Skip to main content
. 2023 Mar 21;13(3):521. doi: 10.3390/brainsci13030521

Table 5.

Characteristics of studies comparing the effects of OSE and CSE on cognitive function in the elderly.

References Participants and Study Protocol
(1. Study Design; 2. Participants; 3. Exercise experience or intervention; and 4. Cognitive tasks; 5. Cognitive functions)
Main Findings
Dai et al. [50]
  1. Cross-sectional study

  2. OSE (n = 16; F:M = 7:9; age: 69.0 ± 3.6 y); CSE (n = 16; F:M = 10:6; age: 69.9 ± 3.6 y); and Control (n = 16; F:M = 14:2; age: 67.3 ± 3.0 y)

  3. OSE (table tennis/tennis; experience: ≥30 min/session; ≥3 times/week; ≥3 months; 13.0 ± 5.7 y); CSE (jogging/swimming; experience: ≥30 min/session; ≥3 times/week; ≥3 months; 11.1 ± 4.5 y); and Control (irregular exercise; experience: <30 min/session; <2 times/week; ≥3 months; 0.7 ± 0.6 y)

  4. Task-switching paradigm

  5. Cognitive flexibility

(Task-switching paradigm)
  • -

    Both OSE and CSE groups had shorter RTs than the control group.

  • -

    A larger P3 amplitude of ERP was observed in both OSE and CSE groups than in the control group.

  • -

    The OSE group showed additional facilitation effects.

Guo et al. [51]
  1. Cross-sectional study

  2. OSE (n = 36; F:M = 19:17; age: 67.6 ± 5.9 y); CSE (n = 38; F:M = 23:15; age: 66.7 ± 5.8 y); and Control (n = 37; F:M = 21:16; age: 66.9 ± 5.9 y)

  3. OSE (table tennis; experience: ≥30 min/session; ≥3 times/week; ≥1 year); CSE (jogging/swimming; experience: ≥30 min/session; ≥3 times/week; ≥1 year); and Control (sedentary and inactivity or low activity)

  4. Visuospatial working memory task; visuospatial short-term memory task; visuospatial mental rotation task

  5. Visuospatial working memory

(Visuospatial working memory task)
  • -

    Both OSE and CSE groups showed higher accuracy than the control group.

(Visuospatial short-term memory task)
  • -

    The OSE group showed higher accuracy than the control group.

(Visuospatial mental rotation task)
  • -

    No difference was observed.

Huang et al. [52]
  1. Cross-sectional study

  2. OSE (n = 20; F:M = 9:11; age: 69.4 ± 3.0 y); CSE (n = 20; F:M = 11:9; age: 70.6 ± 2.6 y); and Control (n = 20; F:M = 14:6; age: 68.3 ± 2.3 y)

  3. OSE (table tennis/tennis/badminton; experience: ≥30 min/session; ≥3 times/week; ≥3 months; 7.8 ± 1.1 y); CSE (jogging/swimming; experience: ≥3 months; 6.7 ± 2.4 y); and Control (irregular exercise)

  4. Eriksen flanker task

  5. Inhibitory control

(Eriksen flanker task)
  • -

    Both OSE and CSE groups had shorter RTs than the control group.

  • -

    The OSE group showed a larger P300 amplitude of ERP at the vertex site than at the frontal site.

Li et al. [53]
  1. Cross-sectional study

  2. OSE (n = 25; F:M = 10:15; age: 69.0 ± 3.4 y); CSE (n = 25; F:M = 17:8; age: 69.8 ± 3.1 y); and Control (n = 25; F:M = 21:4; age: 67.8 ± 2.9 y)

  3. OSE (table tennis/tennis; experience: ≥30 min/session; ≥3 times/week; ≥3 months); CSE (jogging/brisk walking; experience: ≥30 min/session; ≥3 times/week; ≥3 months); and Control (irregular exercise)

  4. Stroop color-word interference test, task-switching paradigm

  5. Inhibitory control, cognitive flexibility

(Stroop color-word interference task)
  • -

    Both OSE and CSE groups had shorter RTs than the control group.

  • -

    The OSE group exhibited smaller N200 and larger P300a amplitudes than the control group.

(Task-switching paradigm)
  • -

    The OSE group displayed a tendency for shorter error-related negativity latencies.

Tsai and Wang [54]
  1. Cross-sectional study

  2. OSE (n = 21; F:M = 7:14; age: 65.4 ± 4.2 y); CSE (n = 22; F:M = 8:14; age: 66.0 ± 4.1 y); and Control (n = 21; F:M = 8:13; age: 63.9 ± 3.4 y)

  3. OSE (table tennis/badminton; experience: ≥30 min/session; ≥3 times/week; ≥2 years); CSE (jogging/swimming; experience: ≥30 min/session; ≥3 times/week; ≥2 years); and Control (sedentary)

  4. Task-switching paradigm

  5. Cognitive flexibility

(Task-switching paradigm)
  • -

    Both OSE and CSE groups had shorter RTs and larger P2 and P3 amplitudes of ERP than the control group.

  • -

    A relatively smaller specific cost, shorter motor RTs, and larger P3 amplitudes of ERP were observed in the switch condition in the OSE group than in the CSE and control groups.

Tsai et al. [55]
  1. Cross-sectional study

  2. OSE (n = 20; F:M = 7:13; age: 65.3 ± 4.1 y); CSE (n = 20; F:M = 6:14; age: 67.0 ± 4.7 y); and Control (n = 20; F:M = 7:13; age: 64.3 ± 3.6 y)

  3. OSE (table tennis/badminton; experience: ≥30 min/session; ≥3 times/week; ≥2 years); CSE (jogging/swimming; experience: ≥30 min/session; ≥3 times/week; ≥2 years); and Control (sedentary; experience: <30 min/session; <2 times/week; ≥2 years)

  4. Central cue Posner paradigm

  5. Visuospatial attention

(Central cue Posner paradigm)
  • -

    Both OSE and CSE groups had shorter RTs and larger P3 amplitudes of ERP than the control group.

  • -

    Only the OSE group displayed better inhibitory control of attention than the control group.

Wang and Guo [56]
  1. Cross-sectional study

  2. OSE (n = 85; F:M = 45:40; age: 66.8 ± 5.5 y); CSE (n = 87; F:M = 49:38; age: 65.5 ± 5.8 y); and Control (n = 87; F:M = 46:41; age: 65.9 ± 6.3 y)

  3. OSE (table tennis/badminton; experience: ≥30 min/session; ≥3 times/week; ≥1 year); CSE (jogging/swimming; experience: ≥30 min/session; ≥3 times/week; ≥1 year); and Control (inactivity or low activity and no regular exercise)

  4. Attention network test

  5. Executive control, orienting, and alerting networks

(Attention network test)
  • -

    The OSE group showed higher executive network efficiency than the CSE and control groups.

  • -

    The CSE group exhibited higher executive network efficiency than the control group.

  • -

    No difference was observed among groups for alerting and orienting networks.

O’Brien et al. [57]
  1. Intervention study

  2. OSE (n = 18; F:M = 17:1; age: 69.2 ± 5.1 y); CSE (n = 19; F:M = 7:12; age: 69.2 ± 4.8 y); and Control (n = 21; F:M = 13:8; age: 70.5 ± 6.9 y)

  3. OSE (tennis/aerobics classes/dance classes; intervention: 80 ± 20 min); CSE (swimming/gym circuits; intervention: 70 ± 20 min); and Control (active retired, meeting, card games; intervention: 60 min)

  4. Sound-induced flash illusion task and forward digit span task

  5. Multisensory perception and memory (immediate memory)

(Sound-induced flash illusion task)
  • -

    The OSE group showed improved sensitivity in audio–visual perception.

(Forward digit span task)
  • -

    Both OSE and CSE groups exhibited improvements in one of the measures of immediate memory.

Tsai et al. [58]
  1. Intervention study

  2. OSE (n = 22; F:M = 0:22; age: 66.9 ± 4.7 y); CSE (n = 21; F:M = 0:21; age: 66.2 ± 4.9 y); and Control (n = 21; F:M = 0:21; age: 65.7 ± 3.5 y)

  3. OSE (table tennis; intervention: 40 min/session; 3 times/week; 6 months); CSE (bike riding/brisk walking/jogging; intervention: 40 min/session; 3 times/week; 6 months); and Control (static stretching and balance training; intervention: 6 months)

  4. Task-switching paradigm and N-back task

  5. Cognitive flexibility and working memory

(Task-switching paradigm)
  • -

    Both OSE and CSE groups had shorter RTs and larger P3 amplitudes of ERP after exercise intervention than the control group.

  • -

    RT facilitation during postexercise relative to pre-exercise only emerged in the OSE group.

(N-back task)
  • -

    Both OSE and CSE groups showed higher ARs and larger P3 amplitudes of ERP after the exercise intervention.

  • -

    The beneficial AR effect only emerged in the CSE group.

Abbreviations: CSE, closed-skill exercise; OSE, open-skill exercise; RT, reaction time; AR, accuracy rate; ERP, event-related potential.