Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2023 Apr 6;18(4):e0284161. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0284161

I wanna hold your hand: Handholding is preferred over gentle stroking for emotion regulation

Haran Sened 1,*, Chen Levin 1, Manar Shehab 1, Naomi Eisenberger 2, Simone Shamay-Tsoory 1
Editor: Rochelle Ackerley3
PMCID: PMC10079127  PMID: 37023083

Abstract

Social touch is an important form of interpersonal emotion regulation. In recent years, the emotion regulation effects of two types of touch have been studied extensively: handholding and stroking (specifically of skin with C-tactile afferents on the forearm, i.e. C-touch). While some studies compare their effectiveness, with mixed results, no study to date has examined which type of touch is subjectively preferred. Given the potential bidirectional communication provided by handholding, we hypothesized that to regulate intense emotions, participants would prefer handholding. In four pre-registered online studies (total N = 287), participants rated handholding and stroking, presented in short videos, as emotion regulation methods. Study 1 examined touch reception preference in hypothetical situations. Study 2 replicated Study 1 while also examining touch provision preferences. Study 3 examined touch reception preferences of participants with blood/injection phobia in hypothetical injection situations. Study 4 examined types of touch participants who have recently given birth recalled receiving during childbirth and their hypothetical preferences. In all studies, participants preferred handholding over stroking; participants who have recently given birth reported receiving handholding more than stroking. This was especially evident in Studies 1–3 in emotionally intense situations. These results demonstrate that handholding is preferred over stroking as a form of emotion regulation, especially in intense situations, and support the importance of two-way sensory communication for emotion regulation via touch. We discuss the results and possible additional mechanisms, including top-down processing and cultural priming.

Introduction

Touch is an important form of interaction in humans as well as in other species [1]. Interpersonal touch serves a variety of functions, including emotion communication [2] and modulating interpersonal bonds [3]. One major reason humans touch one another is to help regulate subjective experiences such as negative emotions and pain, sometimes referred to as consoling or comforting touch. Importantly, people can attempt to use different types of touch to regulate each other’s emotions; each type of touch might be performed on different body locations, in different social contexts, and may be associated with different emotional experiences [4].

Fotopoulou and colleagues [5] argue that one of the functions of touch is homeostatic regulation. In homeostatic regulation, touch helps reset biological functioning to a fixed setpoint after an acute disturbance in homeostasis, as in warming up a cold baby. Shamay-Tsoory and Eisenberger [6] suggest that this kind of regulation involves a feedback loop: one person experiences an acute, unwanted experience (e.g. pain, or a negative emotion) while another person senses that and is motivated to provide comforting touch. The first person, in turn, perceives this touch, which serves to diminish emotional intensity. This feedback loop is homeostatic as it includes a corrective mechanism: feedback from a distressed target allows the toucher to adapt the touch accordingly. Thus, for homeostatic regulation to occur, both parties must establish two-way communication. This suggests that forms of touch which allow for better two-way feedback would be preferable over others, when trying to regulate intense emotional experiences and pain.

Two types of touch have been thoroughly examined in recent years using traditional paradigms testing the effectiveness of touch in distress regulation: handholding and gentle stroking.

Handholding entails grasping the other person’s hand in varying degrees of strength. Studies have found that handholding can reduce pain [7], reduce pre-surgical anxiety [8], and lessen the emotional pain of recalling negative emotional experiences [9]. Handholding has been shown to attenuate pain-related activation in the posterior insula, the anterior cingulate cortex, the orbitofrontal cortex and the prefrontal cortex [10].

Gentle stroking entails slowly stroking skin regions with C-tactile afferents [11], such as hairy skin on the forearm. Although slow stroking activates all types of mechanoreceptive afferents, it is also referred to as C-touch (as C fibers respond optimally to this type of touch). At normal skin temperature (but not at very high or low temperatures), CT afferent firing is correlated with touch pleasantness [12]. Other studies have found that this kind of stroking, activates regions in the posterior insular cortex and the mid-anterior orbitofrontal cortex more than other types of touch [13,14]. Moreover, stroking activates social reward mechanisms [15], is associated with reduced feelings of social exclusion [16], although some of its effects may be attenuated by attachment anxiety[17,18]. It has also been suggested that stroking may be associated with elevated oxytocin levels [19].

Regarding terminology, “Stroking” can mean many things–stroking can be performed in many different parts of the body at various speeds and forms. As the current study aims to match effectiveness studies, unless explicitly stated otherwise, we use “stroking” to mean using one’s hand to stroke another person’s forearm at a speed of 1–10 cm/s, the optimal speed found in most studies. As for “C-touch”, recent studies have found some C-tactile afferents in the palm skin as well, albeit at a much lower density [20]; other studies show that these afferents can be activated by deep pressure and not only by stroking, which might mean that they are activated by handholding as well [21]. As such, we avoid using the term C-touch for stroking in the rest of the manuscript as both types of touch might be activating C-tactile afferents.

The studies cited above have demonstrated many cases in which both handholding and stroking are associated with distress regulation (creating a sense of pleasantness, and reducing pain, anxiety and feelings of social exclusion). However, as detailed above, homeostatic regulation is driven by two-way feedback. While any form of touch allows for two-way sensing, the tactile sensitivity of the palms and fingers is larger by orders of magnitude than that of the forearms [22,23]. In handholding, as opposed to stroking, both participants’ palms and fingers are involved, allowing both of them to sense each other with optimal sensitivity. This two-way tactile communication should help close the feedback loop required for homeostatic regulation by providing bi-directional feedback, as supported by findings showing that handholding promotes synchrony in skin conductance response (SCR) and brain activity [7,24]. Thus, handholding may be more suitable for homeostatic regulation (i.e., regulation of intense, short-term experiences) than gentle stroking.

The importance of handholding as a bidirectional or reciprocal form of touch can also be viewed in the broader context of reciprocity in nonverbal interaction [25]. In general, people are motivated to reciprocate others’ actions if those actions create pleasant arousal [26]. For example, one study found that couples are more likely to reciprocate each others’ touch the more established their relationship is [27]. The use of handholding in emotion regulation is somewhat distinct from classic reciprocity as defined in the literature as the touching person is not reciprocating the touched person’s action (i.e., they are not initiating handholding in response to touch by the other person); instead, they are responding to the other person’s emotion in a manner which, in the case of handholding, has a reciprocal nature. As reciprocity in considered rewarding [28] there could be a general positive reaction to reciprocity; for example, in one study [29] it was found that people who performed reciprocal forms of touch were more highly regarded than people who perform non-reciprocal forms of touch.

The existing studies in the field detailed above have demonstrated the effects of handholding and stroking separately. However, studies comparing the effectiveness of one of these types of touch with the other are scarce, with studies comparing two types of touch often comparing stroking of the forearm to stroking or touching of the hand, but not to handholding. Studies that compared general pleasantness of touch between touching forearm and palm skin found little difference [30]. In one of the only studies directly comparing the emotion regulation properties of stroking of the forearm and handholding, Reddan and colleagues [24] compared participants who received painful thermal stimulation to their legs, while their romantic partners supported them via gentle stroking of the forearm, handholding or no touch. When measuring pain subjectively and through skin conductance response, no significant differences were found between handholding and gentle stroking, although effects of handholding were greater in every case. In sum, studies on the effectiveness of touch on distress regulation has shown that handholding and gentle stroking are effective forms of emotion regulation, with the few studies comparing the two finding no major differences.

While effectiveness studies attempt to explore how people experience various types of touch, they do not examine which touch people actually prefer or choose to provide, when they encounter the need to regulate emotions in real-life. Touch choice may be based on representations of touch effectiveness, but it might also have other considerations. For example, people could be choosing types of touch they have seen others perform or which were depicted culturally (i.e., in various forms of media), or types of touch which require less physical effort. Thus, effectiveness studies are not enough to understand which types of touch people choose to use. The current study aims to address this gap in the literature by examining subjective preference—presenting people with various situations and asking them which types of touch they would prefer to receive or to provide.

As detailed above, the feedback-loop and homeostatic regulation models suggests that handholding might be preferred over stroking. While we could find no studies which directly examined preference, some studies provided indirect evidence. One study examined adolescents undergoing cancer treatment and found that adolescents see handholding as an extremely effective coping method [31]. Another study which developed a scale concerning support in face of pain found that an item asking about handholding was indicative of general partner support in painful situations [32]. Notably, neither study explicitly presented stroking as a valid coping method (We could not find the initial list of items for the scale development paper by Krahé and colleagues [32]–it could be the case that one of the initial items which were discarded due to low factor loadings involved stroking). Still, this indirect evidence, alongside the theoretical model, led us to hypothesize that handholding would be preferred over gentle stroking.

Beyond exploring our main hypothesis regarding preference, we examined several possible moderators. First, we examined whether the hypothesized preference for handholding would be stronger in more intense situations. Based on the homeostatic regulation model it was reasoned that if two-way sensory communication is indeed crucial for regulating acute disturbances in homeostasis, handholding may be more effective, and therefore preferred, as a way to regulate emotions in intense situations. Thus, we hypothesized that the preference for handholding would be stronger in intense situations.

Second, we examined in a more exploratory manner whether situation valence (i.e., whether the experience is positive or negative) and physicality (i.e., whether the experiences involved physical as opposed to purely emotional pain or pleasure) would moderate the effect. Effectiveness studies have examined the effects of touch in both positive and negative contexts (e.g., pleasantness [13] versus pain [17]), and when regulating both emotional [9] and physical [33] pain, but to the best of our knowledge have not compared these contexts to one another. As such, we did not have a specific directional hypothesis concerning valence and physicality.

Third, Study 3 aimed to examine whether including a situation that was specifically relevant to participants–namely, an injection for participants with blood/injection phobia–would induce a different pattern of results. We examined whether the level of phobia would moderate results. Finally, as we could not find effectiveness studies which performed cultural comparisons, we sought to examine whether touch preference would be different in different cultures. To do so, in Study 4 we compared the touch preferences of Arab and Jewish women. Importantly, these last two moderators–blood/injection phobia and culture–build on less established literature. As such, they were only included in one study each (Studies 3 and 4, respectively), and our investigations of them are intended to inform future research, rather than to reach solid final conclusions.

As detailed above, the aim of the current study was to connect effectiveness research with subjective preferences. While many types of stroking exist, stroking researchers have focused on gentle stroking of the forearm by the hand at a velocity of 1–10 cm/s as the optimal stroking speed; in fact, stroking at higher speeds is often used as a control condition (e.g., in a study by von Mohr and colleagues [16]). As our hypothesis was that handholding would be preferred over stroking, we sought to present stroking in the optimal way possible so that our design tests the hypothesis effectively. Therefore, in Studies 1–3 and in the second part of Study 4 we used looping videos instead of verbal labels for representing different types of touch. In the first part of Study 4, we asked participants which types of touch were provided to them during childbirth and provided standard verbal labels–“stroking” and “handholding”. As both optimal and non-optimal forms of stroking would count towards “stroking” in this paradigm, we could be confident that the prevalence of optimal stroking would, if anything, be overestimated.

Notably, the use of videos somewhat resembles paradigms involving vicarious touch–the study of the effects of seeing people touching one another [34]. However, the current study does not ask people about their feelings while watching the videos, or their thoughts about the videos or about the people depicted in them, which are the questions explored by vicarious touch research. Instead, the current study asks participants about hypothetical or recalled scenarios and only uses the videos to describe the types of touch. While some vicarious effects might be triggered by watching the videos, the same is true for actual touch in real life: when touching or being touched by another person, we usually also see the act of touching. Additionally, to the best of our knowledge, no study of vicarious touch has compared observing handholding to observing stroking.

Overview

In four pre-registered studies, we examined whether handholding would be subjectively preferred over gentle stroking, and whether this preference would be stronger in intense emotional situations. In each study participants rated how helpful they would find stroking or handholding in various hypothesized or recalled situations. We also examined whether participants’ choice was moderated by situation intensity, touch reception versus provision, situation valence, emotional versus physical situations, and cultural differences, as detailed above.

Study 1 examined the type of touch people would prefer to receive from a romantic partner in hypothetical situations by comparing ratings as well as discrete choices of each type of touch. Study 2 replicated most of the results of Study 1 while correcting some methodological issues as well as examining the types of touch participants would prefer to provide.

Study 3 examined touch preferences in the context of a more severe stressor. Participants with some level of blood/injection phobia [35] were asked to imagine themselves receiving an injection.

Finally, in addition to hypothesized situations, Study 4 examined recalled situations and behavior. We asked women who recently gave birth to recall types of touch provided by a close person who was present during their labor, which is usually accompanied by intense pain [36], and to rate which type of touch they preferred. Study 4 also examined cultural differences between Arabic-speaking and Hebrew-speaking participants.

Hypotheses and pre-registration

The hypotheses, study design and sample analysis code for all studies were pre-registered prior to analysis (and in the case of Study 1, prior to data collection), and final study data, methods and code were posted on the Open Science Framework (see below for links). The current study examined the following hypotheses:

  1. Subjective rating (All studies): Participants will rate handholding as more helpful than stroking, especially in intense situations.

  2. Subjective choice (Study 1 only): When instructed to choose one type of touch over the other, participants will prefer handholding over stroking, especially in intense situations.

  3. Recollections of touch received during childbirth (Study 4 only): During childbirth, participants will have received handholding more often than stroking.

The main effect (handholding preferred over stroking overall) was pre-registered only in Studies 3 and 4. The interaction effect (handholding preferred more over stroking as intensity increases) was pre-registered in all studies. We also exploratorily examined the effects of valence, physicality, and cultural differences with no specific directional hypothesis, and exploratorily examined the effects of blood/injection phobia expecting blood/injection phobia to be associated with a stronger preference for handholding, especially in intense situations. Results regarding pre-registered hypotheses concerning loneliness in Studies 1 and 2 will be reported as part of a separate project alongside additional studies.

General method

Ethics statement

All procedures were done in accordance with the principles expressed in the declaration of Helsinki for the treatment of human participants. Procedures were approved by the University of Haifa Department of Psychology IRB, approval 048–21. All participants were over 18 years old. Participants’ written consent was obtained and recorded by completing a digital consent form.

Open data and preregistration

We report how we determined our sample size, all data exclusions (if any), all manipulations, and all measures in the study. All four studies were preregistered on the Open Science Framework:

Study 1: https://osf.io/x95f4/?view_only=e272eb9ccc2a46668114685dcb8988e5

Study 2: https://osf.io/nzg6v/?view_only=b9cf1e802d894ce387c80c47ffd7bc97

Study 3: https://osf.io/8n3pu/?view_only=f23a2e3d485946ffb95d32c02e469b85

Study 4: https://osf.io/74szg/?view_only=4b12d6d2a8a04483abbd6ea0c8c225ca.

Study data, analysis code and touch videos for Studies 1–3 are available at the link below, except for touch videos for Study 4 which were not posted as participants who filmed the videos did not consent to wide distribution.

https://osf.io/nju48/?view_only=81dadc972f6545f19d107c83d8dba368

Power analyses

Power analyses were conducted by simulation, using the R package paramtest [37]. Data were simulated with the planned number of participants and study designs, and the theoretical effect sizes detailed below. According to analyses conducted before the project began, a sample size of 100 for Studies 1 and 2 would suffice to detect a main effect with a standardized beta of .25 with very high power (Power > .999) and would have adequate power to detect an interaction effect of .0625 (Power = .812). Studies 3 and 4 were planned with a sample size of 60, with similar power (Power > .999) to detect a .25 main effect but only enough power to detect an interaction effect of .125 (Power = .86 and .904, respectively). Unfortunately, these populations proved to be difficult to recruit and sample sizes were lower than planned.

Study 1

Method

Participants

We recruited a sample of Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk) workers from the United States and the UK. All participants had a masters qualification and declared that they were over 18 years old, and in romantic relationships that lasted six months or more. Participants who completed the study received $1.50 in compensation. One participant did not correctly answer an attention check and was removed. Two additional participants were erroneously removed from the dataset before pre-registration as they made a technical error while submitting their questionnaires (We re-ran all analyses including these participants and all results remain the same. Results for these analyses are provided in the Robustness Tests section in the S1 Appendix).

Of 99 participants, 45 identified as female and 54 identified as male. Mean relationship length was 11.23 years (SD = 10.94). Age data was not collected in this study.

Procedure

All studies were conducted online via the Qualtrics survey platform. Participants began by providing informed consent and indicating the length of their romantic relationship. In the first part of the study, participants were asked to imagine themselves in 8 hypothetical situations in which their romantic partner touched them in order to make them feel better. For each situation, participants rated its intensity and positivity/negativity. They were then asked to select one type of touch which they would prefer to receive. The available types of touch were presence (i.e., no touch), handholding, and optimal stroking. To ensure that the participants understood which type of touch is discussed, the types of touch were not labelled with words (e.g., “handholding”), but instead with looping 5-second videos showing the type of touch (see Fig 1, and details below).

Fig 1. The touch preference screen shown to participants.

Fig 1

The types of touch depicted are, from left to right, stroking, handholding, and no touch. The types of touch were depicted with videos, so the figure shows a still image from each video. Touch type order was randomized.

In the second part of the study, participants saw 8 different situations, and similarly rated intensity and positivity/negativity. However, this time they were asked to provide a rating for each type of touch. Again, the types of touch were labelled with looping videos. As detailed below, each participant saw the same 16 situations which were assigned randomly to one of the two parts of the study. The touch labels were presented in random order.

After these two parts, they completed a loneliness questionnaire for a separate project.

Measures

Situations. A list of hypothetical situations was generated using a pilot study. We showed 21 participants a list of 33 emotional or physical situations generated by the researchers and asked them to rate the valence and intensity of each situation. For each context (emotional or physical), we selected the two situations rated the most positive and intense as the positive intense situations for that context. We similarly selected three additional pairs of situations (negative intense, positive not intense, negative not intense). This process yielded 16 situations classified as intense/not intense (intensity), positive/negative (valence), and physical/emotional (context). Two situations were available for each of the eight possible configurations of these variables. In the actual study, two positive situations originally designated as intense (one physical and one emotional) were each rated less intense than one of the corresponding non-intense positive situations. Thus, the designations of these two situations were switched.

In each part of the study, participants saw 8 situations. Situation order was randomized such that in each part of the study participants saw one situation of each designation (e.g., one physical, non-intense, negative situation; one physical, non-intense, positive situation, etc.).

Situation Intensity and Valence Ratings. Each participant rated the intensity of each situation by responding to the prompt “How intense does this situation feel?” on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from “not intense at all” to “very intense”. They also rated the valence of each situation by responding to the prompt “How positive or negative does this situation feel?” on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from “very negative” to “very positive”.

Touch Preference. In the first part of the study, touch preference was measured using a direct choice. Participants were asked “Which kind of touch would you prefer to receive from your partner in that situation?” and selected one of the types of touch on the screen. Types of touch were presented using looping videos (Fig 1).

In the second part of the study, touch preference was measured more granularly by comparing participants’ ratings of each type of touch. Participants read the following instructions: “Please rate how would that kind of touch affect your feeling?” and responded on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from “would make me feel much worse” to “would make me feel much better”. Types of touch were presented using looping videos (Fig 2– note that this figure includes an additional type of touch which was only added in Study 2).

Fig 2. The touch rating screen shown to participants.

Fig 2

The types of touch depicted are, from top to bottom, no touch, handholding, full palm stroking, and standard stroking. The types of touch were depicted with videos, so the figure shows a still image from each video. Note that full palm stroking was only included in Studies 2 and 3.

Touch Label Videos. The touch literature specifies exact parameters for optimal stroking–stroking of the arm using the index and middle fingers, at speeds of 1–10 cm/s [11,38,39]. As describing this optimal procedure could confuse participants, videos were used instead of verbal labels to depict the different types of touch (Fig 1). The videos showed only the actors’ hands against a flat white background. The videos depicted presence (i.e., two hands not touching each other), stroking, and handholding. In the video depicting handholding the actors were instructed to hold their hands statically. In the stroking videos, the actors were asked to stroke their partner’s arm covering an area of nine cm over a period of three seconds. The videos were then evaluated by judges to confirm that they look natural. Follow-up inspection of videos revealed a range of speeds between 4–12 cm/s.

Videos were recorded by two heterosexual couples who had been in their romantic relationship for over a year. Each couple recorded one video for each type of touch. Videos by one couple were used in the first part of the study, and videos by the other couple were used in the second part, chosen randomly for each participant.

Statistical analysis

All analyses were conducted using R [40]. Analyses were conducted twice, with intensity measured as a rating–the intensity of the situation as self-reported by participants–or as a dichotomous variable–the pre-assigned intensity of the situations. For brevity, we report here only the findings for intensity rating; full results for dichotomous intensity are reported in the S2 Table in S1 Appendix. We state whenever there was a difference in significance between results using dichotomous intensity or intensity rating.

The direct touch type choices performed In the first part of the study were analyzed in a mixed logistical regression using the R package lme4 [41], using the following equation:

Choiceijb0+b1i+b2*Intensityij

The choice of participant i in situation j–stroking or handholding was estimated using a fixed intercept (b0), a random intercept for each participant (b1i), and an intensity effect (b2). Occasions on which participants chose presence over the other types of touch were not analyzed.

The touch ratings collected in the second part of the study were analyzed in a mixed linear model using the R package nlme [42], using the following equation:

Ratingijb0+b1i+b2*Intensityij+b3*Touchtypeij+b4*Intensityij*Touchtypeij

The rating of participant i in situation j was estimated using a fixed intercept (b0), a random intercept for each participant (b1i), an intensity effect (b2), a touch type effect (b3), and an interaction effect between intensity and touch type (b4). Touch type was coded 0.5 for handholding and -0.5 for stroking, so that coefficients for other effects would reflect average effects across the two touch types. In exploratory analyses with additional independent variables, fixed slopes were included for each main effect and for every possible interaction. All dependent variables were person mean-centered.

All analyses are accompanied by partial f2 effect sizes, calculated using the procedures outlined by Selya and colleagues [43]. To calculate f2 for a specific predictor, R2 was calculated for the complete model (R2Total), and for the model without the predictor (R2Omitted); f2 was calculated as (R2Total—R2Omitted) / (1–- R2Total).

To ensure that the effects are not due to specific statistical choices, we have re-run analyses for the main hypotheses using different statistical methods. These include modeling random slopes for all variables, using cumulative link models [44] implemented in the R package ordinal [45] (also with random slopes for all variables), and using simple repeated ANOVA tests to test effects with dichotomous intensity ratings. All of the results stayed the same–no significant effect became non-significant and vice versa. Results for the cumulative link models are provided in Tables 1 and 2, results for the other analyses are provided in the Robustness Tests section in the S1 Appendix. Note that the fully saturated cumulative link model did not converge for the second part of Study 2 (touch provision) when using participant intensity ratings (the model using dichotomous situation classifications converged successfully). Thus we ran that model without a random slope for touch type (but with random slopes for intensity and the interaction between intensity and touch type); this was the only way to have a converging model while removing only one random slope. We performed additional robustness tests on this specific study which are detailed in the S1 Appendix.

Table 1. The effect of intensity measured by intensity ratings and touch type on touch preference analyzed using cumulative link models.
b(SE) z p
Study 1 Intensity .404(.086) 4.69 < .001***
Touch Type .698(.141) 4.97 < .001***
Intensity*Touch Type .402(.133) 3.015 .003**
Study 2 touch provision Intensity 0.439(.096) 4.556 < .001***
Touch Type 1.267(.205) 6.169 < .001***
Intensity*Touch Type 0.51(.128) 3.981 < .001***
Study 2 touch reception Intensity 0.286(.088) 3.262 .001**
Touch Type 1.484(.184) 8.053 < .001***
Intensity*Touch Type 0.495(.12) 4.125 < .001***
Study 3 Intensity 1.058(.259) 4.092 < .001***
Touch Type 3.885(.627) 6.191 < .001***
Intensity*Touch Type 1.406(.339) 4.146 < .001***
Study 1 with omitted participants Intensity .393(.084) 4.705 < .001***
Touch Type .651(.14) 4.64 < .001***
Intensity*Touch Type .426(.131) 3.258 .001**
Table 2. The effect of intensity measured by dichotomous situation classifications and touch type on touch preference analyzed using cumulative link models.
b(SE) z p
Study 1 Intensity .109(.115) 0.949 .343
Touch Type .694(.136) 5.098 < .001***
Intensity*Touch Type .817(.208) 3.933 < .001***
Study 2 touch provision Intensity 0.072(.112) 0.638 .523
Touch Type 1.221(.193) 6.34 < .001***
Intensity*Touch Type 0.624(.186) 3.364 .001***
Study 2 touch reception Intensity 0.112(.122) 0.92 .358
Touch Type 1.424(.179) 7.939 < .001***
Intensity*Touch Type 0.548(.187) 2.927 .003**
Study 3 Intensity 2.207(.547) 4.033 < .001***
Touch Type 3.847(.685) 5.617 < .001***
Intensity*Touch Type 2.977(.779) 3.822 < .001***
Study 1 with omitted participants Intensity .093(.113) 0.824 .410
Touch Type .649(.137) 4.74 < .001***
Intensity*Touch Type .822(.204) 4.025 < .001***

Results

Descriptives

Descriptive values for situation intensity, situation valence, and touch type rating are presented in Table 3 and in Fig 3; A figure depicting all data points is included in the S2 Fig in S1 Appendix. As for choice questions, out of 395 situations classified as intense, handholding was chosen 216 times (54.7%), stroking 87 times (22%), and no touch 92 times (23.3%). Out of 389 situations classified as non-intense, handholding was chosen 133 times (34.2%), stroking 164 times (42.2%) and no touch 92 times (23.7%). These frequencies are presented in Fig 4. The slight difference in the number of situations (389 vs 395) is due to us switching the classification of two of the situations post randomization, as detailed above. The significance of none of the results regarding direct choice changed due to the switch.

