Skip to main content
. 2022 Aug 26;32(1):e13719. doi: 10.1111/jsr.13719

TABLE 2.

Summary of publications, their methods used and their main findings regarding this review

Author Year Method Findings
Bódizs et al. 2005 ECoG 1.5–3.0 Hz activity in the parahippocampus increases at sleep‐onset, thought to be REM‐sleep specific
Bódizs et al. 2008 EEG EEG activity at sleep‐onset resembles REM sleep activity more closely than stage 2 sleep activity
Bosch et al. 2012 Questionnaire‐Study Patients with depression experience hypnagogic states more often than healthy controls
Cancelli et al. 2004 Review Tricyclic antidepressants are linked with increased hypnagogic states
Del Prete & Tressoldi 2005 Experimental Study Hypnosis can induce hypnagogic states successfully
Fortuyn et al. 2009 Questionnaire‐Study Patients with narcolepsy experience hypnagogic states and daytime hypnagogia more often than healthy controls
Fulda et al. 2008 Questionnaire‐Study The prevalence for hypnagogic states was higher both in a sample of not nearer defined psychiatric patients and another sample of sleep‐disordered patients, compared with healthy controls
Germain & Nielsen 2001 Systematic Self‐Observation, EEG Kinaesthetic images were accompanied by prefrontal and frontal delta activation, visual images were accompanied by activation in left‐central and temporal regions at sleep‐onset
Haar Horowitz et al. 2020 Serial Awakening, Dormio The content of hypnagogic experiences can successfully be induced through instruction to think about a specific target before going to sleep
Hayashi et al. 1999 EEG Hypnagogic experiences of landscapes occur earlier during sleep‐onset, followed by dream‐like images, whereas static objects and colour patterns occur later during sleep‐onset
Hinton et al. 2019 Questionnaire‐Study Individuals with PTSD report being bothered more by hypnagogic states (“ghost attacks”) than healthy controls
Horikawa et al. 2013 Machine Learning (fMRI data) Brain areas that process specific stimuli show activation during the perception of corresponding stimuli in the hypnagogic state
Jones et al. 2009 Questionnaire‐Study Intrusive thoughts and undertaking thought suppression are associated with hypnagogic experiences
Jones et al. 2010 Questionnaire‐Study Compared with REM dreams, verbal hypnagogic experiences are less likely to be commanding, but more likely to contain a single clear word rather than sentences, sound more like familiar voices than unknown persons, and talk directly to the individual
Kjaer et al. 2002 PET In stage 1 sleep, relative blood flow increases in visual association cortices, while relative blood flow decreases in the frontal and parietal cortex, the cerebellum and the thalamus
Kussé et al. 2012 Serial Awakening, EEG Playing TETRIS results in hypnagogic images related to the game Anticipating to play the game after sleep results in fewer reports related to the game
Larøi et al. 2019 Questionnaire‐Study Age is associated with a decrease in hypnagogic experiences. Depression and anxiety are associated with an increase in hypnagogic experiences over the lifespan
Lewis‐Hanna et al. 2011 fMRI fMRI measures indicate that individuals with a history of auditory hypnagogic experiences exhibit a significantly greater speech‐evoked activation during wakefulness in the left posterior temporoparietal cortex
McCarthy‐Jones et al. 2011 Questionnaire‐Study Auditory hypnagogic experiences are more prevalent in individuals who report a higher susceptibility to intrusive thoughts
Nielsen et al. 2005 Serial Awakening, EEG The dream‐like quality of hypnagogic states increases after preceding REM sleep deprivation
Nielsen 2017 Sleep‐Onset Observations The content of hypnagogic states may be influenced by self‐generated external stimuli (autosensory imagery) and non‐generated external stimuli (exosensory imagery)
Noreika et al. 2015 Systematic Self‐Observation, EEG Linguistic intrusions are associated with higher alpha and gamma power in the left hemisphere, whereas visual images are associated with higher beta power in the right hemisphere
Ohayon et al. 1996 Questionnaire‐Study Hypnagogic states are much more common than expected
Ohayon 2000 Questionnaire‐Study The most frequent hypnagogic experienced mentioned is the feeling of falling down an abyss, followed by felt‐presence experiences
Ohayon & Shapiro 2000 Questionnaire‐Study Individuals with PTSD classify the emotional quality of their hypnagogic experiences as more terrifying than healthy controls
Pizzagalli et al. 2000 Questionnaire‐Study EEG Individuals with strong beliefs in paranormal phenomena report having hypnagogic experiences more often
Schacter 1976 Review An extensive review on the hypnagogic state
Schmidt & Gendolla 2008 Serial Awakening, EEG Suppressed thoughts during the day are likely to rebound into the hypnagogic state
Sherwood 2012 Questionnaire‐Study Visual experiences, the feeling of falling and feeling a presence in the room are the most frequent hypnagogic experiences, followed by auditory, tactile, bodily and movement sensations. The rarest experiences are olfactory and gustatory
Siclari et al. 2017 Serial Awakening, EEG A bilateral parieto‐occipital “hot zone” correlates with the likelihood of individuals reporting hypnagogic experiences. High activity in this area is associated with dream‐like experiences
Siclari et al. 2013 Serial Awakening, EEG Hypnagogic experiences are less rich and complex, and have less continuity than REM dreams
Soffer‐Dudek & Shahar 2011 Questionnaire‐Study Stress is associated with increased hypnagogic states when sleep quality is poor
Speth et al. 2013 Linguistic Analysis, Serial Awakening, EEG Compared with REM dreams, hypnagogic states are experienced less from a first, second or third perspective. Moreover, there were fewer instances in which the individual or others were involved in the act of speaking
Speth et al. 2016 Linguistic Analysis, Serial Awakening, EEG Memories of the past and thoughts about the present remain fairly constant, while memories of future events decrease throughout sleep‐onset
Speth et al. 2017 Linguistic Analysis, Serial Awakening, EEG Hypnagogic experiences involve fewer acts of speaking and of hearing speech compared with REM dreams
Speth & Speth 2016 Linguistic Analysis, Serial Awakening, EEG Cognitive agencies decrease throughout sleep‐onset, while motor agencies increase
Stenstrom et al. 2012 Serial Awakening, EEG The content of hypnagogic experiences can relate to previous episodic memories
Stickgold et al. 2000 Serial Awakening (Nightcap), EEG Playing TETRIS results in hypnagogic images related to the game. This effect also occurs in patients suffering from dense amnesia
Szklo‐Coxe et al. 2007 Questionnaire‐Study Depression and anxiety are both associated with increased reports of hypnagogic states
Wackermann et al. 2002 Ganzfeld Method, EEG, EOG, EMG Experiences in the ganzfeld and hypnagogic states do not differ in their phenomenology. Vigilance, however, does not decrease in the ganzfeld compared with the hypnagogic state
Wamsley et al. 2010 Serial Awakening (Nightcap), EEG Playing a highly engaging visuomotor video game or observing someone else play results in hypnagogic images related to the game

Abbreviations: ECoG, electrocorticography; EEG, electroencephalogram; EMG, electromyography; EOG, electrooculography; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; PET, positron emission tomography; PTSD, post‐traumatic stress disorder; REM, rapid eye movement.