Abstract
Self-stigma has been associated with reduced accuracy of face emotion recognition in individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis (CHR). Stigma may also relate to slowing of performance during cognitive tasks for which a negative stereotype is relevant. This study aimed to investigate the association of mental illness stigma with face emotion recognition among CHR individuals. Participants were 143 CHR individuals identified using the Structured Interview for Psychosis-Risk Syndromes (SIPS). Face emotion recognition was assessed using the Penn Emotion Recognition Task (ER-40). Stigma was assessed using discrimination, stereotype awareness, and stereotype agreement subscales of the Mental Health Attitudes Interview for CHR. We tested associations of ER-40 accuracy and response times with these stigma variables, including the role of clinical and demographic factors. Racial/ethnic minoritized participants had higher attenuated positive symptoms than non-minoritized participants. Longer ER-40 response times were correlated with greater stereotype agreement (r=.17, p=.045) and discrimination (r=.22, p=.012). A regression model predicting ER-40 response times revealed an interaction of stereotype agreement with minoritized status (p=.008), with slower response times for minoritized participants as stereotype agreement increased. Greater disorganized symptoms and male gender also predicted longer response times. ER-40 accuracy was not associated with stigma. Overall, minoritized CHR individuals with greater internalized stigma took longer to identify face emotions. Future research is needed to assess whether slower response times are specific to social cues, and if internalized stigma interferes with performance in real-world social situations. Reducing stigma may be an important target for interventions that aim to improve social skills.
Keywords: social cognition, stereotype, prodrome, discrimination, race
Stigma may be psychologically harmful to people at clinical high risk (CHR) for psychosis, as this risk syndrome occurs during adolescence and young adulthood when individuals are forming identities (DeLuca, 2020; Yang et al., 2010). Stigma arises from the process of associating human differences with negative stereotypes and labeling and separating individuals with those differences (Link and Phelan, 2001). Stereotypes are learned constructs about groups in which a person rapidly organizes information and generates impressions about individuals based on group membership whether such information is accurate or not. Internalized stigma, or self-stigma, is the internalization of stereotypes, which arises from both an awareness of negative stereotypes (stereotype awareness) and application of those stereotypes to oneself (stereotype agreement). Discrimination is a behavioral response to a stereotype (Corrigan and Watson, 2002), which may occur at the individual level (e.g., rejecting someone because of their mental illness), structural level (e.g., lack of protections for people who are denied rental housing due to their mental illness), or through the stigmatized individuals themselves (e.g., failing to pursue a job promotion because one anticipates failure due to one’s own mental illness).
CHR individuals experience significant self-stigma related to mental illness compared to youth with other mental health conditions (Colizzi et al., 2020; Yang et al., 2015), and report that psychiatric labels impact how they view themselves (Yang et al., 2019). Mental illness self-stigma tends to be higher among racial and ethnic minority groups compared to Whites (Misra et al., 2021). In CHR groups, racial and identity-based discrimination is higher among CHR compared to healthy controls (Saleem et al., 2014; Shaikh et al., 2016). Researchers have also found that race-based trauma is associated with attenuated psychotic experiences (Anglin et al., 2016; Anglin et al., 2014; Anglin et al., 2018) and emerging research has documented how minoritized CHR youth may have unique stigma and coping experiences (Ruiz et al., 2021). Both identity-based discrimination and mental illness stigma have predicted transition to psychosis among CHR individuals (Rüsch et al., 2015; Stowkowy et al., 2016). Individuals with CHR, particularly from minoritized racial and ethnic groups, contend with multiple stressors and cumulative stigma that may lead to various negative clinical and cognitive outcomes (Anglin et al., 2021; DeLuca et al., 2021).
Researchers have suggested that individual and contextual factors including stigma have likely exaggerated the cognitive impairment observed in individuals with schizophrenia (Moritz et al., 2020), which may also be true for the CHR population. Stigma may impact social cognitive processes such as face emotion recognition (FER) given that both are relevant in social situations and are characteristic of individuals with schizophrenia (Barkl et al., 2014; Kohler et al., 2010) and CHR (van Donkersgoed et al., 2015). Individuals with CHR show problems identifying threat-based emotions like fear (Amminger et al., 2012), and failure to distinguish fear and anger from neutral expressions has predicted transition to psychosis (Allott et al., 2014; Corcoran et al., 2015). In one of the first studies to evaluate mental illness stigma and FER in individuals with CHR, greater shame about symptoms was associated with poorer FER accuracy in identification of fearful faces, and misattribution of fear to nonfearful faces (Larsen et al., 2019). Individuals who feel stigmatized may also take longer to judge facial expressions, plausibly resulting in longer response times. FER response time have been related to poorer social functioning in CHR youth (Haining et al., 2020) and have predicted transition to psychosis (Bilgrami et al., 2019), though have not yet been investigated in relation to stigma.
The goal of the present study was to investigate relationships of accuracy and response times during FER with discrimination, stereotype awareness, and stereotype agreement related to mental illness among individuals with CHR, including determining whether associations may be strongest among racial/ethnic minoritized individuals as they have multiple identities that may be the target of stereotypes and discrimination. We hypothesized that poorer accuracy and longer response times to identifying face emotions would be associated with these stigma constructs.
Methods
Participants and Setting
Data were collected as part of a large prospective cohort study assessing stigma and its correlates in CHR conducted at Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center (Boston, MA), Maine Medical Center Research Institute (Portland, ME), and New York State Psychiatric Institute (New York, NY) between November 2012 and December 2015. Present data are from study baseline. This study was approved by all sites’ Institutional Review Boards.
Participants’ ages 12 to 35 self-referred or were recruited from outreach efforts in response to media, public transportation, and online advertisements, as well as from schools, colleges, specialized clinics or other studies. Participants met criteria for a psychosis-risk syndrome using the Structured Interview for Psychosis Risk Syndrome (SIPS) Version 5.0 (Miller et al., 2003) with the exception of ten participants who met criteria based on negative symptoms subscale of the SIPS. Participants were excluded if they had a history of a psychotic disorder, imminent risk of self-harm or violence, any major medical or neurological disorder, or IQ less than 70.
Procedures
Adult participants provided written informed consent. Minors provided written assent and a parent or legal guardian provided written informed consent. Boston and Portland site consent forms discussed examples of symptoms that study participants may experience (e.g., suspiciousness) without using CHR or psychosis terminology, and the New York site consent forms indicated that participants had “a somewhat increased risk of psychosis”. Participants who were not presently engaged with mental health treatment were given a referral to a specialized CHR clinic if desired.
Following consent, participants completed a SIPS interview. The SIPS was administered by masters and doctoral level clinicians, and master level students, who were trained and certified by Yale SIPS trainers. SIPS ratings and classification were confirmed via consensus of all key study clinicians.
Study data were collected in the following order: 1) demographics; 2) Mental Health Attitudes Interview for CHR; 3) ER-40. These assessments were typically administered over the course of different study visits and the time between assessments varied according to patient availability. There were no associations between the inter-assessment interval and our variables of interest.
Measures
Stigma
Discrimination, stereotype awareness, and stereotype agreement were assessed using subscales of the Mental Health Attitudes Interview for CHR administered by clinicians who received extensive training in administration (Table 1). This interview was adapted from stigma measures by Link et al. and adapted for CHR (Link et al., 1989; Yang et al., 2015). Discrimination (related to symptoms) consists of five items (e.g., “because I have had symptoms, people have treated me differently”) rated on a 5-point Likert scale (1=Never, 2=Seldom, 3=Sometimes, 4=Often, 5=Very Often). Stereotype awareness consists of 10 items (e.g., “most people believe that young people with emotional problems may be more dangerous”), and stereotype agreement consists of ten items (e.g., “I believe that young people with emotional problems may be more dangerous”), with items rated on a 4-point Likert scale (1=Strongly Disagree, 2=Somewhat Disagree, 3=Somewhat Agree, 4=Strongly Agree). Scores for each sub-scale were computed as the mean response across items.
Table 1.
Discrimination | Stereotype Awareness / Stereotype Agreement |
---|---|
Because I have had symptoms... 1. People have treated me differently 2. People seem to be less comfortable with me 3. People hang out with me less 4. People are unfair to me 5. People are supportive of me 6. People are a little afraid of me |
Stereotype Awareness: Most people believe that young people with emotional problems are... Stereotype Aareement: I believe that young people with emotional problems are... 1. Just as smart 2. May be more dangerous 3. Have themselves to blame 4. Can be trusted just as much 5. Will have these problems all their lives 6. Can do as well in school 7. Have trouble taking care of themselves 8. Are more creative 9. Are no different 10. Should be treated like everyone else |
Face Emotion Recognition
FER was assessed with the computerized Penn Emotion Recognition Task (ER-40) (Gur et al., 2002). The ER-40 randomly displays an image of a face expressing one of five emotions: happy, sad, anger, fear, and neutral. Participants select their choice among emotions listed on the screen using a computer mouse. There are 40 faces in total and four races are represented: Caucasian (21 faces), Black (11 faces), Hispanic (4 faces), and Asian (4 faces). Stimuli are balanced for gender, age, and ethnicity. This test yields a total accuracy score based on total percent of emotions correctly identified, and an accuracy score for each emotion category. The test also yields a total response time score based on median time to correctly identify all emotions in the task, and a median response time for each emotion category.
CHR Symptomatology
CHR symptoms were obtained from the Structured Interview for Psychosis Risk Syndromes (SIPS) (Miller et al., 2003), which yields scores for positive, negative, disorganized, and general symptoms, as well as a total score. Per SIPS criteria, symptoms could not be better accounted for by another psychiatric disorder.
Demographics
Age, gender, and racial/ethnic minoritized status were self-reported. We coded minoritized status as a binary variable indicating whether or not a participant held a racial or ethnic minority status (minoritized = participant self-identified as an ethnicity and race other than white and/or identified as Hispanic/Latino; non-minoritized = participant identified as white and non-Hispanic/Latino). The highest level of formal education completed by the participant’s mother was used to rate socioeconomic status. IQ was measured using Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) (Wechsler, 1999) and attention was measured with the Continuous Performance Test-Identical Pairs version (CPT-IP).
Analyses
Data from 143 participants were analyzed for the present study using SPSS version 27. Means and standard deviations were calculated for key study variables (stigma, ER-40, and SIPS/SOPS scores – positive, negative, disorganized, general), and Pearson correlations were computed among these variables. The threshold for statistical significance was set at p<0.05. Significant correlations were entered into hierarchical linear regression models.
In step 1, each ER-40 variable was entered as the outcome, and the stigma variable was entered as the predictor. In step 2, we entered symptoms and sociodemographic variables (age, sex, racial/ethnic minoritized status), and in step 3 we entered an interaction between minoritized status and the stigma variable of interest. We also tested whether this interaction would be affected by confounding effects of IQ, site, and maternal education (as a proxy for socioeconomic status). Lastly, we conducted an exploratory linear regression analysis to assess possible specificity of stigma relationships with individual ER-40 emotions (fear, anger, happy, sad, neutral) when entered simultaneously, as past work has found preliminary evidence for fear-specific effects (Larsen et al., 2019).
Results
Sample characteristics
Consistent with most CHR cohorts, the sample comprised adolescents and early adults (mean age=19.1; SD=4.2) and was approximately two-thirds male and two-thirds white (Table 2). Mean IQ was in the average range (mean=109.4; SD=15.1) and correlated with ER-40 response times (r=.226; p=.008). CPT-IP was not correlated with ER-40 response times (r=−.023; p=.818).
Table 2.
Mean (SD) | Range | |
| ||
Age (years) | 19.1 (4.2) | 12–35 |
| ||
N | Percent | |
| ||
Gender (M/F) | 93/48 | 65.5/33.8 |
Maternal Education (n=136) | ||
Some high school | 7 | 5.1 |
Completed high school | 18 | 13.2 |
Some college/tech school | 20 | 14.7 |
Completed college/tech school | 57 | 41.9 |
Some graduate/professional school | 2 | 1.5 |
Completed graduate/professional school | 32 | 23.5 |
Race (n=136) | ||
White (European) | 89 | 65.4 |
Black | 21 | 15.4 |
Interracial | 14 | 10.3 |
Asian | 6 | 4.4 |
Central/South American | 4 | 2.9 |
American Indian | 2 | 1.5 |
Ethnicity (Hispanic or Latino) (n=137) | 17 | 12.4 |
Racial/ethnic Minoritized Status (n=137) | ||
Minoritizeda | 52 | 38.0 |
Non-minoritizedb | 85 | 62.0 |
Note. Number of participants are noted per variable given missing data.
participants who identified as a racial minority and/or Hispanic/Latino.
participants who identified as white and non-Hispanic/Latino.
Means and standard deviations for key variables are presented in Table 3. ER-40 accuracy and response times were not correlated (p=.70). There were no significant differences in stigma variables and ER-40 variables between minoritized and non-minoritized participants. Regarding SIPS symptoms, minoritized participants had higher attenuated positive symptoms compared to non-minoritized participants (t(135)=2.5, p=.014).
Table 3.
Mean (SD) | |
---|---|
| |
ER-40 Response time (ms) | 2078.50 (491.80) |
ER-40 Accuracy | 33.14 (2.85) |
Symptom Discrimination | 1.94 (0.92) |
Stereotype Awareness | 2.56 (0.39) |
Stereotype Agreement | 1.91 (0.39) |
SIPS Total | 47.21 (14.22) |
SIPS-Negative | 15.01 (6.52) |
SIPS-Positive a | 13.73 (4.02) |
SIPS-General | 11.31 (4.16) |
SIPS-Disorganized | 7.21 (3.79) |
Note. Discrimination score ranges from 1 to 5, while stereotype awareness and stereotype agreement scores range from 1 to 4. SIPS = Structured Interview for Psychosis Risk Syndromes. ER-40 = Penn Emotion Recognition Task.
Minoritized participants had higher attenuated positive symptoms than non-minoritized participants.
Relationships between symptoms, ER-40, and stigma
SIPS/SOPS scores were not correlated with ER-40 accuracy. Higher disorganized symptoms were correlated with longer ER-40 response times (r=.28, p = .001).
Greater stereotype awareness was correlated with higher positive (r=.25, p = .003), disorganized (r=.34, p < .001) and general (r=.23, p = .008) symptoms.
Greater stereotype agreement was correlated with higher disorganized symptoms (r=.19, p = .027).
Greater discrimination was correlated with higher negative (r=.20, p = .023), disorganized (r=.30, p = .001), and general (r=.28, p = .001) symptoms.
Relationships between stigma and ER-40 accuracy and response times
There were no correlations between overall ER-40 accuracy and discrimination, stereotype awareness, or stereotype agreement (ps>.36).
ER-40 response time was significantly correlated with discrimination (r=.22, p=.012) and stereotype agreement (r=.17, p=.045; see Figure 1, panels A and B). There was no correlation between ER-40 response time and stereotype awareness (p=.683).
Regression models for stereotype agreement and discrimination predicting ER-40 response times are presented in Table 4. There was a minoritized status x stereotype agreement interaction (Table 4, Model A), with stereotype agreement predicting ER-40 response time only for minoritized individuals (Figure 1, panel C). Disorganized symptoms and gender were also significant predictors in the final model, with stereotype agreement predicting ER-40 response times for male participants with greater disorganized symptoms. The minoritized status x stereotype agreement interaction remained significant when adding site, IQ, and maternal education to the model, and none of these variables were significant predictors.
Table 4.
Step 1 | Step 2 | Step 3 | |||||
|
|||||||
ΔR2=.021 (p=.112) | ΔR2=.125 (p=.003) | ΔR2=.051 (p=.008) | |||||
|
|||||||
(A) Model from Stereotype Agreement | β | p | β | p | β | p | |
| |||||||
Stereotype Agreement | .145 | .112 | .044 | .630 | −.144 | .207 | |
Age | −.085 | .327 | −.084 | .324 | |||
Gender | .257 | .006 | .275 | .003 | |||
Disorganized Symptoms | .175 | .049 | .178 | .040 | |||
Minoritized status | .127 | .143 | .122 | .150 | |||
Minoritized × Stereo. Agreement | .291 | .008 | |||||
| |||||||
Step 1 | Step 2 | Step 3 | |||||
|
|||||||
ΔR2=.078 (p=.002) | ΔR2=.143 (p=.003) | ΔR2=.007 (p=.318) | |||||
|
|||||||
(B) Model from Discrimination | β | p | β | p | β | p | |
| |||||||
Discrimination | .279 | .002 | .262 | .005 | .197 | .083 | |
Age | −.025 | .770 | −.032 | .704 | |||
Gender | .285 | .001 | .280 | <.001 | |||
Minoritized status | .163 | .061 | .176 | .046 | |||
Negative symptoms | −.109 | .342 | −.112 | .330 | |||
General symptoms | −.058 | .615 | −.053 | .641 | |||
Disorganized symptoms | .212 | .043 | .228 | .031 | |||
Minoritized × Discrimination | .106 | .318 |
Note. For both models, dependent variable = ER-40 response time. All continuous predictors are mean-centered. Model (A) Adjusted R2 values: Step 1=.013, Step 2=.109, Step 3=.156. Model (B) Adjusted R2 values: Step 1=.070, Step 2=.173, Step 3=.173. Minoritized refers to the presence of a racial/ethnic minoritized status.
For the model predicting ER-40 response time from discrimination (Table 4, Model B), the minoritized status x discrimination interaction was not significant (Figure 1, panel D), and adding this term to the model in step 3 did not produce an improvement in R2 (ΔR2=.000 (p=.806)). Discrimination remained a significant predictor of ER-40 response times when including sociodemographic variables in the model (Step 2), and disorganized symptoms and gender were also significant predictors in this model.
There were no significant independent effects for discrimination or stereotype agreement predicting individual emotions (anger, fear, sad, happy and neutral) in regression models (all ps > .05).
Discussion
This study investigated the relationship of face emotion recognition (FER) accuracy and response times with three stigma constructs – stereotype awareness, stereotype agreement, and discrimination related to mental health symptoms – among individuals with CHR. As hypothesized, we found a small but significant association between longer FER response times and greater stereotype agreement and discrimination. The relationship between FER response times and stereotype agreement was driven by minoritized participants. These findings remained significant even when accounting for possible confounding by symptoms, site, age, gender, maternal education, and IQ. We did not find relationships between FER accuracy and stigma as we had hypothesized and found in a previous study (Larsen et al., 2019).
The specificity of the ER-40 response time association with stereotype agreement to minoritized participants is consistent with work showing that being from a racial or ethnic minority group may worsen the negative effects of mental illness stigma (Misra et al., 2021), including in CHR individuals (Stowkowy et al., 2016). It is also important to note that minoritized participants in our study had higher attenuated positive symptoms (v. non-minoritized participants). Minoritized individuals experience significant stressors and systematic disadvantages, as well as disparities in early psychosis care (Jones et al., 2021; Oluwoye et al., 2021; Radua et al., 2018).
Individuals with CHR from minoritized groups have multiple social identities that are the target of stereotypes. Intersectional stigma (i.e., the convergence of multiple stigmatized identities in an individual) posits nuanced effects of stigma when considering multiple identity characteristics together rather than in isolation (Crenshaw, 1989; Pachankis et al., 2018; Turan et al., 2019). The combined impact of stereotypes from multiple fronts (racial/ethnic minoritized status and mental illness) can result in a “double disadvantage” (Oexle and Corrigan, 2018). Research has shown that subtle factors such as being asked to indicate race prior to an evaluation, or the race of the test administrator, can lead to poorer performance for minorities (Marx and Goff, 2005; Steele and Aronson, 1995). In addition, an “other-race effect” has been observed for the ER-40, in which Black individuals performed more poorly on this task than White individuals when viewing faces with a race other than their own (Pinkham et al., 2008). This “double disadvantage” may explain why only individuals from racial/ethnic minoritized groups with high internalized mental illness stigma had longer ER-40 response times.
Our findings suggest that mental illness stigma influenced participants’ performance on a social cognitive task. These relations may occur by the same process in which experiences such as stereotype threat (Spencer et al., 2016; Steele and Aronson, 1995), stigma stress (Rüsch et al., 2009b, 2009a), and racial discrimination (Coogan et al., 2020; Keating et al., 2021) prompt a disruptive state of inefficient cognitive processing that undermines performance (Grant and Beck, 2009; Pennington et al., 2016). The mechanisms by which stigma impairs cognitive performance include distraction and loss of focus presumably due to increased stereotyped-related distraction (Cadinu et al., 2005), increased physiological arousal such as decreased heart rate variability (Croizet et al., 2004), increased skin conductance and blood pressure (Osborne, 2006), and increased anxiety (Spencer et al., 1999). The relationship between stigma and social cognitive processing is also supported by work showing that adverse experiences impact cognitive processing of emotional stimuli in individuals with CHR (Tognin et al., 2020) and with evidence for overlap in the neural underpinnings of stigma and emotion detection (Clark et al., 2018).
When cognitive resources are being used for stigma-related processes, a person may fall behind during social interactions or misinterpret crucial social cues. For instance, individuals with schizophrenia exhibited poorer social skills during social interactions in which they were made to think that the other person was aware of their diagnosis compared to those who were told that others knew nothing about them (Henry et al., 2010). In addition, higher disorganized symptoms and male gender partially accounted for longer FER response times in this study, consistent with CHR research demonstrating more social deficits among males (Rietschel et al., 2017). The SIPS disorganized symptoms scale includes trouble with focus and attention, and poor attention may cause processing delays. It is possible that longer FER response times reflect a generalized deficit in processing speed, rather than a specific deficit in processing social cues, which may explain why FER accuracy was not related to stigma in this study. However, our measures of IQ and sustained attention, although not explicit measures of processing speed, were not related to ER-40 response times.
Stereotype awareness did not relate to FER in this study. Stereotype awareness is an observation, whereas stereotype agreement and discrimination are internalized aspects of stigma that can be targeted through treatment. Helpful interventions may include providing feedback on one’s CHR status in a way that enhances the positive effects (e.g., relief, validation) of CHR labeling (Uttinger et al., 2018; Welsh and Tiffin, 2012), directly inquiring about the perceived impact of CHR labeling during feedback (Woodberry et al., 2021), and providing psychoeducation to increase understanding of the CHR condition (Herrera et al., 2021; Mcfarlane et al., 2012). Psychosocial interventions could also address stigma as a barrier to treatment engagement (Ben-David et al., 2019; Gronholm et al., 2017; He et al., 2020; Rusch et al., 2013). Another important implication of this work is that stigma may impact performance on standardized cognitive assessments in clinical and research settings (Moritz et al., 2020).
Future research should investigate the directionality and dynamics of these relationships using longitudinal and multimodal approaches. It would be important to measure the extent to which stigma is related to a generalized cognitive deficit in processing speed compared to a specific deficit in the timely processing of social cues. Racial and ethnic minority groups should be examined individually, and future studies should investigate one’s stigma related to both racial identity and mental illness through self-report and qualitative methods. These relationships may be studied using psychophysiological measures (e.g., heart rate, skin conductance) and within the context of brain imaging (Clark et al., 2018; Todorov, 2012). It may be useful to include measures of stigma beyond self-report, such as the implicit association test (IAT) (Greenwald et al., 1998).
A limitation to this study is that we did not explicitly assess whether longer FER response times were accounted for by a generalized deficit in processing speed. We were unable to compare our sample to individuals with other psychiatric disorders or symptoms, or compare CHR subgroups, such as those who converted to psychosis. Furthermore, we did not have significant power to test each individual minority group or assess participants’ stigma related to their racial identity. There are likely different internalized stereotypes at play accounting for differing relationships between stereotype agreement and response time between racial/ethnic groups. Stigma measures used in this study rely on self-report and thus are susceptible to social desirability bias or selective recall.
Additional research is needed to investigate the social and cognitive implications of mental illness and race-based stigma for CHR individuals, in which symptoms suggest risk for a highly stigmatized mental illness and occur during a critical stage of identity development.
Footnotes
We have no known conflict of interest to disclose. Of note, Dr. McFarlane is the director of the PIER Training Institute, which provides training and consultation to public and non-profit agencies regarding advanced practices for severe mental disorders.
References
- Allott KA, Schäfer MR, Thompson A, Nelson B, Bendall S, Bartholomeusz CF, Yuen HP, McGorry PD, Schlögelhofer M, Bechdolf A, Amminger GP, 2014. Emotion recognition as a predictor of transition to a psychotic disorder in ultra-high risk participants. Schizophr. Res. 153, 25–31. 10.1016/j.schres.2014.01.037 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Amminger GP, Schäfer MR, Papageorgiou K, Klier CM, Schlögelhofer M, Mossaheb N, Werneck-Rohrer S, Nelson B, McGorry PD, 2012. Emotion recognition in individuals at clinical high-risk for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 38, 1030–1039. 10.1093/schbul/sbr015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anglin DM, Greenspoon M, Lighty Q, Ellman LM, 2016. Race-based rejection sensitivity partially accounts for the relationship between racial discrimination and distressing attenuated positive psychotic symptoms. Early Interv. Psychiatry 10, 411–8. 10.1111/eip.12184 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anglin DM, Lighty Q, Greenspoon M, Ellman LM, 2014. Racial discrimination is associated with distressing subthreshold positive psychotic symptoms among US urban ethnic minority young adults. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 49, 1545–1555. 10.1007/s00127-014-0870-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anglin DM, Lui F, Espinosa A, Tikhonov A, Ellman L, 2018. Ethnic identity, racial discrimination and attenuated psychotic symptoms in an urban population of emerging adults. Early Interv. Psychiatry 12, 380–390. 10.1111/eip.12314 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barkl SJ, Lah S, Harris AWF, Williams LM, 2014. Facial emotion identification in early-onset and first-episode psychosis: A systematic review with meta-analysis. Schizophr. Res. 159, 62–69. 10.1016/j.schres.2014.07.049 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ben-David S, Cole A, Brucato G, Girgis RR, Munson MR, 2019. Mental health service use decision-making among young adults at clinical high risk for developing psychosis. Early Interv. Psychiatry 13, 1050–1055. 10.1111/eip.12725 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bilgrami Z, Benavides C, Sarac C, Shaik R, Keilp J, Gur R, Javitt D, & Corcoran C 2019. T35. Speed of Face Processing Predicts Psychosis in At-Risk Youths. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 45(Supplement_2), S216–S217. 10.1093/schbul/sbz019.315 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cadinu M, Maass A, Rosabianca A, Kiesner J, 2005. Why do women underperform under stereotype threat? Evidence for the role of negative thinking. Psychol. Sci. 16, 572–578. 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01577.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cannon TD, Yu C, Addington J, Bearden CE, Cadenhead KS, Cornblatt BA, Heinssen R, Jeffries CD, Mathalon DH, McGlashan TH, Perkins DO, Seidman LJ, Tsuang MT, Walker EF, Woods SW, Kattan MW, 2016. An individualized risk calculator for research in prodromal psychosis. Am. J. Psychiatry 173, 980–988. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.15070890 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark US, Miller ER, Hegde RR, 2018. Experiences of Discrimination Are Associated With Greater Resting Amygdala Activity and Functional Connectivity. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 3, 367–378. 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.11.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colizzi M, Ruggeri M, Lasalvia A, 2020. Should we be concerned about stigma and discrimination in people at risk for psychosis? A systematic review. Psychol. Med 10.1017/S0033291720000148 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corcoran CM, Keilp JG, Kayser J, Klim C, Butler PD, Bruder GE, Gur RC, Javitt DC, 2015. Emotion recognition deficits as predictors of transition in individuals at clinical high risk for schizophrenia: a neurodevelopmental perspective. Psychol. Med. 45, 2959–73. 10.1017/S0033291715000902 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corrigan PW, Watson AC, 2002. Understanding the impact of stigma on people with mental illness. World Psychiatry 1, 16–20. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corrigan PW, Watson AC, Byrne P, Davis KE, 2005. Mental illness stigma: Problem of public health or social justice? Soc. Work 10.1093/sw/50.4.363 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Croizet JC, Després G, Gauzins ME, Huguet P, Leyens JP, Méot A, 2004. Stereotype threat undermines intellectual performance by triggering a disruptive mental load. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 30, 721–731. 10.1177/0146167204263961 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeLuca JS, 2020. Conceptualizing Adolescent Mental Illness Stigma: Youth Stigma Development and Stigma Reduction Programs. Adolesc. Res. Rev. 10.1007/s40894-018-0106-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fusar-Poli P, Deste G, Smieskova R, Barlati S, Yung AR, Howes O, Stieglitz RD, Vita A, McGuire P, Borgwardt S, 2012. Cognitive functioning in prodromal psychosis: A meta-analysis. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 69, 562–571. 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.1592 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant PM, Beck AT, 2009. Defeatist beliefs as a mediator of cognitive impairment, negative symptoms, and functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 35, 798–806. 10.1093/schbul/sbn008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenwald AG, Mcghee DE, Schwartz JLK, 1998. Measuring Individual Differences in Implicit Cognition: The Implicit Association Test, Journal of Personality and Soclal Psychology. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gronholm PC, Thornicroft G, Laurens KR, Evans-Lacko S, 2017. Mental health-related stigma and pathways to care for people at risk of psychotic disorders or experiencing first-episode psychosis: A systematic review. Psychol. Med. 47, 1867–1879. 10.1017/S0033291717000344 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gur RC, Sara R, Hagendoorn M, Marom O, Hughett P, Macy L, Turner T, Bajcsy R, Posner A, Gur RE, 2002. A method for obtaining 3-dimensional facial expressions and its standardization for use in neurocognitive studies. J. Neurosci. Methods 115, 137–143. 10.1016/S0165-0270(02)00006-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haining K, Matrunola C, Mitchell L, Gajwani R, Gross J, Gumley AI, Lawrie SM, Schwannauer M, Schultze-Lutter F, Uhlhaas PJ, 2020. Neuropsychological deficits in participants at clinical high risk for psychosis recruited from the community: Relationships to functioning and clinical symptoms. Psychol. Med. 50, 77–85. 10.1017/S0033291718003975 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henry JD, Hippel C, Shapiro L, 2010. Stereotype threat contributes to social difficulties in people with schizophrenia. Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 49, 31–41. 10.1348/014466509X421963 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kohler CG, Walker JB, Martin EA, Healey KM, Moberg PJ, 2010. Facial emotion perception in schizophrenia: A meta-analytic review. Schizophr. Bull. 10.1093/schbul/sbn192 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krendl AC, Richeson JA, Kelley WM, Heatherton TF, 2008. The negative consequences of threat: A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of the neural mechanisms underlying women’s underperformance in math: Research article. Psychol. Sci. 19, 168–175. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02063.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larsen EM, Herrera S, Bilgrami ZR, Shaik RB, Crump F, Sarac C, Shen J, Yang LH, Corcoran CM, 2019. Self-stigma related feelings of shame and facial fear recognition in individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis: A brief report. Schizophr. Res. 10.1016/j.schres.2019.01.027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Link BG, Cullen FT, Struening E, Shrout PE, Dohrenwend BP, 1989. A Modified Labeling Theory Approach to Mental Disorders: An Empirical Assessment. Am. Sociol. Rev. 54, 400. 10.2307/2095613 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Link BG, Phelan JC, 2001. Conceptualizing Stigma. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 27, 363–385. 10.1146/annurev.soc.27.1.363 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Livingston JD, Boyd JE, 2010. Correlates and consequences of internalized stigma for people living with mental illness: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Soc. Sci. Med. 71, 2150–2161. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.09.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marx DM, Goff PA, 2005. Clearing the air: The effect of experimenter race on target’s test performance and subjective experience. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 44, 645–657. 10.1348/014466604X17948 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McFarlane WR, 2002. Multifamily Groups in the Treatment of Severe Psychiatric Disorders. Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- McFarlane WR, Levin B, Travis L, Lucas FL, Lynch S, Verdi M, Williams D, Adelsheim S, Calkins R, Carter CS, Cornblatt B, Taylor SF, Auther AM, McFarland B, Melton R, Migliorati M, Niendam T, Ragland JD, Sale T, Salvador M, Spring E, 2015. Clinical and functional outcomes after 2 years in the early detection and intervention for the prevention of psychosis multisite effectiveness trial. Schizophr. Bull. 41, 30–43. 10.1093/schbul/sbu108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller TJ, McGlashan TH, Rosen JL, Cadenhead K, Ventura J, McFarlane W, Perkins DO, Pearlson GD, Woods SW, 2003. Prodromal Assessment with the Structured Interview for Prodromal Syndromes and the Scale of Prodromal Symptoms: Predictive Validity, Interrater Reliability, and Training to Reliability, in: Schizophrenia Bulletin. DHHS Public Health Service, pp. 703–715. 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moritz S, Silverstein S, Beblo T, Ozaslan Z, 2020. Much of the Neurocognitive Impairment in Schizophrenia is Due to Factors Other Than Schizophrenia Itself: Implications for Research and Treatment. Schizophr. Bull. Open. 10.1093/schizbullopen/sgaa034 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Oexle N, Corrigan PW, 2018. Understanding mental illness stigma toward persons with multiple stigmatized conditions: Implications of intersectionality theory. Psychiatr. Serv. 69, 587–589. 10.1176/appi.ps.201700312 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Osborne J, 2006. Gender, Stereotype Threat, and Anxiety: Psychophysiological and Cognitive Evidence. Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. [Google Scholar]
- Pachankis JE, Hatzenbuehler ML, Wang K, Burton CL, Crawford FW, Phelan JC, Link BG, 2018. The Burden of Stigma on Health and Well-Being: A Taxonomy of Concealment, Course, Disruptiveness, Aesthetics, Origin, and Peril Across 93 Stigmas. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 44, 451–474. 10.1177/0146167217741313 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pennington CR, Heim D, Levy AR, Larkin DT, 2016. Twenty years of stereotype threat research: A review of psychological mediators. PLoS One 11. 10.1371/journal.pone.0146487 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinkham AE, Sasson NJ, Calkins ME, Richard J, Hughett P, Gur RE, Gur RC, 2008. The other-race effect in face processing among African American and Caucasian individuals with schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 165, 639–645. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07101604 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rüsch N, Corrigan PW, Heekeren K, Theodoridou A, Dvorsky D, Metzler S, Müller M, Walitza S, Rössler W, 2014. Well-being among persons at risk of psychosis: the role of self-labeling, shame, and stigma stress. Psychiatr. Serv. 65, 483–9. 10.1176/appi.ps.201300169 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rüsch N, Corrigan PW, Powell K, Rajah A, Olschewski M, Wilkniss S, Batia K, 2009a. A stress-coping model of mental illness stigma: II. Emotional stress responses, coping behavior and outcome. Schizophr. Res. 110, 65–71. 10.1016/j.schres.2009.01.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rüsch N, Corrigan PW, Wassel A, Michaels P, Olschewski M, Wilkniss S, Batia K, 2009b. A stress-coping model of mental illness stigma: I. Predictors of cognitive stress appraisal. Schizophr. Res. 110, 59–64. 10.1016/j.schres.2009.01.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rüsch N, Heekeren K, Theodoridou A, Müller M, Corrigan PW, Mayer B, Metzler S, Dvorsky D, Walitza S, Rössler W, 2015. Stigma as a stressor and transition to schizophrenia after one year among young people at risk of psychosis. Schizophr. Res. 166, 43–8. 10.1016/j.schres.2015.05.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saleem MM, Stowkowy J, Cadenhead KS, Cannon TD, Cornblatt BA, Mcglashan TH, Perkins DO, Seidman LJ, Tsuang MT, Walker EF, Woods SW, Addington J, 2014. Perceived discrimination in those at clinical high risk for psychosis. Early Interv. Psychiatry 8, 77–81. 10.1111/eip.12058 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shaikh M, Ellett L, Dutt A, Day F, Laing J, Kroll J, Petrella S, McGuire P, Valmaggia LR, 2016. Perceived ethnic discrimination and persecutory paranoia in individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis. Psychiatry Res. 241, 309–14. 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.05.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spencer SJ, Logel C, Davies PG, 2016. Stereotype Threat. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 67, 415–437. 10.1146/annurev-psych-073115-103235 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spencer SJ, Steele CM, Quinn DM, 1999. Stereotype Threat and Women’s Math Performance. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 35, 4–28. 10.1006/jesp.1998.1373 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Steele CM, Aronson J, 1995. Stereotype Threat and the Intellectual Test Performance of African Americans. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 69, 797–811. 10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stowkowy J, Liu L, Cadenhead KS, Cannon TD, Cornblatt BA, McGlashan TH, Perkins DO, Seidman LJ, Tsuang MT, Walker EF, Woods SW, Bearden CE, Mathalon DH, Addington J, 2016. Early traumatic experiences, perceived discrimination and conversion to psychosis in those at clinical high risk for psychosis. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 51, 497–503. 10.1007/s00127-016-1182-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tognin S, Catalan A, Modinos G, Kempton MJ, Bilbao A, Nelson B, Pantelis C, Riecher-Rössler A, Bressan R, Barrantes-Vidal N, Krebs MO, Nordentoft M, Ruhrmann S, Sachs G, Rutten BPF, van Os J, de Haan L, van der Gaag M, McGuire P, Valmaggia LR, 2020. Emotion Recognition and Adverse Childhood Experiences in Individuals at Clinical High Risk of Psychosis. Schizophr. Bull. 46, 823–833. 10.1093/schbul/sbz128 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turan JM, Elafros MA, Logie CH, Banik S, Turan B, Crockett KB, Pescosolido B, Murray SM, 2019. Challenges and opportunities in examining and addressing intersectional stigma and health. BMC Med 17. 10.1186/s12916-018-1246-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Donkersgoed RJM, Wunderink L, Nieboer R, Aleman A, Pijnenborg GHM, 2015. Social cognition in individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis: A meta-analysis. PLoS One 10. 10.1371/journal.pone.0141075 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D, 1999. Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX. [Google Scholar]
- Yang LH, Link BG, Ben-David S, Gill KE, Girgis RR, Brucato G, Wonpat-Borja AJ, Corcoran CM, 2015. Stigma related to labels and symptoms in individuals at clinical high-risk for psychosis. Schizophr. Res. 168, 9–15. 10.1016/j.schres.2015.08.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang LH, Wonpat-Borja AJ, Opler MG, Corcoran CM, 2010. Potential stigma associated with inclusion of the psychosis risk syndrome in the DSM-V: an empirical question. Schizophr. Res. 120, 42–8. 10.1016/j.schres.2010.03.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang LH, Woodberry KA, Link BG, Corcoran CM, Bryant C, Shapiro DI, Downing D, Girgis RR, Brucato G, Huang D, Crump FM, Verdi M, McFarlane WR, Seidman LJ, 2019. Impact of “psychosis risk” identification: Examining predictors of how youth view themselves. Schizophr. Res. 208, 300–307. 10.1016/j.schres.2019.01.037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]