Table 3. Descriptive statistics.
All Situations Intense Situations Non-Intense Situations
Person-level Mean(SD) Within-Person SD Person-level Mean(SD) Within-Person SD Person-level Mean(SD) Within-Person SD
Study 1 Intensity 2.4(0.5) 0.77 2.86(0.56) 0.53 1.99(0.58) 0.6
Valence 4.11(0.34) 2.12 3.93(0.81) 2.24 4.03(0.7) 1.79
Touch rating–- Handhold 3.87(0.59) 0.64 3.97(0.66) 0.59 3.8(0.64) 0.57
Touch rating–- Presence 2.79(0.44) 0.43 2.72(0.5) 0.42 2.81(0.52) 0.36
Touch rating–- Stroke 3.65(0.63) 0.63 3.6(0.66) 0.65 3.71(0.7) 0.54
Study 2 touch reception Intensity 2.51(0.49) 0.81 2.93(0.54) 0.58 2.09(0.61) 0.62
Valence 4.18(0.53) 2.05 4.12(0.6) 2.22 4.25(0.62) 1.82
Touch rating–Full palm stroke 3.67(0.83) 0.64 3.67(0.86) 0.61 3.68(0.89) 0.55
Touch rating–- Handhold 4.13(0.58) 0.6 4.18(0.61) 0.58 4.09(0.63) 0.51
Touch rating–- Presence 2.8(0.57) 0.48 2.75(0.61) 0.5 2.86(0.6) 0.39
Touch rating–- Stroke 3.6(0.79) 0.65 3.57(0.82) 0.62 3.64(0.86) 0.58
Study 2 touch provision Intensity 2.38(0.54) 0.78 2.77(0.61) 0.57 2(0.63) 0.6
Valence 4.17(0.49) 1.94 4.11(0.53) 2.14 4.23(0.56) 1.68
Touch rating–Full palm stroke 3.81(0.89) 0.65 3.76(0.95) 0.64 3.86(0.94) 0.57
Touch rating–- Handhold 4.25(0.8) 0.74 4.32(0.82) 0.74 4.18(0.88) 0.61
Touch rating–- Presence 2.79(0.61) 0.5 2.76(0.68) 0.5 2.82(0.6) 0.41
Touch rating–- Stroke 3.73(0.86) 0.64 3.67(0.9) 0.69 3.78(0.9) 0.52
Study 3 Intensity 2.58(0.48) .85 3.07(0.59) .3 1.59(0.73) 2
Valence 2.82(0.79) .98 2.41(1) .52 3.63(1.2) 2
MBIPI1 66.94(31.06)
Touch rating–Full palm stroke 3.14(1.05) .65 3.25(1.24) .36 2.9(1.08) 2
Touch rating–- Handhold 4.14(0.82) .59 4.38(0.9) .18 3.65(1.09) 2
Touch rating–- Presence 2.84(0.83) .38 2.83(0.95) .18 2.86(0.87) 2
Touch rating–- Stroke 3(1.1) .6 3.06(1.29) .36 2.88(1.07) 2
Study 4 Intensity 0.01(0.63) 0.69 0.4(0.68) 0.37 -0.46(0.82) 0.26
Touch rating–- Handhold 62.84(28.83) 17.09 64.14(28.41) 17.88 64.43(31.2) 12.91
Touch rating–- Presence 22.86(26.22) 8.4 20.46(26.1) 5.88 26.1(31.09) 7.07
Touch rating–- Stroke 38.2(35.16) 14.2 37.12(34.85) 15.58 44.14(36.97) 11.16

1Person-level variables do not differ between situations and as such have no within-person SD or situation-specific values.

2Study 3 had only one non-intense situation.

Fig 3. Study 1 intensity and touch type rating frequencies by situation intensity.

Fig 3

The figure shows how many times each type of touch was chosen as the preferred type of touch for a situation.

Fig 4. Number of participants who chose each type of touch by situation intensity.

Fig 4

The figure plots the distribution of subject mean ratings for each type of touch for each situation (e.g., the mean of one specific subject’s ratings of stroking for 4 intense situations would be one data point). Touch ratings refer to the way participants thought their feelings would change if they received this type of touch.

Direct choice

We performed a Chi-square test for goodness of fit to compare the overall number of times each type of touch was chosen (349 times handholding, 251 times stroking) to a chance (equal) distribution. The test confirmed that the distribution was significantly different from chance, meaning that handholding was significantly preferred over stroking overall (χ2(1) = 16.007, p < .001). We then performed a chi-square test for independence between the type of touch chosen and situation intensity. Touch type chosen and intensity were significantly associated with one another beyond chance (i.e., not statistically independent; χ2(2) = 43.317, p < .001), meaning that handholding was preferred in intense situations. We tested simple effects by comparing the distribution of touch type choices to chance, separately for intense and non-intense situations. Tests for simple effects found that in intense situations handholding was chosen more than stroking beyond chance (χ2(1) = 54.921, p < .001), whereas the difference between stroking and handholding in non-intense situations was not significantly different from chance (χ2(1) = 3.236, p = .072).

To examine whether participants would usually choose handholding over stroking, especially in situations which they rated as more intense (as opposed to the Chi-square tests which compared preferences between situations which were pre-classified as intense or not intense), we conducted a logistic mixed linear regression using the R package lme4 [41] to examine the effect of intensity on touch choice, as detailed above (situations in which “no touch” was chosen were removed). The results confirmed our hypothesis that handholding would be chosen significantly more often stroking (i.e., the intercept was positive and significant; b = .329(SE = .096), z = 3.427, p < .001) and that this effect would be stronger in more intense situations (i.e., an intensity effect, b = .593(SE = .101), z = 5.847, p < .001). As a robustness check, we also ran the model with saturated random slopes. Again, the results confirmed our hypothesis that handholding would be chosen significantly more often stroking (i.e., the intercept was positive and significant; b = .302(SE = .105), z = 2.861, p = .004) and that this effect would be stronger in more intense situations (i.e., an intensity effect, b = .77(SE = .16), z = 4.809, p < .001). This pattern of results also held when including the two omitted participants.

Rating

To examine whether participants would rate handholding as better at regulating their emotions than stroking, especially in situations which they rated as more intense, we conducted a mixed linear regression analysis as detailed above. Handholding was rated significantly higher than stroking, and the interaction between intensity and touch type was significant such that the difference between handholding and stroking was even larger in more intense situations (Full numerical results are provided in Table 4; results are demonstrated in Fig 5).

Table 4. The effect of situation intensity as measured by participant ratings and of touch type on touch rating.
b(SE) 95% CI t(df) p f2
Study 1 Intercept 3.759(.057) 3.65,3.87 66.231(1482) < .001*** 0
Intensity 0.117(.022) 0.07,0.16 5.2(1482) < .001*** .011
Touch Type 0.226(.035) 0.16,0.30 6.375(1482) < .001*** .016
Intensity*Touch Type 0.127(.045) 0.04,0.22 2.829(1482) .005** .003
Study 2 touch provision Intercept 3.886(.066) 3.76,4.02 58.613(2320) < .001*** 0
Intensity 0.105(.02) 0.07,0.14 5.359(2320) < .001*** .007
Touch Type 0.389(.031) 0.33,0.45 12.401(2320) < .001*** .036
Intensity*Touch Type 0.12(.039) 0.04,0.20 3.047(2320) .002** .002
Study 2 touch reception Intercept 3.885(.067) 3.75,4.02 58.129(2320) < .001*** 0
Intensity 0.062(.02) 0.02,0.10 3.104(2320) .002** .002
Touch Type 0.495(.033) 0.43,0.56 15.101(2320) < .001*** .055
Intensity*Touch Type 0.113(.04) 0.04,0.19 2.859(2320) .004** .002
Study 3 Intercept 3.603(.115) 3.38,3.83 31.34(405) < .001*** 0
Intensity 0.193(.052) 0.09,0.30 3.74(405) < .001*** .019
Touch Type 1.069(.092) 0.89,1.25 11.636(405) < .001*** .18
Intensity*Touch Type 0.249(.103) 0.05,0.45 2.404(405) .017* .008
Study 4 Intercept 51.108(4.677) 41.88,60.34 10.926(185) < .001*** .001
Intensity 3.155(2.378) -1.54,7.85 1.327(185) .186 0
Touch Type 22.627(3.456) 15.81,29.45 6.546(185) < .001*** .1
Intensity*Touch Type 0.729(4.686) -8.52,9.97 0.156(185) .877 0
Fig 5. Participants’ touch ratings as predicted by situation intensity and type of touch.

Fig 5

The figure shows the regression results for the association between the ratings for each type of touch and situation intensity.

We performed simple slope analyses via contrasts using the simple_slopes function of the R package reghelper [46]. Handholding was rated significantly higher than stroking at both high and low intensity levels, i.e., one standard deviation above and below mean intensity (when using dichotomous intensity classification, for intensity one standard deviation below the mean, the difference was in the same direction but not significant, p = .059). Ratings for handholding significantly increased with situation intensity. Ratings for stroking did not significantly change with intensity (when using dichotomous intensity classification, ratings for stroking significantly decreased with intensity). Full simple slope results are reported in Table 5.

Table 5. Simple slope analyses for the interaction between intensity measured as a self-reported rating and touch type in Studies 1–3.
b(SE) t(df) p
Study 1 Higher ratings for handholding (vs. stroking) at low intensity (-1 SD) 0.13(0.05) 2.51(1482) .012*
Higher ratings for handholding (vs. stroking) at high intensity (+1 SD) 0.33(0.05) 6.51(1482) < .001***
Higher ratings for handholding as intensity increases 0.18(0.03) 5.68(1482) < .001***
Higher ratings for stroking as intensity increases 0.05(0.03) 1.68(1482) .094
Study 2 touch provision Higher ratings for handholding (vs. stroking) at low intensity (-1 SD) 0.29(0.04) 6.61(2320) < .001***
Higher ratings for handholding (vs. stroking) at high intensity (+1 SD) 0.48(0.04) 10.92(2320) < .001***
Higher ratings for handholding as intensity increases 0.17(0.03) 5.15(2320) < .001***
Higher ratings for stroking as intensity increases 0.05(0.02) 2.00(2320) .045*
Study 2 touch reception Higher ratings for handholding (vs. stroking) at low intensity (-1 SD) 0.40(0.05) 8.65(2320) < .001***
Higher ratings for handholding (vs. stroking) at high intensity (+1 SD) 0.59(0.05) 12.70(2320) < .001***
Higher ratings for handholding as intensity increases 0.12(0.03) 3.65(2320) < .001***
Higher ratings for stroking as intensity increases 0.00(0.02) 0.21(2320) .832
Study 3 Higher ratings for handholding (vs. stroking) at low intensity (-1 SD) 0.85(0.13) 6.52(405) < .001***
Higher ratings for handholding (vs. stroking) at high intensity (+1 SD) 1.29(0.13) 9.92(405) < .001***
Higher ratings for handholding as intensity increases 0.32(0.08) 3.76(405) < .001***
Higher ratings for stroking as intensity increases 0.07(0.06) 1.16(405) .248

Exploratory analyses

We performed exploratory analyses on moderation of these effects by valence and physicality in the second part of the study. Participants rated all types of touch higher in negative situations than in positive situations, especially when those situations were physical. The difference between participants’ ratings of handholding as opposed to stroking was larger in positive than in negative situations and larger in emotional as opposed to physical situations. Full analysis tables are provided in the S1 Appendix.

Study 2

Study 2 aimed to replicate the results of Study 1, while also examining the types of touch participants thought would be better to provide to their partners, and controlling for the amount of skin touching by including a full palm stroking touch type.

Method

Participants

We recruited participants similarly to Study 1, but included workers from all countries. We attempted to recruit 100 participants as per the pre-registration. One participant did not ask for payment through the MTurk platform, resulting in 101 valid entries. Of 101 participants, 49 identified as female and 52 identified as male. Mean relationship length was 11.04 years (SD = 10.41). Age data was not collected in this study. Participants received compensation as in Study 1. All participants completed all of the measures.

Procedure

The procedure was similar to that of Study 1. Study 2 was also divided into two parts. The first part was identical to the second part of Study 1. Participants imagined themselves in eight situations and rated to what extent receiving each type of touch would change their feelings. The second part was the same, but participant were asked to imagine the various situations happening to their partner, and rated the extent to which providing each type of touch would change their partner’s emotions.

Measures

Intensity and valence measures were identical to Study 1. The situations used were identical to Study 1, using the revised situation designation (i.e., with two pairs of situations already switched). An additional type of touch was added to both parts of the study–- full palm stroking, which is identical to the stroking video except that the actors used their entire hand instead of two fingers, thus matching the amount of skin touching in the handholding video.

Results

Descriptives

Descriptive values for situation intensity, situation valence, and touch type rating are presented in Table 3 and Figs 6 and 7; Figures depicting all data points are included in the S3 and S4 Figs in S1 Appendix.

Fig 6. Study 2 part 1 (touch reception) intensity and touch type rating frequencies by situation intensity.

Fig 6

The figure plots the distribution of subject mean ratings for each type of touch for each situation (e.g., the mean of one specific subject’s ratings of stroking for 4 intense situations would be one data point). Touch ratings refer to the way participants thought their feelings would change if they received this type of touch.

Fig 7. Study 2 part 2 (touch provision) intensity and touch type rating frequencies by situation intensity.

Fig 7

The figure plots the distribution of subject mean ratings for each type of touch for each situation (e.g., the mean of one specific subject’s ratings of stroking for 4 intense situations would be one data point). Touch ratings refer to the way participants thought their partners’ feelings would change if they received this type of touch.

Touch Reception–- Rating

As in Study 1, to examine whether participants would rate handholding as better at regulating their emotions than stroking, especially in situations which they rated as more intense, we conducted a mixed linear regression analysis as detailed above. Preliminary analyses found no significant difference between partial stroke and full-palm stroke videos, and thus they were both coded as stroking in analyses.

Handholding was rated significantly higher over stroking, and the interaction between intensity and touch type was significant. Again, the difference between handholding and stroking was even larger in more intense situations (Full numerical results are provided in Table 3; results are demonstrated in Fig 5). As in Study 1, simple slope analyses via contrasts found that (1) handholding was rated significantly higher than stroking at both high and low intensity levels, i.e., one standard deviation above and below mean intensity and touch ratings for handholding, but not for stroking, significantly increased with intensity (this last finding–ratings for handholding increasing with intensity–was in the same direction but not significant when intensity was classified dichotomously, p = .088). Full simple slope results are reported in Table 4.

Touch Provision–Rating

As for touch reception, handholding was rated significantly higher than stroking, the interaction between intensity and touch type was significant. Again, the difference between handholding and stroking was even larger in more intense situations (Full numerical results are provided in Table 3; results are demonstrated in Fig 5). Simple slope analyses via contrasts found that (1) handholding was rated significantly higher than stroking at both high and low intensity levels, i.e., one standard deviation above and below mean intensity and (2) touch ratings for both handholding and stroking, significantly increased with intensity (when intensity was classified dichotomously ratings for stroking significantly decreased with intensity). Full simple slope results are reported in Table 4.

Exploratory analyses

We performed exploratory analyses on moderation of these effects by valence and physicality. For touch reception, participants rated all types of touch higher in negative situations than in positive situations, especially when those situations were physical (when intensity and valence were rated dichotomously, the difference between negative and positive situations was also larger in intense as opposed to non-intense situations). The difference between participants’ ratings of handholding as opposed to stroking was larger in emotional as opposed to physical situations.

For touch provision, participants rated all types of touch higher in emotional as opposed to physical situations, especially when those situations were positive (when intensity and valence were rated dichotomously, participants also rated all types of touch higher in negative as opposed to positive situations, and the effects of valence and physicality were stronger when situations were intense). The difference between participants’ ratings of handholding as opposed to stroking was larger in emotional as opposed to physical situations (this was not significant when intensity and valence were rated dichotomously; instead, the difference between ratings of handholding and striking was larger in negative as opposed to positive situations). Finally, the difference between participants’ ratings of handholding as opposed to stroking was larger in negative and intense situations than in positive, non-intense ones (this was not significant when intensity and valence were rated dichotomously; instead, the difference between ratings of handholding and stroking was larger in emotional and intense situations than in physical and non-intense ones). Full analysis tables are provided in the S1 Appendix.

Study 3

Study 3 aimed to replicate the results of the previous studies using a situation which was known to be relevant to participants, by recruiting participants with blood/injection phobia and asking them about touch preferences while receiving an injection (or a control situation).

Method

Participants

In Study 3 we recruited Hebrew-speaking participants over social media. All participants declared they were over 18 years old, in a romantic relationship lasting over six months, and had some fear of injections or blood. Twenty-one participants were recruited as volunteers. Because recruitment was slow, we added a payment equivalent to $6.00 and recruited an additional 34 participants. Four participants did not complete the entire questionnaire, three of which coded one out of three situations and one coded two out of three situations. Two participants of these four were erroneously not mentioned in the pre-registration document. All analyses used only the 51 participants for whom full data is available.

Of the 51 participants who completed the questionnaire, 32 identified as female and 19 identified as male. Mean relationship length was 6.85 years (SD = 6.84). Age data was not collected in this study. Participants’ mean score on the Multidimensional Blood/Injection Phobia Inventory (MBIPI; see S1 Appendix) was 66.941, slightly higher than the mean previously found in a clinical sample (61.4; [47]). Thus, the group had clinically relevant levels of blood/injection phobia. All participants completed all of the measures.

Procedure

The study was modeled after the second part of Study 1 and the first part of Study 2. Participants were asked to rate how the four types of touch examined in Study 2 would change their feelings in three medically relevant situations (see measures for details). At the end of the study they completed a blood/injection phobia questionnaire.

Measures

Measures for intensity, valence and touch rating were identical to Study 2, but translated to Hebrew.

Situations. Instead of the situations used in Studies 1 and 2, three different situations were presented: getting a vaccination shot, getting a blood test, and getting height and weight measured. For dichotomous intensity analyses (detailed in the S1 Appendix), the first two situations were considered intense and the last was considered not intense.

Blood\Injection Phobia. The level of blood\injection phobia was measured in Study 3 using the Multidimensional Blood/Injury Phobia Inventory (Wenzel & Holt, 2003), which asks participants to rate the extent to which they agree with each one of 40 statements on a 5-point Likert-type scale (from “not at all” to “completely”).

Results

Descriptives

Descriptive values for situation intensity, situation valence, touch type rating and blood/injection phobia are presented in Table 3 and in Fig 8; A figure depicting all data points is included in the S5 Fig in S1 Appendix.

Fig 8. Study 3 intensity and touch type rating frequencies by situation intensity.

Fig 8

The figure plots the distribution of subject mean ratings for each type of touch for each situation intensity (e.g., the mean of one specific subject’s ratings of stroking for the 2 intense situations would be one data point). Touch ratings refer to the way participants thought their feelings would change if they received this type of touch.

Touch Reception–Rating

As in Study 2, handholding was rated significantly higher than stroking, and the interaction between intensity and touch type was significant (Full numerical results are provided in Table 3; results are demonstrated in Fig 5). The difference between handholding and stroking was even larger in more intense situations. Again, simple slope analyses via contrasts found that handholding was rated significantly higher than stroking at both high and low intensity levels, i.e., one standard deviation above and below mean intensity (Full results in 2). Ratings for handholding, but not for stroking, significantly increased with intensity (when intensity was classified dichotomously, ratings for stroking significantly increased).

Exploratory analyses

We performed exploratory analyses on moderation of these effects by valence and by blood/injection phobia. No effects were found for valence. Contrary to our expectations, higher levels of blood/injection phobia were associated with a weaker preference for handholding over stroking; However, simple slope analyses revealed that the preference for handholding remained significant even when injection phobia was 2 standard deviations above the mean (b = .479(SE = .203), t(402) = 2.354, p = .019). Blood/injection phobia levels did not moderate the effects of situation intensity on touch type preference. Full analysis tables are provided in the S1 Appendix.

Study 4

Study 4 aimed to replicate the previous studies using a new situation–childbirth. Instead of hypothetical scenarios, participants answered questions about their recalled experiences while they were giving birth.

Method

Participants

We used social media to recruit 20 Arabic-speaking and 25 Hebrew-speaking participants (i.e., Arabic and Hebrew as their mother tongue, respectively). In Israel, where the study was performed, Hebrew native speakers are overwhelmingly Jewish while Arabic native speakers are overwhelmingly Arab [48], making the use of language as a proxy for culture a common practice in research on these cultural groups (e.g., [49,50]). Participants declared that they were over 18 years old and had given birth during the previous two months. All participants were female. Four Arabic-speaking and five Hebrew-speaking participants completed the questionnaires quite inadequately, with none of the dependent variables, and were removed from the study. Participants’ ages ranged from 22 to 41, with a mean of 30.8 years (SD = 5.03). Relationship length was not examined in this study.

Procedure

After providing informed consent, participants rated the amount of physical pain they experienced during childbirth, rating separately the amount of pain they experienced during contractions and between contractions. They were then asked which types of touch had been provided by a person close to them (e.g., partner or family member) who was present during their labor and delivery. They were then asked to evaluate the extent to which each type of touch helped reduce their physical pain, again answering separately about their experience during contractions, and then between contractions. When a type of touch was not actually provided, they were instructed to rate how they thought it would have affected them.

Then, they reported the amount of emotional pain they experienced, and rated the extent to which each type of touch helped reduce their emotional pain, again answering separately about their experience during contractions, and then between contractions.

All participants answered questions concerning recalled touch and concerning intensity and the effects of touch on physical pain during contractions, and concerning the intensity of physical pain between contractions. 7 participants quitted the survey at that point, leaving 29 participants who answered questions about touch effects on physical pain between contractions. One participant quitted the study at that point, leaving 28 participants who answered questions on intensity and touch effects on emotional pain during contractions. 9 participants quitted the survey at that point, leaving 19 participants who completed the final questions on emotional pain between contractions (We could not find any meaningful differences between participants who quit early and participants who did not; The main effect found for touch—higher ratings for handholding over stroking—was found even when looking only on touch effects on physical pain during contractions, where data was available for all participants).

Measures. Situation Intensity Rating

Intensity was measured using participants’ rating of physical pain as measured on a 0–100 sliding scale and their rating of emotional pain as measured using five items selected from the State-Trait Personality Inventory (STPI; [51]). The measure demonstrated high reliability, with Cronbach’s alpha of .89. Values were normalized to a mean of 0 and a standard deviation of 1 so that physical and emotional intensity would be comparable.

Touch Preference

At the beginning of the study, participants were asked which types of touch a person close to them who was present at childbirth provided. The options were presented verbally, and included handholding, stroking, touching the lower back, hugging, no touch, or “other”; participants could select one, multiple, or no types of touch. When participants were asked to rate types of touch, they were asked to rate the extent to which each of the aforementioned types of touch reduced their pain–physical or emotional–on a 1–100 scale, with no verbal indicators for endpoints. For example, for physical pain the prompt was “For each of the following types of touch, please rate on a scale of 1 to 100 how much did it help reduce your physical pain during the contractions”. Touch types were presented using videos, recorded by a different couple than the actors for the previous studies’ videos. These videos showed actors’ full bodies below the heads, wearing nondescript clothes on a white background. Videos were included for each of the aforementioned types of touch (handholding, stroking, touching the lower back, hugging, or no touch). There was no full palm stroking video in this study. Data on types of touch other than handholding and stroking was not analyzed for the current manuscript as they were only measured in Study 4 and no hypotheses including them were pre-registered.

Results

Descriptive statistics

Descriptive values for situation intensity, situation valence, and touch type rating are presented in Table 3 and in Fig 9; A figure depicting all data points is included in the S6 Fig in S1 Appendix.

Fig 9. Study 4 intensity and touch type rating frequencies by situation.

Fig 9

The figure plots the distribution of subject mean ratings for each type of touch for each situation intensity (e.g., the mean of one specific subject’s ratings of stroking during contractions would be one data point). Touch ratings refer to the way participants thought their feelings would change if they received this type of touch.

Recalled perceptions of touch during childbirth

Out of 36 participants, 7 (19.4%) reported that the helping person stroked them, while 25 (69.4%) reported that the person held their hand. To confirm our hypothesis that handholding would be more prevalent than stroking, we performed a chi-square for goodness of fit test comparing the actual distribution between handholding and stroking (19.4% and 69.4%) with a chance distribution (50% for each type of touch). The test revealed that the distribution was significantly different from chance (χ2(1) = 10.125, p = .001). Importantly, “stroking” was not explicitly specified to be on the hand or forearm. Thus, out of the 7 participants who reported receiving stroking, some may have received stroking in other parts of the body, making the difference in prevalence between handholding and stroking of the hand or forearm even larger.

Touch Reception–Rating

As in previous studies, handholding was rated significantly higher over stroking. However, the interaction between intensity and touch type was in the hypothesized direction but was not significant (Full numerical results are provided in Table 3; results are demonstrated in Fig 4).

Exploratory analyses

We performed exploratory analyses on moderation of these effects by valence, physicality and culture. Participants rated all types of touch higher in emotional, as opposed to physical situations. Arab-speaking participants rated all types of touch higher than Hebrew-speaking participants. No other effects were found. Full analysis tables are provided in the S1 Appendix.

Discussion

The overall pattern of results confirmed our hypotheses. Handholding was preferred over stroking, and in almost all studies that preference was stronger in hypothetical and recalled situations judged to be intense. Handholding was preferred by both recipients and providers and was strongly preferred behaviorally by people assisting women giving birth (as recalled by the women). These results suggest that, at least in hypothetical and recalled situations, participants subjectively preferred handholding over stroking as a type of touch suitable for regulation of acute emotions and pain.

Overall preference for handholding

In all four studies, using a variety of populations and situations and several methodologies, handholding was preferred over stroking as a form of emotion regulation. These findings support the feedback loop theory [6] which suggests that a key part of homeostatic emotion regulation via touch is establishing two-way communication which allows the toucher to optimally react to the other person’s emotional state. In handholding, both people are using their palms–one of the most sensitive parts of the skin [22]–allowing them to establish high quality tactile communication. While the single study we could find comparing effectiveness of handholding and stroking did not find significant differences [24], that could have been due to some specific circumstances of that study, or due to its limited sample size.

However, it could also be the case that the preference for handholding is unrelated to differences in effectiveness. One possibility is that as handholding is more prevalent in society, it is more recognizable and enjoys a familiarity effect [52]. Holding hands is ubiquitous and even appears in some monkey populations [53], and features in a variety of cultures: for example, it is mentioned in well known Western cultural texts [54,55], is a popular public gesture of affection in Vietnamese culture [56], and features in indigenous Taiwanese dances [57].

Another possibility is that top-down processes might lead to handholding being thought of as more effective. While, as discussed in the introduction, bottom-up processes such as the activation of CT fibers [58] may be more dominant in stroking, top-down processes have been identified in both types of touch. For example, studies have shown that participants’ relationship satisfaction moderated the effects of handholding, such that participants with more satisfying relationships exhibited stronger emotion regulation effects [9,59]; another study has shown that participants’ attachment style moderated the pleasantness caused by stroking, such that participants with more anxious attachment styles experienced less pleasantness. While we could find no direct comparison of top-down effects, one study [29] explored difference in the ways people perceive reciprocal types of touch, which include handholding, as opposed to non-reciprocal types of touch, which include stroking (although these two specific types of touch were not directly examined in the study). The study showed that people regarded both participants in reciprocal touch slightly higher (on dimensions of valence and likability) than providers of non-reciprocal touch, and significantly higher than recipients of non-reciprocal touch. Thus, people thinking about providing or receiving handholding might be thinking about themselves as more likeable and with a more positive valence, which might lead them to choose this type of touch over others regardless of its actual bottom-up effects.

Finally, other possible mechanisms include differences between active and passive touch and the regulation effects of holding an object (not necessarily a hand). Regarding active and passive touch, to the best of our knowledge no existing study examined differences in their emotion regulation capacities. However a review of differences in their sensory capabilities found studies pointing to either type of touch as better performing [60]. The authors of the review suggest that the differing factor may be task complexity, with passive touch performing better in sensing simple (as opposed to complex) stimuli. It could be the case that another person’s skin is not an overly complex stimulus, and as such the passive touch involved in handholding might perform better. Additionally, in both handholding and stroking the touch recipient is engaged in passive touch, limiting differences in this regard to the touch provider.

Regarding the effects of holding an object, it is theoretically possible that the effects of handholding are merely the effects of holding an object. However, while holding an object (e.g., a squeeze ball) is a recognized form of distraction in the medical literature, it is not significantly different than other forms of distraction such as watching cartoons or hearing music [61,62]. Thus, there is no specific reason to believe that stroking would not generate similar distraction. Indeed, emotion regulation studies directly comparing holding an object to handholding find larger effects for handholding, suggesting that additional mechanisms are at play [9,63].

