To the Editor:
We appreciate the opportunity to further the discussion regarding the N170 in autism spectrum disorder (ASD) (1). We agree that electroencephalography (EEG) holds promise for deriving biomarkers, but that such applications are not straightforward.
The literature indicates variability in the difference in N170 response to face stimuli between individuals with ASD and those with typical development (TD). This inconsistency is precisely why one would undertake a meta-analysis (2): to synthesize across diverse samples. That a modest relative N170 latency effect for faces was found for ASD, even with significant heterogeneity, allowed us to reject the null hypothesis of no difference between populations. This is notable and uncommon in the search for biomarkers of ASD.
Vettori et al. (3) suggest that a biomarker must index discriminant validity. This is true of a diagnostic biomarker. However, the N170 is likely not simply a standalone diagnostic biomarker of ASD. Instead, there are multiple biomarkers for distinct purposes: a stratification biomarker may classify individuals with ASD into relevant subgroups; a target engagement biomarker may indicate the response of social brain circuitry to intervention; an early efficacy biomarker may indicate a treatment effect more quickly than subjective reports (4).
N170 latency variability as seen in the literature and across development, and responsiveness to experience suggest some potential beyond being a diagnostic biomarker—particularly considering our meta-analysis. For instance, the neural response to faces can reflect changes from intervention (5), demonstrating potential as an early efficacy or target engagement biomarker (4). In addition, indexing individual variation in functional processes is useful to be practicable for dissemination (4,6). Indeed, ASD researchers seek biomarkers for symptoms shared with other disorders, such as schizophrenia or social anxiety. N170 face latency is a good candidate in this regard, as it is implicated in many psychiatric disorders. For instance, N170 response to faces indexes continuously distributed processes that subtend social communication, such as perceptual representation of facial expressions or identity, based on structural and social–communicative criteria, such as rapid social–emotional reactions in the observer (7). Thus, N170 can help indicate the processes underlying such capacities transdiagnostically.
Conversely, N170 may be useful as a stratification biomarker across symptom axes, with strata potentially corresponding to unique syndromes. We identified this as a timely topic for investigation: it is necessary to examine the specificity of the applicability of N170 to ASD versus other populations now that the specificity to face stimuli (vs. nonfaces) in the difference in N170 latency between ASD and TD has been established meta-analytically. Several recent studies have examined this question (8,9)—often with mixed results. This is appropriate to the current development of the literature and demands more nuanced and sophisticated questions regarding when, how, and for whom the N170 face latency difference may function as a biomarker and what sort of biomarker it may be.
Biomarkers should also be sensitive to changes across development, given normative and necessary ontogenetic dynamics of neural systems. ASD is a developmental disorder, and therefore a biomarker would need to capture such variability over time to demarcate discrete stages of developmental canalization (6). N170 largely meets this criterion, as its morphology reflects developmental maturation and can be applied consistently throughout the lifespan with well-established response patterns (6,10). While the variation in N170 morphology across participants and age groups can complicate objective determination of latency and amplitude, atypical developmental morphology is reliably quantifiable with diligent attention across studies even with methodological variability (11). Moreover, differences that become more prominent between ASD and TD across ages (e.g., the amplitude difference found in our meta-analysis) might simply reflect a more consistently evoked ERP, rather than developmental differences in underlying neurophysiology. This is precisely an area for study now that a benchmark population difference has been identified, and this calls for future studies in stricter accordance with guidelines for acquiring and interpreting EEG data in ASD (12,13).
There are several more general points that require some clarification. First, while increased N170 amplitude to facial stimuli is well established, the current literature also supports faster latency to faces than nonfaces in TD (9,11,14). Therefore, the relative delay in ASD to faces (but not to nonfaces) is likely to reflect divergent neural mechanisms of face processing. Second, while the effect size for nonsocial stimuli was medium, it was a nonsignificant effect (95% confidence interval −0.14 to 1.16). This indicates that the null hypothesis should not be rejected (15,16), suggesting that the notion that N170 delay may reflect slower general processing of visual stimuli is not supported on the basis of substantive or statistical significance. Third, we examined the P1 component that reflects basic sensory processes and found that it did not differ between ASD and TD. Therefore, the literature to date suggests that differential processing in ASD does not emerge until the N170 stage (11). Fourth, we wish to clarify that our study compared N170 latency delay in individuals with ASD to age-matched TD control subjects, suggesting that relative N170 latency is delayed in ASD despite an absolute N170 latency decrease. This reveals that the rate of maturation lags behind TD adults, adding support to theories of experience-expectant development of face processing (17,18) and highlighting the N170 as a candidate marker with protracted maturation whose deviation from typical neural activity may reflect a quantitative developmental delay of normative processes that may exacerbate over time.
Finally, we wish to highlight that relative to the current evaluative standards of the field, EEG-based assessment of the N170 offers the promise of a more accessible and efficient evaluation. For instance, EEG-based evaluation offers a more rapid assessment thanks to EEG’s wide applicability (i.e., to a wide developmental and intellectual range), low cost, high accessibility, acute sensitivity, and objectivity in measurement (4). Other practical advantages are evident as well. ASD is presently best diagnosed and treated based on the subjective evaluation of clinical experts or the use of structured tools—both of which exhibit limited availability and potential subjectivity (6). However, biological differences that may be clinically relevant may not be present in overt behaviors, and observable manifestations are not consistently expressed across development. Therefore, any tool that increases objectivity or access would represent a dramatic clinical advance with practical value for investigation, assessment, and treatment of ASD.
Acknowledgments and Disclosures
This work was supported by Simons Foundation Autism Research Initiative Grant No. 381283 (to EK, MDL), National Institute of Mental Health Grant Nos. R01MH110585 (to EK, CMK, MDL) and U19MH108206, R01MH107426, and R01MH100173 (to EJL, JCM), a Brain and Behavior Research Foundation National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression Young Investigator Award (to JHF-F, MDL), an Autism Science Foundation Accelerator Grant (to JHF-F), an Autism Science Foundation Predoctoral Fellowship Award (to CMK), the Seaver Foundation (to JHF-F), and the Arts Connection and the Alan Alda Fund for Communication (to MDL). These funders had no role in the study design, collection, analysis, or interpretation of the data, writing the manuscript, or the decision to submit the paper for publication.
Footnotes
The authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.
See also associated correspondence: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.07.015.
References
- 1.Kang E, Keifer CM, Levy EJ, Foss-Feig JH, McPartland JC, Lerner MD (2018): Atypicality of the N170 event-related potential in autism spectrum disorder: A meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 3:657–666. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Huedo-Medina TB, Sánchez-Meca J, Marín-Martínez F, Botella J (2006): Assessing heterogeneity in meta-analysis: Q statistic or I2 index?. Psychol Methods 11:193–206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Vettori S, Jacques C, Boets B, Rossion B (2019): Can the N170 be used as an electrophysiological biomarker indexing face processing difficulties in autism spectrum disorder?. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 4:321–323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.McPartland JC (2016): Considerations in biomarker development for neurodevelopmental disorders. Curr Opin Neurol 29:118–122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Dawson G, Jones EJH, Merkle K, Venema K, Lowy R, Faja S, et al. (2012): Early behavioral intervention is associated with normalized brain activity in young children with autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 51:1150–1159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.McPartland JC (2017):Developing clinically practicable biomarkers for autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 47:2935–2937. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Hinojosa JA, Mercado F, Carretié L, (2015): N170 sensitivity to facial expression: A meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 55:498–509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Feuerriegel D, Churches O, Hofmann J, Keage HAD (2015): The N170 and face perception in psychiatric and neurological disorders: A systematic review. Clin Neurophysiol 126:1141–1158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Sysoeva OV, Constantino JN, Anokhin AP (2018): Event-related potential (ERP) correlates of face processing in verbal children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) and their ff rst-degree relatives: A family study. Mol Autism 9:41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Taylor MJ, McCarthy G, Saliba E, Degiovanni E (1999): ERP evidence of developmental changes in processing of faces. Clin Neurophysiol 110:910–915. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Taylor MJ, Batty M, Itier RJ (2004): The faces of development: A review of early face processing over childhood. J Cogn Neurosci 16:1426–1442. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Webb SJ, Bernier R, Henderson HA, Johnson MH, Jones EJ, Lerner MD, et al. (2015): Guidelines and best practices for electrophysiological data collection, analysis and reporting in autism. J Autism Dev Disord 45:425–443. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Keil A, Debener S, Gratton G, Junghöfer M, Kappenman ES, Luck SJ, et al. (2014): Committee report: Publication guidelines and recommendations for studies using electroencephalography and magnetoencephalography. Psychophysiology 51:1–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Webb SJ, Dawson G, Bernier R, Panagiotides H (2006): ERP evidence of atypical face processing in young children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 36:881–890. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Ferguson CJ (2009): An effect size primer: A guide for clinicians and researchers. Prof Psychol Res Pract 40:532–538. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Sapp M (2004): Confidence intervals within hypnosis research. Sleep Hypn 6:169–176. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Dawson G, Webb SJ, McPartland JC (2005): Understanding the nature of face processing impairment in autism: Insights from behavioral and electrophysiological studies. Dev Neuropsychol 27:403–424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.McPartland JC, Wu J, Bailey CA, Mayes LC, Schultz RT, Klin A (2011): Atypical neural specialization for social percepts in autism spectrum disorder. Soc Neurosci 6:436–451. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]