Abstract
Introduction
While sexual minority people have been widely considered at risk for developing a range of body image concerns, evidence of body dissatisfaction and shame amongst LGB (lesbian, gay, and bisexual) individuals is mixed. This study investigated differences in body uneasiness, body dissatisfaction, and self-blaming/attacking attitudes between LGB and heterosexual individuals, as well as within LGB groups, while also examining the predictive role of body mass index (BMI).
Methods
A sample of cisgender lesbian women (n = 163), gay men (n = 277), bisexual women (n = 135), bisexual men (n = 39), heterosexual women (n = 398), and heterosexual men (n = 219) completed an online survey assessing different aspects of body image between May and July 2020.
Results
Gay and bisexual men reported greater body image disturbance and self-blaming attitudes relative to heterosexual men. In contrast, lesbian women reported lower body uneasiness than their bisexual and heterosexual counterparts, but greater self-hate. Moreover, lesbian and bisexual women showed more body dissatisfaction than gay men, and bisexual individuals reported more body uneasiness than individuals in other sexual minority subgroups. Higher BMI emerged as a significant predictor of body image concerns and dissatisfaction.
Conclusions
Body image dimensions showed sexual identity–based differences. Determining the specific nuances of body image in LGB individuals can provide important information on potential risk factors that may impact mental health outcomes.
Policy Implications
In-depth knowledge of body dissatisfaction and uneasiness in individuals with LGB identities may have critical implications for the development of personalized prevention and treatment strategies.
Keywords: Body image, Body dissatisfaction, Sexual orientation identity, LGB persons, Body mass index, Objectification
Perceptions, feelings, and thoughts about one’s body can have a significant impact on psychological and social health. Previous studies have outlined that body image is a multidimensional umbrella construct, in terms of both its assessment and its associations with related concepts (Banfield & McCabe, 2002; Cash et al., 2004). Body image disturbance (also referred to as a negative body image) refers primarily to negative experiences related to one’s body weight and shape (Grogan, 2006). It has cognitive-affective, perceptual, and behavioral components, including an excessive emphasis on body weight and/or shape on self-evaluation, poor body size perception accuracy, and repeated body checking (e.g., Glashouwer et al., 2020). While a positive body image has been found to relate to psychological resources such as high self-esteem, a negative body image has been associated with a variety of adverse health outcomes (e.g., Thompson, 2004), including disordered eating behaviors, depression, psychological distress, and poor quality of life (Alvy, 2013; Calzo et al., 2015; Pistella et al., 2019; Stice & Shaw, 2002).
The present study focused on the constructs of body uneasiness and body dissatisfaction, as two of the potential declinations that may accompany body image disturbance (Levitan et al., 2019). Body dissatisfaction reflects discontent about one’s physical appearance (Tatangelo et al., 2016). Body uneasiness, in turn, includes not only dissatisfaction with particular body parts, shapes, or functions, but also a general feeling of uneasiness relating to one’s body or weight, which can lead to avoidance behaviors, compulsive checking behaviors, and detachment and/or estrangement feelings towards the body (Cuzzolaro et al., 2006). The study also considered self-criticizing and self-hating attacking behavior towards the self, as these factors have been found to be associated with shame related to body image (e.g., Ferreira et al., 2019).
Recently, research has begun to explore these issues in social identity groups that have not previously been a focus of body image researchers, including sexual minority adults (Andersen & Swami, 2021). However, the relationship between body image disturbance, gender, and sexual orientation identity (hereinafter referred to as sexual identity) remains controversial. The population of sexual minority people—which includes, but is not limited to, lesbian women, gay men, and bisexual individuals—has been widely considered at risk for developing body dissatisfaction and concerns (Calzo et al., 2017; Dahlenburg et al., 2020; Goldhammer et al., 2019; Mason et al., 2018). Sexual minority individuals also report a greater frequency of disordered eating symptoms (including body image disturbance), relative to their heterosexual counterparts (French et al., 1996; Kamody et al., 2020; Laska et al., 2015; Shearer et al., 2015; Yean et al., 2013).
Assigned gender at birth may represent an additional variable of interest for body image disturbance, as men and women are likely to face different pressures to achieve an ideal body and different levels of weight stigma (Myers & Crowther, 2009). Men tend to strive for a muscular and lean body (Laghi et al., 2013; Schaefer & Thompson, 2018; Tiggemann et al., 2007), whereas women tend to strive for a thin body (e.g., Gordon et al., 2010). Furthermore, several studies have revealed significant differences within men, which are less pronounced in women (Dahlenburg et al., 2020). Of note, similar levels of body dissatisfaction and its correlates have been found in samples of women, regardless of sexual identity, and some investigations have noted that levels of body dissatisfaction in women tend to exceed those in men (Basabas et al., 2019; McGuinness & Taylor, 2016).
Despite increasing interest in the impact of body dissatisfaction and uneasiness in sexual minority individuals, research has primarily focused on gay men, and comparatively fewer studies have been devoted to lesbian women and bisexual individuals (Morrison et al., 2004). Evidence suggests that, compared to heterosexual men, gay men are significantly more likely to experience body image concerns, body dissatisfaction, and body image–related anxiety; to prioritize bodily appearance in appraisals of self-worth; and to strive for thinness (Calzo et al., 2015; Feldman & Meyer, 2007; Levesque & Vichesky, 2006; McClain & Peebles, 2016; Yean et al., 2013). Additionally, gay men with high body dissatisfaction and disordered eating symptoms have been found to have impaired psychological well-being (Gil, 2007; Levesque & Vichesky, 2006), characterized by high self-criticism, low self-esteem, and depressive symptoms (Chaney, 2008; Reilly & Rudd, 2006; Russell & Keel, 2002; Tiggemann et al., 2007; Yelland & Tiggemann, 2003).
Conversely, studies comparing sexual minority and heterosexual women have produced mixed results. Some research has found that, compared with heterosexual women, lesbian women tend to place less emphasis on physical attractiveness (Gettelman & Thompson, 1993; Siever, 1994) and are less likely to have a negative body image, body image concerns, and high body dissatisfaction (Dahlenburg et al., 2020; French et al., 1996; Polimeni et al., 2009; Strong et al., 2000). Furthermore, lesbian sexual identity has been found to predict a positive body image and fewer negative attitudes towards eating and weight (Owens et al., 2003). However, other studies have found no differences between lesbian and heterosexual women regarding body dissatisfaction (e.g., Beren et al., 1996). Two meta-analytic reviews provided evidence that sexual minority and heterosexual women may experience similar levels of body image concerns and body image disturbance. Furthermore, correlates of body image concerns (e.g., sociocultural pressure, high negative affect, low self-esteem) have been found to be generally similar among lesbian, bisexual, and heterosexual women (Mason et al., 2018; Morrison et al., 2004). Several studies have also found equivalent rates of disordered eating symptoms in lesbian, bisexual, and heterosexual women (Feldman & Meyer, 2007; Strong et al., 2000), though some have found a higher rate of such symptoms in heterosexual women (Lakkis et al., 1999; Siever, 1994). Sampling bias and other methodological limitations, such as the wide range of measures employed to evaluate negative body image, may have contributed to these contradictory findings.
Research has also shown that gay men, lesbian women, and heterosexual women tend to have higher levels of negative body image and body dissatisfaction than heterosexual men (Dahlenburg et al., 2020; Peplau et al., 2009). However, only limited research has compared lesbian women and gay men, with preliminary findings suggesting that lesbian women report greater levels of body image disturbance than gay men (Dahlenburg et al., 2020). Furthermore, there is a gap in the literature concerning body image disturbance in bisexual individuals, as very few studies have treated bisexual men and/or women as a specific group for analysis (Feldman & Meyer, 2007; Ryan et al., 2010). Despite the paucity of findings on this topic, current evidence suggests that bisexual individuals, and especially bisexual men, share some of the same body image concerns as other sexual minority men (Filiault et al., 2014).
Body weight and body mass index (BMI) are other under-researched variables in this field. Conceivably, these “objective” indices might influence the “subjective” internalization of weight bias and, in turn, bodily perception (Alvy, 2013). Sexual minority women have been found to report higher BMIs than heterosexual women, and some authors have suggested that they may be more likely to experience elevated rates of body size discrimination and harassment due to social stigmatization of overweight (Bowen et al., 2008; Conron et al., 2010; Johns et al., 2017). Furthermore, body weight and BMI have been found to predict body dissatisfaction in gay men (Frederick & Essayli, 2016; Peplau et al., 2009); however, the findings on lesbian and bisexual women are inconsistent.
Previous efforts to explore sexual identity–based differences in body image have mainly been grounded in sociocultural and objectification theories (Brewster et al., 2014). Sociocultural theories posit that idealized body images or norms differ between sexual minority and heterosexual men, with sexual minority men experiencing higher appearance pressure from peers, partners, the media, and the sexual minority community (Calzo et al., 2017; Jankowski et al., 2014). On the other hand, objectification theory (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997), which was originally developed to account for disordered eating behaviors among women, suggests that societal appearance-related pressure and gender expectations may lead both men and women to view their body as a sexual object, provoking body surveillance behaviors and/or feelings of body-related shame (Gonzales & Blashill, 2021; Matsumoto & Rogers, 2020). Indeed, research has shown that, among gay men, increased sexual objectification may heighten body surveillance, body shame, and restricted eating (Martins et al., 2007; Tiggemann et al., 2007; Wiseman & Moradi, 2010). Among lesbian women, however, the rejection of heteronormative ideals and standards may protect against a negative body image and the development of harmful attitudes around eating and weight (Brown, 1987; Dahlenburg et al., 2020; Owens et al., 2003; Polimeni et al., 2009).
Drawing on the literature, the present study tested the following hypotheses:
(a) There would be significant differences in levels of body image disturbance, body dissatisfaction, and self-criticism/self-hate between heterosexual and sexual minority men. More specifically, gay and bisexual men would show higher overall body uneasiness and its dimensions, as well as greater body dissatisfaction, feelings of inadequacy, and self-hate, compared to heterosexual men (e.g., Dahlenburg et al., 2020; Smith et al., 2011; Yean et al., 2013).
(b) There would be no significant differences in levels of body uneasiness, body dissatisfaction, and self-criticism/self-hate between heterosexual and sexual minority women. Lesbian and bisexual women would show similar levels of body image disturbance and shame compared to heterosexual women (Mason et al., 2018; Morrison et al., 2004).
(c) There would be significant differences between LGB (lesbian, gay, and bisexual) groups. In line with the scarce empirical evidence on sexual minority samples, lesbian women would show more body uneasiness and dissatisfaction compared to gay men (Dahlenburg et al., 2020; Markey et al., 2017).
(d) BMI and body weight would predict levels of body uneasiness, body dissatisfaction, and self-criticism/self-hate in LGB people, even after controlling for age, assigned gender at birth, and sexual identity. Higher BMI and body weight would predict a negative body image and greater feelings of inadequacy and shame, especially in gay men (e.g., Frederick & Essayli, 2016; Peplau et al., 2009).
Methods
Participants and Procedure
Participants met the following inclusion criteria: (a) aged 18 years or older, (b) cisgender, and (c) of Italian nationality. They were asked to respond to an online survey, which was administered over a period of 2 months (May–July 2020). All questionnaires were delivered cross-sectionally through an online Google Forms survey, which was accessible via a designated link and required approximately 30–40 min for completion. All subjects who fulfilled the inclusion criteria were asked to participate in a study on self-image and body representation in LGB people. Information about the study was disseminated via organizations, online forums, listservs, and newsletters geared towards LGBTQ + individuals (by email and posts on websites and social networks), to reach a large number of subjects. All survey items had a forced response, to prevent missing data. Software was used to prevent a single individual from responding to the survey multiple times.
An initial sample of N = 1,248 participants completed the survey. Those who reported a different gender identity than that of their assigned gender (n = 4) and those who identified themselves as transgender woman/man (n = 3) or “other (please specify)” (n = 10) were not included, as such individuals may experience body image and body dissatisfaction uniquely (e.g., Pulice-Farrow et al., 2020; Witcomb et al., 2015). Out of the final study sample of N = 1,231 participants, 163 (13.2%) self-identified as lesbian women, 277 (22.5%) as gay men, 135 (11%) as bisexual women, 39 (3.2%) as bisexual men, 398 (32.3%) as heterosexual women, and 219 (17.8%) as heterosexual men.
Table 1 displays all descriptive characteristics of the study sample. In the overall sample, the mean age was 30.34 years (SD = 10.90). The average BMI was 23.61 kg/m2 and the mean body weight was 68.65 kg (SD = 15.11). All study subjects were White and Italian. The majority of respondents had a high school diploma (N = 440, 35.7%) or bachelor’s degree (N = 364, 29.6%). A slightly lower percentage of participants (N = 262, 21.5%) had a master’s degree, and 127 (10.3%) had a doctoral degree. Only 35 (2.8%) participants had a secondary school degree. Most participants were single (N = 583, 47.3%) or in a stable relationship (N = 508, 41.3%); a minor percentage of respondents were married (N = 113, 9.2%) or separated/divorced (N = 25, 2.1%). Only 2 participants (0.2%) were widowed. In terms of socioeconomic status, 690 (56.1%) declared a medium income level, 267 (21.7%) a low/medium income level, and 219 (17.8%) a medium/high income level; fewer participants declared a low (N = 41, 3.3%) or high (N = 14, 1.1%) income level.
Table 1.
Descriptive Statistics (N = 1,231)
LGB subgroups | Heterosexual subgroups | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Variable | Gay (N = 277) M (SD) |
Lesbian (N = 163) M (SD) |
Bisexual M (N = 39) M (SD) |
Bisexual W (N = 135) M (SD) |
HMa (N = 219) M (SD) |
HWb (N = 398) M (SD) |
Overall sample M (SD) |
Age (years) | 33.84 (12.29) | 27.02 (7.27) | 29.91 (8.84) | 26.56 (7.01) | 33.47 (11.34) | 28.85 (10.12) | 30.34 (10.90) |
BMI (kg/m2) | 23.62 (3.53) | 23.46 (4.50) | 22.71 (4.04) | 23.32 (4.87) | 25.21 (3.74) | 22.94 (4.37) | 23.61 (4.22) |
Body weight | 74.34 (12.87) | 64.28 (13.26) | 72.41 (14.54) | 62.92 (15.04) | 79.68 (13.91) | 62.04 (12.81) | 68.65 (15.11) |
N (%) | N (%) | N (%) | N (%) | N (%) | N (%) | N (%) | |
Marital status | |||||||
Single (never married) | 183 (66.1) | 66 (40.5) | 22 (56.4) | 62 (45.9) | 87 (39.7) | 163 (40.9) | 583 (47.3) |
Partnered | 83 (29.9) | 94 (57.6) | 15 (38.5) | 67 (49.6) | 69 (31.5) | 180 (45.2) | 508 (41.3) |
Married | 10 (3.6) | 2 (1.2) | 2 (5.1) | 3 (2.2) | 54 (24.6) | 42 (10.5) | 113 (9.2) |
Divorced/separated | 1 (0.4) | 1 (0.6) | 0 (0) | 3 (2.2) | 9 (4.1) | 11 (2.7) | 25 (2.1) |
Widowed | 0 (0) | 0 (0) | 0 (0) | 0 (0) | 0 (0) | 2 (0.5) | 2 (0.2) |
Highest education level | |||||||
Secondary school | 4 (1.4) | 5 (3.1) | 2 (5.1) | 3 (2.2) | 16 (7.3) | 5 (1.2) | 35 (2.8) |
High school | 80 (28.9) | 74 (45.4) | 15 (38.4) | 52 (38.5) | 89 (40.6) | 130 (32.6) | 440 (35.7) |
Bachelor degree | 76 (27.4) | 50 (30.7) | 10 (25.6) | 48 (35.5) | 55 (25.1) | 125 (31.4) | 364 (29.6) |
Master degree | 76 (27.4) | 26 (15.9) | 7 (17.9) | 26 (19.2) | 40 (18.2) | 90 (22.6) | 265 (21.5) |
Doctoral degree | 41 (14.8) | 8 (4.9) | 5 (12.8) | 6 (4.4) | 19 (8.7) | 48 (12.1) | 127 (10.3) |
Socioeconomic status | |||||||
Low | 8 (2.8) | 8 (4.9) | 2 (5.1) | 7 (5.2) | 7 (3.2) | 9 (2.3) | 41 (3.3) |
Low/medium | 57 (20.6) | 39 (23.9) | 14 (35.9) | 31(22.9) | 50 (22.8) | 76 (19.1) | 267 (21.7) |
Medium | 151(54.5) | 84 (51.5) | 16 (41.1) | 84 (62.2) | 129 (58.9) | 226 (56.8) | 690 (56.1) |
Medium/high | 53 (19.1) | 32 (19.6) | 6 (15.3) | 13 (9.6) | 30 (13.7) | 85 (21.6) | 219 (17.8) |
High | 8 (2.8) | 0 (0) | 1 (2.5) | 0 (0) | 3 (1.4) | 2 (0.5) | 14 (1.1) |
aHeterosexual men
bHeterosexual women
Significant differences emerged in terms of age (F[5, 1230] = 18.31, p < .001]), BMI (F[5, 1230] = 9.05, p < .001]), and body weight (F[5, 1230] = 68.02, p < .001]). Specifically, post-hoc analyses showed that gay and heterosexual men were older than lesbian, bisexual, and heterosexual women. Heterosexual men also had a higher BMI and body weight compared to all other groups, and gay men had a higher body weight than heterosexual, bisexual, and lesbian women.
The study protocol was reviewed and approved by the local research ethics committee and conducted in accordance with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration. Participation was voluntary. Prior to engaging in the survey, all participants provided electronic informed consent, indicating their understanding of both the study procedures and their right to cease their participation at any time, without penalty.
Measures
Body Uneasiness Test – Form A
The Body Uneasiness Test – Form A (BUT-A; Cuzzolaro et al., 2006) is a 34-item self-report measure of several dimensions of body image in clinical and non-clinical populations, including intense fear of being or becoming fat (e.g., “I’m terrified of putting on weight”), body shape and/or weight dissatisfaction (e.g., “I feel I am fatter than others tell me”), avoidance (e.g., “The thought of some defects of my body torments me so much that it prevents me from being with others”), compulsive control behaviors (e.g., “If I begin to look at myself, I find it difficult to stop”), and detachment and estrangement feelings towards one’s body (e.g., “I have the sensation that my body does not belong to me”). Items are rated on a 6-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (never) to 5 (always). The Global Severity Index (GSI), which represents the average rating of all items, ranges from 0–5, with higher scores indicating greater body uneasiness. In the present study, Cronbach’s α was 0.86 for the GSI, 0.89 for the Weight Phobia (WP) subscale, 0.83 for the Body Image Concerns (BIC) subscale, 0.88 for the Avoidance (A) subscale, 0.78 for the Compulsive Self-Monitoring (CSM) subscale, and 0.86 for the Depersonalization (D) subscale.
Eating Disorder Inventory-3 Referral Form
The Eating Disorder Inventory-3 Referral Form (EDI-3-RF) is an abbreviated version of the original EDI-3 (Garner, 2004), which is a widely used self-report questionnaire for assessing the presence and intensity of psychological traits or symptoms that are clinically relevant to eating disorders in clinical and non-clinical populations. The present study considered the EDI-3-RF Body Dissatisfaction (BD) subscale, which consists of 10 items that explore discontentment with overall body shape and the size of regions of the body that are typically of significant concern to those with eating disorders (i.e., stomach, hips, thighs, buttocks; e.g., “I feel bloated after eating a normal meal”). Compared to previous versions of the scale, the EDI-3-RF has a six-choice format, but scores are recalibrated to a 0–4 format to expand the range of summative scores and improve the psychometric properties with non-clinical populations. The measure has been shown to yield adequate convergent and discriminant validity (Clausen et al., 2011), and all scales included in the EDI-3-RF have been found to have good reliability indices (Garner, 2004). In the present study, Cronbach’s α for the EDI-3-RF Body Dissatisfaction subscale was 0.84.
Forms of Self-Criticizing/Attacking & Self-Reassuring Scale
The Forms of Self-Criticizing/Attacking & Self-Reassuring Scale (FSCRS; Gilbert et al., 2004) is a self-report measure of the tendency to be self-critical and/or self-attacking in response to setbacks or failure. In line with the aims of the present study, which related to problematic attitudes towards one’s body, and in line with studies finding an association between both self-criticizing and self-attacking behaviors and shame related to body image, the present study employed two of the three FSCRS subscales: (a) Inadequate Self, which is comprised of 9 items measuring feelings of personal inadequacy and deficiency (e.g., “I am easily disappointed with myself”); and (b) Hated Self, which is comprised of 5 items that describe self-hating and self-harming behaviors, including those that are directed at the body (e.g., “I have become so angry with myself that I want to hurt or injure myself”). Items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (not at all like me) to 4 (extremely like me). The FSCRS has been shown to have good psychometric properties (Baiᾶo et al., 2015), and it has been previously applied to sexual minority populations (e.g., Petrocchi et al., 2020). In the present study, Cronbach’s α was 0.87 for the Inadequate Self subscale and 0.82 for the Hated Self subscale.
Demographic Information
The survey included a questionnaire that collected demographic data relating to age, assigned gender at birth (i.e., female, male), gender identity (i.e., cisgender woman, cisgender man, transgender woman, transgender man, other), ethnicity, education level, and sexual identity. Respondents indicated their sexual identity by selecting from one of five response options (1 = gay, 2 = lesbian, 3 = bisexual, 4 = heterosexual, 5 = other, please specify). They also provided their height and weight, for the calculation of BMI (measured as kg/m2).
Data Analysis
Power Analyses
A priori power analysis was conducted with G*Power version 3.1.9.7 (Faul et al., 2007), to determine the minimum sample size required for the analyses of principal interest. A multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) in the F-test family was run, with three groups and nine response variables. The input criteria were an error probability of .05, a conventional value of .95 as a threshold power to be reached, and a small effect size (f2) of .10 (Cohen, 1988). The findings showed that the projected minimum sample size was N = 156 participants.
Data Analytic Plan
All statistical analyses were performed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS Version 25) for Windows. First, group differences (in terms of assigned gender at birth and sexual identity) on the BUT-A and FSCRS subscales were analyzed using multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVAs). Subsequently, a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to compare mean body dissatisfaction scores between LGB groups. Furthermore, partial eta squares for effect sizes were calculated. According to Cohen’s (1988) guidelines, partial η2 ≥ 0.01 was treated as a small effect, partial η2 ≥ 0.06 was regarded as a medium effect, and partial η2 ≥ 0.14 was considered a large effect. The Bonferroni test was applied for post-hoc comparisons. Next, hierarchical multiple regressions were conducted to investigate the relevance of BMI and body weight for overall body uneasiness, body dissatisfaction, and self-criticism amongst LGB subgroups, while controlling for participants’ age, assigned gender at birth, and sexual identity. Change in R2 was used to measure the significance of each step (i.e., block). The F test (i.e., F-change) was used to test whether R2 improvement was statistically significant. Partial correlations (i.e., partial r) were also reported to indicate the unique variance in the outcome variable predicted by each independent variable. There was no missing data at the item level, due to the survey’s forced item response.
Results
Comparisons Between Gay, Bisexual, and Heterosexual Men
Table 2 presents the results of the multivariate and univariate analyses of variance and the related effect sizes in the subgroups of gay, bisexual, and heterosexual men. A significant effect for sexual identity was detected for the BUT-A GSI and subscales (Wilks’ lambda = .87; F[2, 533] = 7.52; p < .001, ηp2 = .07), as well as for the FSCRS subscales (Wilks’ lambda = .68, F[2, 533] = 19.91, p < .001, ηp2 = .22). Significant differences also emerged for EDI-3-RF Body Dissatisfaction (F[2, 533] = 8.29, p < .001, ηp2 = .04). Post-hoc comparisons indicated that, compared to heterosexual participants, gay and bisexual men scored significantly higher on the BUT-A GSI and subscales, and EDI-3-RF Body Dissatisfaction.
Table 2.
Differences in Body Image Disturbances between Gay, Bisexual, and Heterosexual Men (N = 535)
Variable | Gay men (n = 277) M (SD) |
Bisexual men (n = 39) M (SD) |
Heterosexual men (n = 219) M (SD) |
F | p | η2 | Post-hoc |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
BUT-Aa | |||||||
Weight Phobia | 1.98 (0.96) | 2.16 (1.09) | 1.13 (0.91) | 36.37 | < .001 | .12 | G = BM > HM |
Body Image Concerns | 1.72 (1.02) | 1.90 (1.28) | 1.04 (0.99) | 22.83 | < .001 | .08 | G = BM > HM |
Compulsive Self-Monitoring | 1.17 (0.89) | 1.32 (0.78) | 0.71 (0.68) | 18.49 | < .001 | .07 | G = BM > HM |
Avoidance | 0.68 (0.67) | 0.95 (0.68) | 0.32 (0.59) | 16.45 | < .001 | .06 | G = BM > HM |
Depersonalization | 0.75 (0.90) | 0.93 (0.99) | 0.35 (0.53) | 18.68 | < .001 | .07 | G = BM > HM |
Global Symptomatic Index | 1.34 (0.98) | 1.54 (1.11) | 0.76 (0.68) | 30.49 | < .001 | .10 | G = BM > HM |
EDI-3-RFb | |||||||
Body Dissatisfaction | 11.37 (5.92) | 10.72 (5.71) | 8.32 (5.45) | 8.29 | < .001 | .04 | G = BM > HM |
FSCRSc | |||||||
Inadequate Self | 4.48 (0.50) | 4.16 (0.65) | 3.37 (0.50) | 43.16 | < .001 | .16 | G > BM > HM |
Hated Self | 4.88 (0.70) | 4.36 (0.87) | 3.56 (0.47) | 45.98 | < .001 | .18 | G > BM > HM |
Gay men also showed greater feelings of inadequacy and self-hate based on FSCRS subscales compared to bisexual men. Bisexual men, in turn, had higher levels of self-criticism than heterosexual men. The greatest effect sizes were found for FSCRS Inadequate Self and Hated Self, and BUT-A Weight Phobia, whereas the smallest effect size was found for EDI-3-RF Body Dissatisfaction.
Comparisons Between Lesbian, Bisexual, and Heterosexual Women
Table 3 displays the results of the multivariate and univariate analyses of variance and the related effect sizes in the subgroups of lesbian, bisexual, and heterosexual women. Similar to the previous results, sexual identity had a significant effect for the BUT-A GSI and subscales (Wilks’ lambda = .94; F[2, 695] = 3.72; p < .001, ηp2 = .03), as well as for the FSCRS subscales (Wilks’ lambda = .92, F[2, 695] = 4.87, p < .001, ηp2 = .05); however, with lower effect sizes than those found in the subgroups of men. Conversely, no significant differences for EDI-3 Body Dissatisfaction were found (F[2, 695] = 1.36, p = ns, ηp2 = .004). More specifically, post-hoc tests revealed that lesbian women scored lower on the BUT-A GSI, Weight Phobia, Body Image Concerns, and Compulsive Self-Monitoring, compared to bisexual and heterosexual women. In turn, bisexual women scored higher on BUT-A Avoidance and Depersonalization than lesbian and heterosexual women. Of note, the FSCRS results were mixed: lesbian women scored lower on Inadequate Self than bisexual women, and bisexual women scored lower on Inadequate Self than heterosexual women; however, lesbian women scored higher on Hated Self than bisexual women, and bisexual women scored higher on Hated Self than heterosexual women. All BUT-A effect sizes were small, whereas all FSCRS effect sizes were medium-large.
Table 3.
Differences in Body Image Disturbances between Lesbian, Bisexual, and Heterosexual Women (N = 696)
Variable | Lesbian women (N = 163) M (SD) |
Bisexual women (N = 135) M (SD) |
Heterosexual women (N = 398) M (SD) |
F | p | η2 | Post-hoc |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
BUT-Aa | |||||||
Weight Phobia | 1.94 (0.99) | 2.44 (1.28) | 2.38 (1.27) | 7.89 | < .001 | .02 | L < BW = HW |
Body Image Concerns | 1.70 (1.33) | 2.16 (1.29) | 1.95 (1.26) | 4.99 | .007 | .01 | L < BW = HW |
Compulsive Self-Monitoring | 1.15 (1.03) | 1.60 (1.10) | 1.40 (1.08) | 6.35 | .002 | .02 | L < BW = HW |
Avoidance | 0.83 (0.72) | 1.11 (0.68) | 0.85 (0.79) | 3.43 | .033 | .01 | BW > L = HW |
Depersonalization | 0.81 (0.98) | 1.22 (1.08) | 0.91 (0.83) | 6.06 | .002 | .02 | BW > L = HW |
Global Symptomatic Index | 1.37 (1.03) | 1.79 (1.04) | 1.59 (1.01) | 6.37 | .002 | .02 | L < BW = HM |
EDI-3-RFb | |||||||
Body Dissatisfaction | 15.15 (6.79) | 16.53 (6.24) | 16.70 (6.10) | 1.36 | .257 | .004 | |
FSCRSc | |||||||
Inadequate Self | 3.31 (0.58) | 4.12 (0.67) | 4.51 (0.61) | 39.22 | < .001 | .14 | L < BW < HW |
Hated Self | 4.98 (0.30) | 4.77 (0.81) | 3.48 (0.65) | 42.32 | < .001 | .16 | L > BW > HW |
Differences Between LGB Subgroups
Table 4 displays the group differences between LGB subgroups. A 3 (sexual identity: lesbian, gay, bisexual) × 2 (assigned gender at birth: man, woman) MANOVA was conducted on body uneasiness (BUT-A GSI and subscales) and self-criticism (FSCRS subscales). The choice of a 3 × 2 design was due to the low number of bisexual men, who were then merged with bisexual women. The analysis revealed a significant effect for sexual identity (Wilks’ lambda = .96; F[2, 614] = 1.90; p = .04, ηp2 = .02), but no significant effect for the assigned gender at birth (Wilks’ lambda = .99; F[2, 614] = .5; p = .75, ηp2 = .004), for the BUT-A subscales. Similarly, a significant effect for sexual identity (Wilks’ lambda = .70; F[2, 614] = 18.6; p < .001, ηp2 = .15), but no significant effect for assigned gender at birth (Wilks’ lambda = 0.98; F[2, 614] = .5; p = .69, ηp2 = .004), was found for the FSCRS subscales.
Table 4.
Differences in Body Image Disturbances among LGB Individuals (N = 614)
Variable | Gay men (N = 277) M (SD) |
Lesbian women (N = 163) M (SD) |
Bisexual individuals (MW) (N = 174) M (SD) |
F | p | η2 | Post-hoc |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
BUT-Aa | |||||||
Weight Phobia | 1.98 (0.96) | 1.94 (0.99) | 2.38 (1.09) | 6.29 | .002 | .02 | BMW > G = L |
Body Image Concerns | 1.72 (1.02) | 1.70 (1.33) | 2.11 (1.28) | 5.81 | .003 | .02 | BMW > G = L |
Compulsive Self-Monitoring | 1.17 (0.89) | 1.15 (1.03) | 1.54 (0.78) | 8.61 | < .001 | .03 | BMW > G = L |
Avoidance | 0.68 (0.67) | 0.83 (0.72) | 1.07 (0.68) | 8.07 | < .001 | .03 | BMW > G = L |
Depersonalization | 0.75 (0.90) | 0.81 (0.98) | 0.97 (0.99) | 6.64 | < .001 | .03 | BMW > G = L |
Global Symptomatic Index | 1.34 (0.98) | 1.37 (1.03) | 1.74 (1.06) | 9.31 | < .001 | .03 | BMW > G = L |
EDI-3-RFb | |||||||
Body Dissatisfaction | 11.37 (5.92) | 15.15 (6.79) | 15.41 (7.20) | 11.99 | < .001 | .04 | L = BMW > G |
FSCRSc | |||||||
Inadequate Self | 4.48 (0.50) | 3.31 (0.58) | 4.13 (0.65) | 38.16 | < .001 | .13 | G > BMW > L |
Hated Self | 4.88 (0.70) | 4.98 (0.30) | 4.69 (0.87) | 35.16 | < .001 | .11 | L > BMW > G |
Significant differences also emerged with respect to EDI-3-RF Body Dissatisfaction (F[2, 614] = 11.99, < .001, ηp2 = .04). Post-hoc comparisons showed that bisexual individuals scored higher on all BUT-A subscales and the GSI compared to lesbian and gay participants, but with small effect sizes. With respect to EDI-3-RF Body Dissatisfaction, lesbian and bisexual participants scored higher than gay men, with small effect sizes. Similar to previous analyses, the FSCRS findings were mixed: gay men scored higher on Inadequate Self than bisexual individuals, and bisexual individuals scored higher on Inadequate Self than lesbian women; however, lesbian women scored higher on Hated Self than bisexual individuals and gay men. The FSCRS effect sizes were medium.
The Predictive Role of BMI and Body Weight Among Sexual Minority Subgroups
A series of four hierarchical multiple regression analyses were performed to assess the role of BMI and body weight in predicting overall body uneasiness (BUT-A GSI), EDI-3-RF Body Dissatisfaction, and FSCRS Self-Criticism in LGB individuals (see Table 5). Age was entered in the first step; assigned gender at birth (i.e., man, woman) and sexual identity (i.e., lesbian/gay, bisexual) were entered in the second step; and body weight and BMI were entered in the third and final step.
Table 5.
Hierarchical Regression Analyses for BMI and Body Weight Predicting Body Uneasiness, Body Dissatisfaction, and Self-Criticism in LGB Subgroups (N = 614)
R | R2 | β | F-change (Model) |
p | Partial r |
|
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Criterion variable: BUT-A GSIa | ||||||
Step 1 | .224 | .050 | 31.988 | < .001 | ||
Age | −0.224 | −.230*** | ||||
Step 2 | .266 | .071 | 6.606 | .001 | ||
Assigned gender (man = 0, woman = 1) | 0.048 | .008 | ||||
Sexual orientation (bisexual = 0, gay/lesbian = 1) | −0.142 | −.158*** | ||||
Step 3 | .426 | .182 | 40.792 | < .001 | ||
Body weight | −0.172 | −.072 | ||||
BMI (kg/m2) | 0.473 | .208*** | ||||
Criterion variable: EDI-3 BDb | ||||||
Step 1 | .185 | .034 | 21.500 | < .001 | ||
Age | −0.185 | −.157*** | ||||
Step 2 | .258 | .067 | 10.485 | < .001 | ||
Assigned gender (man = 0, woman = 1) | 0.037 | .052 | ||||
Sexual orientation (bisexual = 0, gay/lesbian = 1) | −0.164 | −.132*** | ||||
Step 3 | .491 | .241 | 68.893 | < .001 | ||
Body weight | −0.083 | −.036 | ||||
BMI (kg/m2) | 0.487 | .221*** | ||||
Criterion variable: FSCRS ISc | ||||||
Step 1 | .141 | .020 | 12.357 | < .001 | ||
Age | 0.141 | .002 | ||||
Step 2 | .614 | .377 | 77.507 | < .001 | ||
Assigned gender (man = 0, woman = 1) | −0.473 | −.396*** | ||||
Sexual orientation (bisexual = 0, gay/lesbian = 1) | −0.334 | −.386*** | ||||
Step 3 | .620 | .379 | 3.513 | .030 | ||
Body weight | −0.014 | −.007 | ||||
BMI (kg/m2) | −0.073 | −.038 | ||||
Criterion variable: FSCRS ISd | ||||||
Step 1 | .263 | .069 | 26.782 | < .001 | ||
Age | −0.263 | −.086* | ||||
Step 2 | .625 | .416 | 79.367 | < .001 | ||
Assigned gender (man = 0, woman = 1) | 0.497 | .428*** | ||||
Sexual orientation (bisexual = 0, gay/lesbian = 1) | 0.167 | .132** | ||||
Step 3 | .628 | .417 | 1.198 | .257 | ||
Body weight | −0.005 | −.002 | ||||
BMI (kg/m2) | −0.043 | −.018 |
*p ≤ .05; **p ≤ .01; *** p ≤ .001
aBody Uneasiness Scale – A, Global Symptomatic Index (Cuzzolaro et al., 2006)
bEating Disorder Inventory-3 Referral Form, Body Dissatisfaction subscale (Garner, 2004)
cForms of Self-Criticizing/Attacking & Self-Reassuring Scale, Inadequate Self and Hated Self subscales (Gilbert et al., 2004). Partial correlations reported for the third and last step, in which all predictors were included in the regression model
In the final step, younger age (partial r = −.23, p < .001, and partial r = −.16, p < .001, respectively), sexual identity (partial r = −.16, p < .001, and partial r = −.13, p = .001, respectively), and higher BMI (partial r = .21, p < .001, and partial r = .22, p <0.001, respectively) emerged as significant predictors of the BUT-A GSI and EDI-3-RF Body Dissatisfaction, whereas assigned gender and body weight showed no significant effect. The final model explained 18.2% of the variance in overall body uneasiness, as assessed by the BUT-A GSI, and 24.1% of the variance in overall body dissatisfaction, as evaluated by EDI-3-RF Body Dissatisfaction.
Furthermore, in the last step, only assigned gender (partial r = −.39, p < .001) and sexual identity (partial r = −.38, p < .001) emerged as negative predictors of FSCRS Inadequate Self. On the other hand, assigned gender (partial r = .43, p < .001) and sexual identity (partial r = .13, p = .001) emerged as positive predictors of FSCRS Hated Self. The final models explained 37.9% and 41.7% of the variance in FSCRS Inadequate and Hated Self, respectively.
Discussion
In-depth knowledge of the impact of body dissatisfaction and uneasiness among LGB individuals may have critical implications for the prevention and treatment of eating disorders, dysfunctional eating behaviors, and psychological distress among these populations (Thompson, 2000). The present results confirmed the first hypothesis, as gay and bisexual men displayed greater body and weight dissatisfaction, avoidance, compulsive control behaviors, detachment and estrangement feelings towards their body, and body uneasiness relative to heterosexual individuals. These findings are aligned with previous research suggesting that LGB persons—especially gay men—are more likely to experience body image concerns and may be less accurate in their body weight estimations, compared to heterosexual men (Dahlenburg et al., 2020; Morrison et al., 2004; Peplau et al., 2009; Russel & Keel, 2002). The literature suggests that men who are dissatisfied with their body are more likely to report disordered eating patterns, poorer health-related quality of life, greater psychological distress, and lower self-esteem (Bergeron & Tylka, 2007; Tylka, 2011; Wilson et al., 2013). These findings seem particularly relevant to gay men, who have been found to report greater interest in body modification strategies, as well as more pronounced appearance-related pressure and social comparison (Frederick & Essayli, 2016).
Similar results emerged with respect to self-attitude measures, with gay men reporting significantly greater feelings of inadequacy and self-hate than their heterosexual counterparts. As Gilbert et al. (2012) suggested, self-criticism and self-hate are highly associated with shame (Lingiardi et al., 2017), and their dysfunctional effects in LGB persons may derive from related self-directed negative emotions, including anger, disgust, and contempt (Petrocchi et al., 2020), which may also be focused on physical appearance and body shape. Interestingly, gay men also reported higher levels of self-criticism and self-hate compared to bisexual men—who, in turn, reported higher levels of self-blame and self-attack than heterosexual men. Despite the paucity of studies devoted explicitly to the self-attitudes and bodily representations of bisexual men, these results seem to partially diverge from previous research showing no differences between gay and bisexual men (e.g., Bridge et al., 2019). This might be explained by the small sample of bisexual men in the present study (discussed further below).
Sociocultural and objectification theories (Brewster et al., 2014) may provide a potential interpretation of the differences between men, in that several studies have found associations between sexual objectification experiences, internalization of body ideals, body surveillance, body shame, and self-criticism in sexual minority men (e.g., Wiseman & Moradi, 2010). Although some authors have suggested that heterosexual men also internalize body ideals, and are thus potentially subject to self-objectification (Daniel & Bridges, 2010; Martins et al., 2007), most studies have reported that sexual minority men are significantly more affected by this internalization (Duggan & McCreary, 2004; Hobza et al., 2007; Martins et al., 2007; Matsumoto & Rodgers, 2020). Minority stress theory (Meyer, 2003) may offer an additional framework for understanding how sexual minority men experience their bodies within the social environment (Mason & Lewis, 2016; Siconolfi et al., 2016), as the experience and internalization of negative feelings, attitudes, beliefs, behaviors, and assumptions about one’s sexual identity may have relevant consequences for self-perception, at both the identity and the bodily level (Balsam, 2001; Herek, 2000; Meyer, 1995; Rostosky et al., 2007).
Concerning the second hypothesis, the results were mixed. In line with expectations, no group differences were found in body dissatisfaction, as measured by the EDI-3-RF. This result is aligned with Morrison et al.’s (2004) meta-analysis, which found a non-significant effect size for the comparison of heterosexual and lesbian women on body satisfaction. However, with respect to the BUT-A subscales, lesbian women scored lower on Weight Phobia, Body Shape and Weight Dissatisfaction, Estrangement Feelings, Compulsive Control Behaviors, and Overall Body Uneasiness than bisexual and heterosexual women. These findings support the meta-analysis of Dahlenburg et al. (2020), which found that heterosexual women reported more significant body image disturbances compared to lesbian women across all measures considered, even with small effect sizes. They also align with evidence suggesting that lesbian women tend to have fewer disordered eating symptoms than heterosexual women (Lakkis et al., 1999; Strong et al., 2000). The present findings for lesbian women may be explained by a combination of objectification (Fredrikson & Roberts, 1997) and protective (Brown, 1987) theories: whereas women might feel pressurized to meet unrealistic standards of attractiveness, the lesbian subculture’s characteristic rejection of heteronormative standards and ideals may provide some protection against body image disturbances (Beck, 2017), body dissatisfaction (Alvy, 2013; Polimeni et al., 2009), negative attitudes towards weight (Owens et al., 2003), and negative appearance pressure (Hazzard et al., 2019) amongst lesbian women.
Similar findings emerged for FSCRS Self-Criticism and Self-Blaming Attitudes, with lesbian women reporting lower feelings of inadequacy relative to bisexual women—who, in turn, reported lower feelings of inadequacy relative to heterosexual women. Some authors have suggested that lesbian women’s lower feelings of inadequacy may be explained by their generally higher BMIs and preference for curvier figures (Alvy, 2013). However, in the present study, lesbian women also reported higher levels of self-hate and self-attack than bisexual and heterosexual women. Previous research with lesbian women has found that sexual identity–based discrimination is associated with less social support from the family, which, in turn, is related to increased negative affect, social anxiety, and disordered eating symptoms (Bell et al., 2019; Mason et al., 2018). Furthermore, shame and self-hate—primarily related to internalized homophobia—have been shown to be central psychological characteristics of disordered eating symptoms and binge behaviors in lesbian and bisexual women (Bayer et al., 2017).
The present results partially confirmed the third hypothesis, showing that lesbian and bisexual women tended to have more body dissatisfaction than gay men, as well as higher levels of self-hate and self-attack. Dahlenburg et al. (2020) found that, relative to gay men, lesbian women were more prone to experiencing body image disorders and to perceive themselves as physically unattractive (Markey et al., 2017; Swami, 2009). Although there have been very few studies on this topic, the present findings suggest that, while lesbian women may experience fewer body image disturbances than heterosexual women, they may still be influenced by societal pressure to achieve a certain body shape, and subsequently (consciously or subconsciously) experience higher self-objectification and body dissatisfaction relative to their male counterparts.
Nevertheless, contrary to expectations, bisexual individuals, irrespective of their assigned gender at birth, reported more body uneasiness than lesbian women and gay men. Lesbian women and gay men, in turn, reported similar levels of body uneasiness. Studies have shown that bisexual individuals with a same-sex partner tend to report more body image concerns than their heterosexual counterparts (Austin et al., 2004; Hadland et al., 2014). Furthermore, research has found that bisexual people typically report poorer mental health than gay/lesbian people (Conron et al., 2010; Herek, 2007; Kerr et al., 2013; Pistella et al., 2016), probably due to their greater difficulty achieving community and social support, which may hinder the emergence of positive identity dimensions (Baiocco et al., 2018; Petrocchi et al., 2020), even at a bodily level. However, more research on body image in bisexual populations is needed to support these explanations.
Concerning the fourth and final hypothesis, the results showed that younger age, bisexual identity, and higher BMI were significant predictors of the BUT-A GSI and overall body dissatisfaction. Body weight showed no significant effect. These findings are aligned with previous research suggesting that body concerns and dissatisfaction are related to elevated BMI, especially in samples with a diagnosed eating disorder (e.g., Muzi et al., 2020, 2021; Stice & Shaw, 2002). They also support studies confirming BMI as a predictor of weight-related concerns and weight management behaviors, even in non-clinical populations (Markey & Markey, 2005). Despite these observations, the present results and previous findings on the role of BMI in LGB populations are mixed. In fact, contrary to expectations, BMI did not emerge as a significant predictor of self-blame and self-attack. For instance, Frederick and Essayli (2016) found that, although BMI was a strong predictor of body dissatisfaction in sexual minority men, it did not consistently moderate the association between sexual identity and body image. Furthermore, Alvy (2013) suggested that BMI may represent a significant confounding factor in studies of body image in LGB populations, due to differences in self-reported versus directly measured values of height and weight and higher rates of obesity among lesbian women (e.g., Boehmer et al., 2007; Richmond et al., 2012).
Despite these results, the present study suffered from several limitations. First, the design was cross-sectional in nature and restricted to heterosexual and LGB individuals. Although the sample size was adequate, variables related to ethnicity, culture, and nationality were not measured. This limitation is particularly relevant, given the potential effects of ethnic and cultural representations of body ideals, body discrimination, and body image within LGB subgroups (e.g., Deputy & Boehmer, 2014; Gonzales & Blashill, 2021). Moreover, in the present study, the subsample of bisexual men was significantly smaller than the other subsamples. Thus, future research should seek to improve the present investigation by including larger, representative samples—especially with regard to bisexual individuals—and applying more sophisticated measures of sexual orientation.
Third, the data were collected through online sampling techniques, using only self-report measures. This may have generated different results relative to a research design using traditional in-person methodology and a multi-informant approach. However, a recent meta-analysis showed that online-based samples provide comparable results to those generated through in-person methods, suggesting that these methodologies do not significantly differ (Walter et al., 2019). Another limitation pertains to the data collection period, which immediately proceeded the most severe phases of the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic. Several studies have shown the negative consequences of the pandemic and its associated restrictive measures on psychological well-being and mental health, including disordered eating symptoms (Pierce et al., 2020; Prati & Mancini, 2021; Wu et al., 2021). While future studies should consider the potential long-term effect of the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic on body image concerns and disordered eating symptoms in LGB populations, a recent meta-analysis suggested that the pandemic’s adverse effects on eating were highly heterogeneous and challenging to control in empirical research (Sideli et al., 2021). Finally, the present study focused on cisgender LGB populations in comparison with a cisgender heterosexual population. Future studies should also investigate body image, body image disturbances, and internalized stigma among gender minority populations in comparison with a cisgender population, as previous research has shown that transgender and other gender minority individuals may experience significant body image disorders and concerns (e.g., Diemer et al., 2015). Furthermore, the representativeness of other gender and sexual minorities (e.g., queer, pansexual, asexual) should be enhanced, to generate better insight into the issues faced by the broader LGBTQ + community and to limit the dominance of majority voices (often gay men) in empirical studies on this topic (Salvati & Koc, 2022). Future prevention-oriented studies should also include measures of internalized sexual stigma and homonegativity (e.g., Lingiardi et al., 2012), in line with research suggesting a relationship between internalized homophobia and body shame, body image disorders, disordered eating attitudes, and binge eating in sexual minority individuals (e.g., Bayer et al., 2017; Williamson & Spence, 2001).
Social Policy Implications
In-depth investigations into body image and body image disturbances in LGB individuals may generate significant clinical implications for interventions to treat and prevent a wide range of adverse health consequences (Calzo et al., 2015; Pistella et al., 2019). This is particularly relevant, as sexual minorities are exposed to unique stressors (i.e., minority stress in its various forms) that inevitably impact health outcomes (Hatzenbuehler & Pachankis, 2016; Kelleher, 2009; Lingiardi & Capozzi, 2004). In addition to being significantly associated with the onset of eating disorders (e.g., Bell et al., 2019; Simone et al., 2020), body image concerns and body dissatisfaction have also been found to predict the risk of bullying victimization, social discrimination, and diminished quality of life (Kamody et al., 2020; Lingiardi et al., 2020; Meyer, 2012).
From a wider perspective, prejudices and stereotyping attitudes against LGBTQ + people, with respect to body image and shape, are still widespread, both outside and within the LGBTQ + community, leading to potentially harmful consequences (Salvati et al., 2020). For instance, some studies have found that perceived stigma towards one’s body and physical appearance in same-sex couples is associated with self-doubt, sexual dissatisfaction, disempowerment, and lifestyle changes (Goldsmith & Byers, 2016; Markey & Markey, 2014; Markey et al., 2017). Other potential consequences of body dissatisfaction and uneasiness in sexual minority individuals might include an increased use of dating apps (e.g., Breslow et al., 2020), more romantic rejection (Foster-Gimbel & Engeln, 2016), and less professional success (Carels et al., 2022). Untangling the specific nuances of body image concerns and body-related experiences in individuals with different sexual identities and orientations may lead to the development of tailored, person-centered resources to help sexual minority individuals feel more positive about their bodies (Dahlenburg et al., 2020).
Funding
Open access funding provided by Università degli Studi di Perugia within the CRUI-CARE Agreement. The authors declare that no funds, grants, or other support were received during the preparation of this manuscript.
Availability of Data and Material
The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Code Availability
All reasonable requests for code relevant to the analyses of this paper will be met.
Declarations
Ethics Approval
All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and national research committee (Research Ethics Committee of the Department of Dynamic and Clinical Psychology, and Health Studies, Sapienza University of Rome; Approval document reference number: 0000398) and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. This article does not contain any studies with animals performed by any of the authors.
Informed Consent
Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
Conflicts of Interest/Competing Interests
The authors have no relevant financial or non-financial interests to disclose.
Footnotes
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- Alvy LM. Do lesbian women have a better body image? Comparisons with heterosexual women and model of lesbian-specific factors. Body Image. 2013;10(4):524–534. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2013.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersen N, Swami V. Science mapping research on body image: A bibliometric review of publications in Body Image, 2004–2020. Body Image. 2021;38:106–119. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2021.03.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Austin SB, Ziyadeh N, Kahn JA, Camargo CA, Jr, Colditz GA, Field AE. Sexual orientation, weight concerns, and eating-disordered behaviors in adolescent girls and boys. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2004;43(9):1115–1123. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000131139.93862.10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baião R, Gilbert P, McEwan K, Carvalho S. Forms of self-criticising/attacking & self-reassuring scale: Psychometric properties and normative study. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice. 2015;88(4):438–452. doi: 10.1111/papt.12049. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baiocco R, Salvati M, Carone N, Ioverno S, Nappa MR, Pistella J. Identità positiva in persone lesbiche, gay e bisessuali: Un contributo alla validazione italiana della Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual Positive Identity Measure (LGB-PIM) Giornale Italiano Di Psicologia. 2018;45(4):953–978. doi: 10.1421/93010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Balsam KF. Nowhere to hide: Lesbian battering, homophobia, and minority stress. Women & Therapy. 2001;23(3):25–37. doi: 10.1300/J015v23n03_03. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Banfield SS, McCabe M. An evaluation of the construct of body image. Adolescence. 2002;37(146):373–393. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Basabas MC, Greaves L, Barlow FK, Sibley CG, Carmela M, Greaves L, Barlow FK, Chris G. Sexual orientation moderates the effect of gender on body satisfaction: Results from a national probability sample. Journal of Sex Research. 2019;56(9):1091–1100. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2019.1667947. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bayer V, Robert-Mccomb JJ, Clopton JR, Reich DA. Investigating the influence of shame, depression, and distress tolerance on the relationship between internalized homophobia and binge eating in lesbian and bisexual women. Eating Behaviors. 2017;24:39–44. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2016.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck, C. L. (2017). Lesbian body dissatisfaction: The roles of gender identity, body-gender identity incongruence, and internalized appearance ideals. https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_graddiss/4604
- Bell, K., Rieger, E., & Hirsch, J. K. (2019). Eating disorder symptoms and proneness in gay men, lesbian women, and transgender and gender non-conforming adults: Comparative levels and a proposed mediational model. Frontiers in Psychology, 2692. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02692 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Beren SE, Hayden HA, Wilfley DE, Grilo CM. The influence of sexual orientation on body dissatisfaction in adult men and women. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 1996;20:135–141. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(199609)20:2<135::AID-EAT3>3.0.CO;2-H. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bergeron D, Tylka TL. Support for the uniqueness of body dissatisfaction from drive for muscularity among men. Body Image. 2007;4(3):288–295. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2007.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boehmer U, Bowen DJ, Bauer GR. Overweight and obesity in sexual-minority women: Evidence from population-based data. American Journal of Public Health. 2007;97(6):1134–1140. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2006.088419. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bowen DJ, Balsam KF, Ender SR. A review of obesity issues in sexual minority women. Obesity. 2008;16(2):221–228. doi: 10.1038/oby.2007.34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brewster ME, Velez BL, Esposito J, Wong S, Geiger E, Keum BT. Moving beyond the binary with disordered eating research: A test and extension of objectification theory with bisexual women. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2014;61:50–62. doi: 10.1037/a0034748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Breslow AS, Sandil R, Brewster ME, Parent MC, Chan A, Yucel A, Bensmiller N, Glaeser E. Adonis on the apps: Online objectification, self-esteem, and sexual minority men. Psychology of Men & Masculinities. 2020;21(1):25–35. doi: 10.1037/men0000202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bridge L, Smith P, Rimes KA. Sexual orientation differences in the self-esteem of men and women: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychology of Sexual Orientation and Gender Diversity. 2019;6(4):433–446. doi: 10.1037/sgd0000342. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Brown, L. S. (1987). Lesbians, weight, and eating: New analyses and perspectives. Lesbian Psychologies: Explorations and Challenges, 294–309.
- Calzo JP, Blashill AJ, Brown TA, Argenal RL. Eating disorders and disordered weight and shape control behaviors in sexual minority populations. Current Psychiatry Reports. 2017;19(8):49. doi: 10.1007/s11920-017-0801-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calzo JP, Masyn KE, Corliss HL, Scherer EA, Field AE, Austin SB. Patterns of body image concerns and disordered weight- and shape-related behaviors in heterosexual and sexual minority adolescent males. Developmental Psychology. 2015;51(9):1216–1225. doi: 10.1037/dev0000027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carels RA, Miller JC, Shonrock AT, Byrd R, Sall KE, Carraway M. Weight stigma among heterosexual and sexual minority individuals: Dating and hiring preferences. Stigma and Health. 2022;7(4):481–490. doi: 10.1037/sah0000382. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cash TF, Phillips KA, Santos MT, Hrabosky JL. Measuring “negative body image”: Validation of the Body Image Disturbance Questionnaire in a nonclinical population. Body Image. 2004;1:363–372. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2004.10.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Chaney MP. Muscle dysmorphia, self-esteem, and loneliness among gay and bisexual men. International Journal of Men’s Health. 2008;7(2):157–170. doi: 10.3149/jmh.0702.157. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Clausen L, Rosenvinge JH, Friborg O, Rokkedal K. Validating the Eating Disorder Inventory-3 (EDI-3): A comparison between 561 female eating disorders patients and 878 females from the general population. Journal of Psychopathological Behavioral Assessment. 2011;33:101–110. doi: 10.1007/s10862-010-9207-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen S. Psychosocial models of the role of social support in the etiology of physical disease. Health Psychology. 1988;7(3):269–297. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.7.3.269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conron KJ, Mimiaga MJ, Landers SJ. A population-based study of sexual orientation identity and gender differences in adult health. American Journal of Public Health. 2010;100(10):1953–1960. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2009.174169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cuzzolaro M, Vetrone G, Marano G, Garfinkel PE. The Body Uneasiness Test (BUT): Development and validation of a new body image assessment scale. Eating and Weight Disorders. 2006;11(1):1–13. doi: 10.1007/BF03327738. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dahlenburg SC, Gleaves DH, Hutchinson AD, Coro DG. Body image disturbance and sexual orientation: An updated systematic review and meta-analysis. Body Image. 2020;35:126–141. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2020.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daniel S, Bridges SK. The drive for muscularity in men: Media influences and objectification theory. Body Image. 2010;7(1):32–38. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2009.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deputy NP, Boehmer U. Weight status and sexual orientation: Differences by age and within racial and ethnic subgroups. American Journal of Public Health. 2014;104:103–109. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2013301391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diemer EW, Grant JD, Munn-Chernoff MA, Patterson DA, Duncan AE. Gender identity, sexual orientation, and eating-related pathology in a national sample of college students. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2015;57(2):144–149. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2015.03.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duggan SJ, McCreary DR. Body image, eating disorders, and the drive for muscularity in gay and heterosexual men: The influence of media images. Journal of Homosexuality. 2004;47:45–58. doi: 10.1300/J082v47n0303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods. 2007;39:175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldman MB, Meyer IH. Eating disorders in diverse lesbian, gay, and bisexual populations. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2007;40(3):218–226. doi: 10.1002/eat.20360. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferreira C, Dias B, Oliveira S. Behind women’s body image-focused shame: Exploring the role of fears of compassion and self-criticism. Eating Behaviors. 2019;32:12–17. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2018.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Filiault SM, Drummond MJN, Anderson E. Bisexual men and body image. Psychology & Sexuality. 2014;5(3):191–200. doi: 10.1080/19419899.2012.702124. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Foster-Gimbel O, Engeln R. Fat chance! Experiences and expectations of antifat bias in the gay male community. Psychology of Sexual Orientation and Gender Diversity. 2016;3(1):63–70. doi: 10.1037/sgd0000159. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Frederick DA, Essayli JH. Male body image: The roles of sexual orientation and body mass index across five national U.S. studies. Psychology of Men and Masculinity. 2016;17(4):336–351. doi: 10.1037/men0000031. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fredrickson BL, Roberts T-A. Objectification theory: Toward understanding women’s lived experiences and mental health risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 1997;21(2):173–206. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6402.1997.tb00108.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- French SA, Story M, Remafedi G, Resnick MD, Blum RW. Sexual orientation and prevalence of body dissatisfaction and eating disordered behaviors: A population-based study of adolescents. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 1996;19(2):119–126. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-108X(199603)19:2<119::AID-EAT2>3.0.CO;2-Q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garner, D. M. (2004). Eating Disorder Inventory-3: Professional manual. Psychological Assessment Resources.
- Gettelman TE, Thompson JK. Actual differences and stereotypical perceptions in body-image and eating disturbance: A comparison of male and female heterosexual and homosexual samples. Sex Roles. 1993;29:545–562. doi: 10.1007/BF00289327. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gil S. Body image, well-being and sexual satisfaction: A comparison between heterosexual and gay men. Sexual and Relationship Therapy. 2007;22:237–244. doi: 10.1080/14681990600855042. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert P, Clarke M, Hempel S, Miles J, Irons C. Criticizing and reassuring oneself: An exploration of forms, styles and reasons in female students. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2004;43:31–50. doi: 10.1348/014466504772812959. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert P, McEwan K, Gibbons L, Chotai S, Duarte J, Matos M. Fears of compassion and happiness in relation to alexithymia, mindfulness, and self-criticism. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice. 2012;85:374–390. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.2011.02046.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glashouwer KA, Timmerman J, de Jong PJ. A personalized approach-avoidance modification intervention to reduce negative body image. A placebo-controlled pilot study. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 2020;68:101544. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2019.101544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldhammer HB, Maston ED, Keuroghlian AS. Addressing eating disorders and body dissatisfaction in sexual and gender minority youth. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2019;56(2):318–322. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2018.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldsmith KM, Byers ES. Perceived impact of body feedback from romantic partners on young adults’ body image and sexual well-being. Body Image. 2016;17:161–170. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gonzales M, Blashill AJ. Ethnic/racial and gender differences in body image disorders among a diverse sample of sexual minority U.S. adults. Body Image. 2021;36:64–73. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2020.10.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gordon KH, Castro Y, Sitnikov L, Holm-Denoma JM. Cultural body shape ideals and eating disorder symptoms among White, Latina, and Black college women. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology. 2010;16(2):135–143. doi: 10.1037/a0018671. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grogan S. Body image and health: Contemporary perspectives. Journal of Health Psychology. 2006;11(4):523–530. doi: 10.1177/1359105306065013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hadland SE, Austin SB, Goodenow CS, Calzo JP. Weight misperception and unhealthy weight control behaviors among sexual minorities in the general adolescent population. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2014;54(3):296–303. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2013.08.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hatzenbuehler ML, Pachankis JE. Stigma and minority stress as social determinants of health among lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender youth: Research evidence and clinical implications. Pediatric Clinics of North America. 2016;63(6):985–997. doi: 10.1016/j.pcl.2016.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hazzard VM, Schaefer LM, Schaumberg K, Bardone-Cone AM, Frederick DA, Klump KL, Anderson DA, Thompson JK. Testing the tripartite influence model among heterosexual, bisexual, and lesbian women. Body Image. 2019;30:145–149. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2019.07.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herek GM. The psychology of sexual prejudice. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2000;9(1):19–22. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00051. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Herek GM. Confronting sexual stigma and prejudice: Theory and practice. Journal of Social Issues. 2007;63(4):905–925. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2007.00544.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hobza CL, Walker KE, Yakushko O, Peugh JL. What about men? Social comparison and the effects of media images on body and self-esteem. Psychology of Men & Masculinity. 2007;8(3):161–172. doi: 10.1037/1524-9220.8.3.161. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Jankowski GS, Diedrichs PC, Halliwell E. Can appearance conversations explain differences between gay and heterosexual men’s body dissatisfaction? Psychology of Men & Masculinity. 2014;15(1):68–77. doi: 10.1037/a0031796. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Johns MM, Zimmerman M, Harper GW, Bauermeister JA. Resilient minds and bodies: Size discrimination, body image, and mental health among sexual minority women. Psychology of Sexual Orientation and Gender Diversity. 2017;4(1):34–42. doi: 10.1037/sgd0000207. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kamody RC, Grilo CM. Disparities in DSM-5 defined eating disorders by sexual orientation among U. S. adults. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2020;53(2):278–287. doi: 10.1002/eat.23193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelleher C. Minority stress and health: Implications for lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender, and questioning (LGBTQ) young people. Counselling Psychology Quarterly. 2009;22(4):373–379. doi: 10.1080/09515070903334995. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kerr DL, Santurri L, Peters P. A comparison of lesbian, bisexual, and heterosexual college undergraduate women on selected mental health issues. Journal of American College Health. 2013;61(4):185–194. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2013.787619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laghi F, Magistro V, Guarino A, Baumgartner E, Baiocco R. Variables associated with muscle dysmorphia in gay male adolescents. Psicologia Clinica Dello Sviluppo. 2013;17(3):429–448. doi: 10.1449/76227. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lakkis J, Ricciardelli LA, Williams RJ. Role of sexual orientation and gender-related traits in disordered eating. Sex Roles. 1999;41(1–2):1–16. doi: 10.1023/A:1018829506907. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Laska MN, VanKim NA, Erickson DJ, Lust K, Eisenberg ME, Rosser BRS. Disparities in weight and weight behaviors by sexual orientation in college students. American Journal of Public Health. 2015;105:111–121. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2014.302094. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levesque MJ, Vichesky DR. Raising the bar on the body beautiful: An analysis of the body image concerns of homosexual men. Body Image. 2006;3(1):45–55. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2005.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levitan J, Quinn-Nilas C, Milhausen R, Breuer R. The relationship between body image and sexual functioning among gay and bisexual men. Journal of Homosexuality. 2019;66(13):1856–1881. doi: 10.1080/00918369.2018.1519301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lingiardi V, Capozzi P. Psychoanalytic attitudes towards homosexuality: An empirical research. International Journal of Psychoanalysis. 2004;85(1):137–157. doi: 10.1516/002075704322798987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lingiardi V, Baiocco R, Nardelli N. Measure of Internalized Sexual Stigma for Lesbians and Gay Men: A new scale. Journal of Homosexuality. 2012;59(8):1191–1210. doi: 10.1080/00918369.2012.712850. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lingiardi V, Carone N, Semeraro G, Musto C, D’Amico M, Brena S. Mapping Twitter hate speech towards social and sexual minorities: A lexicon-based approach to semantic content analysis. Behaviour & Information Technology. 2020;39(7):711–721. doi: 10.1080/0144929X.2019.1607903. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lingiardi V, McWilliams N, Muzi L. The contribution of Sidney Blatt’s two-polarities model to the Psychodynamic Diagnostic Manual. Research in Psychotherapy: Psychopathology, Process and Outcome. 2017;20(1):242. doi: 10.4081/ripppo.2017.242. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markey CH, Gillen MM, August KJ, Markey PM, Nave CS. Does “body talk” improve body satisfaction among same-sex couples? Body Image. 2017;23:103–108. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2017.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Markey CN, Markey PM. Gender, sexual orientation, and romantic partner influence on body image: An examination of heterosexual and lesbian women and their partners. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2014;31(2):162–177. doi: 10.1177/0265407513489472. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Markey PM, Markey CN. Relations between body image and dieting behaviors: an examination of gender differences. Sex Roles. 2005;53:519–530. doi: 10.1007/s11199-005-7139-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Martins Y, Tiggemann M, Kirkbride A. Those speedos become them: The role of self-objectification in gay and heterosexual men’s body image. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2007;33:634–647. doi: 10.1177/0146167206297403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mason TB, Lewis RJ. Minority stress, body shame, and binge eating among lesbian women: Social anxiety as a linking mechanism. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 2016;40(3):428–440. doi: 10.1177/0361684316635529. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mason TB, Lewis RJ, Heron KE. Disordered eating and body image concerns among sexual minority women: A systematic review and testable model. Psychology of Sexual Orientation and Gender Diversity. 2018;5(4):397–422. doi: 10.1037/sgd0000293. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Matsumoto A, Rodgers RF. A review and integrated theoretical model of the development of body image and eating disorders among midlife and aging men. Clinical Psychology Review. 2020;81(March):101903. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2020.101903. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McClain Z, Peebles R. Body image and eating disorders among lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender youth. Pediatric Clinics of North America. 2016;63(6):1079–1090. doi: 10.1016/j.pcl.2016.07.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGuinness S, Taylor JE. Understanding body image dissatisfaction and disordered eating in midlife adults. New Zealand Journal of Psychology. 2016;45(1):4–12. [Google Scholar]
- Meyer IH. Minority stress and mental health in gay men. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1995;36(1):38–56. doi: 10.2307/2137286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer IH. Prejudice, social stress, and mental health in lesbian, gay, and bisexual populations: Conceptual issues and research evidence. Psychological Bulletin. 2003;129(5):674–697. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.5.674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer IH. The health of sexual minorities. In: Baum A, Revenson TA, Singer J, editors. Handbook of health psychology. Psychology Press; 2012. pp. 595–615. [Google Scholar]
- Morrison MA, Morrison TG, Sager CL. Does body satisfaction differ between gay men and lesbian women and heterosexual men and women? A meta-analytic review. Body Image. 2004;1:127–138. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2004.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muzi L, Tieghi L, Rugo M, Lingiardi V. Personality as a predictor of symptomatic change in a residential treatment setting for anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Eating and Weight Disorders. 2021;26(4):1195–1209. doi: 10.1007/s40519-020-01023-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muzi L, Tieghi L, Rugo MA, Lingiardi, Evaluating empirically valid and clinically meaningful change in intensive residential treatment for severe eating disorders at discharge and at a 6-month follow-up. Eating and Weight Disorders. 2020;25:1609–1620. doi: 10.1007/s40519-019-00798-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Myers TA, Crowther JH. Social comparison as a predictor of body dissatisfaction: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2009;118(4):683–698. doi: 10.1037/a0016763. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Owens LK, Hughes TL, Owens-Nicholson D. The effects of sexual orientation on body image and attitudes about eating and weight. Journal of Lesbian Studies. 2003;7(1):15–33. doi: 10.1300/J155v07n01_02. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peplau LA, Frederick DA, Yee C, Maisel N, Lever J, Ghavami N. Body image satisfaction in heterosexual, gay, and lesbian adults. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2009;38:713–725. doi: 10.1007/s10508-008-9378-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petrocchi N, Pistella J, Salvati M, Carone N, Laghi F, Baiocco R. I embrace my LGB identity: Self-reassurance, social safeness, and the distinctive relevance of authenticity to well-being in Italian lesbians, gay men, and bisexual people. Sexuality Research & Social Policy. 2020;17(1):75–86. doi: 10.1007/s13178-018-0373-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pistella J, Salvati M, Ioverno S, Laghi F, Baiocco R. Coming-out to family members and internalized sexual stigma in bisexual, lesbian and gay people. Journal of Child and Family Studies. 2016;25(12):3694–3701. doi: 10.1007/s10826-016-0528-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pistella J, Ioverno S, Russell ST. The role of peer victimization, sexual identity, and gender on unhealthy weight control behaviors in a representative sample of Texas youth. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2019;52:597–601. doi: 10.1002/eat.23055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polimeni AM, Austin SB, Kavanagh AM. Sexual orientation and weight, body image, and weight control practices among young Australian women. Journal of Women’s Health. 2009;18(3):355–362. doi: 10.1089/jwh.2007.0765. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pierce M, Hope H, Ford T, Hatch S, Hotopf M, John A, Kontopantelis E, Webb R, Wessely S, McManus S, Abel KM. Mental health before and during the COVID-19 pandemic: A longitudinal probability sample survey of the UK population. The Lancet: Psychiatry. 2020;7(10):883–892. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30308-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prati G, Mancini AD. The psychological impact of COVID-19 pandemic lockdowns: A review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies and natural experiments. Psychological Medicine. 2021;51:201–211. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721000015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pulice-Farrow L, Cusack CE, Galupo MP. “Certain Parts of My Body Don’t Belong to Me”: Trans individuals’ descriptions of body-specific gender dysphoria. Sexuality Research & Social Policy. 2020;17:654–667. doi: 10.1007/s13178-019-00423-y. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Reilly A, Rudd NA. Is internalized homonegativity related to body image? Family and Consumer Sciences Research Journal. 2006;35:58–73. doi: 10.1177/1077727X06289430. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Richmond TK, Walls CE, Austin SB. Sexual orientation and bias in self-reported BMI. Obesity. 2012;20:1703–1709. doi: 10.1038/oby.2012.9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rostosky SS, Riggle EDB, Gray BE, Hatton RL. Minority stress experiences in committed same-sex couple relationships. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice. 2007;38(4):392–400. doi: 10.1037/0735-7028.38.4.392. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Russell CJ, Keel PK. Homosexuality as a specific risk factor for eating disorders in men. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2002;31(3):300–306. doi: 10.1002/eat.10036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryan TA, Morrison TG, McDermott DT. Body image investment among gay and bisexual men over the age of 40: A test of social comparison theory and threatened masculinity theory. Gay and Lesbian Issues and Psychology Review. 2010;6:4–19. [Google Scholar]
- Salvati M, De Cristofaro V, Fasoli F, Paolini D, Zotti D. Introduction to the special issue: Sexual prejudice and stereotyping in modern societies. Psicologia Sociale. 2020;15(1):5–14. doi: 10.1482/96291. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Salvati M, Koc Y. Advancing research into the social psychology of sexual orientations and gender identities: Current research and future directions. European Journal of Social Psychology. 2022;52(2):225–232. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2875. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Schaefer LM, Thompson JK. Self-objectification and disordered eating: A meta-analysis. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2018;51:483–502. doi: 10.1002/eat.22854. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shearer A, Russon J, Herres J, Atte T, Kodish T, Diamond G. The relationship between disordered eating and sexuality amongst adolescents and young adults. Eating Behaviors. 2015;19:115–119. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2015.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siconolfi DE, Kapadia F, Moeller RW, Eddy JA, Kupprat SA, Kingdon MJ, Halkitis PN. Body dissatisfaction in a diverse sample of young men who have sex with men: The P18 Cohort Study. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2016;45(5):1227–1239. doi: 10.1007/s10508-015-0592-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sideli L, Lo Coco G, Bonfanti RC, Borsarini B, Fortunato L, Sechi C, Micali N. Effects of COVID-19 lockdown on eating disorders and obesity: A systematic review and meta-analysis. European Eating Disorders Review. 2021;29(6):826–841. doi: 10.1002/erv.2861. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siever MD. Sexual orientation and gender as factors in socioculturally acquired vulnerability to body dissatisfaction and eating disorders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1994;62(2):252–260. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.62.2.252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Simone M, Askew A, Lust K, Eisenberg ME, Pisetsky EM. Disparities in self-reported eating disorders and academic impairment in sexual and gender minority college students relative to their heterosexual and cisgender peers. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2020;53(4):513–524. doi: 10.1002/eat.23226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith AR, Hawkeswood SE, Bodell LP, Joiner TE. Muscularity versus leanness: An examination of body ideals and predictors of disordered eating in heterosexual and gay college students. Body Image. 2011;8:232–236. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stice E, Shaw HE. Role of body dissatisfaction in the onset and maintenance of eating pathology: A synthesis of research findings. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2002;53(5):985–993. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(02)00488-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strong SM, Williamson DA, Netemeyer RG, Geer JH. Eating disorder symptoms and concerns about body differ as a function of gender and sexual orientation. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology. 2000;19(2):240–255. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2000.19.2.240. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Swami V. Body appreciation, media influence, and weight status predict consideration of cosmetic surgery among female undergraduates. Body Image. 2009;6(4):315–317. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2009.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tatangelo G, McCabe M, Mellor D, Mealey A. A systematic review of body dissatisfaction and sociocultural messages related to the body among preschool children. Body Image. 2016;18:86–95. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2016.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson, J. K. (2000). Introduction: Body image, eating disorders, and obesity – An emerging synthesis. In Body image, eating disorders, and obesity: An integrative guide for assessment and treatment (pp. 1–23). American Psychological Association.
- Thompson JK. The (mis)measurement of body image: Ten strategies to improve assessment for applied and research purposes. Body Image. 2004;1(1):7–14. doi: 10.1016/S1740-1445(03)00004-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tiggemann M, Martins Y, Kirkbride A. Oh to be lean and muscular: Body image ideals in gay and heterosexual men. Psychology of Men & Masculinity. 2007;8:15–24. doi: 10.1037/1524-9220.8.1.15. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Tylka TL. Positive psychology perspectives on body image. In: Cash TF, Smolak L, editors. Body image: A handbook of science, practice, and prevention. Guilford Press; 2011. pp. 56–64. [Google Scholar]
- Walter SL, Seibert SE, Goering D, O’Boyle EH., Jr A tale of two sample sources: Do results from online panel data and conventional data converge? Journal of Business and Psychology. 2019;34(4):425–452. doi: 10.1007/s10869-018-9552-y. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Williamson I, Spence K. Towards an understanding of risk factors for eating disturbance amongst gay men. Health Education. 2001;101(5):217–227. doi: 10.1108/EUM0000000005645. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson RE, Latner JD, Hayashi K. More than just body weight: The role of body image in psychological and physical functioning. Body Image. 2013;10(4):644–647. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2013.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiseman MC, Moradi B. Body image and eating disorder symptoms in sexual minority men: A test and extension of objectification theory. Journal of Counselling Psychology. 2010;57:154–166. doi: 10.1037/a0018937. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Witcomb GL, Bouman WP, Brewin N, Richards C, Fernandez-Aranda F, Arcelus J. Body image dissatisfaction and eating-related psychopathology in trans individuals: A matched control study. European Eating Disorders Review. 2015;23(4):287–293. doi: 10.1002/erv.2362. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu T, Jia X, Shi H, Niu J, Yin X, Xie J, Wang X. Prevalence of mental health problems during the COVID-19 pandemic: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2021;281:91–98. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.11.117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yean C, Benau EM, Dakanalis A, Hormes JM, Perone J, Timko CA. The relationship of sex and sexual orientation to self-esteem, body shape satisfaction, and eating disorder symptomatology. Frontiers in Psychology. 2013;4:887. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00887. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yelland C, Tiggemann M. Muscularity and the gay ideal: Body dissatisfaction and disordered eating in homosexual men. Eating Behaviors. 2003;4:107–116. doi: 10.1016/S1471-0153(03)00014-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
All reasonable requests for code relevant to the analyses of this paper will be met.