Intense situations

In Studies 1–3 the preference for handholding (measured either by direct choice or by comparing ratings) was even stronger in intense situations. As detailed in the introduction, handholding allows for better two-way sensory communication than stroking, which could make it more effective in homeostatic emotion regulation. Intense situations might be more likely to cause an abrupt shift in internal states, requiring a quick return to the organism’s baseline state–i.e., preservation of homeostasis. In contrast, non-intense situations might require a shift in the long-term biological setpoint, rather than a return to the norm [5]. Stroking may be especially suited for this role (sometimes termed allostatic regulation [5,64]). For example, in safe and relaxed situations in the presence of one’s partner, stroking may help move the long-term biological setpoint, allowing the individual to be less vigilant in such situations in the future, freeing resources for other tasks. Indeed, gentle stroking has been shown to be prevalent among romantic partners [65] and to play a role in emotional communication of love and affection [66]. Future studies could compare situations like the ones presented in this study to situations which involve even less specific emotions than the low intensity situations used in the current study (e.g., a nondescript evening at home with one’s partner). In such situations, stroking might even be preferred over handholding.

Notably, this effect was not found in Study 4. We attribute this to low power, or alternatively to a ceiling effect. The situation labeled as not intense—during childbirth, between contractions–might have been considered intense relative to everyday situations. Indeed, the main touch type effect in this study was the largest effect size of all four studies, supporting this assumption.

Exploratory findings

Valence and physicality

Exploratory findings for valence and physicality are summarized in S7 Table in the S1 Appendix. Focusing on moderation of touch type preference by valence and physicality, most effects were only significant in a single part of a single study, including three-way interactions and moderation of handholding preference by valence. Thus, the results support the notion that the effects are not limited to positive or negative-valenced situations. While our discussion is somewhat focused on regulation of negative emotions, which are the vast majority of emotion regulation instances reported in everyday life [67], our findings demonstrate that handholding is equally preferred to regulate either type of emotion.

The only effect which was somewhat consistent was the moderation of handholding preference by physicality–in Studies 1 and 2 (except for when using dichotomous measures in the second part of Study 2), the preference for handholding over stroking, as reflected in higher ratings, was even stronger in emotional situations than in physical ones. Simple slope analyses revealed that while ratings for stroking were similar between physical and emotional situations, ratings for handholding were higher in emotional situations, defined here as situations which did not directly involve the body (e.g., winning or losing money) as opposed to physical situations, defined here as situations which directly involved the body (e.g., falling down the stairs, dancing). A possible explanation could be that stroking involves both top-down processes (which may be more prominent in emotional situations) and bottom-up processes (which may be more prominent in physical situations), leading its effects to be comparable across these types of situations; handholding, on the other hand, seems to involve mostly top-down processes, which may explain why it is especially preferred in emotional situations. Still, the overall preference for handholding was still significant in physical situations, indicating that despite this moderation effect the main effects described above exist in both physical and emotional situations. Importantly, as this moderation effect was only an exploratory analysis, it should be treated as provisional until replicated in future studies.

Blood/Injection phobia

Contrary to our expectations, the preference for handholding was weaker for participants with especially high blood/injection phobia. Simple slope analyses revealed that ratings for handholding were similar between participants with varied levels of phobia, while ratings for stroking were higher for participants with high levels of phobia. This effect might stem out of a ceiling effect for ratings of handholding by participants with high levels of phobia. Handholding was rated higher in general in this study than in the first two studies; Importantly, although the difference was not significant, participants with high levels of phobia rated all types of touch higher than participants with low levels of phobia. Thus, it is possible that participants with high levels of phobia rated handholding close to the maximal ratings possible, leaving less room for them to increase those ratings when compared to ratings of stroking. Still, the overall preference for handholding was still significant for participants with high levels of phobia, indicating that despite this moderation effect the main effects described above still exist for people with varied levels of injection phobia. Importantly, as this moderation effect was only an exploratory analysis, it should be treated as provisional until replicated in future studies.

Cultural differences

No moderation of touch preference by culture was found. While this suggests that the differences in touch preference are at least somewhat robust across cultures, the sample size for Study 4 was relatively small, suggesting that this might simply be a null result due to low power.

Implications for future research

The findings of the current study have several implications for future research on emotional regulation via touch. First, as participants have shown a clear preference for handholding, at least in hypothetical and recalled situations, research on touch as a form of emotion regulation should strive to include handholding conditions. Importantly, these studies should also attempt to compare the effectiveness of different types of touch in different top-down contexts (e.g., when trying to regulate intense vs. non-intense emotions). Effectiveness studies could also expand to include other types of touch, such as hugging.

On the other hand, while the current study examined a variety of situations, it could not explore every possible one. More subjective preference research should be conducted to investigate whether there are situations–involving emotion regulation or otherwise–in which people prefer stroking over handholding. Finally, if future effectiveness studies find that stroking is more effective than handholding as a form of emotion regulation, interventions should be developed to encourage people to use stroking in their everyday life.

Limitations

While the four studies discussed in the current paper cover the question of subjective touch preference from a variety of angles, there are still several limitations.

First, while the study included both hypothesized and recalled situations, it did not include live ones. Future studies could put participants in actual situations requiring emotion regulation or use experience sampling methods to track participants as such situations naturally occur. This would allow them to test whether preferences in the moment are similar to hypothesized and recalled ones. Relatedly, in the second part of Study 4 some of the participants received the types of touch they were rating during their actual childbirth experience, while some did not, and were rating them hypothetically, a difference which may have affected the results. As only 7 women (under 20%) received stroking and only 11 women (less than 35%) did not receive handholding, analyzing these subgroups would be underpowered. That said, Studies 1–3 conclusively show that even when all situations are hypothetical, handholding is preferred over stroking.

Second, in Studies 1 and 2 situations were classified as physical or emotional (non-physical). We did not ask each participant to code situations on this axis, and as such cannot verify the validity of this classification. However, the physicality classification has no bearing on our main hypotheses concerning touch preference and intensity.

Third, as detailed above, the study used videos to present optimal stroking without biasing participants with an overly detailed description of stroking compared to handholding. Of course, while the videos were carefully designed to reduce possible biases–by using neutral backgrounds, not showing faces, etc.—some characteristic of the videos which was not controlled for might still have affected results. Some possible factors include video quality (e.g., non-steady camera, shadows), the fact that the actors were all in opposite-sex relationships while participants could have been of any sexual orientation (and in any type of relationship), or the fact that both types of touch were depicted in the air (i.e., not sitting down, or close to the body). This is somewhat mitigated by the first part of Study 4, in which types of touch were presented verbally, without videos, and still “stroking” was overwhelmingly less common as a form of support for women giving birth than “handholding”.

Finally, most participants were from Western, high-income countries, limiting the generalizability of the findings. Study 4 partially addressed this concern by including both Hebrew and Arabic-speaking participants; however, it should be noted that the sample size was small and the study was run in an Israeli (i.e., Western and high-income) context.

Conclusion

In conclusion, the results of four studies demonstrate a consistent subjective preference for handholding over stroking as a form of emotional regulation, especially in intense situations. We suggest several possible explanations for this phenomenon, including the cultural familiarity of handholding, top-down processes having stronger effects than bottom-up processes on subjective preference, handholding being a more passive form of touch, and handholding’s unique capability to establish two-way sensory communication, creating an optimal feedback loop.

Supporting information

S1 Appendix. Additional analyses.

(DOCX)

Data Availability

All four studies were preregistered on the Open Science Framework: Study 1: https://osf.io/x95f4/?view_only=e272eb9ccc2a46668114685dcb8988e5 Study 2: https://osf.io/nzg6v/?view_only=b9cf1e802d894ce387c80c47ffd7bc97 Study 3: https://osf.io/8n3pu/?view_only=f23a2e3d485946ffb95d32c02e469b85 Study 4: https://osf.io/74szg/?view_only=4b12d6d2a8a04483abbd6ea0c8c225ca. Study data, analysis code and touch videos for Studies 1-3 are available at the link below, except for touch videos for Study 4 which were not posted as participants who filmed the videos did not consent to wide distribution. https://osf.io/nju48/?view_only=81dadc972f6545f19d107c83d8dba368.

Funding Statement

The work was funded by a MINDSS grant awarded to the first author by the University of Haifa and by the U.S. - Israel Binational Science Fund (BSF) grant 2015068 awarded to the fourth and fifth authors. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Gliga T, Farroni T, Cascio CJ. Social touch: A new vista for developmental cognitive neuroscience? Developmental cognitive neuroscience. 2019;35:1. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.05.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.McIntyre S, Hauser SC, Kusztor A, Boehme R, Moungou A, Isager PM, et al. The language of social touch is intuitive and quantifiable. Psychological Science. 2022;33(9):1477–94. doi: 10.1177/09567976211059801 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Gallace A, Spence C. The science of interpersonal touch: an overview. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2010;34(2):246–59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Schirmer A, Chiu MH, Croy I. More than one kind: Different sensory signatures and functions divide affectionate touch. Emotion. 2021. doi: 10.1037/emo0000966 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Fotopoulou A, Von Mohr M, Krahé C. Affective regulation through touch: homeostatic and allostatic mechanisms. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2022;43:80–7. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.08.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Shamay-Tsoory S, Eisenberger N. Getting in touch: a neural model of comforting touch. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2021. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.08.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Goldstein P, Weissman-Fogel I, Dumas G, Shamay-Tsoory SG. Brain-to-brain coupling during handholding is associated with pain reduction. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences. 2018;115(11):E2528–37. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1703643115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Sriramka B, Mallik D, Singh J, Khetan M. Effect of hand-holding and conversation alone or with midazolam premedication on preoperative anxiety in adult patients—A randomised controlled trial. Indian Journal of Anaesthesia. 2021;65(2):128. doi: 10.4103/ija.IJA_705_20 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Sahi RS, Dieffenbach MC, Gan S, Lee M, Hazlett LI, Burns SM, et al. The comfort in touch: Immediate and lasting effects of handholding on emotional pain. PloS one. 2021;16(2):e0246753. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0246753 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.López-Solà M, Geuter S, Koban L, Coan JA, Wager TD. Brain mechanisms of social touch-induced analgesia in females. Pain. 2019;160(9):2072–85. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001599 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Löken LS, Wessberg J, McGlone F, Olausson H. Coding of pleasant touch by unmyelinated afferents in humans. Nature neuroscience. 2009;12(5):547. doi: 10.1038/nn.2312 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Ackerley R, Wasling HB, Liljencrantz J, Olausson H, Johnson RD, Wessberg J. Human C-tactile afferents are tuned to the temperature of a skin-stroking caress. Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34(8):2879–83. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2847-13.2014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.McGlone F, Olausson H, Boyle JA, Jones-Gotman M, Dancer C, Guest S, et al. Touching and feeling: differences in pleasant touch processing between glabrous and hairy skin in humans. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;35(11):1782–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08092.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Morrison I. ALE meta-analysis reveals dissociable networks for affective and discriminative aspects of touch. Human brain mapping. 2016;37(4):1308–20. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Pawling R, Trotter PD, McGlone FP, Walker SC. A positive touch: C-tactile afferent targeted skin stimulation carries an appetitive motivational value. Biological Psychology. 2017;129:186–94. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.08.057 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.von Mohr M, Kirsch LP, Fotopoulou A. The soothing function of touch: affective touch reduces feelings of social exclusion. Scientific reports. 2017;7(1):1–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.von Mohr M, Krahé C, Beck B, Fotopoulou A. The social buffering of pain by affective touch: a laser-evoked potential study in romantic couples. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience. 2018;13(11):1121–30. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsy085 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Krahé C, von Mohr M, Gentsch A, Guy L, Vari C, Nolte T, et al. Sensitivity to CT-optimal, affective touch depends on adult attachment style. Scientific reports. 2018;8(1):1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Walker SC, Trotter PD, Swaney WT, Marshall A, Mcglone FP. C-tactile afferents: Cutaneous mediators of oxytocin release during affiliative tactile interactions? Neuropeptides. 2017;64:27–38. doi: 10.1016/j.npep.2017.01.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Watkins RH, Dione M, Ackerley R, Backlund Wasling H, Wessberg J, Löken LS. Evidence for sparse C-tactile afferent innervation of glabrous human hand skin. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2021;125(1):232–7. doi: 10.1152/jn.00587.2020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Case LK, Liljencrantz J, McCall MV, Bradson M, Necaise A, Tubbs J, et al. Pleasant deep pressure: expanding the social touch hypothesis. Neuroscience. 2021;464:3–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2020.07.050 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Gibson GO, Craig JC. Tactile spatial sensitivity and anisotropy. Perception & Psychophysics. 2005;67(6):1061–79. doi: 10.3758/bf03193632 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Morioka M, Whitehouse DJ, Griffin MJ. Vibrotactile thresholds at the fingertip, volar forearm, large toe, and heel. Somatosensory & motor research. 2008;25(2):101–12. doi: 10.1080/08990220802045574 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Reddan MC, Young H, Falkner J, López-Solà M, Wager TD. Touch and social support influence interpersonal synchrony and pain. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2020. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Knapp ML, Hall JA, Horgan TG. Nonverbal communication in human interaction. Cengage Learning; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Patterson ML. An arousal model of interpersonal intimacy. Psychological review. 1976;83(3):235. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Guerrero LK, Andersen PA. Patterns of matching and initiation: Touch behavior and touch avoidance across romantic relationship stages. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. 1994;18:137–53. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Fareri DS, Delgado MR. Social rewards and social networks in the human brain. The Neuroscientist. 2014;20(4):387–402. doi: 10.1177/1073858414521869 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Schirmer A, Reece C, Zhao C, Ng E, Wu E, Yen SC. Reach out to one and you reach out to many: Social touch affects third-party observers. British Journal of Psychology. 2015;106(1):107–32. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12068 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Cruciani G, Zanini L, Russo V, Boccardi E, Spitoni GF. Pleasantness ratings in response to affective touch across hairy and glabrous skin: a meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2021. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.09.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Weekes DP, Kagan SH, James K, Seboni N. The phenomenon of hand holding as a coping strategy in adolescents experiencing treatment-related pain. Journal of Pediatric Oncology Nursing. 1993;10(1):19–25. doi: 10.1177/104345429301000105 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Krahé C, Paloyelis Y, Sambo CF, Fotopoulou A. I like it when my partner holds my hand: Development of the responses and attitudes to support during pain questionnaire (RASP). Frontiers in Psychology. 2014;5:1027. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Habig K, Schänzer A, Schirner W, Lautenschläger G, Dassinger B, Olausson H, et al. Low threshold unmyelinated mechanoafferents can modulate pain. BMC neurology. 2017;17(1):1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Peled-Avron L, Woolley JD. Understanding others through observed touch: neural correlates, developmental aspects, and psychopathology. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2022;43:152–8. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Öst LG. Blood and injection phobia: Background and cognitive, physiological, and behavioral variables. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1992;101(1):68. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.101.1.68 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Brownridge P. The nature and consequences of childbirth pain. European Journal of Obstetrics & Gynecology and Reproductive Biology. 1995;59:S9–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Hughes J. paramtest: Run a Function Iteratively While Varying Parameters [Internet]. 2017. Available from: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=paramtest
  • 38.Essick GK, McGlone F, Dancer C, Fabricant D, Ragin Y, Phillips N, et al. Quantitative assessment of pleasant touch. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2010;34(2):192–203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.McGlone F, Wessberg J, Olausson H. Discriminative and affective touch: sensing and feeling. Neuron. 2014;82(4):737–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing [Internet]. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2022. Available from: https://www.R-project.org/
  • 41.Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software. 2015;67(1):1–48. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Pinheiro J, Bates D, DebRoy S, Sarkar D, R Core Team. nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models [Internet]. 2018. Available from: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Selya AS, Rose JS, Dierker LC, Hedeker D, Mermelstein RJ. A practical guide to calculating Cohen’s f2, a measure of local effect size, from PROC MIXED. Frontiers in psychology. 2012;3:111. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Agresti A. Categorical data analysis. John Wiley & Sons; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Christensen RHB. ordinal—Regression Models for Ordinal Data. 2022. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Hughes J, Beiner D. reghelper: Helper Functions for Regression Analysis [Internet]. 2022. Available from: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=reghelper. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Wenzel A, Holt CS. Validation of the multidimensional Blood/Injury phobia inventory: evidence for a unitary construct. Journal of psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment. 2003;25(3):203–11. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Henkin-Roitfarb R. Hebrew and Arabic in asymmetric contact in Israel. Lodz Papers in Pragmatics. 2011;7(1):61–100. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Rubin E, Bar V, Cohen A. The images of scientists and science among Hebrew-and Arabic-speaking pre-service teachers in Israel. International Journal of Science Education. 2003;25(7):821–46. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Zuzovsky R. The impact of socioeconomic versus linguistic factors on achievement gaps between Hebrew-speaking and Arabic-speaking students in Israel in reading literacy and in mathematics and science achievements. Studies in Educational Evaluation. 2010;36(4):153–61. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Jacobs GA, Latham LE, Brown MS. Test-retest reliability of the state-trait personality inventory and the anger expression scale. Anxiety Research. 1988;1(3):263–5. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Zajonc RB. Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. Journal of personality and social psychology. 1968;9(2p2):1. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Brockett RC, Horwich RH, Jones CB. Hand-holding by Belizean black howler monkeys: intentional communication in a neotropical primate. Folia Primatologica. 2005;76(4):227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Ojennus P. Holding hands in the" Argonautica". The Classical Journal. 2006;253–70. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Welshans M. “So hand in hand they passed”: Hand-holding and monist marriage in paradise lost. Journal for Early Modern Cultural Studies. 2018;18(1):66–97. [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Vũ TL. A cross culture study on using gestures of Vietnamese and American people. Đại học Dân lập Hải Phòng; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Chi-fang C. ‘Holding hands to dance’: Movement as cultural metaphor in the dances of indigenous people in taiwan. In: Identity and diversity. Routledge India; 2020. p. 1–21. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Cascio CJ, Moore D, McGlone F. Social touch and human development. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2019;35:5–11. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.04.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Coan JA, Schaefer HS, Davidson RJ. Lending a hand: Social regulation of the neural response to threat. Psychological science. 2006;17(12):1032–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01832.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Symmons M, Richardson B, Wuillemin. Active versus passive touch: Superiority depends more on the task than the mode. In 2004. p. 179–85. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Aydin D, Şahiner NC, Çiftçi EK. Comparison of the effectiveness of three different methods in decreasing pain during venipuncture in children: ball squeezing, balloon inflating and distraction cards. Journal of clinical nursing. 2016;25(15–16):2328–35. doi: 10.1111/jocn.13321 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Rezai MS, Goudarzian AH, Jafari-Koulaee A, Bagheri-Nesami M. The effect of distraction techniques on the pain of venipuncture in children: A systematic review. Journal of Pediatrics Review. 2017;5(1):26–37. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Master SL, Eisenberger NI, Taylor SE, Naliboff BD, Shirinyan D, Lieberman MD. A picture’s worth: Partner photographs reduce experimentally induced pain. Psychological science. 2009;20(11):1316–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02444.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Burleson MH, Quigley KS. Social interoception and social allostasis through touch: legacy of the somatovisceral afference model of emotion. Social neuroscience. 2021;16(1):92–102. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2019.1702095 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Sorokowska A, Saluja S, Sorokowski P, Frąckowiak T, Karwowski M, Aavik T, et al. Affective interpersonal touch in close relationships: A cross-cultural perspective. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2021;0146167220988373. doi: 10.1177/0146167220988373 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Kirsch LP, Krahé C, Blom N, Crucianelli L, Moro V, Jenkinson PM, et al. Reading the mind in the touch: Neurophysiological specificity in the communication of emotions by touch. Neuropsychologia. 2018;116:136–49. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.05.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Gross JJ, Richards JM, John OP. Emotion Regulation in Everyday Life. In: Emotion regulation in couples and families: Pathways to dysfunction and health. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association; 2006. p. 13–35. [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Jamie Males

15 Aug 2022

PONE-D-22-07411I Wanna Hold Your Hand:

Handholding is Preferred over Gentle Stroking for Acute Emotion RegulationPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Sened,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process. Please pay particular attention to addressing the reviewer's comments regarding the framing of your Introduction and Discussion, and their recommendations regarding the presentation of the methods and results.

Please note that we have only been able to secure a single reviewer to assess your manuscript. We are issuing a decision on your manuscript at this point to prevent further delays in the evaluation of your manuscript. Please be aware that the editor who handles your revised manuscript might find it necessary to invite additional reviewers to assess this work once the revised manuscript is submitted. However, we will aim to proceed on the basis of this single review if possible.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Sep 26 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Jamie Males

Editorial Office

PLOS ONE

Journal requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf".

2. Please provide additional details regarding participant consent. In the ethics statement in the Methods and online submission information, please ensure that you have specified what type you obtained (for instance, written or verbal, and if verbal, how it was documented and witnessed). If your study included minors, state whether you obtained consent from parents or guardians. If the need for consent was waived by the ethics committee, please include this information.

3. Thank you for stating the following financial disclosure:

“The work was funded by a MINDSS grant awarded to the first author by the University of Haifa and by a U.S. - Israel Binational Science Fund (BSF) grant awarded to the fourth and fifth authors.”

Please state what role the funders took in the study.  If the funders had no role, please state: ""The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.""

If this statement is not correct you must amend it as needed.

Please include this amended Role of Funder statement in your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

4. Thank you for stating the following in the Funding Section of your manuscript:

“The first author’s work on the project was funded by a MINDSS grant from the University of Haifa faculty of social sciences. The fourth and fifth author’s work on the project was funded by a grant from the Bi-national Science Fund (BSF).”

We note that you have provided funding information that is not currently declared in your Funding Statement. However, funding information should not appear in the Acknowledgments section or other areas of your manuscript. We will only publish funding information present in the Funding Statement section of the online submission form.

Please remove any funding-related text from the manuscript and let us know how you would like to update your Funding Statement. Currently, your Funding Statement reads as follows:

“The work was funded by a MINDSS grant awarded to the first author by the University of Haifa and by a U.S. - Israel Binational Science Fund (BSF) grant awarded to the fourth and fifth authors.”

Please include your amended statements within your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

5. Your ethics statement should only appear in the Methods section of your manuscript. If your ethics statement is written in any section besides the Methods, please delete it from any other section.

6. We note that you have referenced (Spielberger CD, Jacobs G, Crane R, Russell S, Westberry L, Barker L, et al. Preliminary manual for the state-trait personality inventory (STPI). Unpublished manuscript, University of South Florida, Tampa. 1979; which has currently not yet been accepted for publication. Please remove this from your References and amend this to state in the body of your manuscript:  [Unpublished]”) as detailed online in our guide for authors

http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-reference-style.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: No

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The authors present a series of online studies where handholding is compared to arm stroking, in different intensity situations. They make the excellent distinction that handholding is a truly two-way (active/giving and receiving touch) interaction, whereas stroking is much more one-way (passive/receiving touch). The authors could make even more of this point throughout, where touch becomes a stronger form of communication in handholding. I congratulate the authors on the open way their studies are presented, yet the organization of the results is confusing. Overall, I am not convinced about the statistical analyses used. They seem incomplete, yet repetitive, and do not seem to correct for multiple tests, as well as using unjustified one-way t-tests. Also, the grounding of the work in previous literature needs to be revisited. However, the manuscript is novel and opens up new ideas into social touch mechanisms, adding new insights to the literature.

Introduction

- I do not think the study is well-grounded in the actual findings on affective touch. Many studies investigate the pleasantness of stroking touch and a few have conducted microneurography and shown that C-tactile fibers fire well to slow, gentle touch. However, C-tactile afferents are just one part of affective touch. The authors say, ‘Gentle stroking entails slowly stroking skin regions with C-tactile afferents…’ but this is imprecise, as gentle stroking will readily activate all types of mechanoreceptive afferent. Although CTs fire well to gentle skin stroking, it is clear that affective touch is not strictly linked to CT firing, where the relationship breaks down when other factors are added. Cognitive factors can influence gentle stroking perception, e.g. changes in pleasantness with different odors (Croy et al, 2014 Plos One). Such a breakdown in the CT vs. affective touch relationship has also been demonstrated, when touch was delivered at different temperatures and CT firing no longer correlated with pleasantness (Ackerley et al, 2014 J Neurosci). Further, a number of studies have shown little differences in the perception of pleasant touch on hairy arm and glabrous hand skin (for an overview, see Cruciani et al, 2021 Neurosci Biobehav Rev). Thus, affective touch is more complicated than simply CT afferent firing.

- In the next sentence, they say ‘…activates regions in the posterior insular cortex and the mid-anterior orbitofrontal cortex that are not activated by other types of touch’. Again, this is imprecise, as it is well-known that posterior insula is activated by all types of touch and the meta-analysis by Morrison (2016, Hum Brain Map) found that in general, posterior insula is more likely to be activated for affective touch, S1 for discriminative touch, whereas S2 is well-engaged in both; however, in no way were areas fully selective for one type of touch or the other.

- The authors say ‘no significant differences were found between handholding and gentle stroking, although effects of handholding were greater in every case’ for the work by Reddan et al (2020); however, if there were no significant differences in this work, they cannot claim that the effects of handholding were greater.

- The paper by Schirmer et al (2021, Emotion) on different types of touch may be useful to add to the introduction, in terms of the topographies of touch with others and the social implications.

Methods

- Would it be possible to add age details (e.g. range) for each study? I am also presuming that the sample for Study 4 was only females, but it would be good to state this too.

- It would be good to have the ethical section at the beginning of the methods, as normally done.

Results

- Please can the number of decimal places be reduced in the tables? Only one (maybe two) decimal places are relevant for the data, too many numbers make the table difficult to interpret and I do not think such accuracy is relevant for these ratings (also for the t values).

- I do not understand why the data have been analyzed twice. Two mixed lineal models were conducted with intensity as a dichotomous variable and as a continuous variable, but there is no clear reason for this and it confuses the results.

- On p.22, I do not understand why the authors say (two times) that ‘this was only true in a one-tailed significance test’ and have p values of >0.05. I do not think they are justified in using a one-tailed t-test (why not corrected, post-hoc tests) and these p values are not significant. Also, just underneath, a p value of 0.094 is quoted as significant, when it is not.

- Top of p.23, why are the results for non-intense situations not stated?

Discussion

- Although well-written, the first part of the discussion goes too far. All the studies were online and featured videos and hypothetical situations. Therefore, the authors need to be clear that these were not actual, real, in-person situations, where the results could be different.

- Near the bottom of p. 24, the authors say, ‘…as opposed to stroking, which relies mainly on bottom-up processes’. I do not fully agree that stroking specifically relies mainly on bottom-up processes. Although receiving stroking is passive touch, the situation will determine the perception, including various top-down effects. Such top-down effects may be more relevant for active touch in hand-holding, but they are present in both situations.

- In the middle of p. 25, the authors say, ‘even if handholding is found in the future to be less

effective than stroking’, but what does this mean? Less effective in what way? I am not sure if I follow all the section on ‘effectiveness’, as this concept has not been well-defined throughout. Do you mean how effective touch is to console/comfort someone?

Minor comments

- p. 14, ‘hypothesize themselves in different hypothetical situations’ could be revised for clarity. Imagine themselves?

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Decision Letter 1

Rochelle Ackerley

25 Oct 2022

PONE-D-22-07411R1I Wanna Hold Your Hand:

Handholding is Preferred over Gentle Stroking for Acute Emotion RegulationPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Sened,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Dec 09 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Rochelle Ackerley

Guest Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments:

Thank you for revising your manuscript. I am sorry that the review process has been long for this submission. To explain the situation, Plos One had difficulties finding an Editor. I was the first reviewer in the initial round and I am now the Guest Editor. I sent your manuscript out for review again and three experts have commented on it. I am no longer a reviewer, but I would like you to look over the new reviewers' comments and answer them to your best ability. Your work is very interesting and if you take the comments on board, this will improve your manuscript. It is especially important to consider the points about the analysis and including previous literature. I look forward to seeing another version of the manuscript.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

Reviewer #3: (No Response)

Reviewer #4: (No Response)

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #2: No

Reviewer #3: Yes

Reviewer #4: Partly

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #2: No

Reviewer #3: Yes

Reviewer #4: I Don't Know

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

Reviewer #4: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

Reviewer #4: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)​

Reviewer #2: This is a very interesting and potentially important study. However, there are too many open issues concerning both the study methods and analyses that make it impossible to clearly evaluate the actual value of the data. I list below the things that require further explanation and re-analysis.

Page 3: The idea of allostasis was already implemented in early models of homeostasis. Now, the term allostasis is typically used to refer to deviations that have some sort of clinical implication or are disease relevant. Apart from this issue, I don’t quite see the usefulness of introducing the concept of allostasis as it is not needed for the paper. Indeed all that’s needed is the idea of a feedback loop.

Page 4: In the last paragraph, the critical distinction is that handholding enables reciprocal touch whereas stroking does not. So the classical distinction between reciprocal and non-reciprocal touch really is central here and should be introduced accordingly.

Page 8: There is no value in doing post-hoc power analyses. This should be deleted.

Page 9: I found no touch videos following the link. I only found a zip archive with photos and it wasn’t clear to me how they were being presented. Their names contained numbers and letters that were little intuitive. As this is the key manipulation, what exactly was shown needs to be better explained and illustrated in the main manuscript. Just from looking at Figure 1 it would seem that there was a lot more skin contact for the hand holding condition than for the stroking condition and that would constitute a confound. I believe this was addressed in Study 2 but this isn’t explained or visually supported in any way.

More information about the situations is needed. Please plot means and CIs for ratings for each of the selected situations. Report the rating scale endpoints. As suggested below, it would be useful to do this for each study separately using the study participant data rather than the data obtained in the pre-experimental rating.

Page 11: Please report the software/tool you used to do your power analysis. This is not trivial in the context of mixed modeling.

Page 13: Its a bit disconcerting that intensity flip flops between different samples. This suggests that it is not a stable measure and likely not useful to test your hypotheses. In your results you report analyses using the participant’s own ratings as a variable. Given these issues that would be the more reasonable way to go. So I would do away with the pre-experimental ratings and trying to categorize situations into intense and not intense. Your stats is a bit dense as it is. No need to add to this by including analyses that aren’t informative… Note that this also raises concerns about how reliable/consistent your valence and physicality ratings were. Why would we trust those? Again, I would suggest dropping those. As a reader I am not convinced…

The reader is variously referred to S1 appendix and supplementary materials. Are they the same? I could only find S1 appendix with this submission. But some of the information (e.g., situation intensity ratings for the different studies) was not provided.

Page 15: Here we learn for the first time that there are two different stroking conditions. This is a bit confusing. I also don’t understand how the videos were assigned to situations. What exactly were the “video sets”? Again this is the key manipulation and needs to be fully explained.

The description of the task is a bit misleading. What you say in the text and what is shown in Figure 1 are at odds. If I understand Figure 1 correctly, then participants did not select what type of touch condition they preferred. Instead, they rated for each touch condition, how it would make them feel in a given situation.

Page 16: A 5-point Likert scale is not a continuous measure. It is an ordinal measure and should be treated as such. I recommend the ordinal package in R.

Please report the software and packages you used to conduct your statistical analysis.

How did you convert what you call “preference rating” (which is a misnomer) into numbers? This is necessary to understand the data in Table 1. Later sections of that table are non-legible due to line breaks. There are variables in there that haven’t been introduced (e.g., UCLA). The table really isn’t helpful. I suggest presenting key information in figures, and place a cleaned up version of the table in your supplementary materials. Current figure 2 just shows the models not the data. Please find a proper way to plot your data. I suggest rain-cloud plots for their transparency (https://www.r-bloggers.com/2021/07/ggdist-make-a-raincloud-plot-to-visualize-distribution-in-ggplot2/).

So I assume you had 8 values for each cell per participant on average? If you don’t model random slopes, you are increasing your type 1 error. If you enter more than one value per cell per participant you should add random slopes or include situation as a factor. Alternatively, if these models don’t converge, as you are not interested in situation you should use a mean across situations. Also please report missing cells as a function of subjects. I would assume there are some. One would hope there are not too many including cases that rate each situation equally intense…

Earlier in the manuscript you state that you tested both positive and negative situations and different types of what you call physicality. What happened with these manipulations in your analyses? This goes back to the issue that its not clear what exactly was presented in terms of combinations of the situations and touch types.

Please report how you determined effect sizes from your mixed models. This is a bit tricky for LMEs.

How did you follow-up interactions? Did you do this within the full model or did you run simpler separate models? Did you look at the intensity effect for each level of touch? Please spell things out for your reader.

Page 25: Now here you really do look at preference. Please be clear throughout your manuscript what your items actually measure.

I’m unclear what you did with the logistic regression. It would seem that you treated data points within participants as independent. That would be inappropriate. How can you have a significant touch effect when touch preference (handholding vs stroking) was your DV? Or what was your DV? More information is needed.

For study 4, did you specifically ask about hand holding and hand stroking? What if there was other stroking touch that didn’t target the hand? That would seem to be a problem with interpreting the frequency of handholding vs stroking.

The results section ends with reports on valence and physicality effects. As mentioned earlier I’m unclear how these variables intersect with what else was presented in each study. No information is provided about how they were statistically analyzed. Hence, results cannot be evaluated.

I am not reading the discussion as I don’t see much point. There simply are too many question marks concerning the paradigm and analyses. In my opinion this needs another major revision in which the methods and result sections are improved. The results should include only appropriate analyses that are methodologically justified and properly explained.

Reviewer #3: In this four-experiments study, authors aimed to show that handholding was a stronger social touch than stroking to regulate intense situations. This is an interesting and very relevant study – however I have several comments that could be taken into account in order to improve the manuscript.

Overall, it is not clear why the authors chose handholding and stroking over other types of social touch. Especially in the abstract stating that “social touch highlights two forms of touch: handholding and stroking”, is quite limiting (and not true – see for ex Hertenstein et al. 2009; McIntyre et al. 2022). Authors should rephrase and discuss limitations accordingly. And in the introduction, page 4 please back up your discourse by references when stating that there are two main forms of consoling touch.

Moreover, there seems to be a general bias towards the authors hypothesis of preference for handholding in general: indeed, by looking at the gif presented to the participants, the naturalness of the handholding is much stronger than for the stroking. Authors could try to control for this (asking new participants to rate naturalness) and/or add it as a strong limitation. On the same line, the stimuli picturing the stroking conditions are not very well filmed (camera moving) and it is rare to perform this type of touch on the move / in the air. Moreover, there are quite a lot of different types of stroking so authors should acknowledge this.

The study by Burgoon 1991 is interesting however indeed they did not test arm stroking, so authors should rephrase their conclusion/reformulate as arm touching is quite different to arm stroking (stroking hasn’t been shown to communicate dominance but more comfort). Also, slow vs fast stroking does not communicate the same emotions (see for ex Kirsch et al. 2018).

I would advise to refrain from talking about C-touch for slow caress like stroking, just use the slow gentle stroking or caress like stroking terminology.

In the introduction – before the overview – it would be nice to justify the reason why you are investigating “touch provision vs reception, situation valence, emotional vs physical and cultural differences”. Moreover, actually you are not testing cultural differences (or results missing).

Authors should justify from the introduction the reason why they are conducting the studies only in people being in a heterosexual romantic relationship. And then discuss the implied limitations.

In study 4 – authors state that ‘in cases where they were not provided a particular type of touch..’: how often does this happened? As it is very different to rate received vs hypothetical touch.

For the touch preference rating: this section is very unclear. Please clarify what the participants had to do: rate each video or choose the one they preferred and rate only this one? Probably the terminology used is confusing: as it is not a preference rating but how it would make people feel. Please reformulate.

For data analyses: why coding 0,5 vs -0,5 the type of touch, and not use a categorical variable? Moreover, could you have run simple ANOVA on your data? Please better justify you analyses.

It is unclear what is the difference between table p18 and table p20? Why including UCLA ratings there is you are not analyzing it? Moreover, it is unclear in these tables how you take into account valence (positive vs. negative scenario).

Table 2 is unclear: if it represents the results of the mixed linear model could you please add the b, CI and R2? Also as in the text you are mentioning slopes p24 but they are not reported.

Please add in the Statistical Analysis section all the other analyses you have run: how you analyzed the subjective preference, recalled perception etc.. As for example the chi-square analyses are unclear.

Figures in general should be improved (titles, axes, legends). For example, in figure 2 what is ‘improvement’? The same scales should be used in all the graphs to be comparable; and in the legend please indicate what are the scales - for example why is intensity represented from -2 to 2, when the participants answer on a 4-point Likert scale?

Discussion:

It is unclear how results support this statement “It is much more likely to be performed in real life situations involving intense emotions”. Please justify and clarify.

Please add all limitations mentioned in points above.

Reviewer #4: It was a pleasure to able to review this manuscript titled: I wanna hold your hand: handholding is preferred over gentle stroking for acute emotion regulation. I have read the manuscript with great interest as it (supposedly) addresses an open question in the field of social touch: what types of touch are more suited to provide emotional support. I found the study interesting in principle and well powered. I really appreciate the large sample size and the fact that all the studies were preregistered. However, I found the process of reading and understanding the paper very challenging. I provide more specific comments below, but generally my main concerns are regarding the introduction and the discussion that should be completely revised, in my opinion. Too many generalizations are made, working hypotheses are cited as evidence, and scientific rigor is lacking. Furthermore, the present studies are not grounded in the literature, the results are very difficult to follow, the discussion does not do what a discussion is meant to do, which is to discuss the results in the context of what was already known in the field. Overall, I have noticed a lack of clarity and precision throughout the manuscript, and the conclusions are not fully supported by the results. Therefore, I am not in the position to recommend acceptance of the manuscript in the present form. I am providing some suggestions that could potentially help to improve the manuscript in turn below:

• Abstract: It is not clear from the abstract that the studies were conducted online. This should absolutely be included in the abstract, otherwise it can be misleading.

• Page 5: On the second page of the introduction, it remains unclear whether “consoling touch” should be classified as a touch that targets homeostatic or allostatic regulation. I think it is important to clarify this, or to say that consoling touch can address both needs…?

• Page 5: When talking about the positive effects of handholding and touch more generally, I think it is important to stress the fact that such effects are modulated by individual differences in patterns of relating (e.g., attachment). Furthermore, there is a large body of studies that are very relevant and have just been neglected here. Please see below:

Krahé, C., Paloyelis, Y., Sambo, C. F., & Fotopoulou, A. (2014). I like it when my partner holds my hand: Development of the responses and attitudes to support during pain questionnaire (RASP). Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1027.

von Mohr, M., Kirsch, L. P., & Fotopoulou, A. (2017). The soothing function of touch: affective touch reduces feelings of social exclusion. Scientific reports, 7(1), 1-9.

von Mohr, M., Krahé, C., Beck, B., & Fotopoulou, A. (2018). The social buffering of pain by affective touch: a laser-evoked potential study in romantic couples. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 13(11), 1121-1130.

Krahé, C., von Mohr, M., Gentsch, A., Guy, L., Vari, C., Nolte, T., & Fotopoulou, A. (2018). Sensitivity to CT-optimal, affective touch depends on adult attachment style. Scientific reports, 8(1), 1-10.

Krahé, C., Paloyelis, Y., Condon, H., Jenkinson, P. M., Williams, S. C., & Fotopoulou, A. (2015). Attachment style moderates partner presence effects on pain: a laser-evoked potentials study. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 10(8), 1030-1037.

Krahé, C., & Fotopoulou, A. (2018). Psychological and neurobiological processes in coping with pain: The role of social interactions. In The Routledge International Handbook of Psychobiology (pp. 73-92). Routledge.

Burleson, M. H., & Quigley, K. S. (2021). Social interoception and social allostasis through touch: legacy of the somatovisceral afference model of emotion. Social neuroscience, 16(1), 92-102.

• Page 6: “ …stroking is uniquely associated with elevated oxytocin levels (13)”. I invite the Authors to be more cautious about this. I would advise to edit this sentence as to clarify that this is a working hypothesis. (e.g., ….it has been suggested that …).

• Page 6: When saying that handolding and C-touch types of touch are effective forms of emotion regulation, I would like to think this is true and it is something that many labs have been trying to prove, but the authors must be more precise and should avoid mentioning this as a fact. Also, which types of emotions are they referring to? Citing some papers to support this statement would be helpful.

Updated after continuing reading: But then, when I continue reading, I find the paragraph saying that “existing studies in the field have focused on testing the effectiveness of touch as a method of emotion regulation, with most studies …”. So, the Authors now question something that was given as certain a few lines above? Perhaps this section should come first, or the Authors might want to be a bit more clear here.

• Page 6: “…as opposed to stroking, handholding allows both participants to sense each other equally and simultaneously using their hands, which are more effective as tactile sensory organs than the forearms”. I find sentences like this one quite problematic for the following reasons:

1. The sensory organ in touch is always the skin. The hands and the forearms are not sensory organs, they are body sites.

2. Stroking can also refer to palm against palms, and as such, the proposed difference between handling and stroking would not apply anymore.

3. Stroking can also be delivered from forearm to forearm, and as such both individuals would be equally and simultaneously touched

4. Touch is always a two-way sensory communication, so I find this statement very weak as a basis for the hypothesis of this study.

5. To me, one of the main differences between stroking and handholding (that is not mentioned at all) has more to do with the fact that handholding is static, while stroking is a dynamic type of touch.

• Page 6: “….studies compared general pleasantness of touch between touching arm and hand skin found little difference (18).” The Authors are citing the meta-analysis of Cruciani et al in an incorrect way, for what I understood. Cruciani and colleagues discussed differences between hairy and non-hairy skin. Here the Authors discuss differences between arm and hand. The hand has both hairy (dorsal part of the hand) and non-hairy skin (palms), so I think the Authors should double check and be consistent with the use of terms throughout the manuscript, otherwise it is all very confusing.

• Generally, in the introduction, the Authors do not mention various, very important pieces of knowledge in the field of social touch that can be very relevant to build the framework for this paper:

1. CTs can be found also on the non-hairy skin of the body (e.g., palms); this body part is involved in handholding: Watkins, R. H., Dione, M., Ackerley, R., Backlund Wasling, H., Wessberg, J., & Löken, L. S. (2021). Evidence for sparse C-tactile afferent innervation of glabrous human hand skin. Journal of Neurophysiology, 125(1), 232-237.

2. CTs have been associated also to deep pressure touch – which is supposedly is involved in handholding: Case, L. K., Liljencrantz, J., McCall, M. V., Bradson, M., Necaise, A., Tubbs, J., ... & Bushnell, M. C. (2021). Pleasant deep pressure: expanding the social touch hypothesis. Neuroscience, 464, 3-11.

3. There are studies already showing individual preferences for the types of touch (see for example, Perini, I., Olausson, H., & Morrison, I. (2015). Seeking pleasant touch: neural correlates of behavioral preferences for skin stroking. Frontiers in behavioral neuroscience, 9, 8.)

• Page 6: When citing the work of Reddan and colleagues, I think it is very important and informative to also cite the (several) publications from Charlotte Krahé and Aikaterini Fotopoulou that have investigated the role of touch in modulating pain (I have mentioned them in one of my points above).

• Page 7: “While research on the effectiveness of these types of touch has achieved mixed results…” What are these mixed results the Authors are referring too? I do not thing that have been discussed properly. What are the controversies/open questions/outstanding issues in this field? This is unclear and should be discussed here.

• Page 8: I think this paper would be very relevant here, and I am very surprised it has not been mentioned at all:

von Mohr, M., Kirsch, L. P., & Fotopoulou, A. (2017). The soothing function of touch: affective touch reduces feelings of social exclusion. Scientific reports, 7(1), 1-9.

• Page 8: “We did not have any hypothesis concerning valence and physicality”. It is very hard to understand what the Authors mean with valence. I suggest including some examples in parathesis to explain what you mean by “valence”. Furthermore, I am very surprised that no hypothesis was formulated about valence. Surely one would expect that a positive or a negative emotion would make a difference (if I understand what the Authors mean by valence, as it has not been spelled out)…

• Page 8: “…we hypothesized that the preference for handholding would be stronger in intense situations, involving strong, acute emotions”. Again, this is very unclear. What do you mean by “strong, acute emotions”?

• Page 9: Why the Authors did choose Arabic-speaking and Hebrew-speaking participants to investigate cultural differences? This implies some differences in emotion regulation or in the use of touch due to the spoken language, but this is what I imagine, as it has not been explained, contextualized, rationalized here.

• Page 10, Open data and preregistration: I must say that this is the first time I see the word “videos” so far and it came a bit of nowhere. I think that the fact that the experiments are done online, and videos are used should be introduced much earlier in the manuscript (e.g., introduction). Readers go through the hypotheses without knowing a very important detail about the used approach. There are several studies that used videos of touch, and this opens the door to a field of studies (sometime referred to as vicarious touch) that has not been mentioned at all in the introduction and I think it is of crucial importance here. Participants are not simply imagining of being touched (as I originally thought) but they are watching videos of people touching. I strongly encourage the Authors to consider adding some references from the vicarious touch relevant and introduce this topic in the introduction.

• Page 17, Statistical analysis: Which software/package was used for data analysis? This information should be added.

• Page 25-18, Results: I must say that I found extremely difficult to follow and understand the results. The results are presented in tables with no sufficient explanation, and I think that the main results should also be reported in the text. Furthermore, expressions such as “almost always” “in almost all studies” should be avoided in the results section. I would like to see more precision and clarity. I am wondering if reporting the results study by study rather than organized in topics would be more helpful.

• Page 30, Discussion: I think that it is quite confusing and beyond the point to answer the question “Why is handholding preferred over stroking?” by referring to Greek literature and the story of Adam and Eve. The Authors should stay closer to the data and what it is known in the scientific literature in humans. The discussion overall is too brief and does not help in understanding the results.

• Page 30-31, it is unclear to me why top-down factors play a more important role in handholding as compared to stroking. There is a huge increase in research showing the impact of intentionality, cognitive factors, and top-down factors in stroking studies, and these would go completely against this explanation. See for example:

Sailer, U., & Leknes, S. (2022). Meaning makes touch affective. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 44, 101099.

Sailer, U., Hausmann, M., & Croy, I. (2020). Pleasantness only? How sensory and affective attributes describe touch targeting C-tactile fibers. Experimental Psychology, 67(4), 224.

Ellingsen, D. M., Leknes, S., Løseth, G., Wessberg, J., & Olausson, H. (2016). The neurobiology shaping affective touch: expectation, motivation, and meaning in the multisensory context. Frontiers in psychology, 6, 1986.

McCabe, C., Rolls, E. T., Bilderbeck, A., & McGlone, F. (2008). Cognitive influences on the affective representation of touch and the sight of touch in the human brain. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 3(2), 97-108.

Furthermore, it is also unclear why the paper of Cascio et al that is cited here would support this view.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #2: No

Reviewer #3: No

Reviewer #4: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Decision Letter 2

Rochelle Ackerley

17 Feb 2023

PONE-D-22-07411R2I Wanna Hold Your Hand:

Handholding is Preferred over Gentle Stroking for Emotion RegulationPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Sened,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Apr 03 2023 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Rochelle Ackerley

Guest Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments:

Thank you for your revised manuscript and your patience in the review process. The expert reviewers do see the interest and significance of your work, where two suggest minor/no changes, whereas one suggests more in-depth revision. The review comments are important to take into account and I would appreciate it if the authors could address all the comments posed.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

Reviewer #3: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #4: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #2: No

Reviewer #3: Yes

Reviewer #4: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #2: No

Reviewer #3: Yes

Reviewer #4: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #2: No

Reviewer #3: Yes

Reviewer #4: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

Reviewer #4: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #2: Although the authors have made a significant effort to revise their manuscript, important issues have not been resolved. Moreover, I am concerned that their statements are too broad and unjustified by the data. They send the wrong message and may do more harm than good for the field. Below I explain my main theoretical/conceptual issues and then elaborate on some of the outstanding methodological/analytical problems.

I previously asked that the authors introduce the distinction between reciprocal and non-reciprocal touch in their introduction when discussing differences between handholding and stroking and the bidirectionality of the former, which is lacking for the latter. They wrote in their response that there is no research about this. However, this distinction has been around for decades. I suggest looking at the book by Hall and Knapp (https://www.amazon.com/Nonverbal-Communication-Human-Interaction-Knapp/dp/1133311598). Again, I consider this distinction central to the comparison they make and something that should be raised when introducing their research question. Ultimately, their handholding condition is active, whereas their stroking condition is passive and it is more than simply tactile processes that differ here. There are additional motor and proprioceptive processes that contribute to hand-holding. There is also the issue that simply holding onto something (be it another person or another object) could help regulate discomfort or pain.

I was now able to view the gif files. One thing I noted is that stroking seemed significantly faster than 3 cm/s. Perhaps time changed in the process of creating the gifs? Can the authors measure the speed in the images and document that?

In general, the stroking looks a bit mechanical rather than affectionate. Moreover, I don’t think the position of the people stroking is typical for the manner in which people stroke each other. When would you stroke someone’s forearm standing side by side? It is uncomfortable. This position is much more natural for handholding. For several of the clips the action is not in center and moves out of the frame. There are also significant shadows of the body parts on the walls that could affect perception.

I wonder whether body part matters. Could a rub on the back be more comfortable/preferred than rubbing of the forearm? In any case, I think that a generalization to all kinds of stroking is not possible here. The authors don’t compare hand-holding with stroking, they compare hand-holding with forearm stroking (from an unnatural position with a certain speed).

I had asked previously that more information about the situations in Study 1 and 2 be provided. I have not found this in the revision. I’m wondering what kind of emotions were examined (e.g., sadness, anger, fear)? Could stroking be more effective for low-arousal states such as dejection and handholding for high-arousal states such as fear? Indeed, I’m skeptical that the data allow the authors to conclude that emotion regulation in general benefits more from handholding than stroking. Emotion regulation is a very broad class of processes that includes the enhancement, maintenance and reduction of any kind of emotion.

Relatedly, it seems from the discussion that some of the situations were positive? Did participants opt for handholding to increase/maintain positive and/or reduce negative emotions? Much of the paper is written in a way that suggests touch is used to down-regulate negative feelings but it seems the data may not clearly speak to that and may be more nuanced.

So with all that’s said above, it is not appropriate to say that handholding is more effective than stroking for emotion regulation. Instead, the authors should carefully reflect on their methods/results and offer a more nuanced discussion and summary of findings in their title, abstract and main body of the paper.

Other concerns:

Page 14: „Continuous Situation Intensity and Valence“ I had mentioned in my previous review that these are not continuous measures.

I’m unclear about what the authors mean by intensity? How is it different from being very positive or negative? How were participants instructed to do the rating?

The logistic regression should include random intercepts and slopes. I’m also confused about the choice of test here. It would seem a chi-square test would be more appropriate. This is indeed what is used in Study 4.

I had asked previously that the rating analyses be revised in line with the statistical properties of the data. I don’t understand why the revised analyses are in the supplementary materials rather than the main body of the manuscript? Also, the analyses there are inappropriate. To analyze the ratings, the authors should run an ordinal model with all random slopes and intercepts, rather than an ordinal model with only random intercepts and a linear model with random intercepts and slopes, which is what they have now placed in the supplementary materials. Tables S10 and 11 show no degrees of freedom. They are also missing in the main text.

Follow-up tests of the interaction between intensity and touch were not reported. The authors say what package they used, but do not explain what they actually did. Did they use the simple_contrast function on their LME model? For the results, the reader is sent to the Appendix.

The figures have not improved. The authors count data points and participants without discriminating between the two. So they don’t present subject means and do not allow readers to get a sense of the variance in the data. There are a couple of rain cloud plots, but these are not fully presented. It seems the distributions were very broad and are cut off at the margin of the figure.

Page 30: „physical or emotional – on a 1-100 scale, with no verbal indicators for

endpoint“ How can the authors be certain that all participants used this scale as intended by them? I could consider ratings as reflecting the amount of pain that is being reduced or the amount that pain is being reduced to.

Page 32: „was strongly preferred behaviorally by people assisting women giving birth“ This was not examined. You simply examined the kind of touch women giving birth recall having received from their partner.

I looked at the OSF archives. I could only find some MTURK pilot data that was the same for Study 1 and 2. Otherwise I found no data.

Reviewer #3: Authors have addressed all my comments.

However, I have some comments on the revised version:

- Please make sure you proofread the paper as some grammatical errors are present (e.g. "was significantly differed")

- Some comments seem to not be included in the main text; while mentioned in the response to reviewers, please justify / change it. For example for experiment 1, authors states that they elaborated the chi-square results "beyond asking participants to rate ..." but I cannot find this paragraph in the revised version of the manuscript.

- On that note, the second part of this added paragraph is not totally clear to me. "Touch type chosen and intensity were not statistically independent (χ2(2)= 43.317, p < .001), meaning that handholding was preferred in intense situations." Could you please reformulate/correct ?

- Figure legends: could you expand the legends, especially for Fig1 and 2, explaining what are the images picturing? For fig 2: are these the 4 types of touch participants rate in all studies ? Add this info in the figure legend. For fig 1, is the first image picturing stroking or no touch ?

For figure 6 to 9 what does the legend "touch rating" represent ? please explain in the legend and on the figure as "touch rating" is not specific enough. Please be clearer on what is presented, as for now figures are difficult to understand.

- Study 2: why not include the chi-square analyses results in the descriptive part ?

- Page 31: touch reception ratings in study 4: which figure are you referring to? Probably figure 9. On that note, why representing the results differently here from the other studies ?

- Overall, I would advise to go again over the results section of each study to improve clarity.

Reviewer #4: The Authors have addressed my concerns to a satisfactory extent, and I believe that the manuscript is now suitable for publication.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #2: No

Reviewer #3: No

Reviewer #4: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Decision Letter 3

Rochelle Ackerley

27 Mar 2023

I Wanna Hold Your Hand:

Handholding is Preferred over Gentle Stroking for Emotion Regulation

PONE-D-22-07411R3

Dear Dr. Sened,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Rochelle Ackerley

Guest Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Thank you again for your revision and for your patience. I understand that this has been a long time in revision, but I really do appreciate how well you have responded to the reviewers and dealt with this process. I am very pleased to accept this very interesting paper and congratulate you on this in-depth work.

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Rochelle Ackerley

29 Mar 2023

PONE-D-22-07411R3

I Wanna Hold Your Hand: Handholding is Preferred over Gentle Stroking for Emotion Regulation

Dear Dr. Sened:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Rochelle Ackerley

Guest Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Appendix. Additional analyses.

    (DOCX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Consoling touch reviewer response final.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Reviewer Comment Revision 2 Final.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Reviewer response final.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All four studies were preregistered on the Open Science Framework: Study 1: https://osf.io/x95f4/?view_only=e272eb9ccc2a46668114685dcb8988e5 Study 2: https://osf.io/nzg6v/?view_only=b9cf1e802d894ce387c80c47ffd7bc97 Study 3: https://osf.io/8n3pu/?view_only=f23a2e3d485946ffb95d32c02e469b85 Study 4: https://osf.io/74szg/?view_only=4b12d6d2a8a04483abbd6ea0c8c225ca. Study data, analysis code and touch videos for Studies 1-3 are available at the link below, except for touch videos for Study 4 which were not posted as participants who filmed the videos did not consent to wide distribution. https://osf.io/nju48/?view_only=81dadc972f6545f19d107c83d8dba368.


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES