Skip to main content
Movement Disorders Clinical Practice logoLink to Movement Disorders Clinical Practice
. 2023 Feb 13;10(4):617–624. doi: 10.1002/mdc3.13684

Phantom Boarder Relates to Experimentally‐Induced Presence Hallucinations in Parkinson's Disease

Olaf Blanke 1,2,, Fosco Bernasconi 1, Jevita Potheegadoo 1
PMCID: PMC10105112  PMID: 37070043

Abstract

Background

Phantom boarder (PB) is the sensation that someone uninvited is in the patient's home despite evidence to the contrary. It is mostly reported by patients with neurodegenerative disorders such as Alzheimer's disease, dementia with Lewy bodies or Parkinson's disease (PD). Presence hallucination (PH) is frequent in neurodegenerative disease, shares several aspects with PB, and is the sensation that someone is nearby, behind or next to the patient (when nobody is actually there). Recent work developed a sensorimotor method to robotically induce PH (robot‐induced PH, riPH) and demonstrated that a subgroup of PD patients showed abnormal sensitivity for riPH.

Objective

We investigated if PD patients with PB (PD‐PB) would (1) show elevated sensitivity for riPH that (2) is comparable to that of patients reporting PH, but not PB (PD‐PH).

Methods

We studied the sensitivity of non‐demented PD patients in a sensorimotor stimulation paradigm, during which three groups of patients (PD‐PB; PD‐PH; PD patients without hallucinations, PD‐nPH) were exposed to different conditions of conflicting sensorimotor stimulation.

Results

We show that PD‐PB and PD‐PH groups had a higher sensitivity to riPH (compared to PD‐nPH). PD‐PB and PD‐PH groups did not differ in riPH sensitivity. Together with interview data, these behavioral data on riPH show that PB is associated with PH, suggesting that both share some underlying brain mechanisms, although interview data also revealed phenomenological differences.

Conclusions

Because PD‐PB patients did not suffer from dementia nor delusions, we argue that these shared mechanisms are of perceptual‐hallucinatory nature, involving sensorimotor signals and their integration.

Keywords: Parkinson's disease psychosis, sensed presences, sensorimotor processing


Patients with phantom boarder phenomenon (PB) report that someone uninvited has entered or lives in the patient's home, despite of evidence to the contrary. 1 The unsolicited visitor is mostly experienced by patients as an unfamiliar intruder with malevolent intentions (e.g., harm or rob the patient) or hassling behaviors (e.g., make noise), although the visitor may also be experienced as a friend or family member. 1 , 2 , 3 , 4 PB is clinically relevant as it occurs repeatedly, and is a compelling experience, which is often destabilizing for patients, caregivers, and their relationship. Furthermore, PB has been associated with earlier home placement and delirium. 5 PB is prevalent in several neurodegenerative diseases associated with dementia, such as dementia with Lewy bodies (DLB), 6 , 7 Alzheimer's disease (AD), 8 , 9 vascular dementia 10 and Parkinson's disease (PD). 6 PB is often described as the most common “delusion” (together with paranoid ideation) in such diseases, with Aarsland et al. 6 reporting PB in 41% of DLB and 17% of PD dementia (PDD) patients.

Despite this clinical relevance, understanding of the involved brain mechanisms of PB remains limited and its “exact nosology (…) debatable”. 11 In his initial clinical description, Rowan 1 described PB as a delusional symptom and subsequent authors classified PB among delusional misidentification syndromes such as Capgras syndrome, mirror or television misidentification. 9 , 10 , 12 Others have considered PB as a complex hallucination, based on the observation that in some cases the PB is also heard or seen. 8 , 10 , 13 Either classification (hallucination, delusional misidentification) has received some support from the results of symptom cluster analysis. 7 , 8 , 14 These studies and findings are sparse, however, and further complicated by the fact that research was carried out in patients with different neurodegenerative diseases. Thus, Ballard 8 tested patients with dementia of different etiologies and linked PB with complex visual hallucinations (their Factor 1), whereas Nagahama et al. 7 , 14 studied DLB patients and associated PB with delusional misidentification (their Factor 1). PB is further complicated by the importance of additional psychological and social factors. Thus, fear and anxiety, 1 , 8 , 10 social isolation, lack of interpersonal relationships and personal loss 15 have been reported in patients with PB. Although most patients with PB suffer from dementia, past work showed that PB is independent of the degree of severity of the associated cognitive deficits objectified by neuropsychological evaluation 8 , 16 and can even be observed in patients without any clinical signs of dementia. 1 , 3 Finally, regarding the neural correlates of PB, a SPECT (single photon emission computed tomography) study of a large group DLB patients 14 showed involvement of a network consisting of insula, inferior frontal gyrus, hippocampus, and the striatum.

Presence hallucination (PH) defined as the vivid sensation that somebody is nearby when no one is actually there and can neither be seen or heard. 17 PH shares several clinical aspects with PB and yet, PH has not been considered in relation to PB and their potential relationship has not been investigated experimentally. PH is grouped among minor hallucinations (together with passage hallucinations and visual illusions) 18 and is frequent in PD patients. 17 , 19 PH may occur in recurrent fashion, affecting about half of patients, 20 , 21 and often precedes the onset of structured visual hallucinations. 22 , 23 PH also occurs in elderly healthy individuals, 24 in extreme conditions (sailors, explorers or extreme mountain climbers), 25 , 26 psychiatric illnesses, 27 , 28 and neurological cases and illnesses other than PD (epilepsy, DLB). 29 , 30 , 31 These presences observed in those different populations share phenomenological aspects with the PH in PD, as subjects describe feeling someone close to the body, behind, above their shoulder or to one side. PB and PH share other characteristics: in PB the uninvited intruder or person is often an unfamiliar person as is most often the case for the presence in PH. 17 , 32 , 33 In PH, the sensed presence often appears at home, which is a defining feature of PB. Typically, the sensed presence in PH cannot be seen or heard, again as the phantom boarder, and both are frequently reported by patients with DLB and PD. 29 , 34 Although at first sight, PH and PB may share features with extracampine hallucinations (EH) and include the hallucination of a person in one's home, PH and PB should be distinguished from EH. EH were first described by Eugen Bleuler 35 and defined as hallucinations outside a given sensory modality's perceptual field: thus outside the visual fields in the case of a visual EH (see also Sato & Berrios). 36 Concerning PH and PB, one important difference with EH is that they are seen by the patient in the case of visual EH (i.e., case 1, case 5 from Bleuler 35 ; heard in the case of an auditory EH). This is not the case for PH 37 or PB, 1 which are neither seen, heard nor felt (if, in rare instances, they are heard or felt, they still differ, because such cases of PH are not extracampine, but intracampine hallucinations). Therefore, EH that are seen or heard or felt, are a broader class of hallucinations (visual, tactile, auditory) and, as already described by Bleuler, 35 are not specific for people, beings, or animals, as for PH and PB. Based on these clinical similarities between PH and PB and the previous link of PB to hallucinations, 8 , 10 , 13 we here studied whether PB is associated with PH by exposing a group of PD patients with PB to a sensorimotor stimulation protocol that has been shown to induce experimentally‐controlled or robot‐induced PH (riPH) 38 in healthy participants 37 and in PD patients. 32 The sensitivity of PD patients with PB (PD‐PB) to riPH was tested behaviorally and compared with those of two other groups of PD patients (patients with PH but no PB: PD‐PH; patients without PB nor PH: PD‐nPH). Based on the assumption that PB and PH share common mechanisms, we predicted that PD‐PB patients would show similar sensitivity to riPH as described previously in PD‐PH patients 32 and elevated sensitivity compared to PD patients without hallucinations (PD‐nPH).

Materials and Methods

Participants

In a group of PD patients that was recruited for a previous study, 32 we retrospectively searched for patients with PB (PD‐PB, N = 4; 16.7%) and compared them with two groups of PD patients without PB: patients with PH, but without PB (PD‐PH, N = 7; 29.2%) and patients with neither PB nor PH (PD‐nPH, N = 13; 54.2%). Three of the PD‐PB patients had both PB and PH and only one patient had PB without PH.

All patients gave written informed consent prior to participating in the study approved by the local ethics committee. Diagnosis of PD was made by trained neurologists. Patients were not suffering from other neurological disorders, had neither psychiatric illnesses nor substance abuse disorders. The study procedures were administered in the following order: cognitive examination, experimental robotic paradigm, neuropsychiatric assessment and debriefing session. All patients were tested on their regular pharmacological treatment and at similar time of the day.

Clinical Assessment

Global cognitive functioning and screening for dementia were carried out by means of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA). 39 Motor symptoms severity of PD were assessed by trained neurologists using the Part III of the Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale (MDS‐UPDRS). 40 The frequency and phenomenology of PH and other hallucinations, PB, and delusions were obtained through a semi‐structured interview conducted by a trained neuropsychologist. We used the Prodromal Questionnaire (PQ‐16) 41 to assess the occurrence of psychotic prodromal signs (Part 1) including hallucinations and delusional ideas, and the level of distress associated to such signs (Part 2).

Robot‐Induced PH Paradigm

The patients were seated and equipped with a blindfold and headphones delivering constant white noise to isolate them from external stimulations and distractions. As described in Bernasconi et al. 38 participants were instructed to perform back and forth movements with their most affected hand by means of a robotic device (Geomagic Touch®) placed in front of them. The hand movement performed by patients was reproduced by another robotic device located behind them, delivering tactile feedback on the participants’ back (see Fig. 1A). Participants were asked to perform the sensorimotor task, while a randomized delay from 0 to 500 ms (6 delays with steps of 100 ms) was introduced between the hand movement and the tactile feedback on the back. On each trial, each patient performed 10 successive poking movements (automatically counted) and immediately afterwards gave a Yes or No answer to the following question assessing riPH (robot‐induced PH): “Did you feel as if someone was standing close by—behind or next to you?”. In total, each patient underwent three sessions of 18 trials (three repetitions per delay). Breaks between sessions allowed patients to avoid physical discomfort and fatigue. This experimental procedure does not only involve tactile stimulation on the back but also proprioceptive and motor cues from the upper limb (and additional robotically controlled spatiotemporal cues related to the incongruency between these proprioceptive‐tactile‐motor signals). Therefore, tactile cues alone are not sufficient to induce PH, as they are present in the same way in all experimental conditions experimental condition. What differs is the spatiotemporal congruency between the involved sensorimotor (proprioceptive, tactile, and motor) signals.

Fig 1.

Fig 1

riPH. (A) Robotic setup. (B) Higher proneness to riPH and higher delay sensitivity in PD‐PB (purple) vs. PD‐nPH (orange). Thick line: mean of fitted model; shaded area: 95% confidence interval.

Data Analysis

For each participant (except for one patient from the PD‐PB group who was not able to perform the task due to severe dyskinesia), responses to the Yes/No riPH were averaged for each delay (leading to one value per delay, per participant). Responses were then analyzed with linear mixed‐effects models (lme4 and lmerTest packages). 42 , 43 The model included delay (6 delays) and group of patients (PD‐PB, PD‐PH, PD‐nPH) as fixed effect (interaction between the two), and included a random intercept for patients. Significance of fixed effects was estimated using Satterthwaite's approximation for degrees of freedom of F statistics.

Data Sharing

Data and scripts used to generate the analyses presented in the paper are available here: https://gitlab.epfl.ch/fbernasc/phantom_boarder_pd.git.

Results

We assessed whether riPH depends on the degree of conflict applied during sensorimotor stimulation (i.e., different delays inserted between the movements of the front robot and the back robot) and here, especially, whether this differs between the three groups of patients (PD‐PB; PD‐PH; PD‐nPH). Behavioral results show 1 that the intensity of riPH increased with increasing spatiotemporal conflict for all three groups (delay dependency) (main effect of Delay; F (1,110) = 12, p‐value < 0.01). Critically, this delay dependency differed between the three patient groups (interaction: F(2,110) = 3.11, p‐value = 0.048; Figure 1B). Post‐hoc analysis revealed that both PD‐PB and PD‐PH had a higher delay sensitivity compared to PD‐nPH (PD‐PB vs. PD‐nPH: interaction: F (1,75) = 4.3; p‐value = 0.04; PD‐PH vs. PD‐nPH: F (1,95) = 6.4, p‐value = 0.01). There was no delay sensitivity difference between PD‐PB and PD‐PH (interaction: F (1, 50) = 0.06, p‐value = 0.8). Critically, there was also a main effect of group and PD‐PB had a higher intercept (bias) compared to PD‐nPH (PD‐PB vs. PD‐nPH: main effect of Group: F (1, 17) = 4.2, p‐value = 0.04) (Fig. 1B), whereas the intercept did not differ between PD‐PH vs. PD‐nPH (main effect of Group: F (1, 20) = 3.8, p‐value = 0.06) or between PD‐PB and PD‐PH (main effect of Group: F (1, 11) = 0.07, p‐value = 0.8).

Antiparkinsonian medication, motor impairment, age, and gender did not differ between the three groups. All patients presented normal cognition to mild cognitive impairment, and MoCA scores did not differ between the three groups (see Table 1). There was only one difference in a questionnaire screening for risk of developing psychosis (PQ‐16 score, Part 1), which was elevated for both, PD‐PB and for PD‐PH groups, versus the PD‐nPH group (PD‐PB and PD‐PH did not differ in PQ‐16).

TABLE 1.

Demographic and Clinical Data for Each Group of Patients with PD. Statistics Were Calculated Comparing the Three Groups of Patients

PD‐PB (N = 4) a PD‐PH (N = 7) a PD‐nPH (N = 13) a p‐value b
Age (years) 63 (14) 62 (15) 66 (8) >0.9
Sex at birth 0.20
F 2 (50%) 2 (29%) 9 (69%)
M 2 (50%) 5 (71%) 4 (31%)
Clinical characteristics of PD
Duration of PD (years) 13 (3) 7 (4) 9 (6) 0.12
UPDRS III 24 (16) 19 (12) 19 (18) 0.80
PQ16 (Part 1) 3 (2) 5 (2) 1 (1) <0.001
PQ16 (Part 2) 3 (3) 4 (6) 1 (3) 0.13
Levodopa equivalent daily dose 559 (166) 646 (437) 786 (653) >0.9
(LEDD including dopamine agonists) (mg/day)
Cognitive examination
MoCA 26 (2) 27 (2) 25 (2) 0.30
a

Mean (SD); n (%).

b

Kruskal‐Wallis rank sum test; Fisher's exact test.

Detailed interviews with all patients revealed the following additional findings. For three PD‐PB, the phantom boarder was experienced as an unfamiliar person, as was the felt presence experienced during PH. The remaining PD‐PB patient experienced her own children (or other family members) as phantom boarders and her sister (or mother) as the presence during PH. Neither the PB nor the PH was seen or heard by any of the four patients. All PB occurred in places familiar to the patients, either at home or at secondary well‐known vacation home, while PH occurred either at home (two patients) or outside the patients’ home (one patient). However, all three patients having both PH and PB clearly distinguished one from each other. Thus, for all three patients who experienced both PH and PB, the two phenomena always occurred at different times. Moreover, whereas these patients felt the presence (PH) to be in the same room as the patient, to be located very close to them (0–3 m), and at a fixed location either next to or behind them, this was different for the PB. The PB was always experienced at larger and more variable distances from the patient's body and never in the same room as the one occupied by the patient (Fig. 2).

Fig 2.

Fig 2

Illustration of PB and PH in an ecological setting. PB (A) is located on the first floor and PH (B) next to the patient (indicated as sitting on couch). Hallucinated PB and PH are indicated by the white silhouette.

In addition to PH, PD‐PB patients also reported other hallucinations (Table 2). Thus, among the four PD‐PB patients, three reported complex visual hallucinations, two reported visual illusions, two simple auditory hallucinations (sounds, noise), and one had passage hallucinations. These hallucinations were never reported at the same time as the PB. One of the PD‐PB patients reported mild theft delusions, always during PB instances. Visual and auditory hallucinations did not differ between PD‐PB and PD‐PH (p > 0.9).

Table 2.

Other Hallucinations Experienced by Patients of the PD‐PB Group

Types of hallucinations/delusions in the PD‐PB group Patient 1 Patient 2 Patient 3 Patient 4
• Phantom boarder (PB)
• Presence hallucinations (PH)
• Passage hallucinations
• Visual illusions (e.g., pareidolia)
• Visual hallucinations
• Auditory hallucinations
• Somatosensory hallucinations
• Olfactory/gustatory hallucinations

✓: presence of hallucination.

✗: absence of hallucination.

Discussion

The present study investigates PB and PH in patients with PD that share several phenomenological characteristics concerning sensed presences, but also differ in other aspects. To the best of our knowledge, our study is the first to provide behavioral indications that PD patients with PB show 1 elevated proneness to riPH, similar to patients with PD‐PH, and 2 a higher delay sensitivity compared to PD‐nPH patients when exposed to a robotic procedure that has been shown to experimentally induce PH in different patient populations. 32 , 37 , 44 Moreover, elevated proneness and higher delay sensitivity for riPH in PD‐PB 3 did not differ from PD‐PH patients. Our robotic procedure exposed participants to different levels of conflicting sensorimotor stimulation and allowed us to measure riPH in real‐time and across several controlled delay conditions. First, the finding of a higher riPH proneness (stronger bias) in reporting PH shows that PD‐PB patients behave abnormally, compared to PD‐nPH, when exposed to the robotic procedure, linking the symptom of PB that PD‐PB patients experienced in daily life to experimentally‐induced and real‐time PH in the laboratory. Second, evidence of higher delay sensitivity during sensorimotor stimulation indicates that PD‐PB, compared to PD‐nPH, is compatible with an alteration of sensorimotor brain processes in fronto‐temporal regions 32 and altered sensorimotor processes in hallucinations in the present PD‐PB patients. 45 , 46 , 47 Third, the present data linked PB to PH by showing that riPH proneness and delay sensitivity did not differ between PD‐PB and PD‐PH. These findings are not related to a general response bias that PD patients have when exposed to the procedure because riPHs were much weaker in PD‐nPH and because all patient groups showed delay sensitivity. Moreover, there were no significant demographic and clinical differences between the three groups. Antiparkinsonian medication, motor impairment, age, illness duration or gender cannot account for the reported differences in experimentally riPH (Table 1). Moreover, the present PD patients were all without dementia and only showed mild cognitive impairments (MoCA > 22), not differing between the three tested groups.

We argue that PB share underlying brain mechanisms with PH and that these mechanisms are of perceptual nature involving sensorimotor processes. 32 , 48 Thus, an altered sensorimotor integration of bodily signals would not only favor the occurrence of PH as shown previously, 32 , 49 but also of PB (present study). Involvement of perceptual mechanisms in PB is compatible with previous clinical work on PB, showing that in some cases the phantom boarder is heard or seen 8 , 10 and because some authors have used cluster analysis (of symptoms) and associated PB with complex visual hallucinations 8 , 10 and auditory hallucinations. 10 While, these previous data are compatible with a hallucinatory and perceptual origin of PB, the present data from the robotic paradigm suggest that the perceptual mechanisms of PB may involve primarily the sensorimotor system, including tactile, proprioceptive and motor signals and their integration. We note that these sensorimotor signals have been shown to be among the first and most prominent to be altered in PD. 45 , 50 , 51 The present robotic procedure 38 was motivated by earlier neurological data showing that invasive electrical stimulation in temporo‐parietal cortex can reliably induce the PH and that disturbed processing of sensorimotor own body signals (tactile, proprioceptive, and motor cues) modulates PH. 31 A limitation of the present study is that we could not test riPH in a group of PD patients who had pure PB (without PH). It could thus be argued that not PB, but the occurrence of PH (and PB) in the present PD‐PB patients, led to the elevated riPH proneness and delay sensitivity. We did report a patient with isolated PB, but were not able to test him fully with the riPH procedure. However, we report a higher bias (intercept) for riPH in PD‐PB compared to PD‐nPH, whereas this was not the case for PD‐PH, suggesting that the occurrence of PB and not PH is related to this difference in riPH bias. None of our PD‐PB patients had a history of symptoms related to delusional misidentifications and only one reported a mild transitory theft delusion directly related to the experience of PB, providing no support for the involvement of delusional‐cognitive mechanisms in the present cases of PB.

Interview data revealed similarities, but also phenomenological differences between PB and PH. Concerning similarities, all PD‐PB patients reported sensing the presence of another person during PB and PH, always without seeing or hearing the person. The sensed person was most often an unknown person, in PB and PH (except in one patient), and in all PD‐PB patients the PH and PB always appeared and disappeared suddenly. However, all patients who reported PB as well PH clearly distinguished between the two phenomena, considering them not to be the same presences, and not associated with the same emotional valence, and context of occurrence. First, the presence (PH) sensed by all PD‐PB patients (and PD‐PH patients), was experienced very close (0–3 m) to their body either behind or to the side of the patient, and within the current room at a very specific location. 17 , 32 Patients very often report PH “as if someone was bending over” their shoulders. This differed for the phantom boarder, who was experienced by all PD‐PB patients at much larger distances, outside the room (where the patient would be located at the moment) or even on a different floor or rooms, and also at more various and less defined locations inside the patients' home (Fig. 2). As indicated by a patient from the PD‐PB group, “while the PH stays close to me, the other presences (PB) seem to wander in my apartment”. Another marked difference between PB and PH is that they never occurred at the same time in the present PD‐PB patients. Other differences between PB and PH are that the PB can represent multiple presences in all of our four PD‐PB patients, whereas this was not reported by PD‐PH. Moreover, past work 32 , 48 showed that PH may occur at different places and also outside one's home, whereas PB are limited to very familiar places, such as the patients’ home (or secondary home). The present PD‐PB patients’ reports are not compatible with EH, as the sensed presence was neither seen nor heard as required for EH. 35 , 36 The PD‐PB patients also did not report to be touched by the hallucinated presences. EH require an extracampine tactile, auditory, or visual hallucinatory component, which is not the case for PH or PB. However, borderline cases may exist for whom it is difficult to draw a clear line between the three phenomena, for example when patients indicate to feel being touched by a hallucinated presence who is nearby and in their home, but is perceived as remaining outside their tactile field of perception (i.e., a presence experienced at a distance, yet felt as touching the patient; a presence in the immediate proximity felt to be touching the patient; the former being rather a tactile EH and the latter case a PH associated with a tactile intracampine component). More work is needed on this topic, by acquiring detailed data about patients’ experience, through the use of specialized questionnaires, and by searching for behavioral and neural differences and similarities (e.g., testing EH patients with riPH or other procedures), between these three fascinating feats of the human mind. Overall, these interview data on the phenomenological characteristics of PB and PH suggest that additional mechanisms, beyond the described sensorimotor mechanisms of PH, are likely to also be involved in PB. These may especially concern brain regions involving familiarity processing (compatible with prior work linking PB to misidentification or persons and places), 52 but also anxiety, 1 , 8 , 10 lack of interpersonal relationships, personal loss and living alone. 15 Nagahama et al. 7 included “the feeling of presence”—PH—in their factor analysis for psychotic symptoms in DLB, but did not relate PH to PB in their study. Future work should investigate how these additional mechanisms of PB interact with sensorimotor mechanisms of PH. The present robotic procedure has the advantage that it can be combined with tasks examining such additional processes: for example, by performing behavioral tasks during robotic stimulation. 44 , 53

The present study suffers from several limitations. More experimental data in larger groups of patients, including a group with PB without PH (pure PD‐PB), are needed to further investigate the brain mechanisms of presences and phantom boarders in PD, and to determine the clinical, behavioral and neural similarities and differences between both symptoms. Future studies should therefore acquire imaging data, investigating whether patients with PB recruit networks linked to PH 32 and/or whether brain regions involved in PD patients with PB rather overlap with networks of visual hallucinations. 54 , 55 , 56 Extended neuropsychological examination would be relevant in understanding the relationship between cognitive dysfunctions (or decline) and PB, and the cognitive mechanisms involved. 52 , 57 Additionally, a clearly defined scale for assessing PB and PH would allow to refine their phenomenological differences and similarities. Finally, delusional misidentification syndrome (e.g., Capgras, reduplicative paramnesia) was not evaluated in depth in our interviews nor tested behaviorally and should be included in future studies.

Author Roles

(1) Research project: A. Conception, B. Organization, C. Execution; (2) Statistical Analysis: A. Design, B. Execution, C. Review and Critique; (3) Manuscript Preparation: A. Writing of the first draft, B. Review and Critique.

O.B.: 1A, 1B, 2A, 2C, 3A, 3B.

F.B.: 1C, 2A, 2B, 2C, 3B.

J.P.: 1A, 1B, 1C, 2C, 3A, 3B.

Disclosures

Ethical Compliance Statement: The Ethics Committee of the Canton of Vaud (CER‐VD) and the Ethics Committee of the Canton of Geneva (CCER‐GE) in Switzerland evaluated and approved the study protocol. All participants were presented to the study orally and by means of a written information sheet. All participants gave their written informed consent prior enrollment in the study. We confirm that we have read the Journal's position on issues involved in ethical publication and affirm that this work is consistent with those guidelines.

Financial Disclosures for the Previous 12 Months: The authors declare that there are no additional disclosures to report.

Funding Sources and Conflicts of Interest: This project was supported by two generous donors advised by CARIGEST SA, the first one wishing to remain anonymous and second one being Fondazione Teofilo Rossi di Montelera e di Premuda; the Bertarelli Foundation; the Swiss National Science Foundation (grant number FNS 320030_188798). OB is one of the inventors on patent US 10,286,555 B2 (Title: Robot‐controlled induction of the feeling of a presence) held by the Swiss Federal Institute (EPFL) that covers the robot‐controlled induction of the feeling of a presence (PH). O.B. is one of the inventors on patent US 10,349,899 B2 (Title: System and method for predicting hallucinations) held by the Swiss Federal Institute (EPFL) that covers a robotic system for the prediction of hallucinations for diagnostic and therapeutic purposes.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to all the patients for their time and for sharing their subjective experience of hallucinations with us. We also thank Mr. Arthur Trivier for 3D design of Figure 1A and Figure 2. Open access funding provided by Ecole Polytechnique Federale de Lausanne.

[Corrections added on 11 April 2023, after online publication. PD‐nH has been revised to PD‐nPH throghout the article.]

References

  • 1. Rowan EL. Phantom boarders as a symptom of late paraphrenia. Am J Psychiatry 1984;141(4):580–581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Kasahara H, Tsumura M, Kada H, Hashidume T, Ito T, Ochiai Y, et al. Perspectives on phantom boarder symptom. Psychogeriatrics 2005;5(3):103–107. [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Lek SJ, Trinh S, Gavito‐Higuera J, Salazar R. Delusional misidentification syndrome: Phantom boarder syndrome presenting with intact general cognition and functional brain imaging evidence of bilateral posterior and orbitofrontal Hypometabolism. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2019;31(2):175–177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Terada S, Ishizu H, Fujisawa Y, et al. Delusion of theft and phantom intruder delusion in demented elderly patients in Japan. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 2005;18(3):142–148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Gottesman RT, Stern Y. Behavioral and psychiatric symptoms of dementia and rate of decline in Alzheimer's disease. Front Pharmacol 2019;10:1062. Available from: https://www.frontiersin.org/article/. 10.3389/fphar.2019.01062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Aarsland D, Ballard C, Larsen JP, McKeith I. A comparative study of psychiatric symptoms in dementia with Lewy bodies and Parkinson's disease with and without dementia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2001;16(5):528–536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Nagahama Y, Okina T, Suzuki N, Matsuda M, Fukao K, Murai T. Classification of psychotic symptoms in dementia with Lewy bodies. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2007;15(11):961–967. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Ballard CG, Bannister CL, Patel A, Graham C, Oyebode F, Wilcock G, Chung MC. Classification of psychotic symptoms in dementia sufferers. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1995;92(1):63–68. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Burns A, Jacoby R, Levy R. Psychiatric phenomena in Alzheimer's disease. II: Disorders of perception. Br J Psychiatry J Ment Sci 1990;157(76–81):92–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Hwang JP, Yang CH, Tsai SJ. Phantom boarder symptom in dementia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2003;18(5):417–420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Burns A, Jacoby R, Levy R. Psychiatric phenomena in Alzheimer's disease. I: Disorders of thought content. Br J Psychiatry J Ment Sci 1990;157(72–6):92–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Förstl H, Burns A, Jacoby R, Levy R. Neuroanatomical correlates of clinical misidentification and misperception in senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. J Clin Psychiatry 1991;52(6):268–271. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Asano H, Kondo H, Takahashi Y, Kikuchi Y. Mental disease on senile dementia‐phantom boarders symptom. Sendaishiritsubyoin Ishi 2001;21(3–8). [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Nagahama Y, Okina T, Suzuki N, Matsuda M. Neural correlates of psychotic symptoms in dementia with Lewy bodies. Brain 2010;133(2):557–567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Fukatsu T, Kanemoto K. Phantom boarder symptom in elderly Japanese. Psychogeriatrics 2022;22(1):108–112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Nagaratnam N, Irving J, Kalouche H. Misidentification in patients with dementia. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 2003;37(3):195–202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Fénelon G, Soulas T, De Langavant LC, Trinkler I, Bachoud‐Lévi AC. Feeling of presence in Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2011;82(11):1219–1224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Ffytche DH, Creese B, Politis M, Chaudhuri KR, Weintraub D, Ballard C, et al. The psychosis spectrum in Parkinson disease. Nat Rev Neurol 2017;13(2):81–95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Lenka A, Pagonabarraga J, Pal PK, Bejr‐Kasem H, Kulisvesky J. Minor hallucinations in Parkinson disease. Neurology 2019;93(6):259–266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Diederich NJ, Fénelon G, Stebbins G, Goetz CG. Hallucinations in Parkinson disease. Nat Rev Neurol 2009;5(6):331–342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Kataoka H, Ueno S. Predictable risk factors for the feeling of presence in patients with Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord Clin Pract 2015;2(4):407–412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Marinus J, Zhu K, Marras C, Aarsland D, van Hilten JJ. Risk factors for non‐motor symptoms in Parkinson's disease. Lancet Neurol 2018;17(6):559–568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Zhu K, van Hilten JJ, Putter H, Marinus J. Risk factors for hallucinations in Parkinson's disease: Results from a large prospective cohort study. Mov Disord 2013;28(6):755–762. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Castelnovo A, Cavallotti S, Gambini O, D'Agostino A. Post‐bereavement hallucinatory experiences: A critical overview of population and clinical studies. J Affect Disord 2015;1(186):266–274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Geiger J. The Third Man Factor: Surviving the Impossible. New York, NY: Weinstein Books; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Suedfeld P, Geiger J. The sensed presence as a coping resource in extreme environments. Miracles: God, Science, and Psychology in the Paranormal: Parapsychological Perspectives. Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers; 2008:1–15. [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Llorca PM, Pereira B, Jardri R, Chereau‐Boudet I, Brousse G, Misdrahi D, et al. Hallucinations in schizophrenia and Parkinson's disease: An analysis of sensory modalities involved and the repercussion on patients. Sci Rep 2016;1(6):38152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Jaspers K. Allgemeine Psychopathologie. 8th ed. Berlin: Springer‐Verlag; 1973. Available from: //www.springer.com/de/book/9783540033400. [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Nicastro N, Stripeikyte G, Assal F, Garibotto V, Blanke O. Premotor and fronto‐striatal mechanisms associated with presence hallucinations in dementia with Lewy bodies. NeuroImage Clin 2021;32:102791. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Brugger P, Regard M, Landis T. Unilaterally felt “presences”: The neuropsychiatry of one's invisible doppelgänger. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 1996;9:114–122. [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Arzy S, Seeck M, Ortigue S, Spinelli L, Blanke O. Induction of an illusory shadow person. Nature 2006;443(August):287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Bernasconi F, Blondiaux E, Potheegadoo J, Stripeikyte G, Pagonabarraga J, Bejr‐Kasem H, et al. Robot‐induced hallucinations in Parkinson's disease depend on altered sensorimotor processing in fronto‐temporal network. Sci Transl Med 2021;13(591):eabc8362. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Wood RA, Hopkins SA, Moodley KK, Chan D. Fifty percent prevalence of Extracampine hallucinations in Parkinson's disease patients. Front Neurol 2015;6:263. Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4685050/. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Nicastro N, Eger AF, Assal F, Garibotto V. Feeling of presence in dementia with Lewy bodies is related to reduced left frontoparietal metabolism. Brain Imaging Behav 2018;14:1199–1207. 10.1007/s11682-018-9997-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Bleuler E. Extracampine Hallucinationen. Psychiatrisch‐neurologische Wochenschrift 1903;25:261–264. [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Sato Y, Berrios GE. Extracampine hallucinations. The Lancet 2003;361(9367):1479–1480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Blanke O, Pozeg P, Hara M, et al. Neurological and robot‐controlled induction of an apparition. Curr Biol CB 2014;24(22):2681–2686. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Bernasconi F, Blondiaux E, Rognini G, Dhanis H, Jenni L, Potheegadoo J, et al. Neuroscience robotics for controlled induction and real‐time assessment of hallucinations. Nat Protoc 2022;12:1–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Nasreddine ZS, Patel BB. Validation of Montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA, alternate French versions. Can J Neurol Sci J Can Sci Neurol 2016;43(5):665–671. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Goetz CG, Tilley BC, Shaftman SR, et al. Movement Disorder Society‐sponsored revision of the unified Parkinson's disease rating scale (MDS‐UPDRS): Scale presentation and clinimetric testing results. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc 2008;23(15):2129–2170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Loewy RL, Pearson R, Vinogradov S, Bearden CE, Cannon TD. Psychosis risk screening with the prodromal questionnaire – Brief version (PQ‐B). Schizophr Res 2011;129(1):42–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting linear mixed‐effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 2015;67:1–48. [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Kuznetsova A, Brockhoff PB, Christensen HB. lmerTest: tests for random and fixed effects for linear mixed effect models (lmer objects of lme4 package), v. 2.0–25. See Httpcran R‐Proj Orgpackage LmerTest. 2014.
  • 44. Salomon R, Progin P, Griffa A, Rognini G, Do KQ, Conus P, et al. Sensorimotor induction of auditory misattribution in early psychosis. Schizophr Bull 2020;46:947–954. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Conte A, Khan N, Defazio G, Rothwell JC, Berardelli A. Pathophysiology of somatosensory abnormalities in Parkinson disease. Nat Rev Neurol 2013;9(12):687–697. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Corlett PR, Murray GK, Honey GD, et al. Disrupted prediction‐error signal in psychosis: Evidence for an associative account of delusions. Brain J Neurol 2007;130(Pt 9):2387–2400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Fletcher PC, Frith CD. Perceiving is believing: A Bayesian approach to explaining the positive symptoms of schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci 2009;10(1):48–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Potheegadoo J, Dhanis H, Horvath J, Burkhard PR, Blanke O. Presence hallucinations during locomotion in patients with Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord Clin Pract 2022;9(1):127–129. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Goetz CG, Vaughan CL, Goldman JG, Stebbins GT. I finally see what you see: Parkinson's disease visual hallucinations captured with functional neuroimaging. Mov Disord 2014;29(1):115–117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Lewis GN, Byblow WD. Altered sensorimotor integration in Parkinson's disease. Brain J Neurol 2002;125(9):2089–2099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Rossini PM, Filippi MM, Vernieri F. Neurophysiology of sensorimotor integration in Parkinson's disease. Clin Neurosci N Y N 1998;5(2):121–130. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Cipriani G, Vedovello M, Ulivi M, Lucetti C, Di Fiorino A, Nuti A. Delusional misidentification syndromes and dementia: A border zone between neurology and psychiatry. Am J Alzheimers Dis Dementiasr 2013;28(7):671–678. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Serino A, Pozeg P, Bernasconi F, Solcà M, Hara M, Progin P, et al. Thought consciousness and source monitoring depend on robotically controlled sensorimotor conflicts and illusory states. iScience 2021;24(1):101955. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Lenka A, Jhunjhunwala KR, Saini J, Pal PK. Structural and functional neuroimaging in patients with Parkinson's disease and visual hallucinations: A critical review. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2015;21(7):683–691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Marques A, Taylor NL, Roquet D, et al. Structural and functional correlates of hallucinations and illusions in Parkinson's disease. J Parkinsons Dis 2021;12:397–409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Nishio Y, Yokoi K, Uchiyama M, et al. Deconstructing psychosis and misperception symptoms in Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2017;88:722–729. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Pagonabarraga J, Llebaria G, García‐Sánchez C, Pascual‐Sedano B, Gironell A, Kulisevsky J. A prospective study of delusional misidentification syndromes in Parkinson's disease with dementia. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc 2008;23(3):443–448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Movement Disorders Clinical Practice are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES