Abstract
Leisure engagement is believed to offer emotional benefits. However, relatively few studies have examined how leisure engagement influences emotional well-being in the context of individuals’ everyday lives. The current study examined the associations between leisure engagement and daily emotional well-being, as measured by participants’ mean levels of positive affect (PA), PA variability, and PA reactivity to stressors. Using smartphones, participants (n = 176) completed ecological momentary assessments (EMAs) 5 times per day for 14 consecutive days. The Pearson correlations indicated that the participants who engaged in leisure more frequently experienced higher mean levels of PA and lower PA variability in daily life compared to those with less frequent leisure engagement. However, multilevel models indicated that individuals’ differences in frequency of leisure engagement did not influence their emotional reactivity in response to daily stressors. The findings extend the importance of leisure engagement to consider the dynamics of daily emotional well-being.
Keywords: leisure benefits, positive affect, affect variability, emotional reactivity, ecological momentary assessment
Introduction
Leisure engagement, which is defined as engagement in enjoyable activities during the time free from obligations or responsibilities (Pressman et al., 2009), has demonstrable benefits for emotional well-being (Newman, Tay, & Diener, 2014). One way that leisure engagement is beneficial is that it promotes positive mental states that foster the accumulation of psychological resources (e.g., building social support, increasing optimism; Mitas, Qian, Yarnal, & Kerstetter, 2011). Drawing on the broaden-and-build theoretical framework (Fredrickson, 2001), Denovan and Macaskill (2017) suggested that leisure engagement can help individuals develop psychological resources, which in turn enhance positive affect (PA) and counteract the experience of stress. Indeed, individuals who maintain high levels of and stable (i.e., low variability) PA exhibit better mental and physical health than individuals with lower levels of and more variable PA (Gruber, Kogan, Quoidbach, & Mauss, 2013). Another way that leisure engagement leads to emotional well-being is by buffering individuals from the effects of stress (Coleman & Iso-Ahola, 1993; Iwasaki & Mannell, 2000). In particular, individuals’ emotional responses to everyday stress are predictive of long-term mental and physical health (Piazza, Charles, Sliwinski, Mogle, & Almeida, 2013), and leisure engagement is related to more positive emotional profiles (Qian, Yarnal, & Almeida, 2013). However, relatively few studies have examined how leisure engagement relates to individuals’ emotional experiences and responses to stress in the context of their everyday lives. To this end, this study examines how leisure engagement affects individuals’ experiences of PA in their everyday lives and how it may moderate the effects of daily stress on the individuals’ PA levels.
Leisure Engagement and the Development of Psychological Resources
Psychological resources are “characteristics that people draw upon to help them withstand threats posed by events and objects in their environment” (Pearlin & Schooler, 1978, p. 5). An important way that psychological resources are accumulated is through the experiencing of positive emotions (Tugade, Fredrickson, & Barrett, 2004). The broaden-and-build theory suggests that experiencing positive emotions can “broaden people’s momentary thought-action repertoires and build their enduring personal resources” (Fredrickson, 2001, p. 3). During the broadening process, positive emotions expand individuals’ attention and actions (Fredrickson, 2001). These broadened scopes of attention and actions may help individuals cope effectively with stressors because they enable the individuals to step back from their current situations and reappraise the stressors from various angles (Fredrickson & Joiner, 2002). During the building process, on the other hand, positive emotions help individuals develop a variety of personal resources, such as social resources (e.g., friendships; Aron, Norman, Aron, McKenna, & Heyman, 2000), psychological resources (e.g., resilience; Fredrickson, Tugade, Waugh, & Larkin, 2003), and physical resources (e.g., physical skills; Danner, Snowdon, & Friesen, 2001). These personal resources, and especially psychological resources, enhance individuals’ coping abilities and contribute to emotional well-being (Fredrickson, 2001).
Leisure engagement often increases positive emotions and triggers the accumulation of psychological resources, which in turn positively contributes to emotional well-being by the broaden-and-build process (see Figure 1; Carruthers & Hood, 2007; Chang, Yarnal, & Chick, 2016). For example, Mitas et al. (2011) found that participants broadened their thoughts and actions (e.g., meeting new people, trying new activities) and built resources (e.g., social support, optimism) while experiencing leisure, which in turn helped them cope with stressors such as partner loss and caregiving demands. More recently, Denovan and Macaskill (2017) suggested that leisure coping was associated with PA, counteracting perceived stress and promoting well-being. This, too, is consistent with the broaden-and-build theory. Together, leisure engagement can help individuals develop psychological resources through the broaden-and-build process, which may lead to higher levels of emotional well-being and buffer against stress over time.
PA is an important type of emotional well-being that can be strengthened by leisure engagement, according to the broaden-and-build theory (Hood & Carruthers, 2002). PA refers to the subjective experience of positive feelings such as joy, interest, or contentment (Fredrickson, 2001). Although individuals’ global reports of their PA (e.g., how they feel in general or how they have felt over the previous month) tend to be positively correlated with frequency of engagement in leisure (Hicks & Siedlecki, 2017; Pressman et al., 2009), little is known about how leisure engagement relates to how people experience PA in everyday life. Given that PA is dynamic and varies over time, information derived from multiple time point assessments in everyday life can be informative (Zautra, Affleck, Tennen, Reich, & Davis, 2005). In addition, few researchers have focused on how leisure engagement influences PA variability, which is a construct that captures the degree to which an individual’s level of PA varies over time (Jenkins, Hunter, Cross, Acevedo, & Pressman, 2018; Ram & Gerstorf, 2009). Studies have suggested that greater PA variability is associated with lower life satisfaction and more depressive symptoms (Gruber et al., 2013), more psychological distress (Hardy & Segerstrom, 2016), and a stronger immune response to influenza vaccination (Jenkins et al., 2018). Understanding how leisure engagement impacts daily PA and PA variability may help to explain the association between leisure and emotional well-being in the context of individuals’ everyday lives.
Leisure Engagement and Buffers from Emotional Responses to Daily Stressors
Daily stressors refer to “routine challenges, such as the everyday concerns of work, caring for other people, and commuting between work and home” (Almeida, 2005, p. 62). Although daily stressors are less intense than stressful major life events, they are linked with adverse same-day health-related outcomes, which can accumulate to negatively impact long-term health (Almeida, Piazza, Stawski, & Klein, 2011). A growing body of research has suggested that on days when individuals encounter stressors, they report lower levels of PA, higher levels of negative affect (Almeida, 2005; Mroczek et al., 2015), greater frequency of memory problems (Rickenbach, Condeelis, & Haley, 2015), and poorer cardiovascular function (Sin, Sloan, McKinley, & Almeida, 2016) than they do on stressor-free days.
Although daily stressors matter, emotional reactivity to stressors—that is, how individuals emotionally respond to these negative experiences—seems to be especially crucial to long-term health-related outcomes (Sin, Graham-Engeland, Ong, & Almeida, 2015). Piazza et al. (2013) suggested that a higher magnitude of acute emotional reactivity in response to daily stressors can lead to worse long-term physical health. Similarly, Charles, Piazza, Mogle, Sliwinski, and Almeida (2013) found that individuals whose inferred emotional states were more affected by the experience of daily stressors had an increased risk of affective distress and affective disorders 10 years later than those who were less affected by the experience of daily stressors. Given that a lack of emotional stability in the face of stressors may lead to poor health outcomes, understanding how health behaviors such as leisure engagement as a way for individuals to buffer themselves from emotional reactivity to daily stressors may provide useful information on long-term physical and mental health outcomes.
Relatively little work has examined the relationship between leisure engagement and emotional reactivity to daily stressors. In one of the first studies on this topic, Qian et al. (2013) showed that increases in the amount of free time available daily for enjoyable activities helped individuals cope with daily stressors. Their findings provide evidence for theoretical models that suggest leisure can be a stress buffer for individuals who have experienced a life crisis (e.g., Coleman & Iso-Ahola, 1993; Iwasaki & Mannell, 2000); that is, participating in enjoyable activities (e.g., taking a vacation, participating in sports with friends, listening to relaxing music) during free time may reduce the negative effects of stress on the individuals’ well-being (Coleman & Iso-Ahola, 1993; Hutchinson, Bland, & Kleiber, 2008; Iwasaki & Mannell, 2000). Tugade et al. (2004) similarly suggested that developing psychological resources through healthy behaviors, such as habitual engagement in leisure, can be seen as a proactive or protective approach that may reduce emotional reactivity in response to stressors. Indeed, individuals who more frequently engage in leisure tend to have more effective self-regulation to facilitate stress coping and maintain PA compared to those who less frequently engage in leisure (Patry, Blanchard, & Mask, 2007). However, Qian et al. (2014) further suggested that adults’ (n = 2,022) differences in the average amount of free time they have daily for enjoyable activities did not moderate the effects of stressors on PA across eight days’ worth of diary data. Although Qian and colleagues’ study (2014) showed little agreement regarding the effects of leisure engagement on emotional reactivity to daily stressors, the authors called for more studies examining the effects of leisure on daily emotional well-being, such as emotional reactivity to daily stressors. Our study is a response to this call, extending the results of Qian et al. (2014) by examining PA reactivity to stressors over a more finely grained time scale (e.g., a single day, multiple hours).
Study Purpose and Research Hypotheses
The purpose of the current study was to examine the associations between leisure engagement and daily emotional well-being, as measured by mean levels of PA, PA variability, and PA reactivity, in response to daily stressors over 14 days of EMAs in a diverse probability sample of adults. In recent years, EMAs have been become a popular form of data collection in the field of leisure and recreation because they can reduce recall biases and capture how leisure behavior interacts with emotional well-being in individuals’ everyday lives (Scott, Wozencroft, & Waller, 2019). Given that PA is a time-varying construct, in this study, EMAs were used to distinguish variance that differentiated among individuals with higher or lower frequencies of leisure engagement from the variance that differentiated among multiple measurement moments nested within individuals.
In sum, a total of three research hypotheses were examined in the study:
H1: Individuals who report participating more frequently in leisure activities experience higher mean levels of PA in their daily lives compared to individuals who report participating less frequently in leisure activities.
H2: Individuals who report participating more frequently in leisure activities experience lower levels of PA variability in their daily lives compared to individuals who report participating less frequently in leisure activities.
H3: Individuals who report participating more frequently in leisure activities experience smaller decreases in PA in response to daily stressors compared to individuals who report participating less frequently in leisure activities.
Method
We used data from the Effects of Stress on Cognitive Aging, Physiology and Emotion (ESCAPE) project, a longitudinal study that collected intensive repeated measures of its titular variables at multiple time scales. Because ESCAPE did not measure variables of leisure engagement during the first wave, we restricted our use of the study’s data to the second wave. Most participants completing the second wave participated in 2014. Further information on the study protocol can be found in Scott et al. (2015).
Participants
A sampling frame was developed based on New York City Registered Voter Lists (RVL) obtained from the Board of Elections. A racially and economically diverse sample of participants were recruited via systematic probability sampling of New York City’s registered voter lists for the zip code 10475 (Bronx, NY). The sample included 176 adults (67% women) aged 25–66 years (Mean = 47.83, SD = 11.21). The participants were representative of the sampling area (race-ethnicity: Non-Hispanic White: 9.04%, Non-Hispanic Black: 65.54%, Hispanic White: 17.51%, Hispanic Black: 3.95%, Asian: 0.56%, Other: 3.39%). As the race-ethnicity distribution suggests, a relatively high percentage of the participants were Non-Hispanic Black, offering a unique “minority-majority” perspective in the study. The sample was diverse in terms of participants’ education levels (Less than high school: 6.25%, Completed high school or received GED: 18.18%, Some college: 28.98%, College degree: 28.98%, Graduate or professional degree: 17.61%) and annual incomes (< $4,999: 8.02%; $5,000-$19,999: 19.75%; $20,000-$39,999: 24.69%; $40,000- $59,999: 15.43%; $60,000- $79,999: 17.90%; $80,000- $99,999: 4.32%; $100,000- $149,000: 9.26%; > $150,000: 0.62%).
Measures
Data about leisure engagement and demographics (e.g., age, gender, education, and work status) from the baseline questionnaire and data about PA and stressors from the momentary assessment were used to examine the research hypotheses.
Leisure engagement.
The Pittsburgh Enjoyable Activities Test (PEAT; Pressman et al., 2009) was used to measure participants’ baseline frequency of enjoyable activities. Participants were given a list of enjoyable activities (10 items) and asked to rate how often they had spent time doing each of the activities in the previous month: (1) spending quiet time alone; (2) spending time unwinding at the end of the day; (3) visiting friends and/or relatives; (4) going out for meals with friends and/or relatives; (5) doing fun things with others; (6) doing club, fellowship, and religious group participation; (7) going away on holiday/vacation; (8) being in outdoor settings such as gardens, parks, countryside; (9) actively engaging in physical activity (e.g., walking, sports, hiking); and (10) involving in hobbies. The response scale ranged from 1 (Never), 2 (Occasionally), 3 (Quite often), 4 (Frequently), to 5 (Everyday). The PEAT scores were calculated as the sum of responses (range = 10 to 50) and centered at the sample mean (24.93). The Cronbach α test indicating the reliability of the 10 items was 0.84, indicating reasonable reliability (Cortina, 1993). Pressman et al. (2009) reported that most items were loaded > 0.40 on the PEAT scale, suggesting that the PEAT scale encompasses many different types of enjoyable activities. Their study also demonstrated the adequate reliability and validity of the PEAT scale, which can be used for testing the associations between the PEAT and multiple psychological outcomes (e.g., PA, negative affect, perceived stressful life events).
Momentary PA.
PA was calculated as the average of each participant’s ratings of four PA items: happy, enjoyable, joyful, and pleased (Diener & Emmons, 1984). For each PA item, participants responded to the question, “How____ do you feel right now?” The PA items were assessed using a slider that ranged from 0 (Not at all) to 100 (Extremely), with higher scores indicating greater PA. PA variability was operationalized by computing each individual’s standard deviation (iSD) from their own mean level of PA (Ram & Gerstorf, 2009).
Momentary stressors.
Momentary stressors were assessed by asking participants to select 1 (Yes) or 0 (No) in response to the question, “Did anything stressful occur since the last survey? A stressful event is any event, even a minor one, which negatively affected you” (Almeida, Wethington, & Kessler, 2002).
Covariates.
Age, gender, education, and work status were assessed in the mailed baseline questionnaire and were included as covariates. The covariates were coded as follows. Age (centered at the sample mean) was verified using the participants’ dates of birth. Gender was coded as male = 1, female = 2. Education (centered at the sample mean) was coded as less than high school diploma = 0, high school diploma or some college = 1, and college degree or higher = 2. Work status was coded as employed = 1, retired = 2, unemployed and looking for work = 3, and unemployed and not looking for work = 4.
Data Collection Procedures
There were three phases of data collection in the second wave of the ESCAPE: baseline surveys, EMA practice, and smartphone surveys. We describe each phase of data collection below.
Baseline surveys.
Participants were mailed a baseline survey to complete at home. The questionnaire was intended to assess the participants’ demographics and their frequency of leisure engagement. Participants were required to visit a laboratory for specific training on how to use their smartphones as part of the forthcoming EMA protocol.
EMA practice.
During the EMA practice phase, participants returned the baseline survey; they also learned how to operate the specially programmed smartphones (Droid X) during their laboratory visit. Participants practiced taking the smartphone survey (described in the Measures section) with trained research assistants. The participants were then asked to complete a two-day EMA data collection during which they became familiar with the protocol. After these two days, participants returned to the laboratory and their compliance with study protocol was calculated. Those participants who completed 80% or more of the smartphone surveys during the EMA practice phase were invited to complete 14-day smartphone surveys.
Smartphones surveys phases.
Participants took short smartphone surveys 5 times per day for 14 consecutive days. The average amount of time between the smartphone surveys was approximately 2.5 hours. At particular times during the day, the smartphone produced an audible alert (“beep”) signaling participants to complete the smartphone surveys about their recent experiences and psychological states such as stressors and current affects. Participants completed each smartphone survey around 2 minutes and 59 seconds. To prevent participants from anticipating the beeps, the beeps were scheduled with quasi-random timing. Participants’ smartphone survey responses were stored on the smartphones and data were sent to a secure server upon completion of all survey. At the end of the 14 days, the participants returned the smartphones to the laboratory. Each participant who had completed the entire protocol received $160.
Data Analysis
Data analysis was conducted using SAS PROC MIXED (version 9.4) and IBM SPSS 25.0 (Chicago, IL). Incomplete data were treated as missing at random. Statistical significance was evaluated at α = 0.05.
Between-person correlations.
We conducted Pearson correlation analyses to examine whether individuals who reported participating more frequently in leisure activities experienced higher mean levels of PA (H1) and lower levels of PA variability (H2) in their daily lives compared to individuals who reported participating less frequently in leisure activities. Using Pearson correlation analyses allowed us to confirm the relationship among leisure engagement, momentary PA, and momentary PA variability. For the analyses, we calculated each participant’s average momentary PA and PA variability across 14 days.
Multilevel models.
We used multilevel models (Snijders & Bosker, 1999) to examine whether individuals who reported participating more frequently in leisure activities experienced smaller decreases in PA in response to daily stressors compared to individuals who reported participating less frequently in leisure activities (H3). Using multilevel models enabled us to capture whether leisure engagement moderated the association between stressors and levels of PA at the momentary level. Each person’s average percentage of stressful events across 14 days (Stressors_pmni) was included in the model. To make the values more interpretable, the momentary-level stressor was centered by subtracting each individual’s average percentage of stressful events across 14 days (Stressors_pmni) from the individual’s stressor. Equations (1–3) offer a description of the multilevel models.
Level 1 (Within-Person) Model
(1) |
Equation (1) in the Level 1 Model specifies the within-person changes in PA as a function of changes in stressors in daily life. The PA for person i at time t is a function of an intercept parameter for person i (across the t times for which the person provided data), βoi; a slope parameter that captures momentary fluctuation in stressors for person i, β1i; and the within-individual random error, eit.
Level 2 (Between-Person) Model
(2) |
(3) |
Equations (2) and (3) in the Level 2 Model describe between-person differences (e.g., frequency of enjoyable activities) in within-person change. The between-person intercept and coefficient are expressed as a function of a fixed intercept, fixed slope, and a between-person error term for intercept u0i and slope u1i, respectively. γ00 indicates the intercept of PA for a person with average momentary stressors; γ01 refers to the effect of frequency of enjoyable activities on the initial level of PA for person i; γ02 refers to the effect of average momentary stressors on the initial level of PA for person i; γ03 to γ06 reflect the effects of control variables (age, gender, education, and work status, respectively) on the initial level of PA for person i; u0i indicates the error of βoi; γ10 indicates the slope of PA on momentary stressors; γ11 refers to the slope of PA on momentary stressors with an interactive effect of frequency of enjoyable activities; and u1i indicates the error of β1i.
Results
Descriptive Statistics
A total of 1,972 stressors were reported across the study. Each participant reported having at least one stressor on 43% of the 14 total days and in 17% of responses to smartphone surveys. Table 1 displays means, standard deviations, and correlations of the study variables. Momentary measures (i.e., PA and stressors) were averaged across all measurements to compute the between-person correlations. As shown in Table 1, participants reported moderate PA (M = 58.71 on a 0–100 scale) and moderate leisure engagement (M = 24.93 on a 10–50 scale). Age was correlated positively with leisure engagement (r = 0.21, p < .01) and average momentary PA (r = 0.24, p < .001), indicating that older adults tended to report more frequent leisure engagement and higher levels of PA than younger adults. Higher age was not associated with increased reporting of stressors (r = −0.01, p > .05). After adjusting for all covariates (age, gender, education, and work status), average momentary PA was correlated negatively with PA variability (r = −0.24, p < .001). The number of stressors experienced and leisure engagement showed no association (r = −0.05, p > .05), adjusting for all covariates.
Table 1.
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||||
1. Age | — | 0.01 | 0.12 | 0.09 | 0.21** | 0.24** | −0.19* | −0.01 |
2. Gender | — | — | 0.01 | −0.06 | −0.02 | −0.01 | 0.11 | −0.05 |
3. Education | — | — | — | −0.23** | 0.09 | −0.10 | −0.08 | 0.18* |
4. Work status | — | — | — | — | 0.06 | 0.14 | −0.07 | −0.20** |
5. Leisure engagement | — | — | — | — | — | 0.37*** | −0.27** | −0.05 |
6. PA b | — | — | — | — | 0.35*** | — | −0.24** | −0.21** |
7. PA variability b | — | — | — | — | −0.23** | −0.24** | — | 0.29*** |
8. Stressors b | — | — | — | — | −0.05 | −0.15 | 0.31*** | — |
| ||||||||
M | 47.83 | 1.67 | 1.40 | 2.02 | 24.93 | 58.71 | 15.29 | 0.17 |
SD | 11.21 | 0.47 | 0.61 | 1.10 | 7.05 | 18.68 | 7.14 | 0.18 |
Notes. Below the diagonal are correlation coefficients controlled for covariates (age, gender, education, and work status). Above the diagonal are correlation coefficients that did not control for covariates. M = mean; SD = standard deviation; PA = positive affect
Mean value reflects average taken for each individual
Mean value reflects average taken across days and beeps for each individual.
p < .05
p < .01
p < .001.
Research hypothesis 1: Individuals who participate more frequently in leisure activities experience higher mean levels of PA in their daily lives.
Leisure engagement was found to be moderately correlated with mean levels of PA (r = 0.35, p < .001; Cohen, 1988), adjusting for all covariates. Individuals who participated more frequently in leisure activities experienced higher mean levels of PA in everyday life. The results are displayed in Table 1 and Figure 2.
Research hypothesis 2: Individuals who participate more frequently in leisure activities experience lower levels of PA variability in their daily lives.
As shown in Table 1 and Figure 3, leisure engagement was negatively correlated with PA variability (r = −0.23, p < .01), adjusting for all covariates. That is, individuals who participated more frequently in leisure activities experienced lower levels of PA variability in everyday life.
Research hypothesis 3: Individuals who participate more frequently in leisure activities experience smaller decreases in PA in response to daily stressors in their daily lives.
Table 2 displays the results from our multilevel models examining leisure engagement and momentary stressors as predictors of momentary PA. Engagement in leisure activities did not moderate the associations between momentary stressors and momentary PA (p > .05), adjusting for all covariates. That is, the effect of experiencing a stressor on PA did not differ among individuals with varying levels of leisure engagement. Figure 4 shows that individuals with either more or less frequent leisure engagement did not have elevated PA when stressors occurred.
Table 2.
Momentary PA | ||
---|---|---|
| ||
Fixed Effects | ||
Intercept | 57.81*** | (4.01) |
Age | 0.30* | (0.12) |
Gender | −0.20 | (2.68) |
Education | −2.78 | (2.18) |
Work status | 0.46 | (1.21) |
Momentary stressors | −14.57*** | (1.16) |
Average momentary stressors | −14.94** | (6.55) |
Leisure engagement | 0.76*** | (0.22) |
Momentary stressors × Leisure engagement | 0.18 | (0.22) |
Random Effects | ||
Variance intercept | 301.51*** | (34.64) |
Variance stressors | 153.23*** | (22.90) |
Covariance intercept and stressors | −90.47*** | (22.69) |
Residual variance | 247.34*** | (3.37) |
| ||
−2LL | 93583.0 | |
AIC | 93591.0 |
Notes. N = 176. SE = standard errors; PA = positive affect; LL = log-likelihood; AIC = Akaike information criterion.
p < .01
p < .001.
Discussion
The current study investigated the association between leisure engagement and daily emotional well-being in 176 adults from diverse backgrounds. Our findings based on EMA data collected over a 14-day period suggest that more frequent leisure engagement was associated with higher mean levels of PA and lower PA variability in daily life compared to less frequent leisure engagement. However, individuals’ differences in leisure engagement did not influence their emotional reactivity in response to daily stressors. Our model, which was drawn on the broaden-and-build theoretical framework to illustrate the relationships between leisure engagement and daily emotional well-being, is shown in Figure 5.
Our findings contribute to leisure literature in the following ways. First, our findings provide empirical evidence that individuals who engage in leisure activities more frequently enjoy higher average levels of experienced PA in everyday life. This finding is consistent with those of other studies examining the association between leisure engagement and PA based on the broaden-and-build theory (Denovan & Macaskill, 2017). It is also in line with Newman and colleagues’ (2014) argument that positive feelings are “based on a weighting of key life domains such as leisure” (p. 558). The researchers indicated that engaging in leisure might lead to an aggregation of positive feelings by strengthening individuals’ multiple psychological resources, such as autonomy, mastery, and affiliation. Additionally, our findings support the Leisure and Well-being Model proposed by Carruthers and Hood (2007), which indicates that leisure engagement is linked to positive emotions that set the stage for the development of psychological resources that in turn promote everyday well-being. As such, individuals who participate more frequently in leisure activities may perceive themselves as having psychological resources, which subsequently leads to their perception of higher mean levels of PA compared to those who participate less frequently in leisure.
Second, our findings indicate that people who more frequently engage in leisure tend to have less variability in PA in their daily lives. This finding is consistent with those of other studies showing that more positive leisure experiences are associated with a more stable emotional life, which is characterized by emotional regulation (Freire & Teixeira, 2018). Indeed, emotional regulation can be enhanced by taking vacations, suggesting that travel experiences are opportunities to regulate emotions by sharing emotions and maintaining good relationships with significant others (Gao, Kerstetter, Mowen, & Hickerson, 2018). In addition, researchers have demonstrated that highly resilient individuals tend to have greater control over their emotional stability (Ong, Bergeman, Bisconti, & Wallace, 2006) and have proposed that resilience is a psychological resource for emotional regulation (Hildebrandt, McCall, Engen, & Singer, 2016). Leisure engagement may play a role in cultivating resilience by the broaden-and-build process (Chang et al., 2016), which in turn increases individuals’ emotion regulation abilities and promote PA stability.
Third, our findings indicate that leisure engagement did not minimize the effects of reactions to stressors on PA in daily life. These results support Qian and colleagues’ (2014) finding that the amount of leisure time does not influence individuals’ emotional responses to stressors in daily life. There are several possible explanations for these findings. One is that leisure engagement may help individuals develop psychological resources at particular times during the recovery process in response to stressors and threats (Hutchinson et al., 2008; Newman et al., 2014), since individuals’ reactions to stressful events are immediate (Dickerson & Kemeny, 2004). That is, the precise timing of leisure engagement may be important in its ability to affect stress-buffering mechanisms. Research has shown that positive experiences generated by uplifting events (i.e., having fun, socializing) can have buffering effects during stressful events (Sin & Almeida, 2018; Sin, Ong, Stawski, & Almeida, 2017). Another potential explanation for the findings is that the everyday stressors individuals reported in the current study did not reach a level of severity sufficient for leisure engagement to exhibit buffering effects. Coleman and Iso-Ahola (1993) argued that “leisure impacts health by providing buffering mechanisms that come into play when life presents significant problems. On the other hand, when life stress is relatively low leisure’s contribution to health is expected to less substantial” (p. 113). Perhaps leisure engagement buffers individuals from the stress caused by major adverse life events rather than that caused by minor daily hassles. Future studies might examine how the effects of leisure are related to stressor severity.
Limitations
There are some limitations that should be considered when interpreting the present results. First, due to the use of secondary data, leisure engagement was measured at a baseline before the 14 consecutive days of data collection. Thus, we were unable to test key mechanisms of the Leisure and Health Model (Coleman & Iso-Ahola, 1993) by assessing the longitudinal effects of leisure engagement after periods of stress response. Future studies might determine whether individuals’ emotional reactivity to stressors can be buffered by engaging in leisure using a finer-grained time scale (e.g., across hours) measured by EMAs. Second, the PEAT is a brief self-rating tool that may not accurately represent a broad array of leisure activities. The PEAT only indicates individuals’ frequency of engagement in a certain spectrum of enjoyable activities. Additionally, some overlap is possible among the PEAT items (e.g., doing fun things with others versus engaging in physical activity). Thus, further studies using a more detailed list of enjoyable activities performed at different frequencies are warranted. Finally, the current study is a cross-sectional study and cannot be taken as determining causality. Future studies using experimental designs to examine affect and stressors are recommended to develop a more robust understanding of the moderating role of leisure in daily experiences.
Conclusion
In sum, our findings provide further evidence that leisure engagement is related to emotional well-being (Coleman & Iso-Ahola, 1993; Iwasaki & Mannell, 2000; Newman, Tay, & Diener, 2014). Whereas previous research has focused on the associations between leisure engagement and its benefits on emotional well-being, as determined by global assessments, this study considered leisure engagement’s benefits for emotional well-being in the context of individuals’ everyday lives using an EMA-based approach. The findings supported the broaden-and-build theory (Fredrickson, 2001), which suggests that leisure engagement may have beneficial consequences (increasing mean levels of PA and lowering PA variability) for daily emotional well-being. Additionally, leisure engagement did not minimize emotional reactivity to daily stressors, suggesting that the timing of leisure engagement appears to matter because of its ability to buffer against negative experiences in daily life. These findings show the importance of leisure engagement to the dynamics of daily emotional well-being.
Acknowledgments
An earlier version of this work was orally presented at the 2018 National Recreation and Park Association (NRPA) conference in Indianapolis, IN. We thank the conference participants for their constructive comments.
Funding
This research was supported by the National Institutes of Health (NIH) [R03AG050798 to S.B.S., RO1AG039409 to M.J.S.]. Any opinions, findings, and conclusions expressed in this study are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the views of the NIH.
Footnotes
Declaration of Competing Interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
References
- Almeida DM (2005). Resilience and vulnerability to daily stressors assessed via diary methods. Psychological Science, 14(2), 64–68. doi: 10.1111/j.09637214.2005.00336.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Almeida DM, Piazza JR, Stawski RS, & Klein LC (2011). The speedometer of life: Stress, health and aging. In Schaie KW & Willis SL (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (7th ed.). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Almeida DM, Wethington E, & Kessler RC (2002). The daily inventory of stressful events: an interview-based approach for measuring daily stressors. Assessment, 9(1), 41–55. 10.1177/1073191102091006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Aron A, Norman CC, Aron EN, McKenna C, & Heyman RE (2000). Couples’ shared participation in novel and arousing activities and experienced relationship quality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(2), 273–284. 10.1037/0022-3514.78.2.273 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brajša-Žganec A, Merkaš M, & Šverko I (2011). Quality of life and leisure activities: How do leisure activities contribute to subjective well-being? Social Indicators Research, 102(1), 81–91. doi: 10.1007/s11205-010-9724-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Carruthers C, & Hood CD (2007). Build a life of meaning through therapeutic recreation: The leisure and well-being model, part I. Therapeutic Recreation Journal, 41(4), 276–297. [Google Scholar]
- Chang PJ, Yarnal C, & Chick G (2016). The longitudinal association between playfulness and resilience in older women engaged in The Red Hat Society. Journal of Leisure Research, 48(3), 210–227. doi: 10.18666/JLR-2016-V48-I3-6256 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Charles ST, Piazza JR, Mogle J, Sliwinski MJ, & Almeida DM (2013). The wear and tear of daily stressors on mental health. Psychological Science, 24(5), 733–741. doi: 10.1177/0956797612462222 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
- Coleman D, & Iso-Ahola SE (1993). Leisure and health: The role of social support and self-determination. Journal of Leisure Research, 25(2), 111–128. doi: 10.1080/00222216.1993.11969913 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cortina JM (1993). What is coefficient alpha? An examination of theory and applications. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78(1), 98–104. 10.1037/0021-9010.78.1.98 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Danner DD, Snowdon DA, & Friesen WV (2001). Positive emotions in early life and longevity: Findings from the nun study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(5), 804–813. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.5.804 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Denovan A, & Macaskill A (2017). Stress, resilience and leisure coping among university students: Applying the broaden-and-build theory. Leisure Studies, 36(6), 852–865. 10.1080/02614367.2016.1240220 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dickerson SS, & Kemeny ME (2004). Acute stressors and cortisol responses: a theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychological Bulletin, 130(3), 355–391. 10.1037/0033-2909.130.3.355 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diener E, & Emmons RA (1984). The independence of positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(5), 1105–1117. doi: 10.1037//00223514.47.5.1105 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fredrickson BL (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology. American Psychologist, 56(3), 218–226. doi: 10.1037//0003-066X.56.3.218 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fredrickson BL, & Joiner T (2002). Positive emotions trigger upward spirals toward emotional well-being. Psychological Science, 13(2), 172–175. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00431 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fredrickson BL, Tugade MM, Waugh CE, & Larkin GR (2003). What good are positive emotions in crises? A prospective study of resilience and emotions following the terrorist attacks on the United States on September 11th, 2001. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(2), 365–376. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.365 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Freire T, & Teixeira A (2018). The Influence of leisure attitudes and leisure satisfaction on adolescents’ positive functioning: The role of emotion regulation. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1349. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01349 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gao J, Kerstetter DL, Mowen AJ, & Hickerson B (2018). Changes in tourists’ perception of well-being based on their use of emotion regulation strategies during vacation. Journal of Travel & Tourism Marketing, 35(5), 567–582. 10.1080/10548408.2017.1374908 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gruber J, Kogan A, Quoidbach J, & Mauss IB (2013). Happiness is best kept stable: Positive emotion variability is associated with poorer psychological health. Emotion, 13(1), 1–6. 10.1037/a0030262 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hardy J, & Segerstrom SC (2016). Intra-individual variability and psychological flexibility: Affect and health in a National US sample. Journal of Research in Personality, 69, 13–21. 10.1016/j.jrp.2016.04.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hicks SA, & Siedlecki KL (2017). Leisure activity engagement and positive affect partially mediate the relationship between positive views on aging and physical health. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 72(2), 259–267. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbw049 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hildebrandt LK, McCall C, Engen HG, & Singer T (2016). Cognitive flexibility, heart rate variability, and resilience predict fine-grained regulation of arousal during prolonged threat. Psychophysiology, 53(6), 880–890. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12632 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hood CD, & Carruthers CP (2002). Coping skills theory as an underlying framework for therapeutic recreation services. Therapeutic Recreation Journal, 36(2), 137–153. [Google Scholar]
- Hutchinson SL, Bland AD, & Kleiber DA (2008). Leisure and stress-coping: Implications for therapeutic recreation practice. Therapeutic Recreation Journal, 42(1), 9–23. [Google Scholar]
- Iwasaki Y, & Mannell RC (2000). Hierarchical dimensions of leisure stress coping. Leisure Sciences, 22(3), 163–181. 10.1080/01490409950121843 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Jenkins BN, Hunter JF, Cross MP, Acevedo AM, & Pressman SD (2018). When is affect variability bad for health? The association between affect variability and immune response to the influenza vaccination. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 104, 41–47. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2017.11.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kleiber DA, Walker GJ, & Mannell RC (2011). A social psychology of leisure (2nd ed.). State College, PA: Venture Publishing, Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Lazarus RS, Kanner AD, & Folkman S (1980). Emotions: A cognitive-phenomenological analysis. In Plutchik R & Kellerman H (Eds.), Theories of emotion (pp. 189–217). New York: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Mitas O, Qian XL, Yarnal C, & Kerstetter D (2011). The fun begins now!” Broadening and building processes in Red Hat Society participation. Journal of Leisure Research, 43(1), 30–55. 10.1080/00222216.2011.11950225 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mroczek DK, Stawski RS, Turiano NA, Chan W, Almeida DM, Neupert SD, & Spiro A, (2015). Emotional reactivity and mortality: Longitudinal findings from the va normative aging study. The journals of gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 70(3), 398–406. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbt107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newman DB, Tay L, & Diener E (2014). Leisure and subjective well-being: A model of psychological mechanisms as mediating factors. Journal of Happiness Studies, 15(3), 555–578. doi: 10.1007/s10902-013-9435-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ong AD, Bergeman CS, Bisconti TL, & Wallace KA (2006). Psychological resilience, positive emotions, and successful adaptation to stress in later life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9(4), 730–749. 10.1037/0022-3514.91.4.730 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patry DA, Blanchard CM, & Mask L (2007). Measuring university students’ regulatory leisure coping styles: planned breathers or avoidance? Leisure Sciences, 29(3), 247–265. 10.1080/01490400701257963 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pearlin L, & Schooler C (1978). The structure of coping. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 19(1), 2–21. doi: 10.2307/2136319 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piazza JR, Charles ST, Sliwinski MJ, Mogle J, & Almeida DM (2013). Affective reactivity to daily stressors and long-term risk of reporting a chronic physical health condition. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 45(1), 110–120. doi: 10.1007/s12160-012-9423-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pressman SD, Matthews KA, Cohen S, Martire LM, Scheier M, Baum A, & Schulz R (2009). Association of enjoyable leisure activities with psychological and physical well-being. Psychosomatic Medicine, 71(1), 725–721. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181ad7978 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qian X, Yarnal CM, & Almeida DM (2013). Does leisure time as a stress coping resource increase affective complexity? Applying the dynamic model of affect (DMA). Journal of Leisure Research, 45(3), 393–314. doi: 10.18666/jlr-2013-v45-i3-3157 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qian X, Yarnal CM, & Almeida DM (2014). Does leisure time moderate or mediate the effect of daily stress on positive affect? An examination using eight-day diary data. Journal of Leisure Research, 46(1), 106–124. doi: 10.1080/00222216.2014.11950315 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ram N, & Gerstorf D (2009). Time-structured and net intraindividual variability: Tools for examining the development of dynamic characteristics and processes. Psychology and Aging, 24(4), 778–791. doi: 10.1037/a0017915 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rickenbach EH, Condeelis KL, & Haley WE (2015). Daily stressors and emotional reactivity in individuals with mild cognitive impairment and cognitively healthy controls. Psychology and Aging, 30(2), 420–431. 10.1037/a0038973 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott JL, Wozencroft AJ, & Waller SN (2019). Ecological momentary assessment research in therapeutic recreation. Therapeutic Recreation Journal, 53(1), 76–85. doi: 10.18666/TRJ-2019-V53-I1-8797 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Scott SB, Graham-Engeland JE, Engeland CG, Smyth JM, Almeida DM, Katz MJ, . . . Sliwinski MJ (2015). The effects of stress on cognitive aging, physiology, and emotion (escape) project. BMC Psychiatry, 15,146–159. doi: 10.1186/s12888-015-0497-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sin NL, & Almeida DM (2018). Daily positive experiences and health: Biobehavioral pathways and resilience to daily stress. In Ryff CD & Krueger RF (Eds.), The oxford handbook of integrative health science (pp. 1–34). Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sin NL, Graham-Engeland JE, Ong AD, & Almeida DM (2015). Affective reactivity to daily stressors is associated with elevated inflammation. Health Psychology, 34(12), 1154–1165. doi: 10.1037/hea0000240 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sin NL, Ong AD, Stawski RS, & Almeida DM (2017). Daily positive events and diurnal cortisol rhythms: Examination of between-person differences and within-person variation. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 83, 91–100. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.06.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sin NL, Sloan RP, McKinley PS, & Almeida DM (2016). Linking daily stress processes and laboratory-based heart rate variability in a national sample of midlife and older adults. Psychosomatic Medicine, 78(5), 573–582. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000306 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snijders TAB, & Bosker RJ (1999). Multilevel analysis: An introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. [Google Scholar]
- Tugade MM, Fredrickson BL, & Barrett LF (2004). Psychological resilience and positive emotional granularity: Examining the benefits of positive emotions on coping and health. Journal of Personality, 72(6), 1161–1190. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00294.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zautra A, Affleck GG, Tennen H, Reich JW, & Davis MC (2005). Dynamic approaches to emotions and stress in everyday life: Bolger and Zuckerman reloaded with positive as well as negative affects. Journal of Personality, 73(6), 1511–1538. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2005.00357.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]