Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2023 Mar 24;13:4831. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-31907-y

Evidence for cultural differences in affect during mother–infant interactions

Miada Abu Salih 1,#, Maayan Abargil 1,#, Saja Badarneh 1, Nathalie klein Selle 2, Merav Irani 1, Shir Atzil 1,
PMCID: PMC10039016  PMID: 36964204

Abstract

Maternal care is considered a universal and even cross-species set of typical behaviors, which are necessary to determine the social development of children. In humans, most research on mother–infant bonding is based on Western cultures and conducted in European and American countries. Thus, it is still unknown which aspects of mother–infant behaviors are universal and which vary with culture. Here we test whether typical mother–infant behaviors of affect-communication and affect-regulation are equally represented during spontaneous interaction in Palestinian-Arab and Jewish cultures. 30 Palestinian-Arab and 43 Jewish mother–infant dyads were recruited and videotaped. Using AffectRegulation Coding System (ARCS), we behaviorally analyzed the second-by-second display of valence and arousal in each participant and calculated the dynamic patterns of affect co-regulation. The results show that Palestinian-Arab infants express more positive valence than Jewish infants and that Palestinian-Arab mothers express higher arousal compared to Jewish mothers. Moreover, we found culturally-distinct strategies to regulate the infant: increased arousal in Palestinian-Arab dyads and increased mutual affective match in Jewish dyads. Such cross-cultural differences in affect indicate that basic features of emotion that are often considered universal are differentially represented in different cultures. Affect communication and regulation patterns can be transmitted across generations in early-life socialization with caregivers.

Subject terms: Human behaviour, Psychology

Introduction

Emotion regulation is the process of modulating the duration or amplitude of internal feeling states1 in an instrumental way2 for the service of self and social adaptation3,4. Emotion regulation is fundamental for health and well-being throughout the lifespan57. Deficits in emotion regulation underlie myriad forms of mental health adversities8,9 and contribute to deficits in relationships10, decision-making11, and job performance12. It is well known that emotion regulation develops over early childhood6,13 into adolescence14 and is thought to be scaffolded by caregivers’ socialization behaviors1519. Although it is intuitive that parental care may promote infants’ emotion regulation, the behavioral mechanisms by which caregiver behavior translates into infant regulation is still relatively little understood.

Maternal care is considered a universal and even cross-species set of typical behaviors2023, which are necessary to determine the social, cognitive, and emotion development of children2426. Among the most typical and basic behaviors that mothers and infants spontaneously demonstrate are affect communication and affect regulation27. Affect communication and regulation are considered universal ingredients of mother–infant interactions2830. However, the universality of affective communication in the mother–infant relationship requires further investigation, as the majority of research on the mother–infant relationship is based on Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic (WEIRD) cultures, and conducted in European and American countries25,26.

Communication of affect is a core ingredient in mother–infant interaction31,32 and refers to the social communication of arousal and valence33. Since human infants are born helpless in maintaining their ongoing physiological demands, they depend on their caregivers for biological and psychological regulation and communicate their regulatory needs using affective cues33. Infants communicate negative changes in regulation (e.g., pain, hunger, frustration, or fatigue) by frowning, fussing, or crying. Infants communicate positive changes in regulation (e.g., soothing, joy and interest) by smiling, laughing, and engaging. Thus, infants’ affective signals bare information about their regulatory requirements, which is salient for caregivers. Accordingly, caregivers tend to be highly attentive to their infant's affective cues, which effectively elicit parental dopaminergic secretion33 and parental care behaviors34. This interactive process allows the parent to learn and predict the infant's needs to provide attuned care, which is defined here as social regulation of allostasis3537. Affect communication is thus necessary for the social regulation of allostasis15. Moreover, parents regulate the infant affect via affective attunement, which supports infant development35,3840, as through affective communication and affective attunement caregivers teach infants emotion concepts, self-regulation, and the value of phenomena in the environment15,28,41. Affect is thus a basic feature of the parent–infant relationship, that is central to infant survival and development27,38,4245. Despite being a basic and necessary feature, it is still unknown which aspects of affect are universal and which vary with culture.

Amongst affect, cultural differences were found in a variety of mother–infant behaviors46. For example, African mothers talk less to their infants compared to mothers from the Far East, Europe, and South and North America47. Indian mothers and infants tend to vocalize simultaneously compared to French and American mothers and infants, who take turns in vocalizations48. Moreover, Lebanese infants show a faster vocal response to their mother compared to Western infants49. Cross-cultural differences were also found in attachment styles, as in western cultures the dominant insecure attachment style is avoidant, whereas in non-western cultures the dominant insecure attachment style is resistant50. Such cross-cultural differences were suggested to rely on different cultural values and beliefs about parenting51. The cultural differences in attachment are of particular interest, given the previously reported association between attachment and the development of affect regulation, which is necessary for resilience52. For example, securely attached children were reported to show increased affective communication and to modulate their emotional responses53. Thus, cross-cultural differences in attachment could be routed in cultural differences in affect communication.

In adults, while some studies refer to affect communication54,55 and affect attunement56 as universal, other studies show that affect communication varies between cultures57. For example, the understanding of certain facial motions as prototypical of a specific emotion is based on cultural knowledge58,59. Moreover, some facial expressions are conveyed and recognized only in specific cultures60. Studies also show cross-cultural variation in the intensity of emotion expression61,62. Like affect, the tendency to synchronize in attunement with a partner during interaction is mostly considered a universal phenomenon63. Yet, studies show cross-cultural variation in some aspects of interpersonal interactions64. For example, while in Western countries only positive interactions are considered synchronous65,66, in Japan negative interactions are perceived as synchronous as well67.

The Arab society is under-studied6870 and Palestinian Arabs are particularly under-represented in scientific investigation across disciplines, including social, medical, and psychological research71, and particularly in the field of maternal bonding. Along with East-Asian cultures, Arab cultures are considered "Eastern" collectivist cultures72,73. Collectivist cultures emphasize family values such as family cohesion, mutual assistance, and close relations with extended family members74. However, Arab cultures are distinguished from East-Asian collectivistic cultures in multiple aspects, including geography, language, family structure75, expression of values76, and attitude toward authority7779. Unjustified generalization of cultures diminishes distinguished cultural features80. Moreover, it can indirectly lead to the consistent exclusion of certain populations and their misrepresentation in scientific literature81. For example, while Eastern collectivistic cultures, such as the Indian and Sudanese, have low post-partum depression82, the postpartum depression rates in Arab mothers are higher than in Western cultures immediately after birth83. This emphasizes the importance of including Arab mothers in medical research.

In contrast to the Palestinian-Arab society, the Israeli-Jewish society has been extensively studied in the context of mother–infant relations8489. The Jewish population in Israel tends to resemble Western cultures more than the Palestinian-Arab society90,91. Unlike the collectivist family identity of Arab culture72,73, the Jewish family identity has shifted towards diversity, individualism92, and egalitarianism91,93,94. Despite modernization processes seen in the Palestinian-Arab society, a cultural gap in values between the two societies is still evident95, with potential consequences to parenting and family relationships. For example, Arab adolescents report having a closer relationship with their parents compared to Jewish adolescents91, and Arab adults tend to rely on familial help to manage emotional problems, rather than professional help96. Along these lines, Israeli-Jewish children describe a less cohesive family structure compared to Israeli-Arab children90.

It is of particular interest to compare Palestinian-Arab and Jewish cultures living in Israel. Palestinian-Arab and Jewish societies are involved in a long-standing conflict on one hand, and are based on proximal geographical territories and sharing environmental impact on the other hand. Thus, this specific investigation represents a cultural comparison that goes beyond the common east vs. west or WEIRD vs. non-WEIRD studies, which can shed light on additional sources of cultural differences and similarities in affect. A few cross-cultural studies that include Palestinian-Arab mothers were conducted in Israel, in which the population comprises a relatively individualistic Jewish society (74.4%) and a more collectivistic Palestinian-Arab society (20.9%)97. It was demonstrated that Palestinian-Arab parents better adapt to work following the first childbirth compared to Jewish parents, as infants were cared for by a family member98. One previous study compared Jewish and Palestinian-Arab parent–infant dyads and found increased social gaze and active touch in Jewish dyads compared to Palestinian-Arab dyads, which showed increased proximity between parent and infant and reduced negative emotionality compared to Jewish dyads99. Interestingly, Jewish parents demonstrated higher levels of parental reciprocity with the infant during interaction compared to Palestinian-Arab parents98. With that, despite the different patterns of parent and infant behavior, levels of infant self-regulation98 and attachment styles100 were comparable, suggesting that different behavioral patterns can achieve similar developmental purposes. Synthesizing these findings points out that affective communication in the parent-infant dyad may depend on culture. Specifically, research in adults suggests that Jewish and Palestinian mothers may have distinct patterns of affective communication101,102. Moreover, developmental research on the impact of parents on infant development of emotion regulation99,103105, points out that the cultural differences in maternal affect may underlie a culturally distinct pattern of affect regulation in Jewish and Palestinian infants.

Here we use Affect Regulation Coding System (ARCS), which captures in a dynamic way how valence and arousal are spontaneously communicated by infants and mothers during free interactions, to test whether affect communication and co-regulation show cross-cultural variation among Jewish and Palestinian-Arab families. Using ARCS, we aim to discover (1) whether there are cultural differences in the regulation of valence and arousal in Palestinian and Jewish mothers; (2) whether those mediate further cultural differences in the regulation of valence and arousal of Palestinian and Jewish infants; and (3) whether Palestinian and Jewish mothers use different strategies to regulate their infants.

Results

Cultural differences in mother–infant valence and arousal

Multivariate GLM demonstrates a statistically significant difference between cultures in Global Scores of valence and arousal during mother–infant interactions: Palestinian-Arab mothers have increased arousal compared to Jewish mothers (F (1, 69) = 26.068, p < 0.001), and Palestinian-Arab infants have more positive valence compared to Jewish infants (F (1, 69) = 4.561, p = 0.036) (Fig. 1). Specifically, Maternal arousal: Palestinian-Arab mothers have higher arousal while interacting with their infant compared to Jewish mothers (Arab: M = 6.87, SE = 0.19, 95% CI 6.8, 7.25]; Jewish: M = 5.47, SE = 0.16, 95% CI [5.16, 5.79], p < 0.001, 95% CI [− 1.480, − 0.495]). (C) Infant valence: Palestinian-Arab infants are tending to show increased valence when interacting with their mothers compared to Jewish infants (Arabs: M = 0.103, SE = 0.049, 95% CI [0.005, 0.201]; Jewish: M = − 0.41, SE = 0.04, 95% CI [− 0.121, 0.039]; p = 0.036, 95% CI [0.01, 0.279]). (D) Maternal valence: there is no significant difference in valence among Palestinian-Arab and Jewish (Arabs: M = 0.652, SE = 0.06, 95% CI [0.534, 0.770]; Jewish: M = 0.603, SE = 0.05, 95% CI [0.507, 0.700]; p = 0.466, 95% CI [− 0.211, 0.114]). (E) Infant arousal: there is no significant difference between Palestinian-Arab and Jewish infants in arousal when interacting with their mothers (Arabs: (M = 4.13, SE = 0.173, 95% CI [3.056, 3.719]; Jewish: M = 3.73, SE = 0.141, 95% CI [3.45, 4.02]; p = 0.267, 95% CI [− 0.707, 0.199]).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Cultural differences in valence and arousal during free mother–infant interaction. (A) The horizontal axis tracks the expression of valence, and the vertical axis tracks the intensity of the affective expression. Jewish participants are represented in shades of blue (dark for the mothers and light for the infants), and Palestinian-Arab participants are represented in shades of yellow (dark for the mothers and light for the infants). (B) Cultural differences are found in maternal arousal. (C) Cultural differences are found in infant valence. (D) Cultural differences were not found in maternal valence. (E) Cultural differences were not found in infant arousal.

Mediation analysis on the relationship of culture, infants’ valence, and mothers’ arousal

Mediation analysis demonstrates that the cultural difference in infant valence is mediated by maternal arousal (Simple Mediation Analysis: Total effect = 0.14 p = 0.036, Direct effect = 0.017 p = 0.82, Indirect effect = 0.13 95% CI [0.049, 0.23]) (Fig. 2). We also examine a mediation model with the opposite direction, where infant valence is a mediator of the cultural effects in maternal arousal (Total effect = 1.396, p < 0.001, Direct effect = 1.193 p < 0.001, Indirect effect = 0.203, 95% CI [0.035, 0.402]). In the first mediation model, the direct affect becomes insignificant when including the mediator, suggesting that the variation in infant valence is explained by maternal arousal. In the second model, the direct effect remained significant, suggesting that while some of the variation in maternal arousal is explained by the infant valence, culture also directly explains the variation in maternal arousal. Importantly, since the data are not longitudinal these mediation analyses cannot count for causality.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Maternal arousal mediates the association between culture and infants’ valence.

Cultural differences in affect regulation

Cultural differences were not found in dynamic synchrony but were found in maternal responses during events of infant regulation and in mutual affective match.

No cultural differences found in mother–infant dynamic synchrony of valence and arousal

Results demonstrate individual differences in dyadic synchrony of valence and arousal in both cultures, where some mothers and infants are highly synchronized, and some are poorly synchronized. No cultural differences were found in the distributions of dynamic synchrony of valence and arousal (Valence: t (71) =  − 0.97, p = 0.336, two-tailed; Jewish dyads mean r = 0.108, 95% CI [0.066, 0.15], Palestinian-Arab dyads mean r = 0.080, 95% CI [− 0.050, 0.066]. Arousal: t (65) =  − 0.05, p = 0.964 two-tailed; Jewish dyads mean r = 0.19, 95% CI [0.143, 0.237], Palestinian-Arab dyads (mean r = 0.19, 95% CI [0.150, 0.230]) (Figure S2).

Cultural differences in maternal affect during infant affective regulation

Cultural differences were found in how mothers respond to events of infant regulation (Fig. 3). ARCS provides second-by-second measures of dyadic affect, which enables to assess in a dynamic way how maternal affect unfolds during specific events of infant affective regulation. Maternal responding was assessed during four types of infant regulatory events: infant engagement into play, infant unwinding, infant distress, and infant calming down. The results show that Palestinian-Arab mothers maintain higher arousal compared to Jewish mothers regardless of the infant's affect. However, Palestinian-Arab mothers maintained more positive valence compared to Jewish mothers only at times of infant distress (Fig. 3). See statistical analysis for analyses of all time points in Supplementary Table S2.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Maternal changes in valence and arousal during infant engagement, distress, unwinding, and calming down. The X-axes represent time, where multiple events of infant play or distress are temporally aligned in time at x = 0. Y-axes represent maternal social display of valence or arousal during infant play, distress, unwinding, and calming down. Error bars represent 1 standard error (SE). (A) Maternal arousal during infant engagement. (B) Maternal valence during infant engagement. (C) Maternal arousal during infant unwinding. (D) Maternal valence during infant unwinding. (E) Maternal arousal during infant distress. (F) Maternal valence during infant distress. (G) Maternal arousal during infant calming down. (H) Maternal valence during infant calming down.

Maternal arousal during infant play

Palestinian-Arab mothers express elevated levels of arousal compared to Jewish mothers during events of infant engagement into play (Repeated Measure GLM, F = 18.25, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.091). While Palestinian-Arab mothers remain in high arousal throughout the infant transition into play, Jewish mothers show an increase in arousal that precedes the infant transition into play (Δ stages [before] vs [transition] =  − 2.32, p = 0.021 two-tailed, 95% CI [− 0.789, − 0.066]) (Fig. 3A).

Maternal valence during infant play

There is no cultural difference in maternal valence between Palestinian-Arab and Jewish dyads during infant engagement into play (Repeated Measure GLM, F = 0.02, p = 0.889, η2 = 0.00) (Fig. 3B). Palestinian-Arab and Jewish mothers show a gradual increase in positive valence that precedes the moment of infant positive transition (Palestinian-Arab mothers: Δ stages [before] vs [transition]: t = − 2.16, p = 0.034 two-tailed, 95% CI [− 0.153, − 0.006], Δ stages [before] vs [after]: t = − 2.32, p = 0.022 two-tailed, 95% CI [− 0.159, − 0.012]; Jewish mothers: Δ stages [before] vs [transition]: t = − 1.92, p = 0.056 two-tailed, 95% CI [− 0.147, 0.002]) (Fig. 3B).

Maternal arousal during infant unwinding

Palestinian-Arab mothers express elevated levels of arousal compared to Jewish mothers during events of infant unwinding (Repeated Measure GLM, F = 26.25, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.134). While Palestinian-Arab mothers remain in high arousal throughout the infant transition into neutral, Jewish mothers show a decrease in arousal that precedes the infant transition into neutral (Δ stages [before] vs [after]: t = 2.5, p = 0.013 two-tailed, 95% CI [0.116, 0.995]) (Fig. 3C).

Maternal valence during infant unwinding

There is no cultural difference in maternal valence between Palestinian-Arab and Jewish dyads (Repeated Measure GLM, F = 0.001, p = 0.972, η2 = 0.00.) Palestinian-Arab mothers show a gradual decrease in positive valence that precedes the moment of infant transition from positive to neutral, a similar trend is observed in Jewish mothers (Palestinian-Arab mothers: Δ stages [before] vs [after]: t = 2.15, p = 0.035 two-tailed, 95% CI [0.006, 0.165]; Jewish mothers: Δ stages [before] vs [after]: t = 1.38, p = 0. 352 two-tailed, 95% CI [− 0.019, 0.106]) (Fig. 3D).

Maternal arousal during infant distress

Palestinian-Arab mothers express overall elevated levels of arousal compared to Jewish mothers during events of infant distress (Repeated Measure GLM, F = 10.68, p = 0.001, η2 = 0.09). There were no significant temporal changes in maternal arousal (Fig. 3E).

Maternal valence during infant distress

There is no cultural difference in maternal valence between Palestinian-Arab and Jewish dyads (Repeated Measure GLM, F = 1.542, p = 0.217, η2 = 0.014). Jewish mothers show a gradual decrease in valence that precedes the moment of infant display of distress, a similar trend was observed in Palestinian Arab mothers (Jewish mothers: Δ stages [before] vs [after] t = 2.72, p = 0.007 two-tailed, 95% CI [0.07, 0.441]; Palestinian-Arab mothers: Δ stages [before] vs [after]t = 2.01, p = 0.11 two-tailed, 95% CI [− 0.05, 0.342]) (Fig. 3F).

Maternal arousal during infant calming down

Palestinian-Arab mothers express elevated levels of arousal compared to Jewish mothers during events of infant transition from negative to neutral (Repeated Measure GLM, F = 27.396, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.209). Palestinian-Arab mothers remain in high arousal throughout the infant transition into neutral, whereas Jewish mothers remain in low arousal throughout the infant transition to neutral. There were no significant temporal changes in maternal arousal (Fig. 3G).

Maternal valence during infant calming down

There is no cultural difference in maternal valence during events of infant calming down (Repeated Measure GLM, F = 1.424, p = 0.235, η2 = 0.014). There were no significant temporal changes in maternal valence (Fig. 3H).

Cultural differences in mother–infant affective matching

Both Palestinian-Arab and Jewish dyads show strong valence mutual match, while Jewish dyads are more matched in their arousal (Fig. 4). The mutual affective match is the Pearson correlation between the Global Valence or Arousal Scores of the mothers and the infants.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Mother–infant mutual affective match in Arab and Jewish cultures. (A) Mutual match between mothers and their infants in arousal only in Jewish dyads. (B) Mutual match between mothers and their infants in valence in Jewish and Palestinian-Arab dyads.

Mutual match in arousal

Mothers and infants are highly correlated in their global scores of arousal in Jewish dyads (r = 0.533, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.284, 0.731]), and not significantly correlated in Palestinian-Arab dyads (r = 0.038, p = 0.843, 95% CI [− 0.318, 0.343]). This cultural difference is significant (z = 2.23, p = 0.026, two-tailed).

Mutual match in valence

Mothers and infants are highly correlated in their global scores of valence in both Jewish and Palestinian-Arab dyads (Jewish, r = 0.664, p < 0.001, 95% CI [0.438, 0.833], Palestinian-Arab, r = 0.484, p = 0.007, 95% CI [0.144, 0.728]). There is no statistical difference in valence mutual match between Arab and Jewish cultures (z = 1.09, p = 0.275, two-tailed).

Discussion

This study demonstrates that basic features of maternal and infants’ affect during free interaction, including affect communication and co-regulation, are not universal and show cross-cultural variation. Specifically, Palestinian-Arab mothers show elevated arousal and Palestinian-Arab infants display more positive valence during free interaction compared to Jewish dyads. The levels of maternal arousal mediate the cross-cultural difference in infant valence. While Palestinian-Arab mothers maintain stable levels of high arousal, Jewish mothers show variation in their arousal levels, which co-varies with infant arousal. During times of infant distress, Palestinian-Arab mothers maintained more positive valence compared to Jewish mothers who were more matched to the infant valence. This points to a cross-cultural difference in maternal strategies for infant regulation: Palestinian-Arab mothers maintain more positive infants, while Jewish mothers maintain a better mutual affective match with their infants. This study shows that basic maternal behaviors that are often considered typical and universal manifest differently across cultures, suggesting that mother–infant interactions can serve as a mechanism via which cultural socialization of emotion can be transmitted between generations106.

While no cultural differences were found in dynamic synchronization across the entire interaction, ARCS enables us to zoom into discrete moments of infants’ regulatory transitions, where cultural differences do emerge. The results show that infant engagement is consistently preceded by upregulation of positive valence in Palestinian-Arab mothers, and upregulation of both valence and arousal in Jewish mothers, potentially as a strategy to engage the infant. When calming down, Jewish mothers down-regulate their arousal in attunement with the infant, unlike Palestinian-Arab mothers who maintain high arousal while their infants calm down. Moreover, unlike Jewish mothers, Palestinian-Arab mothers use positive valence to soothe their distressed infants. With that, despite the cultural specifications during positive transitions, both Palestinian-Arab and Jewish mothers are equally attuned to their infants during negative transitions (distress and unwinding). The cross-cultural commonality in the response to infant distress could reflect its immediate allostatic consequences that are relevant for survival. On the other hand, in times of infant engagement, cultural expressions become more pronounced.

The elevated arousal of the Palestinian-Arab mothers and the increased mutual affective match of the Jewish mothers can represent two strategies of infant regulation. Higher arousal was found to be related to greater infant attention to their mothers107. Parent-infant similarities were also found to promote joined attention, as well as the development of complex affective expression, and the ability to abstract108. Therefore, the high levels of arousal that were found in the Palestinian-Arab mothers and the affective match in Jewish mothers can both serve as strategies for the social regulation of infants’ attention that maintain their engagement. This suggests that while the need to maintain infants engaged and regulated is a universal feature of parental care, different regulation strategies can be applied, and those may vary cross-culturally.

Adult literature reported that Palestinian-Arab and Jewish cultures show differential patterns of affective communication101,102. Tracing cultural differences previously reported in adults to the early mother–infant bond can advance our understanding of socialization of affect and emotion. The study demonstrates that Palestinian-Arab infants show more positive affect while interacting with their mothers and that this cross-cultural effect is mediated by maternal arousal. This result is consistent with previous cross-cultural studies that found that Arab infants showed fewer negative affect compared to Jewish infants99. Collectivist cultures associate positive feelings with social harmony109, which may elicit maintained positivity in Arab culture. Accordingly, the high arousal of the Palestinian-Arab mothers in our study may reflect an effort to keep the infant engaged in a positive state. Together, these findings in adults and infants point out that self-regulation can be rooted in cultural norms, and sculpted at an early age via mother–infant interactions110. By responding to the infant's affective signals, mothers and other caregivers provide immediate regulation but also help the infant to become more independent in self-regulation39,111114. Indeed, from an early age, infants learn to adjust their response to their caregiver's behavior115, and thus parents can shape their infant's behavior according to their cultural patterns. Therefore, the culture-specific pattern of both mothers’ and infants’ behaviors can represent a behavioral mechanism for the cross-cultural transmission of behavioral norms.

Despite cultural differences, a similar percentage of mothers chose to interact in a face-to-face position (87% of Palestinian-Arab mothers, and 79% of Jewish mothers). Previous research has demonstrated that face-to-face positions are common in individualist cultures, which are characterized by active eye-gaze transactions. In collectivist cultures, there is more physical proximity and continuous physical contact with less direct eye-gaze interactions99,116. This suggests that the individualist/collectivist distinction may not apply in comparing Jewish to Palestinian-Arabs in Israel and emphasizes the importance of studying specific cultures. Another cultural similarity was demonstrated in dynamic synchrony, where no cultural differences emerged, as a similar variance of individual differences in dyadic synchrony of valence and arousal was observed in both cultures. Such distribution of individual differences in mother–infant synchrony aligns with previous research117,118, demonstrating the reliability of ARCS measurements. This supports previous research proposing that the tendency to synchronize in attunement with a partner during interaction is a universal phenomenon63. Future cross-cultural research is needed to determine this in many individuals across multiple cultures.

This research holds some limitations. The extent of the compatibility of experimental constructs to both cultures, such as the behavioral coding of valence and arousal, may vary as specific measurements can be more prominent in certain cultural settings. Moreover, research constructs can be defined differently, depending on culture, and impact the results of the study115. However, importantly, the method used in this research was developed by Palestinian-Arab and Jewish scientists on a sample of both Palestinian-Arab and Jewish populations. Another limitation is that the developmental consequences of such cultural differences were not assessed in this study. Previous cross-cultural longitudinal research found that similar maternal behaviors, such as maternal intrusiveness in infancy, can lead to different outcomes across cultures119. In this study, the statistical control over infant ages interferes with assessing the developmental trajectory of cultural differences. Thus, we are unable to determine whether Jewish and Palestinian-Arabs mothers adapt differently to their infants’ affective development. This possibility is supported by the second mediation model showing that infant valence partly mediates the cultural differences in maternal arousal. Given prior literature suggesting that early-life socialization is a bidirectional process120,121, future longitudinal research is needed to determine the developmental outcomes of the early cross-cultural differences found here on both infants and parents, whether they remain consistent, and how they affect child development.

In sum, cultural differences in affect communication and regulation are evident in early life. Such cross-cultural differences indicate that basic features of emotion that are often considered universal, are differentially represented in different cultures. Most studies of cultural differences between mothers and children focus on comparing east with west, or WEIRD with Non-WEIRD cultures. Thus, including the Palestinian-Arab culture that is underrepresented in medical and psychological research, and comparing it to the geographically adjacent Jewish culture, can provide novel evidence on the origins of cultural similarities and differences.

Methods

Participants

Seventy-three mother–infant dyads participated in the study, 30 mothers who identified as Palestinian-Arabs (13 female infants and 17 males) and 43 mothers who identified as Jewish (17 female infants, 26 males), all of them hold Israeli citizenship. Infants’ ages ranged between 2.5 and 48 weeks and mothers’ ages ranged between 22 and 47 years. The sample size was determined with power analysis calculated for multivariate General Linear Model (GLM) using G*Power software122, given an effect size of 0.4, and power of 0.8 for comparing mother–infant interactions within two different cultures, relying on data from our previous research on mother–infant bonding behavior123,124. Palestinian-Arab and Jewish families were matched in demographic variables including education (Mean Jewish years of education = 16.88 [SD = 1.48], Mean Palestinian-Arab years of education = 15.67 [SD = 1.83]), income level (Jewish income score = 3.3 (SD = 1.66), Palestinian-Arab income score = 3.23 (SD = 1.73) and marital status (all mothers were married). Group differences were found in infant age (Mean Jewish infant age = 13.5 weeks; Mean Palestinian-Arab infant age = 24 weeks) and the number of children in the family (Mean Jewish = 1.6, Mean Palestinian-Arab = 2.4), accordingly, all the statistical analyses that compare Jewish to Arab cultures control for variability explained by these variables, unless otherwise stated. The Institution Ethics Committee of the Hebrew University approved the study, the research protocol was performed in accordance with ethics guidelines, and all the mothers signed informed consent before participation and were remunerated for their participation. And filled out questionnaires in the presence of the experimenter, so that it was possible to get used to his presence at this time.

Procedure

Mothers were recruited for the study via social media ads. Mothers were not informed about the research questions before participation to optimize the ecological validity of the experimental procedure. Participation included questionnaires and a video recording of a 2-min free interaction with the infant. Mothers spend up to twenty minutes participating and were remunerated. Videos were captured in the families’ homes or their local community center. Trained research personnel filmed the interaction while in the room, based on our previous research33,34,42,125. Mothers and infants had time to adjust to the research personnel before filming. Interactions were filmed using one camera, while research personnel confirmed that the face and body of both dyadic partners are captured in the frame throughout the interaction33,34,42,125. Seconds in which faces were not captured were removed from the analysis (Infants’ face: mean = 1.47 s, STD = 4.15, median = 0; Mothers’ face: mean = 0.59 s, STD = 2.04, median = 0). There were no missing data for participants’ bodies. It is acknowledged despite the relatively naturalistic out-of-lab setting of the filming, the presence of the experimenter may affect the spontaneous behavior of mothers and infants. Mothers were instructed to freely interact with their infants as they usually do, without any restrictions, specific toys, or guidelines. This setting was found to be highly representative of natural mother–infant interactions126,127, as it allows each mother to determine her own setup and interaction choices and tendencies with no constraints. Interestingly, 87% of Palestinian-Arab and 79% of Jewish mothers chose to interact in a face-to-face position.

Capturing 2-min interactions optimizes the trade-off between the high validity of the data on one hand and minimal intrusion to post-partum women and their infants on the other hand. Accordingly, an experimental setup of 2-min mother–infant interactions is commonly applied38,123125,128135. While short and thus susceptible to being affected by non-representative events, this data is rich in spontaneous interactive behaviors, providing a multi-dimensional matrix of 120 repeated data points per each behavioral variable for each dyadic partner. Such a matrix is highly detailed and more ecologically valid compared to questionnaires or lab-based tasks. To confirm the reliability of each measurement across the interaction, a Kolmogorov–Smirnov test was applied (Supplementary analysis 2). The free mother–infant interactions were then imported to the lab and behaviorally coded and analyzed using ARCS by trained personnel.

Behavioral analyses

Trained coders coded behavior in the videos of the mothers and infants interacting. Coders tracked concrete behaviors that represent the expression of valence and arousal. The coders tracked the second-by-second behavioral display of valence and arousal, separately for each ARCS scale for each dyadic partner, throughout the interaction (see ARCS scales in Table 1). Coders observed the videos of the interactions on Windows Media Player and stopped after each second to code the specific ARCS scale. Each second during the interaction video was given a score per each ARCS scale. The coding is then quantified either as global scores, i.e., average across 120 s (in Figs. 1, 2, 4), or used in temporal analyses as second-by-second value (Fig. 3 and dynamic synchrony analysis).

Table 1.

ARCS—affect regulation coding system.

Axis Behavioral parameter Codes
Valence Facial expressions

Negative (for example crying, angry face, curving the lips downwards)

Neutral (the absence of signs of negative or positive valence)

Positive (for example smiling, laughing, curling the lips upwards)

Arousal 1. Physical effort—bodily movement

(− 2) Sleeping

(− 1) Falling asleep

(0) Calm with no movement

(1) Subtle movement (for example, gentle hand movements, petting an object)

(2) Significant movement (for example, large movements with the hands, lifting objects)

(3) Intensive movement (for example, a movement that involves the whole body, walking)

2. Agency—gaze and attention

(0) Unfocused look (for example staring into space

(1) Directed gaze (for example, the gaze is focused on a specific object

(2) Switching gaze (for example, the gaze is moving from one object to another)

3. Vocalization—pitch and intensity

Mothers:

(0) No vocalization (for example the mother is silence)

(1) Low pitch (for example the mother whispered)

(2) Adult speech (for example the mother talk regularly)

(3) High pitch intensity (for example the mother talking in a high pitch, the mother is talking in a childish way or storytelling)

Infants:

(0) The infant is silent

(1) The infant makes small noises or weak syllables

(2) The infant makes a sound at regular volume

(3) The infant cries, laughs or shouts

4. Expression intensity—the amplitude of the facial expression

(0) No facial effort (no facial expression)

(1) Vocalization with no expression)

(2) Mild expression (for example, a weak smile)

(3) Strong expression (for example, an expression of surprise, a big smile, a sullen expression)

ARCS—affect regulation coding system

Valence

Valence codes included scores of 1 (positive), 0 (neutral), or − 1 (negative), based on the pleasantness of the facial expression.

Arousal

The arousal score is based on coding the behavioral expressions of four continuous sub-scales: (1) physical effort (measured with body motion)136; (2) agency (measured with gaze and attention)137,138; (3) vocalizations (measured with pitch and intensity of the vocalization)139; and (4) expression intensity (measured with by the amplitude of the facial expression). More information on how to apply ARCS will be freely provided by the corresponding author upon request.

Compute regulation scores of mothers and infants during interaction

After the initial coding of infants’ and mothers’ valence and arousal, we used MATLAB R2016a (The MathWorks, Natick, MA) and Python (version 3.7) to plot the scores of each subject on a bi-dimensional space of valence and arousal140, and computed score for General Valence (Eq. 1), General Arousal (Eq. 2), and dynamic measurements of regulation during the interaction (Fig. 1).

Equation (1) :General Valence Score: across 120 s of the interaction (N).

v¯=viN

Equation (2): General Arousal Score: relies on a summary of four subscales: physical effort, agency, vocalization, and expression intensity (Table 1), which were coded separately. Then, the four sub-scales were normalized on a scale of 0–10 and averaged per each second. Last, for each participant, the multi-scale score is averaged across 120 s (N).

a¯=aieffort+aiagency+aivocalization+aiexpressionintensityN

Inter-rater reliability

For each ARCS variable, at least 8% of videos were coded by multiple coders to validate the reliability of ARCS coding. The reliability was calculated for the entire interaction. Krippendorff’s alpha test was used to estimate the inter-rater reliability between coders, given its accuracy in estimating the level of agreement between raters for categorical variables with multiple levels141. The inter-rater reliability scores for maternal behavior: Krippendorff’s α = 0.85; and for infant behaviors: Krippendorff’s α = 0.89, which is considered moderate to high141 (see Supplementary Table S1 for reliability scores of each variable).

Validity of the coding scheme

The empirical implementation of the axes relies on previous literature, showing that the behavioral display of physical effort136, agency and attention137,138, and vocalization139 are related to levels of arousal. It also relies on previous methods that code affect or valence during interactions42,125. ARCS is rooted in the theoretical framework of the affective circumplex by James Russel140 and sets to map the interplay of valence and arousal on a bi-dimensional space in two partners simultaneously, to trace dyadic patterns of co-regulation. ARCS analyses demonstrate expected phenomena, including expected variation in parent-infant dynamic synchrony142, and arousal responsivity to salient stimuli143.

Test the cultural differences in arousal, valence, and co-regulation

Test the cultural differences of arousal and valence in mothers and infants

The General Valence and Arousal scores of mothers and infants in each cultural group were mapped on a bi-dimensional space of valence and arousal144 (Palestinian-Arab = 30, Jewish = 43). We applied a multivariate GLM to test the group differences in general valence and arousal scores between Palestinian-Arab and Jewish mothers and infants (Fig. 1).

Test if maternal affect mediates the cross-cultural difference in infants’ affect

We used a Simple Mediation Model with 1000 bootstrap samples to calculate the direct effect of culture on infant affect, and whether the cross-cultural differences in infant affect are mediated by maternal affect, or whether the cross-cultural differences in maternal affect are mediated by infant affect. Mediational analysis was performed via Hayes's (2013) “PROCESS" macro, model 4, v2.16 mediation script for SPSS (Fig. 2).

Test for cultural differences in affect regulation

Cultural differences were tested in three dyadic patterns that reflect co-regulation: (a) dynamic synchrony; (b) maternal responding during events of infant regulation; (c) mutual affective match.Dynamic synchrony of valence and arousal

Dynamic synchrony of valence and arousal

To evaluate the cross-cultural difference in dynamic synchrony between mothers and infants, we calculated the Pearson correlations for arousal and Cramer’s coefficient for valence145 across the 120 s of the interaction. Scores were calculated in each culture separately, and the distributions of synchrony scores were compared cross-culturally.

Maternal responding during events of infant regulation: engagement, distress, unwinding, and calming down

In each type of infant event, maternal valence and arousal were averaged across all participants for a given second and compared cross-culturally with repeated measure GLM (Fig. 3). Post hoc LSD assessed the simple effects in each culture (Supplementary Table S2).

Infant engagement Events of infant engagement were defined as transitioning from neutral to positive valence. We located all events where infants transitioned from neutral to positive valence across all the free interaction videos aligned in time (187 events across 48 mother–infant interactions: Palestinian Arabs = 109, Jews = 78). We then calculated the average scores of maternal valence and arousal 3 s before and 3 s after the infant transition to dynamically map maternal affect during events of infant engagement.

Infant unwinding Events of infant unwind were defined as events where infants transitioned from positive to neutral valence: we located all events where infants transitioned from positive to neutral valence across all of the free interaction videos aligned in time (174 events across 47 mother–infant interactions: Palestinian-Arabs = 105, Jews = 69). We then calculated the average scores of maternal valence and arousal 3 s before and 3 s after the infant transition to dynamically map maternal affect during events of infant unwinding,

Infant distress Events of infant distress were defined as events where infants transitioned from neutral to negative valence. We located all events where infants transitioned from neutral to negative valence across all the free interaction videos aligned in time (112 events across 38 mother–infant interactions: Palestinian Arabs = 40, Jews = 72). We then calculated the average scores of maternal valence and arousal 3 s before and 3 s after the infant transition to dynamically map maternal affect during events of infant distress.

Infant calming down Events of infant calming down were defined as events where infants transitioned from negative to neutral valence: We located all events where infants transitioned from negative to neutral valence across all the free interaction videos aligned in time (108 events across 37 mother–infant interactions: Palestinian-Arabs = 32, Jews = 76). We then calculated the average scores of maternal valence and arousal 3 s before and 3 s after the infant transition to dynamically map maternal affect during events of the infant calming down.

Mutual affective match between mothers and infants

In addition to dynamic patterns of co-regulation, we also assessed the cross-cultural difference in Mutual Affective Match between mothers and infants. The mutual affective match was calculated as the Pearson correlation between the Global Scores of the mother and her infant, separately in each culture. Then, the r values were transformed using Fisher r-to-z and statistically compared cross-culturally (Fig. 4).

In all analyses, we computed the bootstrapped confidence intervals (CI). All analyses were corrected for multiple hypotheses testing using Bonferroni correction and were controlled for infant age and the number of children in the family.

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Information. (311.9KB, docx)

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Kfir Toledano for the drawings in Figures 1, 3 and S1.

Author contributions

M.A.S.—data collection, analysis, graph preparation, writing. M.A.—methodology, analysis, graph preparation, writing. S.B.—conceptualization, data collection. M.I.—conceptualization, data collection. N.k.S.—data analysis. S.A.—conceptualization, analysis, graph preparation, writing, supervision.

Data availability

All data was uploaded to osf.io and can become publicly available upon request after publication.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

These authors contributed equally: Miada Abu Salih and Maayan Abargil.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-023-31907-y.

References

  • 1.Cole PM, Martin SE, Dennis TA. Emotion regulation as a scientific construct: Methodological challenges and directions for child development research. Child Dev. 2004;75(2):317–333. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00673.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Tamir M, Mitchell C, Gross JJ. Hedonic and instrumental motives in anger regulation. Psychol. Sci. 2008;19(4):324–328. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02088.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Eisenberg N, Spinrad TL. Emotion-related regulation: Sharpening the definition. Child Dev. 2004;75(2):334–339. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00674.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Shamay-Tsoory SG, Levy-Gigi E. You name it: Interpersonal affect labeling diminishes distress in romantic couples. Behav. Ther. 2021;52(2):455–464. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2020.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Gordon AM, Mendes WB. A large-scale study of stress, emotions, and blood pressure in daily life using a digital platform. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2021;118(31):e2105573118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2105573118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Wakschlag LS, Perlman SB, Blair RJ, Leibenluft E, Briggs-Gowan MJ, Pine DS. The neurodevelopmental basis of early childhood disruptive behavior: Irritable and callous phenotypes as exemplars. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2018;175(2):114–130. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.17010045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Wakschlag LS, Roberts MY, Flynn RM, Smith JD, Krogh-Jespersen S, Kaat AJ, Gray L, Walkup J, Marino BS, Norton ES, Davis MM. Future directions for early childhood prevention of mental disorders: A road map to mental health, earlier. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2019;48(3):539–554. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2018.1561296. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Eisenberg N, Fabes RA. Child Psychology: A Handbook of Contemporary Issues. 2. Psychology Press; 2006. Emotion regulation and children’s socioemotional competence; pp. 357–381. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Gross JJ, Muñoz RF. Emotion regulation and mental health. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 1995;2(2):151–164. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2850.1995.tb00036.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Bloch L, Haase CM, Levenson RW. Emotion regulation predicts marital satisfaction: More than a wives’ tale. Emotion. 2014;14(1):130–144. doi: 10.1037/a0034272. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Martin LN, Delgado MR. The influence of emotion regulation on decision-making under risk. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2011;23(9):2569–2581. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2011.21618. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Newman DA, Joseph DL, MacCann C. Emotional intelligence and job performance: The importance of emotion regulation and emotional labor context. Ind. Organ. Psychol. 2010;3(2):159–164. doi: 10.1111/j.1754-9434.2010.01218.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Morris AS, Silk JS, Steinberg L, Myers SS, Robinson LR. The role of the family context in the development of emotion regulation. Soc. Dev. 2007;16(2):361–388. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00389.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Silvers JA, McRae K, Gabrieli JDE, Gross JJ, Remy KA, Ochsner KN. Age-related differences in emotional reactivity, regulation, and rejection sensitivity in adolescence. Emotion. 2012;12(6):1235–1247. doi: 10.1037/a0028297. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Atzil S, Gendron M. Bio-behavioral synchrony promotes the development of conceptualized emotions. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2017;17:162–169. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.07.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Cole PM, Michel MK, Teti LO. The development of emotion regulation and dysregulation: A clinical perspective. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 1994;59(2/3):73–100. doi: 10.2307/1166139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Karnilowicz HR, Waters SF, Mendes WB. Not in front of the kids: Effects of parental suppression on socialization behaviors during cooperative parent–child interactions. Emotion. 2019;19(7):1183–1191. doi: 10.1037/emo0000527. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Puhlmann LM, Derome M, Morosan L, Kilicel D, Vrtička P, Debbané M. Longitudinal associations between self-reported attachment dimensions and neurostructural development from adolescence to early adulthood. Attach. Human Dev. 2021 doi: 10.1080/14616734.2021.1993628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Shamay-Tsoory SG, Saporta N, Marton-Alper IZ, Gvirts HZ. Herding brains: A core neural mechanism for social alignment. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2019;23(3):174–186. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2019.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Brett ZH, Humphreys KL, Fleming AS, Kraemer GW, Drury SS. Using cross-species comparisons and a neurobiological framework to understand early social deprivation effects on behavioral development. Dev. Psychopathol. 2015;27(2):347–367. doi: 10.1017/S0954579415000036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Callaghan BL, Sullivan RM, Howell B, Tottenham N. The international society for developmental psychobiology Sackler symposium: Early adversity and the maturation of emotion circuits—a cross-species analysis. Dev. Psychobiol. 2014;56(8):1635–1650. doi: 10.1002/dev.21260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Callaghan BL, Tottenham N. The neuro-environmental loop of plasticity: A cross-species analysis of parental effects on emotion circuitry development following typical and adverse caregiving. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;41(1):163–176. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Feldman R. The neurobiology of mammalian parenting and the biosocial context of human caregiving. Horm. Behav. 2016;77:3–17. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.DiPietro JA, Kivlighan KT, Voegtline KM, Costigan KA, Moore GA. It takes two: An antenatal to postnatal RDoC framework for investigating the origins of maternal attachment and mother–infant social communication. Dev. Psychopathol. 2021;33(5):1539–1553. doi: 10.1017/S0954579421000997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Provenzi L, Scotto di Minico G, Giusti L, Guida E, Müller M. Disentangling the dyadic dance: Theoretical, methodological and outcomes systematic review of mother–infant dyadic processes. Front. Psychol. 2018 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00348. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Rocha NACF, dos Santos Silva FP, dos Santos MM, Dusing SC. Impact of mother–infant interaction on development during the first year of life: A systematic review. J. Child Health Care. 2020;24(3):365–385. doi: 10.1177/1367493519864742. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Feldman R. Parent–infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry. 2007;48(3–4):329–354. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01701.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.DePasquale CE. A systematic review of caregiver–child physiological synchrony across systems: Associations with behavior and child functioning. Dev. Psychopathol. 2020;32(5):1754–1777. doi: 10.1017/S0954579420001236. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Feldman R. Bio-behavioral synchrony: A model for integrating biological and microsocial behavioral processes in the study of parenting. Parenting. 2012;12(2–3):154–164. doi: 10.1080/15295192.2012.683342. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Lavelli M, Fogel A. Interdyad differences in early mother–infant face-to-face communication: Real-time dynamics and developmental pathways. Dev. Psychol. 2013;49(12):2257–2271. doi: 10.1037/a0032268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Boeve JL, Beeghly M, Stacks AM, Manning JH, Thomason ME. Using the actor-partner interdependence model to assess maternal and infant contributions to mother–infant affective exchanges during the still-face paradigm. Infant Behav. Dev. 2019;57:101351. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2019.101351. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Tronick E, Beeghly M. Infants’ meaning-making and the development of mental health problems. Am. Psychol. 2011;66(2):107–119. doi: 10.1037/a0021631. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Zeevi L, Irani M, Catana C, Feldman Barrett L, Atzil S. Maternal dopamine encodes affective signals of human infants. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2021 doi: 10.1093/scan/nsab116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Atzil S, Touroutoglou A, Rudy T, Salcedo S, Feldman R, Hooker JM, Dickerson BC, Catana C, Barrett LF. Dopamine in the medial amygdala network mediates human bonding. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2017;114(9):2361–2366. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1612233114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Atzil S, Gao W, Fradkin I, Barrett LF. Growing a social brain. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2018;2(9):624–636. doi: 10.1038/s41562-018-0384-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Atzil S, Barrett LF. Social regulation of allostasis: Commentary on “Mentalizing homeostasis: The social origins of interoceptive inference” by Fotopoulou and Tsakiris. Neuropsychoanalysis. 2017;19(1):29–33. doi: 10.1080/15294145.2017.1295214. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Sterling P. Allostasis: A model of predictive regulation. Physiol. Behav. 2012;106(1):5–15. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Feldman R. Parent–infant synchrony: Biological foundations and developmental outcomes. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2007;16(6):340–345. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00532.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Granat A, Gadassi R, Gilboa-Schechtman E, Feldman R. Maternal depression and anxiety, social synchrony, and infant regulation of negative and positive emotions. Emotion. 2017;17(1):11–27. doi: 10.1037/emo0000204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Moore C, Dunham PJ, Dunham P. Joint Attention: Its Origins and Role in Development. Psychology Press; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Bell MA. Chapter Six—mother–child behavioral and physiological synchrony. In: Benson JB, editor. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, ***Vol 58. JAI; 2020. pp. 163–188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Atzil S, Hendler T, Feldman R. Specifying the neurobiological basis of human attachment: Brain, hormones, and behavior in synchronous and intrusive mothers. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36(13):2603–2615. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Beebe B, Messinger D, Bahrick LE, Margolis A, Buck KA, Chen H. A systems view of mother–infant face-to-face communication. Dev. Psychol. 2016;52(4):556–571. doi: 10.1037/a0040085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Beebe B, Steele M. How does microanalysis of mother–infant communication inform maternal sensitivity and infant attachment? Attach. Hum. Dev. 2013;15(5–6):583–602. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2013.841050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Margolis AE, Lee SH, Peterson BS, Beebe B. Profiles of infant communicative behavior. Dev. Psychol. 2019;55(8):1594–1604. doi: 10.1037/dev0000745. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Barry ES, McKenna JJ. Reasons mothers bedshare: A review of its effects on infant behavior and development. Infant Behav. Dev. 2022;66:101684. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Bornstein MH, Putnick DL, Cote LR, Haynes OM, Suwalsky JTD. Mother–infant contingent vocalizations in 11 countries. Psychol. Sci. 2015;26(8):1272–1284. doi: 10.1177/0956797615586796. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Gratier M. Expressive timing and interactional synchrony between mothers and infants: Cultural similarities, cultural differences, and the immigration experience. Cogn. Dev. 2003;18(4):533–554. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2003.09.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Farran LK, Yoo H, Lee C-C, Bowman DD, Oller DK. Temporal coordination in mother–infant vocal interaction: A cross-cultural comparison. Front. Psychol. 2019 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02374. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Ding Y, Xu X, Wang Z, Li H, Wang W. Study of mother–infant attachment patterns and influence factors in Shanghai. Early Human Dev. 2012;88(5):295–300. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2011.08.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Rothbaum F, Kakinuma M, Nagaoka R, Azuma H. Attachment and Amae: Parent—child closeness in the United States and Japan. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 2007;38(4):465–486. doi: 10.1177/0022022107302315. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Cooke JE, Kochendorfer LB, Stuart-Parrigon KL, Koehn AJ, Kerns KA. Parent–child attachment and children’s experience and regulation of emotion: A meta-analytic review. Emotion. 2019;19:1103–1126. doi: 10.1037/emo0000504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Brumariu LE. Parent-child attachment and emotion regulation. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 2015;2015(148):31–45. doi: 10.1002/cad.20098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Cowen AS, Keltner D, Schroff F, Jou B, Adam H, Prasad G. Sixteen facial expressions occur in similar contexts worldwide. Nature. 2021;589(7841):7841. doi: 10.1038/s41586-020-3037-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Wataraka Gamage, K., Dang, T., Sethu, V., Epps, J., & Ambikairajah, E. (2018). Speech-based continuous emotion prediction by learning perception responses related to salient events: A study based on vocal affect bursts and cross-cultural affect in AVEC 2018. In Proceedings of the 2018 on Audio/Visual Emotion Challenge and Workshop, 47–55. 10.1145/3266302.3266314.
  • 56.Gelfand MJ, Caluori N, Jackson JC, Taylor MK. The cultural evolutionary trade-off of ritualistic synchrony. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2020;375(1805):20190432. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0432. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Jackson JC, Watts J, Henry TR, List J-M, Forkel R, Mucha PJ, Greenhill SJ, Gray RD, Lindquist KA. Emotion semantics show both cultural variation and universal structure. Science. 2019;366(6472):1517–1522. doi: 10.1126/science.aaw8160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Gendron M, Crivelli C, Barrett LF. Universality reconsidered: Diversity in making meaning of facial expressions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2018;27(4):211–219. doi: 10.1177/0963721417746794. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Gendron M, Hoemann K, Crittenden AN, Mangola SM, Ruark GA, Barrett LF. Emotion perception in hadza hunter-gatherers. Sci. Rep. 2020;10(1):3867. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-60257-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Srinivasan R, Martinez AM. Cross-cultural and cultural-specific production and perception of facial expressions of emotion in the wild. IEEE Trans. Affect. Comput. 2021;12(3):707–721. doi: 10.1109/TAFFC.2018.2887267. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Bhugra D, McKenzie K. Expressed emotion across cultures. Adv. Psychiatr. Treat. 2003;9(5):342–348. doi: 10.1192/apt.9.5.342. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.O’Driscoll C, Sener SB, Angmark A, Shaikh M. Caregiving processes and expressed emotion in psychosis, a cross-cultural, meta-analytic review. Schizophr. Res. 2019;208:8–15. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2019.03.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Koban L, Ramamoorthy A, Konvalinka I. Why do we fall into sync with others? Interpersonal synchronization and the brain’s optimization principle. Soc. Neurosci. 2019;14(1):1–9. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2017.1400463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Kim YY. Achieving synchrony: A foundational dimension of intercultural communication competence. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 2015;48:27–37. doi: 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2015.03.016. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Charny EJ, Condon WS. Psychosomatic manifestations of rapport in psychotherapy. Psychosom. Med. 1966;20:305–315. doi: 10.1097/00006842-196607000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Kinreich S, Djalovski A, Kraus L, Louzoun Y, Feldman R. Brain-to-brain synchrony during naturalistic social interactions. Sci. Rep. 2017;7(1):1. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-17339-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Kimura M, Daibo I. Interactional synchrony in conversations about emotional episodes: A measurement by “the between-participants pseudosynchrony experimental paradigm”. J. Nonverbal Behav. 2006;30(3):115–126. doi: 10.1007/s10919-006-0011-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Keshet Y, Popper-Giveon A, Liberman I. Intersectionality and underrepresentation among health care workforce: The case of Arab physicians in Israel. Isr. J. Health Policy Res. 2015;4(1):18. doi: 10.1186/s13584-015-0004-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Moustafa K. Promoting an academic culture in the Arab world. Avicenna J. Med. 2018;8(03):120–123. doi: 10.4103/ajm.AJM_166_17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Zeinoun P, Akl EA, Maalouf FT, Meho LI. The Arab Region’s contribution to global mental health research (2009–2018): A bibliometric analysis. Front. Psychiatry. 2020 doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Dwairy MA, Jagelman J. Cross-cultural counseling: The Arab-Palestinian case. Routledge. 2020 doi: 10.4324/9781315809946. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Dwairy M. Addressing the repressed needs of the Arabic Client. Cult. Divers. Ment. Health. 1997;3:1–12. doi: 10.1037/1099-9809.3.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Azaiza F, Cohen M. Between traditional and modern perceptions of breast and cervical cancer screenings: A qualitative study of Arab women in Israel. Psychooncology. 2008;17(1):34–41. doi: 10.1002/pon.1180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Ben-Arieh A, Khoury-Kassabri M, Haj-Yahia MM. Generational, ethnic, and national differences in attitudes toward the rights of children in Israel and Palestine. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry. 2006;76(3):381–388. doi: 10.1037/0002-9432.76.3.381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Alnunu M, Amin A, Abu-Rayya HM. The susceptibility to Persuasion strategies among Arab Muslims: The role of culture and acculturation. Front. Psychol. 2021 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.574115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Leung AK-Y, Cohen D. Within- and between-culture variation: Individual differences and the cultural logics of honor, face, and dignity cultures. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2011;100(3):507–526. doi: 10.1037/a0022151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Hofstede G. Culture’s Consequences: Comparing Values, Behaviors. SAGE Publications; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Schwartz S. A theory of cultural value orientations: Explication and applications. Comp. Sociol. 2006;5(2–3):137–182. doi: 10.1163/156913306778667357. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.San Martin A, Sinaceur M, Madi A, Tompson S, Maddux WW, Kitayama S. Self-assertive interdependence in Arab culture. Nat. Human Behav. 2018;2(11):11. doi: 10.1038/s41562-018-0435-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Vignoles VL, Owe E, Becker M, Smith PB, Easterbrook MJ, Brown R, González R, Didier N, Carrasco D, Cadena MP, Lay S, Schwartz SJ, Des Rosiers SE, Villamar JA, Gavreliuc A, Zinkeng M, Kreuzbauer R, Baguma P, Martin M, Bond MH. Beyond the ‘east–west’ dichotomy: Global variation in cultural models of selfhood. J. Exp. Psychol. Gener. 2016;145(8):966–1000. doi: 10.1037/xge0000175. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Kozlowski D, Larivière V, Sugimoto CR, Monroe-White T. Intersectional inequalities in science. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2022;119(2):e2113067119. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2113067119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Wang Z, Liu J, Shuai H, Cai Z, Fu X, Liu Y, Xiao X, Zhang W, Krabbendam E, Liu S, Liu Z, Li Z, Yang BX. Mapping global prevalence of depression among postpartum women. Transl. Psychiatry. 2021;11(1):1. doi: 10.1038/s41398-021-01663-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Badr LK, Ayvazian N, Lameh S, Charafeddine L. Is the effect of postpartum depression on mother–infant bonding universal? Infant Behav. Dev. 2018;51:15–23. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2018.02.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Keren M, Feldman R, Eidelman AI, Sirota L, Lester B. Clinical Interview for high-risk Parents of premature infants (CLIP) as a predictor of early disruptions in the mother–infant relationship at the nursery. Infant Ment. Health J. 2003;24(2):93–110. doi: 10.1002/imhj.10049. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Harel J, Scher A. Insufficient responsiveness in ambivalent mother–infant relationships: Contextual and affective aspects. Infant Behav. Dev. 2003;26(3):371–383. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(03)00036-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Ninio A. Picture-book reading in mother–infant dyads belonging to two subgroups in Israel. Child Dev. 1980;51(2):587–590. doi: 10.2307/1129299. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Kraemer RS. Jewish mothers and daughters in the Greco-Roman world. In: Cohen SJD, editor. The Jewish Family in Antiquity. Brown Judaic Studies; 2020. pp. 89–112. [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Ziv Y, Aviezer O, Gini M, Sagi A, Karie NK. Emotional availability in the mother–infant dyad as related to the quality of infant–mother attachment relationship. Attach. Hum. Dev. 2000;2(2):149–169. doi: 10.1080/14616730050085536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Feldman R, Eidelman AI. Neonatal state organization, neuromaturation, mother–infant interaction, and cognitive development in small-for-gestational-age premature infants. Pediatrics. 2006;118(3):e869–e878. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-2040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Florian V, Mikulincer M, Weller A. Does culture affect perceived family dynamics? A comparison of Arab and Jewish adolescents in Israel. J. Comp. Fam. Stud. 1993;24(2):189–201. doi: 10.3138/jcfs.24.2.189. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Mikulincer M, Weller A, Florian V. Sense of closeness to parents and family rules: A study of Arab and Jewish Youth in Israel. Int. J. Psychol. 1993;28(3):323–335. doi: 10.1080/00207599308246925. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Ben-Rafael E, Schoeps JH, Sternberg Y, Glöckner O, Weberling A. Handbook of Israel: Major Debates. Walter de Gruyter GmbH & Co KG; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Florian V, Har-Even D. Cultural patterns in the choice of leisure time activity frameworks: A study of Jewish and Arab Youth in Isreal. J. Leis. Res. 1984;16(4):330–337. doi: 10.1080/00222216.1984.11969604. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Malach Pines A. Occupational burnout: A cross-cultural Israeli Jewish-Arab perspective and its implications for career counselling. Career Dev. Int. 2003;8(2):97–106. doi: 10.1108/13620430310465516. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Azaiza F. Processes of conservation and change in Arab society in Israel: Implications for the health and welfare of the Arab population. Int. J. Soc. Welf. 2013;22(1):15–24. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2397.2011.00866.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Pines AM, Zaidman N. Gender, culture, and social support: A male–female. Isr. Jewish-Arab Comp. Sex Roles. 2003;49(11):571–586. doi: 10.1023/B:SERS.0000003128.99279.94. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Puddister S, Ali-Saleh O, Cohen-Dar M, Baron-Epel O. Health may be compromised by social interactions depending on culture among postpartum Arab and Jewish Israeli women. BMC Pregn. Childb. 2020;20(1):480. doi: 10.1186/s12884-020-03168-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Feldman R, Masalha S, Nadam R. Cultural perspective on work and family: Dual-earner Israeli Jewish and Arab families at the transition to parenthood. J. Fam. Psychol. 2001;15(3):492–509. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.15.3.492. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Feldman R, Masalha S, Alony D. Microregulatory patterns of family interactions: Cultural pathways to toddlers’ self-regulation. J. Fam. Psychol. 2006;20(4):614–623. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.20.4.614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Zreik G, Oppenheim D, Sagi-Schwartz A. Infant attachment and maternal sensitivity in the Arab Minority in Israel. Child Dev. 2017;88(4):1338–1349. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Arar K, Oplatka I. Emotion display and suppression among Arab and Jewish assistant principals in Israel: The key role of culture, gender and ethnicity. J. Professional Capital Community. 2018;3(3):173–191. doi: 10.1108/JPCC-12-2017-0030. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Biadsy-Ashkar A, Peleg O. The relationship between differentiation of self and satisfaction with life amongst Israeli women: A cross cultural perspective. Health. 2013;05(09):09. doi: 10.4236/health.2013.59200. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Friedlmeier W, Corapci F, Cole PM. Emotion socialization in cross-cultural perspective. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass. 2011;5(7):410–427. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00362.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Lavelli M, Carra C, Rossi G, Keller H. Culture-specific development of early mother–infant emotional co-regulation: Italian, Cameroonian, and West African immigrant dyads. Dev. Psychol. 2019;55:1850–1867. doi: 10.1037/dev0000696. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Friedlmeier W, Trommsdorff G. Emotion regulation in early childhood: A cross-cultural comparison between German and Japanese toddlers. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 1999;30(6):684–711. doi: 10.1177/0022022199030006002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Takada A. Pragmatic reframing from distress to playfulness: !Xun caregiver responses to infant crying. J. Pragmat. 2021;181:180–195. doi: 10.1016/j.pragma.2021.05.021. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Cirelli LK, Jurewicz ZB, Trehub SE. Effects of maternal singing style on mother–infant arousal and behavior. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2020;32(7):1213–1220. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Feldman R. On the origins of background emotions: From affect synchrony to symbolic expression. Emotion. 2007;7(3):601–611. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Kitayama S, Mesquita B, Karasawa M. Cultural affordances and emotional experience: Socially engaging and disengaging emotions in Japan and the United States. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2006;91(5):890–903. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.5.890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Bozicevic L, De Pascalis L, Montirosso R, Ferrari PF, Giusti L, Cooper PJ, Murray L. Sculpting culture: Early maternal responsiveness and child emotion regulation—A UK-Italy comparison. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 2021;52(1):22–42. doi: 10.1177/0022022120971353. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Beeghly M, Perry BD, Tronick E. Self-regulatory processes in early development. Oxf. Handb. Treat. Processes Outcomes Psychol. 2016 doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199739134.013.3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Buck R. Chapter Two—Motivation, emotion, cognition, and communication: Definitions and notes toward a grand theory. In: Elliot AJ, editor. Advances in Motivation Science, Vol 6*** Elsevier; 2019. pp. 27–69. [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Meins E, Fernyhough C, Wainwright R, Das Gupta M, Fradley E, Tuckey M. Maternal mind-mindedness and attachment security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. Child Dev. 2002;73(6):1715–1726. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Provenzi L, Casini E, de Simone P, Reni G, Borgatti R, Montirosso R. Mother–infant dyadic reparation and individual differences in vagal tone affect 4-month-old infants’ social stress regulation. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2015;140:158–170. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Negayama K, Delafield-Butt JT, Momose K, Ishijima K, Kawahara N. Comparison of Japanese and Scottish mother–infant intersubjectivity: Resonance of timing, anticipation, and empathy during feeding. Front. Psychol. 2021 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.724871. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Rothbaum F, Weisz J, Pott M, Miyake K, Morelli G. Attachment and culture: Security in the United States and Japan. Am. Psychol. 2000;55(10):1093–1104. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.10.1093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Cote LR, Bornstein MH. Synchrony in mother–infant vocal interactions revealed through timed event sequences. Infant Behav. Dev. 2021;64:101599. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Abney DH, da Silva EB, Bertenthal BI. Associations between infant–mother physiological synchrony and 4- and 6-month-old infants’ emotion regulation. Dev. Psychobiol. 2021;63(6):e22161. doi: 10.1002/dev.22161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Ispa JM, Fine MA, Halgunseth LC, Harper S, Robinson J, Boyce L, Brooks-Gunn J, Brady-Smith C. Maternal intrusiveness, maternal warmth, and mother–toddler relationship outcomes: Variations across low-income ethnic and acculturation groups. Child Dev. 2004;75(6):1613–1631. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00806.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Jenkins JM, McGowan P, Knafo-Noam A. Parent–offspring transaction: Mechanisms and the value of within family designs. Horm. Behav. 2016;77:53–61. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.06.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Davidov M, Knafo-Noam A, Serbin LA, Moss E. The influential child: How children affect their environment and influence their own risk and resilience. Dev. Psychopathol. 2015;27(4pt1):947–951. doi: 10.1017/S0954579415000619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods. 2007;39(2):175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Atzil S, Touroutoglou A, Rudy T, Salcedo S, Feldman R, Hooker JM, Dickerson BC, Catana C, Barrett LF. Dopamine in the medial amygdala network mediates human bonding. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2017;114(9):2361–2366. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1612233114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Atzil S, Hendler T, Feldman R. Specifying the neurobiological basis of human attachment: Brain, hormones, and behavior in synchronous and intrusive mothers. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36:2603–2615. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Atzil S, Hendler T, Zagoory-Sharon O, Winetraub Y, Feldman R. Synchrony and specificity in the maternal and the paternal brain: Relations to oxytocin and vasopressin. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2012;51(8):798–811. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.06.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Belsky J. Mother–infant interaction at home and in the laboratory: A comparative study. J. Genet. Psychol. 1980;137(1):37–47. doi: 10.1080/00221325.1980.10532800. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Gardner F. Methodological issues in the direct observation of parent–child interaction: Do observational findings reflect the natural behavior of participants? Clin. Child. Fam. Psychol. Rev. 2000;3(3):185–198. doi: 10.1023/A:1009503409699. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Atzil S, Hendler T, Feldman R. The brain basis of social synchrony. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2014;9(8):1193–1202. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Hane AA, LaCoursiere JN, Mitsuyama M, Wieman S, Ludwig RJ, Kwon KYV, Browne J, Austin JM, Myers M, Welch MG. The Welch emotional connection screen: Validation of a brief mother–infant relational health screen. Acta Paediatr. 2019;108(4):615–625. doi: 10.1111/apa.14483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Müller D, Teismann T, Hirschfeld G, Zmyj N, Fuths S, Vocks S, Schneider S, Seehagen S. The course of maternal repetitive negative thinking at the transition to motherhood and early mother–infant interactions: Is there a link? Dev. Psychopathol. 2019;31(4):1411–1421. doi: 10.1017/S0954579418000883. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Sahlén Helmer C, Birberg Thornberg U, Frostell A, Örtenstrand A, Mörelius E. A randomized trial of continuous versus intermittent skin-to-skin contact after premature birth and the effects on mother–infant interaction. Adv. Neonatal Care. 2020;20(3):E48. doi: 10.1097/ANC.0000000000000675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Tang, C., Zheng, W., Zong, Y., Cui, Z., Qiu, N., Yan, S., & Ke, X. (2018). Automatic smile detection of infants in mother–infant interaction via CNN-based feature learning. In Proceedings of the Joint Workshop of the 4th Workshop on Affective Social Multimedia Computing and First Multi-Modal Affective Computing of Large-Scale Multimedia Data, 35–40. 10.1145/3267935.3267951
  • 133.Williams LR. The impact of infant carrying on adolescent mother–infant interactions during the still-face task. Infant Child Dev. 2020;29(3):e2169. doi: 10.1002/icd.2169. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Calkins SD, Dedmon SE, Gill KL, Lomax LE, Johnson LM. Frustration in infancy: Implications for emotion regulation, physiological processes, and temperament. Infancy. 2002;3(2):175–197. doi: 10.1207/S15327078IN0302_4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Siller M, Sigman M. The behaviors of parents of children with autism predict the subsequent development of their children’s communication. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2002;32(2):77–89. doi: 10.1023/A:1014884404276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Pribram KH, McGuinness D. Arousal, activation, and effort in the control of attention. Psychol. Rev. 1975;82(2):116–149. doi: 10.1037/H0076780. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Jarick M, Bencic R. Eye contact is a two-way street: Arousal is elicited by the sending and receiving of eye gaze information. Front. Psychol. 2019 doi: 10.3389/FPSYG.2019.01262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Stephenson LJ, Edwards SG, Howard EE, Bayliss AP. Eyes that bind us: Gaze leading induces an implicit sense of agency. Cognition. 2018;172:124–133. doi: 10.1016/J.COGNITION.2017.12.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Filippi P, Congdon JV, Hoang J, Bowling DL, Reber SA, Pašukonis A, Hoeschele M, Ocklenburg S, de Boer B, Sturdy CB, Newen A, Güntürkün O. Humans recognize emotional arousal in vocalizations across all classes of terrestrial vertebrates: Evidence for acoustic universals. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2017;284:1859. doi: 10.1098/RSPB.2017.0990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Russell JA. A circumplex model of affect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1980;39(6):1161–1178. doi: 10.1037/h0077714. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Hayes AF, Krippendorff K. Answering the call for a standard reliability measure for coding data. Commun. Methods Meas. 2007;1(1):77–89. doi: 10.1080/19312450709336664. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Feldman R. Parent-infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2007;48(3–4):329–354. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01701.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Lang PJ, Greenwald MK, Bradley MM, Hamm AO. Looking at pictures: Affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions. Psychophysiology. 1993;30(3):261–273. doi: 10.1111/J.1469-8986.1993.TB03352.X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 144.Irani, M., et al (n.d.). Infant communication of valence and arousal as a behavioral mechanism for social regulation.
  • 145.Shishkina, T., Farmus, L., & Cribbie, R. (2018). Testing for a Lack of Relationship Among Categorical Variables. https://yorkspace.library.yorku.ca/xmlui/handle/10315/34630.

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Information. (311.9KB, docx)

Data Availability Statement

All data was uploaded to osf.io and can become publicly available upon request after publication.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES