Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2023 Apr 11;18(4):e0284271. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0284271

Effect of frequency and rhythmicity on flicker light-induced hallucinatory phenomena

Ioanna Alicia Amaya 1,2,3, Nele Behrens 1,4, David John Schwartzman 5, Trevor Hewitt 5, Timo Torsten Schmidt 1,*
Editor: Manuel Spitschan6
PMCID: PMC10089352  PMID: 37040392

Abstract

Flicker light stimulation (FLS) uses stroboscopic light on closed eyes to induce transient visual hallucinatory phenomena, such as the perception of geometric patterns, motion, and colours. It remains an open question where the neural correlates of these hallucinatory experiences emerge along the visual pathway. To allow future testing of suggested underlying mechanisms (e.g., changes in functional connectivity, neural entrainment), we sought to systematically characterise the effects of frequency (3 Hz, 8 Hz, 10 Hz and 18 Hz) and rhythmicity (rhythmic and arrhythmic conditions) on flicker-induced subjective experiences. Using a novel questionnaire, we found that flicker frequency and rhythmicity significantly influenced the degree to which participants experienced simple visual hallucinations, particularly the perception of Klüver forms and dynamics (e.g., motion). Participants reported their experience of geometric patterns and dynamics was at highest intensity during 10 Hz rhythmic stimulation. Further, we found that frequency-matched arrhythmic FLS strongly reduced these subjective effects compared to equivalent rhythmic stimulation. Together, these results provide evidence that flicker rhythmicity critically contributes to the effects of FLS beyond the effects of frequency alone, indicating that neural entrainment may drive the induced phenomenal experience.

Introduction

Flicker light stimulation (FLS) reliably induces simple visual hallucinations in healthy participants via closed-eye ocular stimulation with stroboscopic light [13]. Simple visual hallucinations, synonymous with elementary visual hallucinations, refer to the subjective experience of colours and geometric patterns that are devoid of semantic content. The experience is often accompanied by other phenomenological changes, such as altered mood, arousal, and sense of time passing [2]. Simple visual hallucinations experienced under FLS display marked similarities to the perceptual changes associated with migraine aura [4,5], epileptic seizures [6] and Charles Bonnet Syndrome (hallucinatory experiences due to sensory deprivation resulting from macular degeneration) [7] as well as drug-induced psychedelic experiences [2,8]. In recent years, FLS has been used as an experimental tool to study the neural underpinnings of visual hallucinations [9]. However, in order to draw links between neural mechanisms underlying specific forms of visual hallucinations, it is first important to establish a thorough characterisation of the experienced phenomena.

Flicker-induced effects were first formally described by Purkinje in 1819 [10]; thereafter, the phenomenon was relatively unexplored until the invention of the electroencephalogram (EEG) in the 1920s [11]. This allowed the observation of synchronised brain oscillations when FLS was presented in the alpha frequency range (8–12 Hz) [12]. Later, it was explored recreationally in the 1960s with the creation of the “Dreamachine”, a low-fi method of delivering FLS using a record player [11]. Today, FLS can be delivered using specially programmed electronic lamps with precise manipulation of flicker frequency, rhythmicity (i.e., the temporal pattern of flashes), and brightness.

Initial studies attempting to characterise visual experiences arising from altered and pathological states employed illustrations and open report methods, which revealed a striking universality in the types of visual patterns experienced [13,14]. Four of these commonly occurring patterns are collectively named the Klüver form constants [15], which are comprised of grids, spirals, tunnels, and targets (see [16,17] for illustrations). FLS-induced perception of motion and colours are also shared across various pathologies and altered states. For example, migraine sufferers often report seeing red, yellow, and blue in addition to bright white [18] and vivid colours are one of the most frequently reported characteristics of N, N-dimethyltryptamine (N, N-DMT) experiences [19]. In addition, FLS also induces other types of imagery that have lower levels of pattern organisation and higher degrees of noise, such as TV static and floating, scattered blobs and dots. These are sometimes referred to as phosphene forms and likely occur due to retinal stimulation with a strong light source [20]. Aside from simple hallucinations, complex visual hallucinations (i.e., realistic scenes, objects, and faces) have also been reported during FLS, albeit less frequently [2,3]. Recent research further explored if FLS experiences relate to person-specific factors, such as the personality trait of Absorption [2,21]. It was also found that people with Aphantasia (i.e., lack of mentally simulated visual imagery [22]) report fewer FLS-induced visual effects [23].

Recent studies have used standardised methods to assess the flicker-induced subjective experience, e.g., Bartossek et al. [2] administered the Altered States of Consciousness Rating Scale (5D-ASC/11-ASC; [24]) and the Phenomenology of Consciousness Inventory (PCI) [25]. These questionnaires are well-established and validated to assess a whole spectrum of altered experiences and thereby allow comparisons across different types of altered states of consciousness [8]. However, due to their breadth of measured phenomena, they are limited in capturing a high level of detail of visual effects that would enable differentiation between different types of visual hallucinations. Using an analogue slider to assess experience intensity, Schwartzman et al. [3] were able to differentiate the intensity of experiences between different frequencies of FLS, which was otherwise not captured via 5D-ASC ratings. These observations highlight the need for a careful and detailed assessment of the types of visual phenomena experienced during FLS that extends beyond the currently available tools.

The link between phenomenology and neurophysiology can be used to shed light on the neural mechanisms underlying FLS-induced visual hallucinations. Using periodic flicker (i.e., FLS with regular inter-flash intervals; also called rhythmic flicker), it was found that FLS at alpha frequency (8–12 Hz) induces stronger simple visual hallucinations than other frequencies [2,3,17] and additionally enhances the amplitude of EEG oscillations at the targeted frequency band of stimulation [26,27]. This indicates that entrainment (i.e., synchronisation of brain oscillations with periodic external driving stimulation) may contribute to the generation of simple visual hallucinations. To further test whether entrainment is indeed a driving factor in generating the subjective experience associated with FLS, one must compare the subjective effects between traditional rhythmic stimulation and frequency-matched arrhythmic stimulation. Theoretically, removing the rhythmicity of the stimulation should abolish entrainment. Therefore, if arrhythmic frequency-matched FLS produces fewer subjective effects, it would indicate that neural entrainment contributes to the generation of FLS-induced simple visual hallucinations.

Here, we aim to determine the effects of frequency and rhythmicity on flicker-induced phenomenology. Based on previous findings, we expect that rhythmic FLS within the alpha frequency range (8–12 Hz) will lead to increased reports of simple visual hallucinations, Klüver forms and visual experiences that are more dynamic (i.e., moving patterns, patterns changing frequently over time) and visually detailed, compared to other frequencies. We further expect that rhythmic FLS will generate more simple visual hallucinations compared to frequency-matched arrhythmic stimulation. We utilise two arrhythmic conditions that vary in their degree of arrhythmicity. We hypothesise that higher variability in inter-flash intervals (i.e., greater arrhythmicity) will lead to a greater reduction in subjective effects. In contrast, as all FLS conditions deliver the same total duration of light stimulation, we hypothesise that seeing phosphene forms (e.g., blobs, TV static) will be frequency- and rhythmicity-independent. This is because they have low levels of pattern organisation, making it likely they are caused by retinal stimulation and not higher-order neural mechanisms. We will also explore whether frequency and rhythmicity affect reports of complex visual hallucinations and types of colours that are observed during the flicker experience.

Methods

Participants

Healthy participants were recruited (N = 20; 12 female, 7 male, 1 diverse; age range 20–37 years, M = 24.78, SD = 4.25) that met the following inclusion criteria, as established by Bartossek et al. [2]: no history of epilepsy, migraines, psychological problems (e.g., depression, anxiety disorders), no current consumption of any psychotropic drugs (e.g., antidepressants, neuroleptics). To mitigate the risk of an adverse reaction, we only included subjects who had previously used FLS for recreational purposes. Alternatively, an EEG examination was performed to screen for indicators of photosensitive epilepsy, which would lead to exclusion. The recruitment took place via student mailing lists and through word-of-mouth. Participants gave their written consent before commencing the experiment. All materials and procedures were approved by the ethics committee at Freie Universität Berlin (application reference: 045/2021). Seventeen participants filled out the questionnaires in German, of which fifteen were native speakers, while the remaining three participants preferred English, two of which were native speakers.

Materials

Flicker light stimulation

A custom stroboscope was constructed by Lumenate Growth Inc. (Bristol, United Kingdom) to generate light stimulation. It consists of twelve 4500k J2 6V white LEDs organised in a three-by-four grid with dimensions 128 x 176mm (width x height). The lamp was set to deliver 5,520 Lumens over participants’ eyes (maximum capacity is 10,360 Lumens). Positioned approximately 150cm from participants, the LEDs were within a visual angle of approximately 6.5°, while the setup assured that illumination of the visual field was experienced as homogenous. The lamp was interfaced with an Arduino (v1.8.16) to deliver FLS at different frequencies and rhythmicities. Three rhythmicity conditions were used across four levels of frequency (3 Hz, 8 Hz, 10 Hz and 18 Hz) [Fig 1A]. Rhythmicity levels were: Rhythmic, which consists of periodic light stimulation following a 0.3 duty cycle (30% ON time); normally distributed arrhythmic stimulation (Arrhythmicnorm), where inter-flash intervals (IFIs) were sampled from a normal distribution with mean IFI equal to one OFF time during periodic stimulation at matched frequency (e.g., 70 ms at 10 Hz) and standard deviation equal to 0.45*OFF time at matched frequency; and paired arrhythmic stimulation (Arrhythmicpairs), which involved paired high frequency flashes (similar to the luminance control condition used by Ffytche [28]). The inter-pair OFF time was calculated as 100/freq ms and adjusted to 10 ms if the value would otherwise be lower. The flash pairs were embedded within a set of intervals that were sampled from an exponential probability distribution, where the mean IFI was equal to one OFF time at frequency-matched periodic stimulation. As IFIs were calculated for every second, 3 Hz Arrhythmicpairs used one pair and one single flash for every second. All conditions delivered 300 ms of light stimulation per second (30% ON time). While previous studies used 50% ON time [2,3], we decided to shorten the ON period as this allowed for a greater degree of variation in the arrhythmic IFIs. The Arrhythmicpairs condition contains IFIs with higher variability than the Arrhythmicnorm at each frequency level (excluding the constant inter-pair interval). This was determined using the root mean squared of successive differences (RMSSD), which is commonly used for calculating heart rate variability [29] (e.g., at 10Hz, Arrhythmicpairs: RMSSD = 166.57, Arrhythmicnorm: 64.22). For an illustration of the ON/OFF flicker sequences see Fig 1A.

Fig 1.

Fig 1

(A) The experiment comprised a 3x4 factorial design with 3 levels of rhythmicity (Rhythmic, Arrhythmicnorm and Arrhythmicpairs) and 4 levels of frequency (3 Hz, 8 Hz, 10 Hz, 18 Hz). (B) In the experimental setup, participants were seated in a dark room 150 cm away from the stroboscope (Lumenate Growth Inc., Bristol, United Kingdom). The initial assessment involved the completion of TAS questionnaire. In the training phase, participants were exposed to FLS and familiarised with the Stroboscopic Visual Experience Survey (SVES) and ASC-R items. The subsequent flicker session consisted of twelve two-minute stimulation periods presented in a fully randomised order of conditions. Following each stimulation period, participants rated their experience using SVES and ASC-R items. A second session took place within a week of the first.

Questionnaires

Three questionnaires were used in the study: The Tellegen Absorption scale (TAS), selected items from the Altered States of Consciousness Rating Scale (ASC-R) and an abridged version of the novel Stroboscopic Visual Experience Survey (SVES). Participants were able to answer the questionnaires in English or German.

TAS

TAS assesses the personality trait Absorptiveness. It captures the openness to experiencing mind-altering states with 34 items rated on a five-point Likert scale (0 = “Not at all” to 4 = “Very much”) [21].

ASC-R

Eleven questions were taken from the Altered State of Consciousness Scale [24]. The ASC-R is a standardised and validated research tool widely used to investigate ASCs [8,24]. Items are rated using a visual analogue scale (VAS) ranging from “no, not more than usually” to “yes, much more than usually”. The 96-item questionnaire can be decomposed into 11 factors, including subscales from the main five dimensions: Oceanic Boundlessness, Visionary Reconstruction, Dread of Ego Dissolution, Auditory Alterations, Vigilance Reduction) [30]. As FLS primarily induces visual effects, we selected all items from the Elementary Imagery subscale: (“I saw regular patterns [with closed eyes or in complete darkness.]”; “I saw colors [with closed eyes or in complete darkness.]”), excluding the item “I saw brightness or flashes of light with eyes closed or in complete darkness” as the experience is inherent to FLS. Further, we selected all items from the Complex Imagery subscale of the Visionary Reconstruction scale: “I saw whole scenes roll by [with closed eyes or in complete darkness]”; “I could see images from my memory or imagination with extreme clarity”; “My imagination was extremely vivid”. As it has been reported that other altered state phenomena can additionally arise from FLS [2], we also selected two items from the Positive Derealisation subscale: “I felt as if in a wonderful other world.”; “The boundaries between myself and my surroundings seemed to blur”, the item “My sense of space and time was altered as if I was dreaming” from Altered Perception of Time subscale and “I had the impression I was out of my body.” from the Positive Depersonalisation subscale of the Oceanic Boundlessness scale. Finally, to measure participants alertness during each trial we included “I felt sleepy” from the Reduction of Vigilance scale.

SVES

An abridged version of the Stroboscopic Visual Experience Survey, which is currently under development, was used. The SVES is a computer-based questionnaire designed to allow participants to capture aspects of their FLS experience more accurately, implemented using the SoSci Survey platform. It was originally constructed in English and was translated into German for the purposes of this study. The SVES begins with an instruction page that explains how to answer each item. Thereafter, participants are asked “How well do you recall your visual experience right now” and “What colours did you see? Select all that apply”. There are twenty-four colour options, which correspond to the following Natural Colour System (NCS) IDs: S1040-R, S2070 Y80R, S4050 Y90R, S2050-Y50R, S1070-Y70R, S3050-Y80R, S0550-Y20R, S0580-Y30R, S1060-Y40R, S2070-G70R, S2070-G60Y, S5040-B70G, S0520-B, S3050-R70B, S4050-B10G, S1020-R50B, S5020-R70B, S4050-R50B, S2050-R20B, S5010-B70G, S6010-G10Y, S4050-R20B as well as black and white. Participants are then asked to rate the occurrence of different patterns and forms during the preceding FLS experience (See S1 Appendix for full list of items). The patterns used in these questions are based on geometric patterns that were reported in previous FLS studies [1,16,28] and additional piloting. The patterns vary between Klüver form constants, phosphenes forms and other possible geometric patterns that could appear, as well as one geometric pattern that is unlikely to occur (akin to a control pattern). In addition, overarching visual aspects of the FLS experience are assessed with another ten items, such as “Did your visual experience continuously change or evolve over time?” and “Did your visual experience contain a high level of randomness or chaos?”. These items use a visual analogue scale (VAS) ranging from 0 (no, not at all) to 100 (yes, very much), which was used to increase comparability of effect sizes with the ASC-R. Three example pictures are given to demonstrate the range of possibilities across the scale. Item 1 was excluded due to technical difficulties. Item 16 and Item 22 were excluded due to high response variability. For the subsequent analysis, items were grouped together that conceptually measured the same visual phenomena. Items were grouped into the following scales: colors (Item 2), simple visual hallucinations (Item 3—Item 10), phosphene forms (Item 11 & Item 12), detail (Item 13), dynamics (Item 14 & Item 15), paisley (Item 17), complex visual hallucinations (Item 18) and absorption (Item 21). The paisley pattern represents a geometrically simple pattern that is unlikely to occur. Two subscales of the simple visual hallucinations scale were also determined: Klüver (Item 3 –Item 6) and other (Item 8 and Item 10) in order to assess whether there were differences in reported pattern subtypes.

Experimental procedure

Initial assessment

A semi-structured interview, which followed the guidelines published by Bartossek et al. [2], took place to screen participants for eligibility. During the initial assessment [Fig 1B], participants were given an information sheet and then filled out the consent form. A pseudo-anonymised subject ID was created to link data from the two experimental sessions. Participants completed the TAS via tablet.

Training phase

Participants wore noise-cancelling headphones and were seated on a comfortable chair with headrest 150 cm away from the lamp in a dark room. The training phase consisted of four one-minute stimulation periods: constant light, 3 Hz, 10 Hz and 18 Hz of rhythmic flicker light. This allowed participants to accustomise to the light intensity and type of experience. Next, participants were asked to evaluate a static image using the SVES to gain familiarity with the questionnaire items.

Experimental sessions

If participants had no further questions, the experimental phase could begin. This involved presentation of twelve two-minute stimulation periods with a fully randomised order of conditions. The conditions were comprised of three levels of rhythmicity (Rhythmic, Arrhythmicnorm and Arrhythmicpairs) and four levels of frequency (3 Hz, 8 Hz, 10 Hz, 18 Hz). Following each stimulation period, participants answered the SVES and ASC-R items to evaluate their phenomenal experience. A second experimental session took place at the same time of day 1–7 days after the first.

Statistical analysis

All statistical analysis was conducted using Rstudio (v1.4.1103). To test whether participant ratings differed across the two test sessions, 3x4x2 ANOVAs with rhythmicity, frequency and test session as factors were run. For each participant, the mean rating of each scale between the two sessions was used for further analysis. To test the effects of rhythmicity and frequency, we ran 3x4 ANOVAs with rhythmicity and frequency as factors. Post-hoc Tukey HSD-Tests were used to compare the distribution of ratings in different conditions. As there was insufficient evidence to assume normality of data for some of the assessed scales, shown with Shapiro-Wilks normality tests, we used Kruskal-Wallis tests to confirm the ANOVA results with nonparametric testing. To test the effect of rhythmicity and frequency on colour selection, we ran a repeated measures logistic regression model for each colour using the lme4 package in R. In the regression model, Participant ID was included as a random effect term while frequency and rhythmicity were fixed effect terms. Further, we used Pearson product-moment correlation to explore whether there were associations between the personality trait Absorptiveness and the occurrence of complex visual hallucinations.

Results

Effects of test session order on flicker-induced phenomena

To test for the effects of test session order, we performed a 3x4x2 ANOVA for each SVES scale with frequency, rhythmicity, and test session as factors. We found effects of test session for the Simple Visual Hallucination (F(1, 456) = 11.93, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.03), Dynamics (F(1, 456) = 16.41, p < 0.001 η2p = 0.03) and Paisley SVES scales (F(1, 456) = 3.87, p = 0.05, η2p < 0.01), albeit with small effect sizes, where ratings were higher in the first session compared to the second. When using nonparametric Wilcoxon Rank Sum tests to assess differences in ratings for each condition, there was no significant differences between test sessions for any SVES scale. Therefore, for subsequent analysis, the mean score was calculated from the first and second session for each participant.

Effect of frequency and rhythmicity on simple visual hallucinations

We sought to test if frequency and rhythmicity affects how participants rated, between 0 and 100, the occurrence of simple visual hallucinations in their flicker experience. First, we ran a 3x4 ANOVA on the Simple Visual Hallucination scale of the SVES. We found main effects of frequency (F(3, 228) = 46.19, p < 0.001, η2p = .38) and rhythmicity (F(2, 228) = 27.91, p < 0.001, η2p = .19) and a significant interaction effect (F(6, 228) = 3.78, p = 0.001, η2p = 0.09). Nonparametric Kruskal-Wallis testing confirmed a significant effect of frequency and rhythmicity (H(3) = 79.3, p < 0.001; H(2) = 26.5, p < 0.001). Post-hoc Tukey tests found that 8 Hz, 10 Hz and 18 Hz stimulation elicited higher ratings of simple visual hallucinations than 3 Hz (all p < 0.001). Furthermore, all rhythmicity levels were significantly different from each other, where rhythmic was higher than Arrhythmicnorm (p = 0.001) and Arrhythmicpairs (p < 0.001) and Arrhythmicnorm was higher than Arrhythmicpairs (p < 0.001). For interaction effects, post-hoc Tukey tests showed that the highest increase in reports of simple visual hallucination was between Rhythmic and Arrhythmicpairs stimulation at 10Hz (p < .001) and remained significant for 18Hz (p = .003) and 8Hz (p = .01) [See S1 Table for full report of Tukey tests]. Furthermore, during Rhythmic stimulation, reports of simple visual hallucinations are significantly higher during 8Hz, 10Hz and 18Hz compared to 3Hz (all p < .001) [See S1 Table for full report of Tukey tests]. Fig 2A summarises the interaction between rhythmicity and frequency on ratings of simple visual hallucinations. Secondly, we tested for effects on simple visual hallucinations via scores of the ASC-R Elementary Imagery scale. Here, a 3x4 ANOVA revealed a main effect of frequency (F(3, 228) = 5.89, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.07), which was further confirmed by Kruskal-Wallis testing (H(3) = 18.6, p < 0.001). Again, post-hoc Tukey tests found that 8 Hz, 10 Hz and 18 Hz stimulation generated higher ratings of simple visual hallucinations than 3 Hz (all p < 0.01).

Fig 2. Differential effects of rhythmicity and frequency on FLS-induced hallucinatory phenomena.

Fig 2

(A) Effects of rhythmicity and frequency on reports of simple visual hallucinations. Ratings are shown from the SVES Simple Visual Hallucinations and ASC-R Elementary Imagery scales. (B) Effects of rhythmicity and frequency on visual dynamics, which encompasses motion and how much the experience changes over time. (C) Effects of rhythmicity and frequency on different types of visual patterns. Klüver forms SVES subscale consists of spirals, cobwebs, targets and grids. Other forms include SVES items of rippling items and flowing lines. Phosphene forms refers to lower order forms and includes SVES items for TV snow and blobs of light or colour. Bar charts display the difference in ratings between arrhythmic controls and rhythmic stimulation for each frequency. Significance is determined by Tukey tests comparing ratings between rhythmicity conditions at each frequency level [See S1 Table for Tukey test results].

Effects of frequency and rhythmicity on dynamics and detail

To test if frequency and rhythmicity would affect the visual dynamics and detail of hallucinatory phenomena, we ran 3x4 ANOVAs on the Dynamics and Detail SVES scales. We found a main effect of rhythmicity (F(2, 228) = 33.83, p < 0.001, η2p = .23) and frequency (F(3, 228) = 46.32, p < 0.001, η2p = .38) as well as an interaction effect (F(6, 228) = 3.50, p = .0025, η2p = .08) on visual dynamics (Fig 2B). Kruskal-Wallis testing confirmed the significant effect of frequency and rhythmicity on visual dynamics (H(3) = 76.3, p < 0.001; H(2) = 33.1, p < 0.001). Post-hoc Tukey tests revealed that 8 Hz, 10 Hz and 18 Hz stimulation elicited higher ratings of visual dynamics than 3 Hz (all p < 0.001). Moreover, ratings were higher for Rhythmic compared to Arrhythmicnorm and Arrhythmicpairs (p < 0.001). For interaction effects, post-hoc Tukey tests showed that, during Rhythmic stimulation, 8 Hz, 10 Hz and 18 Hz elicited higher ratings of visual dynamics than 3 Hz (p < .001). Moreover, ratings were higher for Rhythmic compared to Arrhythmicpairs at 8 Hz, 10 Hz and 18 Hz (all p < .001) [See S1 Table for Tukey test results]. At 10Hz, visual dynamics were also higher for Rhythmic compared to Arrhythmicnorm (p = .03) [See S1 Table for full report of Tukey tests]. Further, we found a significant effect of rhythmicity (F(2, 228) = 9.25, p < 0.001, η2p = .08) and frequency (F(3, 228) = 26.60, p < 0.001, η2p = .26) on visual detail. Kruskal-Wallis testing confirmed the significant effect of frequency and rhythmicity on ratings of visual detail (H(3) = 57.9, p < 0.001; H(2) = 13.5, p = 0.001). Post-hoc Tukey tests revealed that visual detail was higher at 8 Hz, 10 Hz and 18 Hz compared to 3 Hz (p < 0.001) [See S1 Table for Tukey test results]. Ratings of visual detail were also higher for Rhythmic compared to Arrhythmicpairs (p < 0.001).

Effects of frequency and rhythmicity on seeing different pattern types

To test if frequency and rhythmicity would affect the types of patterns experienced during simple visual hallucinations, we ran 3x4 ANOVAs on the Klüver forms, Other forms, Phosphene forms and Paisley SVES scales. We found that frequency had a main effect on ratings of seeing Klüver forms (F(3, 228) = 34.92, p < 0.001, η2p = .31) and Other forms (F(3, 228) = 24.78, p < 0.001, η2p = .13) (Fig 2C]. Kruskal-Wallis testing confirmed significant effects of frequency on seeing Kluver forms (H(3) = 69.6, p < 0.001) and Other forms ((H(3) = 57.6, p < 0.001). ANOVA testing also revealed a significant main effect of rhythmicity on Klüver form (F(2, 228) = 17.47, p < 0.001, η2p = .13) and Other form ratings (F(2, 228) = 16.69, p < 0.001, η2p = .13), which was further confirmed by Kruskal-Wallis testing (Kluver forms: H(2) = 20.3, p < 0.001); Other forms: H(2) = 22.1, p < 0.001). Additionally, there was a significant interaction effect on ratings of Klüver forms (F(6, 228) = 3.26, p = 0.004, η2p = 0.08). Post-hoc Tukey tests found that Klüver and Other forms generated higher ratings at 8 Hz, 10 Hz and 18 Hz than at 3 Hz (all p < 0.001). Ratings of Klüver and Other forms were also significantly higher in the Rhythmic condition compared to arrhythmic controls [See S1 Table for Tukey test results]. While ANOVA testing identified a small effect of rhythmicity on ratings of Phosphene forms (F(2,228) = 3.07, p = 0.05, η2p = .03), Kruskal-Wallis testing found no effect of rhythmicity or frequency on Phosphene forms. Similarly, ANOVA testing identified a small effect of frequency (F(3, 228) = 2.71, p = 0.05, η2p = .03) and rhythmicity (F(2, 228) = 4.83, p = 0.009, η2p = .04) on perception of Paisley patterns, which was not supported by Kruskal-Wallis testing (i.e., no significant effects were found).

Effect of rhythmicity and frequency on observed colours

Next, we explored whether there were categorical shifts in the spectrum of perceived colours during FLS at different frequencies and rhythmicity. No a priori hypotheses were set. The probability of each colour being selected is shown in Fig 3. From this, it appears there were no major shifts in the proportions of colours that were perceived across conditions. Descriptively, prominent peaks for reds, white and black can be observed, which increase in amplitude as the frequency increases. Logistic regression models were used to assess the relationship between frequency and rhythmicity and colour selection. The alpha threshold was Bonferroni corrected to 0.002 (0.05/24 due to 24 colours being tested). We found that frequency affected colour selection of white, light yellow and bright blue. The odds of participants selecting white were 6.0, 5.6 and 10.7 times greater during 8 Hz, 10 Hz and 18 Hz, respectively, compared to 3 Hz stimulation (all p < 0.001). White was chosen in 53.3% of trials at 3 Hz, 78.3% of trials at 8 Hz, 76.7% of trials at 10 Hz and 82.5% of trials at 18 Hz. Additionally, during 8 Hz stimulation, the odds of selecting light yellow were increased by 3.2-fold compared to 3 Hz stimulation (p < 0.001). At 18 Hz, the odds of selecting bright blue were 6.7 times higher than at 3 Hz (p < 0.001). Rhythmicity had a significant interaction with frequency on selection of white, whereby odds were 7.1 times higher during rhythmic stimulation at 10 Hz (p = 0.002).

Fig 3. Effects of rhythmicity and frequency on colour selection, depicted as a probability of each colour being selected.

Fig 3

This is expressed as sum of selections over the n = 20 participants. The mean selection probability is displayed, where for each participant the probability was based on the average of the first and second session (0, 0.5 or 1).

Effect of frequency and rhythmicity on complex visual hallucinations

Next, we tested the effects of frequency and rhythmicity on the occurrence of complex visual hallucinations. To this end, we performed a 3x4 ANOVA on the ratings of the SVES Complex Imagery scale. We found a main effect of rhythmicity (F(2, 228) = 4.35, p = 0.01, η2p = .04), which was not found using nonparametric testing. However, nonparametric Kruskal-Wallis testing identified a significant effect of frequency on SVES complex imagery ratings (H(3) = 11.6, p = 0.009). When testing for complex hallucinations via the ASC-R Complex Imagery scale, no significant effect was found. Note that the ratings for complex visual hallucinations were overall relatively low, however showing a relevant variability (i.e., for SVES scale ratings: 3 Hz: 5.2 ± 12.4 M ± SD; 8 Hz: 10.3 ± 19.0; 10 Hz: 12.7 ± 19.4; 18 Hz: 11.1 ± 16.3), which motivates further exploration to identify factors that determine if a participant will experience complex hallucinations or not.

Testing for a relationship between the personality trait absorptiveness and FLS-effects

In our sample, TAS scores, which indicate the personality trait Absorptiveness, ranged between 33 and 110 (maximum possible range: 0–136) across participants (M = 64.61, SD = 19.09). We used Pearson product-moment correlation to test if absorptiveness relates to the occurrence of simple and complex visual hallucinations. Across the four frequency conditions, no correlations (p<0.05) were found for SVES nor ASC-R ratings of simple visual hallucinations. Following the literature suggestion that the occurrence of complex hallucinations might be driven by a persons’ absorptiveness, we tested within the four rhythmic frequency conditions for correlations of TAS with Complex Imagery scales of the SVES and ASC-R. We found a positive correlation for the 10 Hz condition, which was significant when assessed with the SVES (r = .61, p = .004) even after correction for multiple comparisons (Bonferroni: 0.05/8 = 0.00625). When testing with the ASC-R scores, this correlation was also present (r = .49, p = .03), however did not survive correction for multiple comparisons.

Discussion

In this study, we aimed to determine the effects of flicker frequency and rhythmicity on FLS-induced hallucinatory effects. We used the well-established ASC-R in combination with an abridged version of the novel SVES to quantitatively assess the visual experience elicited by different FLS frequencies. This combination provided a direct comparison to previous data while also allowing a more thorough assessment of visual phenomena than previously attempted. We found effects of frequency on occurrence of simple visual hallucinations, especially perception of Klüver forms, as well as visual dynamics (e.g., motion) and the degree of visual detail. Here, participants reported that they experienced the most geometric patterns (e.g., Klüver forms) and visual dynamics during 10 Hz FLS. Furthermore, to test the influence of FLS rhythmicity on inducing simple visual hallucinations, we compared the subjective effects of rhythmic versus arrhythmic stimulation. We found that, even though arrhythmic stimulation delivered the same amount of physical light stimulation per second as rhythmic stimulation, it resulted in substantially reduced visual effects, including reduced perception of geometric patterns and visual dynamics. This may suggest that neural entrainment, elicited by rhythmic FLS, plays a significant role in the generation of simple visual hallucinations. The reduction in visual effects was most pronounced for the Arrhythmicpairs condition at 10 Hz, supporting its future use in investigations of the neuronal mechanisms underlying the flicker experience.

Assessment of phenomenology

To draw conclusions from phenomenological data, it is first important to establish whether the employed assessment tools provide an accurate representation of the subjective experience. Based on recent work [1,2,17], we designed and administered an abridged version of the novel SVES to assess FLS-induced visual effects. We found that ratings of simple visual hallucinations were similar across ASC-R and SVES measures. Given that the ASC-R is well validated [22], the parity of these results indicates construct validity of the SVES, which should be formally tested in future studies. Furthermore, larger effect sizes were found using SVES ratings compared to ASC-R ratings. This is likely because the SVES was designed to capture specific details and pattern subtypes within simple visual hallucinations while ASC-R targets an overarching altered state experience. By differentiating pattern subtypes, we found that some visual phenomena occur irrespective of frequency and rhythmicity (i.e., phosphene forms; see Fig 2C), thus the SVES preserves this information while the ASC-R only captures gross visual phenomena: “I saw patterns”. Indeed, recent studies did not find differences in ASC-R ratings of simple visual hallucinations at different FLS frequencies [2,3], even though the experience intensity was rated differently [3]. While the ASC-R questionnaire remains useful for comparisons across altered state induction methods [2,8], the SVES enables a more detailed assessment of visual hallucinatory characteristics.

Effects of frequency and rhythmicity on visual hallucinatory phenomena

First, we tested how the phenomenal characteristics of FLS-induced hallucinatory phenomena were affected by flicker frequency. We found that simple visual hallucinations, such as perception of Klüver forms, were experienced most intensely at 10 Hz rhythmic FLS. This not only confirms previous findings, where 10 Hz FLS was identified as generating the greatest hallucinatory effects [2,3,17,28], but offers an extension by distinguishing phenomenal components within the experience. For example, by differentiating simple visual hallucinations into patterns subtypes, we found that Klüver forms (i.e., grids, cobwebs, spirals, tunnels) were the most reported pattern subtype during rhythmic FLS at 8 Hz, 10 Hz and 18 Hz. Furthermore, we found that 10 Hz rhythmic stimulation elicited the most visually dynamic experiences. Visual dynamics encompass perceived motion and how much the experience changes over time. FLS-induced moving patterns have been previously documented [1,31,32]. We extend this by finding that flicker frequency had the largest effect on dynamics compared to all other FLS-induced subjective qualities, emphasising that it constitutes a highly relevant characteristic of FLS effects. Future studies could incorporate eye tracking sensors that monitor participant eye movements during the flicker experience to explore whether participants’ eye position and movement adds to variability in the subjective experience. Altogether, out of the tested frequencies, our results identify 10 Hz FLS as the frequency that induced the greatest perceptual changes.

Next, we investigated whether rhythmicity affected the phenomenal characteristics induced by FLS. We found that arrhythmicity significantly reduced simple visual hallucinations and visual dynamics. The relative reduction of effects was largest for Arrhythmicpairs at 10 Hz, compared to rhythmic 10 Hz. The use of paired flashes as a control condition was first utilised by Ffytche [28], where it was found that paired flashes led to significant decreases in occipitotemporal activity, measured via EEG, compared to periodic FLS. However, it should be noted that Ffytche did not include a phenomenal characterisation of the flicker conditions. Furthermore, we applied an arrhythmic version of the paired flash stimulation whereby inter-pair intervals were sampled from an exponential probability distribution. In case of higher frequencies (i.e., 18 Hz), flicker trains of higher frequency due to IFI randomization were more likely to occur than in the other conditions, with local frequency of up to 37 Hz (compare Fig 1). Due to the randomization of IFIs within one second, these trains were very short and interrupted by longer IFIs, making it unlikely that they were majorly driving the subjective experiences. Overall, we present considerable evidence to show that frequency-matched arrhythmic FLS reduces hallucinatory effects compared to rhythmic stimulation, underscoring the importance of rhythmicity in determining the intensity of FLS effects.

Simple visual patterns, such as those reported in our study, are also commonly reported following administration of a range of psychedelic drugs (serotonin-2A receptor agonists) [19,33]. Indeed, Klüver forms were first identified in the context of mescaline-induced hallucinations [15] and have also been reported in migraine aura [5] and Charles Bonnet Syndrome (i.e., reported mosaic patterns as a form of grid) [34]. Similarly, perceived motion of visual imagery also occurs in epileptic seizures [35] and during migraine aura [4]. The similarities in reports of simple visual hallucinations across aetiologically distinct origins (including FLS) indicate shared underlying neural mechanisms. Seminal computational modelling work suggests that the structure of simple visual hallucinations is to some extent determined by the neuronal architecture between the retina and brain [14,16,36,37]. Therefore, simple visual hallucinations may reveal the hidden architecture of visual areas of the brain. Future neurophysiology research can therefore draw upon research from various domains to formulate a better understanding of how hallucinatory phenomena are generated.

Aside from simple hallucinatory phenomena, there were small effects of rhythmicity and frequency on reports of complex visual hallucinations. Complex hallucinations involve the perception of realistic objects, scenes, and faces (i.e., containing semantic value). While previous reports found them to be more prevalent at 3 Hz stimulation [3], we found that ratings increased with frequency. Still, complex visual hallucinations remained relatively low throughout all FLS conditions and occurred to a lesser extent than simple hallucinations, which is in line with previous work [2]. This reinstates that FLS reliably induces simple visual hallucinations, while phenomena that involve semantically meaningful content occur only occasionally.

To explore what factors may influence the extent of experiencing complex hallucinations, we tested their occurrence in relation to the personality trait “absorptiveness”, following from previous work [2]. We found a positive correlation between absorptiveness and complex imagery ratings for 10 Hz rhythmic FLS. Absorptiveness positively correlates with hypnotisability [21,38], a term that precedes the recently introduced concept of “phenomenological control” [39], which describes one’s capacity to alter their subjective experience in order to meet expectations. This could suggest that participants with high absorptiveness experience more hallucinations due to expectation that they will occur. Interestingly, however, absorptiveness did not correlate with simple visual hallucinations. Following from this, predictive coding models suggest that altered hierarchical processing, more specifically prior distributions, on either lower or higher levels of the visual hierarchy relate to simple or complex hallucinations, respectively [4042]. In light of this distinction, it is plausible that personality traits like absorptiveness and phenomenological control influence hierarchically higher regions and thereby increase likelihood of experiencing complex hallucinations. Further research could expand the scope of assessing how inter-individual differences influence FLS-induced phenomenology and neural processing by measuring a wider variety of participant traits, such as phenomenological control.

Exploration of FLS-induced perception of colours

We explored the types of colours that participants reported during different levels of flicker frequency and rhythmicity. We aimed to decipher whether there were categorical shifts in the spectrum of perceived colours depending on the type of FLS. We found that increasing flicker frequency increased the chances of participants reporting the perception of white, light yellow and bright blue. Rhythmicity also influenced the selection of white, whereby rhythmic stimulation led to higher chances of perceiving white during 10 Hz stimulation. Previous research identified that colours are often experienced during rhythmic FLS [2,3], Ganzfeld stimulation [43], psychedelic drug-induced experiences [19,33], epileptic seizures [6] and Charles Bonnet Syndrome [34], however we are not aware of literature that has further classified the specific colours experienced or their respective proportions within an experience. The VES enables group-level quantification of each colour perceived during different FLS conditions. It is interesting to note that colour perception is only weakly modulated by frequency and rhythmicity, especially when compared against simple patterns and visual dynamics. This could indicate that hallucinatory colour perception arises in the lower levels of the visual pathway (e.g., from retinal stimulation; entoptic phenomena), while patterns and other hallucinatory phenomena depend on frequency and rhythmicity-dependent neural mechanisms, such as neural entrainment. These exploratory findings can be used to formulate hypotheses of flicker-induced colour perception in future studies.

Potential underlying neural mechanisms

Ultimately, it is of interest to explain phenomenal characteristics in relation to their underlying neural mechanisms. While there have been some neuroimaging studies of FLS [3,2628], the direct link between FLS-induced phenomena and neural activity is yet to be established. Currently, there are three main views addressing the neural mechanisms that lead to FLS-induced visual hallucinatory phenomena.

Firstly, it is likely that neural entrainment plays an important role. Haegens [44] defined entrainment as the phase alignment of existing brain oscillations to an external periodic stimulus, which continues for several cycles after stimulus termination. Previous EEG studies found that rhythmic flicker at alpha frequency increases neural entrainment at that frequency [3,12,4547]. Further, it was found that rhythmic flicker produced stronger phase locking than arrhythmic stimulation when presented with high light intensity at a stimulation frequency close to the individual’s dominant intrinsic frequency [4547]. In our study, we found the greatest differences in reported hallucinatory phenomena between Rhythmic and the Arrhythmicpairs control, which has more arrhythmicity than the normally distributed control, as determined by the RMSSD of IFIs (see Methods). This finding could suggest that the relationship between rhythmicity and visual effects exists as a continuum where the degree of arrhythmicity affects the extent to which effects are reduced. However, it is important to note that we did not directly assess the neural effects of arrhythmic stimulation. In this light, it should be considered that other mechanisms can also contribute to oscillatory activity, such as the superposition of event-related responses, which are evoked cortical responses to visual stimulation that add onto, but do not interact with, ongoing oscillations [48]. Moreover, a recent study using rhythmic flickering checkerboards found evidence for both frequency-specific neural responses, supporting the entrainment model, and frequency-independent resonance phenomena, supporting the superposition model [49]. As we found that intensity of FLS effects was affected by frequency, it is likely that underlying frequency-specific neural responses, such as entrainment, contribute to FLS effects. However, future EEG research is necessary to test whether FLS-induced neural responses satisfy the criteria for entrainment [44] and if there is markedly less entrainment elicited by arrhythmic conditions. In doing so, evidence can be provided to determine whether neural entrainment mediates the effect of frequency and rhythmicity on the flicker-induced subjective experience.

Secondly, the Ermentrout-Cowan model proposes that the perception of Klüver forms corresponds to self-organised striped cortical activity in the primary visual cortex (V1) [14,16,17,50]. Due to the nonlinear transformation of retinal to cortical coordinates, the model demonstrates that striped activation in V1 translates into spirals, tunnels and other Klüver forms when mapped onto retinal coordinates. The model incorporates anatomical knowledge of the visual cortex, such as the size of V1 hypercolumns, their lateral inhibitory connections and orientation selectivity [14,16]. Nevertheless, it cannot explain the entirety of reported simple visual hallucinations as there are other pattern types and characteristics that it does not account for. For example, Ffytche [28] found that FLS led to increased V4 activity, which may correspond to perceived colour or motion of patterns. Still, our findings lend some support to the model as we found that Klüver forms are reported to a greater degree than other patterns subtypes, reinforcing their relevance in the study of simple visual hallucinations. Moreover, as expected, flicker frequency and rhythmicity did not influence whether phosphene forms were perceived. This supports the notion that phosphene forms, such as TV snow and blobs, are generated from retinal stimulation with a bright light source [17] as all frequency and rhythmicity levels produced the same amount of light input (300ms of light stimulation per second). The additional patterns, such as Klüver forms, are more likely to be a result of frequency-dependent modulation of neural activity via periodic light stimulation.

Finally, one can look to models developed in other domains to inform predictions of how FLS-induced phenomena could arise. The cortico-striato-thalamo-cortical (CSTC) model proposes that drug- and pathology-induced hallucinations are associated with aberrant modulation of thalamus activity leading to thalamocortical dysconnectivity [51,52]. This is supported by studies that found drug-induced alterations in sensory perception to be positively correlated with the functional connectivity between thalamus and primary sensory cortices [53]. Such increased thalamocortical functional connectivity has also been found during flicker-induced hallucinations [28] and psychosis [5456]. Further, thalamocortical dysconnectivity has been implicated in Ganzfeld-induced altered states [57] and thalamocortical functional and structural dysconnectivity is present in patients with epilepsy [5860] and migraine [6163]. These findings hint that thalamocortical dysconnectivity may also play a key role in FLS-induced effects.

It is likely that the three views are not mutually exclusive but that their proposed mechanisms interact or influence each other at different levels. For example, thalamocortical dysconnectivity may arise from neural entrainment at specific frequencies. Furthermore, the Ermentrout-Cowan model may explain specific properties of the visual experience, such as perception of Klüver forms, but requires additional inputs from other models to encapsulate the entire phenomenal experience. The aim of future research should be to tie together the interacting mechanisms in order to formulate an overarching model of how simple visual hallucinations are generated in the brain.

Outlook

Here, we have presented the effects of flicker light rhythmicity and frequency on aspects of the flicker-induced experience, such as simple visual hallucinations, visual dynamics and perceived colours. The applied SVES generated similar ratings to the ASC-R, but with larger effect sizes, which suggests that SVES can capture the FLS-induced phenomenology with a higher level of detail. Further, we found that flicker arrhythmicity significantly reduced visual effects, which implies that neural entrainment may be critical to the generation of simple visual hallucinations. From two frequency-matched arrhythmic control conditions, we identified the Arrhythmicpairs condition as most effective in reducing simple visual hallucinations when compared against rhythmic stimulation, especially at 10 Hz. Using this, future neuroimaging studies can investigate the neural mechanisms that mediate the effects of rhythmicity on the flicker-induced hallucinatory experience.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Tables for full report of Tukey test results.

(PDF)

S1 Appendix. Abridged version of Stroboscopic Visual Experience Survey.

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Tom Galea, Jay Conlon and Lumenate Growth Inc. for the helpful discussions and generous provision of experimental hardware.

Data Availability

All questionnaire data files are available on the OSF data repository (URL: https://osf.io/5d29g/).

Funding Statement

The investigator-initiated study was financially supported by a donation from Lumenate Growth Ltd to Freie Universität Berlin allocated to TTS. There was no additional external funding received for this study.

References

  • 1.Allefeld C, Pütz P, Kastner K, Wackermann J. Flicker-light induced visual phenomena: Frequency dependence and specificity of whole percepts and percept features. Conscious Cogn. 2011;20: 1344–1362. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.10.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Bartossek MT, Kemmerer J, Schmidt TT. Altered states phenomena induced by visual flicker light stimulation. Plos One. 2021;16: e0253779. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0253779 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Schwartzman DJ, Schartner M, Ador BB, Simonelli F, Chang AY-C, Seth AK. Increased spontaneous EEG signal diversity during stroboscopically-induced altered states of consciousness. Biorxiv. 2019; 511766. doi: 10.1101/511766 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Schott GD. Exploring the visual hallucinations of migraine aura: the tacit contribution of illustration. Brain. 2007;130: 1690–1703. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl348 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Cowan JD. Visual Hallucinations and Migraine Aura. Encyclopedia of Computational Neuroscience. 2013; 1–11. doi: [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Panayiotopoulos CP. Elementary visual hallucinations in migraine and epilepsy. J Neurology Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1994;57: 1371. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.57.11.1371 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Jan T, Castillo J del. Visual Hallucinations: Charles Bonnet Syndrome. West J Emerg Medicine Integrating Emerg Care Popul Heal. 2012;13: 544–547. doi: 10.5811/westjem.2012.7.12891 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Schmidt TT, Berkemeyer H. The Altered States Database: Psychometric Data of Altered States of Consciousness. Front Psychol. 2018;9: 1028. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Rogers S, Keogh R, Pearson J. Hallucinations on demand: the utility of experimentally induced phenomena in hallucination research. Philosophical Transactions Royal Soc B. 2021;376: 20200233. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2020.0233 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Purkinje von J. Beiträge zur Kenntniss des Sehens in subjectiver Hinsicht. Prag: In Commission bei Johann Gottfried Calve; 1819. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Meulen BC ter, Tavy D, Jacobs BC. From Stroboscope to Dream Machine: A History of Flicker-Induced Hallucinations. Eur Neurol. 2009;62: 316–320. doi: 10.1159/000235945 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Adrian ED, Matthews BHC. The Berger Rhythm: Potential Changes from the Occipital Lobes in Man. Brain. 1934;57: 355–385. doi: 10.1093/brain/57.4.355 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Ffytche DH. Visual hallucinations and the charles bonnet syndrome. Curr Psychiat Rep. 2005;7: 168–179. doi: 10.1007/s11920-005-0050-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Ermentrout GB, Cowan JD. A mathematical theory of visual hallucination patterns. Biol Cybern. 1979;34: 137–150. doi: 10.1007/BF00336965 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Klüver H. Mescal and Mechanisms of Hallucinations. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1966. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Bressloff PC, Cowan JD, Golubitsky M, Thomas PJ, Wiener MC. What Geometric Visual Hallucinations Tell Us about the Visual Cortex. Neural Comput. 2002;14: 473–491. doi: 10.1162/089976602317250861 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Billock VA, Tsou BH. Elementary Visual Hallucinations and Their Relationships to Neural Pattern-Forming Mechanisms. Psychol Bull. 2012;138: 744–774. doi: 10.1037/a0027580 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Richards W. The Fortification Illusions of Migraines. Sci Am. 1971;224: 88–96. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican0571-88 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Lawrence DW, Carhart-Harris R, Griffiths R, Timmermann C. Phenomenology and content of the inhaled N, N-dimethyltryptamine (N, N-DMT) experience. Sci Rep-uk. 2022;12: 8562. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-11999-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Billock VA, Tsou BH. Neural interactions between flicker-induced self-organized visual hallucinations and physical stimuli. Proc National Acad Sci. 2007;104: 8490–8495. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0610813104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Tellegen A, Atkinson G. Openness to absorbing and self-altering experiences (“absorption”), a trait related to hypnotic susceptibility. J Abnorm Psychol. 1974;83: 268–277. doi: 10.1037/h0036681 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Keogh R, Pearson J. The blind mind: No sensory visual imagery in aphantasia. Cortex. 2018;105: 53–60. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.10.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Königsmark VT, Bergmann J, Reeder RR. The Ganzflicker experience: High probability of seeing vivid and complex pseudo-hallucinations with imagery but not aphantasia. Cortex. 2021;141: 522–534. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2021.05.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Dittrich A. The Standardized Psychometric Assessment of Altered States of Consciousness (ASCs) in Humans. Pharmacopsychiatry. 1998;31: 80–84. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-979351 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Pekala RJ. Quantifying Consciousness, An Empirical Approach. 1991; 127–143. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4899-0629-8_8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Becker C, Gramann K, Müller HJ, Elliott MA. Electrophysiological correlates of flicker-induced color hallucinations. Conscious Cogn. 2009;18: 266–276. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Herrmann CS. Human EEG responses to 1–100 Hz flicker: resonance phenomena in visual cortex and their potential correlation to cognitive phenomena. Exp Brain Res. 2001;137: 346–353. doi: 10.1007/s002210100682 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Ffytche DH. The hodology of hallucinations. Cortex. 2008;44: 1067–1083. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.04.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Shaffer F, Ginsberg JP. An Overview of Heart Rate Variability Metrics and Norms. Frontiers Public Heal. 2017;5: 258. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2017.00258 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Studerus E, Gamma A, Vollenweider FX. Psychometric Evaluation of the Altered States of Consciousness Rating Scale (OAV). Plos One. 2010;5: e12412. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012412 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Smythies JR. The Stroboscopic Patterns. Brit J Psychol. 1960;51: 247–255. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1960.tb00747.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Herrmann CS, Elliott MA. Fechner’s colors are induced by flickering monochromatic light. In: Sommerfeld E, Kompass R, Lachmann T, editors. Pabst Science Publishers; 2001. pp. 427–431. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Zamberlan F, Sanz C, Vivot RM, Pallavicini C, Erowid F, Erowid E, et al. The Varieties of the Psychedelic Experience: A Preliminary Study of the Association Between the Reported Subjective Effects and the Binding Affinity Profiles of Substituted Phenethylamines and Tryptamines. Frontiers Integr Neurosci. 2018;12: 54. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2018.00054 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Vacchiano V, Tonon C, Mitolo M, Evangelisti S, Carbonelli M, Liguori R, et al. Functional MRI study in a case of Charles Bonnet syndrome related to LHON. Bmc Neurol. 2019;19: 350. doi: 10.1186/s12883-019-1579-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Wilkinson F. Auras and other hallucinations: windows on the visual brain. Prog Brain Res. 2004;144: 305–320. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(03)14421-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Butler TC, Benayoun M, Wallace E, Drongelen W van, Goldenfeld N, Cowan J. Evolutionary constraints on visual cortex architecture from the dynamics of hallucinations. Proc National Acad Sci. 2012;109: 606–609. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1118672109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Rule M, Stoffregen M, Ermentrout B. A Model for the Origin and Properties of Flicker-Induced Geometric Phosphenes. Plos Comput Biol. 2011;7: e1002158. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002158 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Glisky ML, Tataryn DJ, Tobias BA, Kihlstrom JF, McConkey KM. Absorption, openness to experience, and hypnotizability. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1991;60: 263–272. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.60.2.263 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Lush P, Scott RB, Seth AK, Dienes Z. The Phenomenological Control Scale: Measuring the Capacity for Creating Illusory Nonvolition, Hallucination and Delusion. Collabra Psychology. 2021;7. doi: 10.1525/collabra.29542 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Suzuki K, Roseboom W, Schwartzman DJ, Seth AK. A Deep-Dream Virtual Reality Platform for Studying Altered Perceptual Phenomenology. Sci Rep-uk. 2017;7: 15982. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-16316-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Corlett PR, Horga G, Fletcher PC, Alderson-Day B, Schmack K, Powers AR. Hallucinations and Strong Priors. Trends Cogn Sci. 2019;23: 114–127. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.12.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Reichert DP, Seriès P, Storkey AJ. Charles Bonnet Syndrome: Evidence for a Generative Model in the Cortex? Plos Comput Biol. 2013;9: e1003134. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003134 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Schmidt TT, Prein JC. The Ganzfeld experience—A stably inducible altered state of consciousness: Effects of different auditory homogenizations. Psych J. 2019;8: 66–81. doi: 10.1002/pchj.262 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Haegens S. Entrainment revisited: a commentary on Meyer, Sun, and Martin (2020). Lang Cognition Neurosci. 2020;35: 1119–1123. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2020.1758335 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Mathewson KE, Prudhomme C, Fabiani M, Beck DM, Lleras A, Gratton G. Making Waves in the Stream of Consciousness: Entraining Oscillations in EEG Alpha and Fluctuations in Visual Awareness with Rhythmic Visual Stimulation. J Cognitive Neurosci. 2012;24: 2321–2333. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00288 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Notbohm A, Herrmann CS. Flicker Regularity Is Crucial for Entrainment of Alpha Oscillations. Front Hum Neurosci. 2016;10: 503. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00503 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Notbohm A, Kurths J, Herrmann CS. Modification of Brain Oscillations via Rhythmic Light Stimulation Provides Evidence for Entrainment but Not for Superposition of Event-Related Responses. Front Hum Neurosci. 2016;10: 10. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Capilla A, Pazo-Alvarez P, Darriba A, Campo P, Gross J. Steady-State Visual Evoked Potentials Can Be Explained by Temporal Superposition of Transient Event-Related Responses. Plos One. 2011;6: e14543. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0014543 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Nuttall R, Jäger C, Zimmermann J, Archila-Melendez ME, Preibisch C, Taylor P, et al. Evoked responses to rhythmic visual stimulation vary across sources of intrinsic alpha activity in humans. Sci Rep-uk. 2022;12: 5986. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-09922-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Ermentrout GB, Billock VA. Flicker-Induced Phosphenes. Encyclopedia of Computational Neuroscience. 2018; 1–6. doi: [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Vollenweider FX, Geyer MA. A systems model of altered consciousness: integrating natural and drug-induced psychoses. Brain Res Bull. 2001;56: 495–507. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(01)00646-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Geyer MA, Vollenweider FX. Serotonin research: contributions to understanding psychoses. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2008;29: 445–453. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2008.06.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Müller F, Lenz C, Dolder P, Lang U, Schmidt A, Liechti M, et al. Increased thalamic resting‐state connectivity as a core driver of LSD‐induced hallucinations. Acta Psychiat Scand. 2017;136: 648–657. doi: 10.1111/acps.12818 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Ramsay IS. An Activation Likelihood Estimate Meta-analysis of Thalamocortical Dysconnectivity in Psychosis. Biological Psychiatry Cognitive Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2019;4: 859–869. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2019.04.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Avram M, Rogg H, Korda A, Andreou C, Müller F, Borgwardt S. Bridging the Gap? Altered Thalamocortical Connectivity in Psychotic and Psychedelic States. Frontiers Psychiatry. 2021;12: 706017. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.706017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Sheffield JM, Huang AS, Rogers BP, Giraldo-Chica M, Landman BA, Blackford JU, et al. Thalamocortical Anatomical Connectivity in Schizophrenia and Psychotic Bipolar Disorder. Schizophrenia Bull. 2020;46: 1062–1071. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbaa022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Schmidt TT, Jagannathan N, Ljubljanac M, Xavier A, Nierhaus T. The multimodal Ganzfeld-induced altered state of consciousness induces decreased thalamo-cortical coupling. Sci Rep-uk. 2020;10: 18686. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-75019-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Chen Y, Fallon N, Kreilkamp BAK, Denby C, Bracewell M, Das K, et al. Probabilistic mapping of thalamic nuclei and thalamocortical functional connectivity in idiopathic generalised epilepsy. Hum Brain Mapp. 2021;42: 5648–5664. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25644 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Wang Z, Zhang Z, Jiao Q, Liao W, Chen G, Sun K, et al. Impairments of Thalamic Nuclei in Idiopathic Generalized Epilepsy Revealed by a Study Combining Morphological and Functional Connectivity MRI. Plos One. 2012;7: e39701. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0039701 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Kim JB, Suh S, Seo W, Oh K, Koh S, Kim JH. Altered thalamocortical functional connectivity in idiopathic generalized epilepsy. Epilepsia. 2014;55: 592–600. doi: 10.1111/epi.12580 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Bolay H. Thalamocortical Network Interruption: A Fresh View for Migraine Symptoms. Turk J Med Sci. 2020;50: 1651–1654. doi: 10.3906/sag-2005-21 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Tu Y, Fu Z, Zeng F, Maleki N, Lan L, Li Z, et al. Abnormal thalamocortical network dynamics in migraine. Neurology. 2019;92: e2706–e2716. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000007607 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Martinelli D, Castellazzi G, Icco RD, Bacila A, Allena M, Faggioli A, et al. Thalamocortical Connectivity in Experimentally-Induced Migraine Attacks: A Pilot Study. Brain Sci. 2021;11: 165. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11020165 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Manuel Spitschan

26 Jan 2023

PONE-D-22-33362Effect of frequency and rhythmicity on flicker light-induced hallucinatory phenomenaPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Schmidt,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

==============================

Please address all reviewers' comments.

==============================

Please submit your revised manuscript by Mar 12 2023 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Manuel Spitschan

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. We note that the grant information you provided in the ‘Funding Information’ and ‘Financial Disclosure’ sections do not match. 

When you resubmit, please ensure that you provide the correct grant numbers for the awards you received for your study in the ‘Funding Information’ section.

3. Thank you for stating in your Funding Statement: 

"The investigator-initiated study was financially supported by a donation from Lumenate Growth Ltd to TTS."

Please provide an amended statement that declares *all* the funding or sources of support (whether external or internal to your organization) received during this study, as detailed online in our guide for authors at http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submit-now.  Please also include the statement “There was no additional external funding received for this study.” in your updated Funding Statement. 

Please include your amended Funding Statement within your cover letter. We will change the online submission form on your behalf.

4. Thank you for stating the following in the Competing Interests section:

"I have read the journal's policy and the authors of this manuscript have

the following competing interests:

TTS: This research was supported by a unrestricted donation from Lumenate

Growth ltd to Freie Universität Berlin allocated to TTS."

We note that you received funding from a commercial source: Lumenate Growth ltd 

Please provide an amended Competing Interests Statement that explicitly states this commercial funder, along with any other relevant declarations relating to employment, consultancy, patents, products in development, marketed products, etc. 

Within this Competing Interests Statement, please confirm that this does not alter your adherence to all PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials by including the following statement: ""This does not alter our adherence to PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials.” (as detailed online in our guide for authors http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/competing-interests).  If there are restrictions on sharing of data and/or materials, please state these. Please note that we cannot proceed with consideration of your article until this information has been declared. 

Please include your amended Competing Interests Statement within your cover letter. We will change the online submission form on your behalf.

5. We note that Figures 1 and S1 Appendix in your submission contain copyrighted images. All PLOS content is published under the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY 4.0), which means that the manuscript, images, and Supporting Information files will be freely available online, and any third party is permitted to access, download, copy, distribute, and use these materials in any way, even commercially, with proper attribution. For more information, see our copyright guidelines: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/licenses-and-copyright.

We require you to either (1) present written permission from the copyright holder to publish these figures specifically under the CC BY 4.0 license, or (2) remove the figures from your submission:

a. You may seek permission from the original copyright holder of Figures 1 and S1 Appendix to publish the content specifically under the CC BY 4.0 license. 

We recommend that you contact the original copyright holder with the Content Permission Form (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=7c09/content-permission-form.pdf) and the following text:

“I request permission for the open-access journal PLOS ONE to publish XXX under the Creative Commons Attribution License (CCAL) CC BY 4.0 (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/). Please be aware that this license allows unrestricted use and distribution, even commercially, by third parties. Please reply and provide explicit written permission to publish XXX under a CC BY license and complete the attached form.”

Please upload the completed Content Permission Form or other proof of granted permissions as an ""Other"" file with your submission. 

In the figure caption of the copyrighted figure, please include the following text: “Reprinted from [ref] under a CC BY license, with permission from [name of publisher], original copyright [original copyright year].”

b. If you are unable to obtain permission from the original copyright holder to publish these figures under the CC BY 4.0 license or if the copyright holder’s requirements are incompatible with the CC BY 4.0 license, please either i) remove the figure or ii) supply a replacement figure that complies with the CC BY 4.0 license. Please check copyright information on all replacement figures and update the figure caption with source information. If applicable, please specify in the figure caption text when a figure is similar but not identical to the original image and is therefore for illustrative purposes only.

6. We note that Figure S1 Appendix includes an image of a participant in the study. 

As per the PLOS ONE policy (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-human-subjects-research) on papers that include identifying, or potentially identifying, information, the individual(s) or parent(s)/guardian(s) must be informed of the terms of the PLOS open-access (CC-BY) license and provide specific permission for publication of these details under the terms of this license. Please download the Consent Form for Publication in a PLOS Journal (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=8ce6/plos-consent-form-english.pdf). The signed consent form should not be submitted with the manuscript, but should be securely filed in the individual's case notes. Please amend the methods section and ethics statement of the manuscript to explicitly state that the patient/participant has provided consent for publication: “The individual in this manuscript has given written informed consent (as outlined in PLOS consent form) to publish these case details”. 

If you are unable to obtain consent from the subject of the photograph, you will need to remove the figure and any other textual identifying information or case descriptions for this individual.

7. Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Review of “Effect of frequency and rhythmicity on flicker light-induced hallucinatory phenomena”.

The authors report an experiment investigating the frequency and rhythmicity of flickering light on simple visual hallucinations. They found the experience of visual hallucinations to be maximal at 10 Hz and that rhythmic light resulted in stronger hallucinations than an arrhythmic control condition.

Overall I find no major flaws in the study. This is a very interesting topic and an important step towards understanding the phenomena.

The following should be considered minor revisions/suggestions.

Some additional information on the experimental setup should be added to the methods section. There were 12 LEDs, presumably these were white LEDs? This should be stated for clarity.

What pattern were the LEDs positioned in, e.g. in a circle, a grid? How far apart?

Importantly, the visual angle of the light source should be reported because peripheral flickering light is perceived differently from flickering light on the fovea. Although the light is diffuse because the eyes are closed, the visual angle of the light source could be a factor on the resulting perceptions. This can simply be calculated from the distance and width of the light source.

A potential factor which should be considered is the position of the eyes when eyelids are closed. In my experience participants sometimes report moving their eyes to look away from a bright light sources with eyes closed, as this reduces the unpleasantness of bright light. Furthermore, it is known that it is difficult to maintain a consistent position of the eye with eyes closed, so the position might change over time. This is not a critical problem for the current study because there is no reason to suspect that the eye position was different across control conditions, but it should be mentioned in the discussion as a potential source of variability in the subjective experience.

A second control condition using paired arrhythmic stimulation was used. It is not clear why an additional control condition is needed, when the arrhythmic condition controls for rhythmicity. Perhaps the authors could explain the decision to add an extra control condition in more detail in the introduction. For example, do the authors hypothesize that the decreases in occipitotemporal EEG activity found by Ffytche is not present in the normal arrhythmic control condition? The authors report that the paired arrhythmic stimulation can be considered more arrhythmic, it would be good if they could elaborate on why this is the case. Is it simply a greater variation in inter flash intervals? Do the authors have any hypothesis as to why the reduction in visual effects was most pronounced for the arrhythmic pairs condition?

The authors discuss the possibility of neural entrainment being responsible for the generation of simple visual hallucinations. While I agree that neural entrainment is a possible (even likely) explanation, there is a debate in the EEG literature as to whether the frequency specific neural responses to visual flicker (SSVEPs) are the result of entrainment of ongoing oscillations, or a resonance phenomena where the cortex has a preferred frequency of stimulation. For example Capilla et. al. (2011) argued SSVEPs can be explained as a superposition of transient responses to the visual stimulation. In a recent study we have shown that different cortical sources of alpha oscillations show very different steady state responses to visual flicker, which vary depending on the distance of the flicker frequency and the individual’s alpha frequency (Nuttall et. al. 2022). In the current study the authors have shown that rhythmicity is important for the hallucinatory visual effect, so an interaction between neural oscillations and the flicker is likely, but entrainment is only one possible explanation. Distinguishing between entrainment and resonance is difficult and the exact definition of entrainment is not always clearly defined, see Haegens (2020) for a good discussion. It is quite possible that different brain areas respond to rhythmic stimulation in a variety of ways, with some showing entrainment of an ongoing oscillation, and some simply responding preferentially to an optimal frequency of rhythmic input. I appreciate that a full discussion of these issues is beyond the scope of the current article, but it would be good to acknowledge the possibility of different neural mechanisms which might explain this effect.

James Dowsett

References:

Capilla A, Pazo-Alvarez P, Darriba A, Campo P, Gross J (2011) Steady-State Visual Evoked Potentials Can Be Explained by Temporal Superposition of Transient Event-Related Responses. PLoS ONE 6(1): e14543.

Nuttall, R., Jäger, C., Zimmermann, J., Archila-Melendez, M. E., Preibisch, C., Taylor, P., ... & Dowsett, J. (2022). Evoked responses to rhythmic visual stimulation vary across sources of intrinsic alpha activity in humans. Scientific reports

Saskia Haegens (2020): Entrainment revisited: a commentary on Meyer, Sun,

and Martin (2020), Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, DOI: 10.1080/23273798.2020.1758335

Reviewer #2: This paper examines – as summarised in the title – the effect of frequency and rhythmicity on various flicker-induced visual hallucinations. As described in the paper, 10-Hz visual stimulation has been reported to lead to strongest hallucinations (as compared to other stimulation rates), but the effect of rhythmicity remained unclear due to the lack of well-designed arrhythmic control conditions. Such conditions, along with novel questionnaires, were used in the current study. The authors were able to confirm the advantage of 10-Hz flicker (among several other rates) to induce visual hallucinations. In addition, they show that, in particular for 10 Hz, rhythmic stimulation leads to stronger hallucinatory effects than arrhythmic control stimulation.

This paper is very well written and the rationale is easy to follow. I cannot see any major issues with this work and believe that this is a well-designed and executed study. I do have two minor comments:

- I was a bit confused about arrhythmic pairs control condition. The rate (frequency) of paired flashes always seems faster than in the corresponding rhythmic condition. Given the effect of rate on perception, I wonder if this can lead to an apparent effect of rhythmicity that is due to this difference in “local” frequency/rate.

- For some statistical tests, the authors report a reliable interaction of rhythmicity and frequency. In some cases (e.g., page 13), it remains unclear which combination of conditions has produced the interaction. This could be described in more detail, in particular as main effects can be difficult to interpret in the presence of such an interaction.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: James Dowsett

Reviewer #2: Yes: Benedikt Zoefel

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2023 Apr 11;18(4):e0284271. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0284271.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


9 Mar 2023

Reviewer 1: We thank you for the positive reception of our manuscript and for the helpful comments, suggestions, and additional literature. We have incorporated all your comments to the revised manuscript.

Reviewer 2: We thank you for the positive feedback and constructive comments for improving our manuscript. We have incorporated all your points to the revised manuscript.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Manuel Spitschan

29 Mar 2023

Effect of frequency and rhythmicity on flicker light-induced hallucinatory phenomena

PONE-D-22-33362R1

Dear Dr. Schmidt,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Manuel Spitschan

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: James Dowsett

Reviewer #2: Yes: Benedikt Zoefel

**********

Acceptance letter

Manuel Spitschan

3 Apr 2023

PONE-D-22-33362R1

Effect of frequency and rhythmicity on flicker light-induced hallucinatory phenomena

Dear Dr. Schmidt:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Manuel Spitschan

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Table. Tables for full report of Tukey test results.

    (PDF)

    S1 Appendix. Abridged version of Stroboscopic Visual Experience Survey.

    (PDF)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All questionnaire data files are available on the OSF data repository (URL: https://osf.io/5d29g/).


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES