Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2023 Feb 28;12:e80517. doi: 10.7554/eLife.80517

Prolonged partner separation erodes nucleus accumbens transcriptional signatures of pair bonding in male prairie voles

Julie M Sadino 1, Xander G Bradeen 2,3, Conor J Kelly 1,4, Liza E Brusman 1, Deena M Walker 5, Zoe R Donaldson 1,2,
Editors: Margaret M McCarthy6, Kate M Wassum7
PMCID: PMC10112888  PMID: 36852906

Abstract

The loss of a spouse is often cited as the most traumatic event in a person’s life. However, for most people, the severity of grief and its maladaptive effects subside over time via an understudied adaptive process. Like humans, socially monogamous prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) form opposite-sex pair bonds, and upon partner separation, show stress phenotypes that diminish over time. We test the hypothesis that extended partner separation diminishes pair bond-associated behaviors and causes pair bond transcriptional signatures to erode. Opposite-sex or same-sex paired males were cohoused for 2 weeks and then either remained paired or were separated for 48 hours or 4 weeks before collecting fresh nucleus accumbens tissue for RNAseq. In a separate cohort, we assessed partner-directed affiliation at these time points. We found that these behaviors persist despite prolonged separation in both same-sex and opposite-sex paired voles. Opposite-sex pair bonding led to changes in accumbal transcription that were stably maintained while animals remained paired but eroded following prolonged partner separation. Eroded genes are associated with gliogenesis and myelination, suggesting a previously undescribed role for glia in pair bonding and loss. Further, we pioneered neuron-specific translating ribosomal affinity purification in voles. Neuronally enriched transcriptional changes revealed dopaminergic-, mitochondrial-, and steroid hormone signaling-associated gene clusters sensitive to acute pair bond disruption and loss adaptation. Our results suggest that partner separation erodes transcriptomic signatures of pair bonding despite core behavioral features of the bond remaining intact, revealing potential molecular processes priming a vole to be able to form a new bond.

Research organism: Vole

eLife digest

Losing a spouse or life partner is a deeply traumatic event that can have long-term repercussions. Given enough time, however, most surviving partners are able to process their grief. The neural processes that enable people to adapt to their loss remain unknown.

To explore this question, scientists often turn to animals that form long-term mating based pair bonds and can be raised in the laboratory. Monogamous prairie voles enter lifelong partnerships where the two individuals live together, prefer to cuddle with each other, and take care of their pups as a team. After having lost their mate, they show signs of distress that eventually subside with time.

Sadino et al. examined the biological impact of partner loss in these animals by focusing on the nucleus accumbens, a brain region important for social connections. This involved tracking gene expression – which genes were switched on and off in this area – as the voles established their pair bonds, and then at different time points after one of the partners had been removed.

The experiments revealed that establishing a relationship leads to a stable shift in nucleus accumbens gene expression, which may help maintain bonds over time. In particular, genes related to glia (the non-neuronal cells which assist neurons in their tasks) see their expression levels increase, indicating a previously undescribed role for this cell type in regulating pair bonding. Having their partner removed led to an erosion of the gene expression pattern that had emerged during pair bonding; this may help the remaining vole adapt to its loss and go on to form a new bond. In addition, Sadino et al. explored the gene expression of only neurons in the nucleus accumbens and uncovered biological processes distinct from those that occur in glia after partner separation. Together, these results shed light on the genetic and neuronal mechanisms which underlie adaptation to loss; this knowledge could one day inform how to better support individuals during this time.

Introduction

The death of a romantic partner is cited as one of the most traumatic experiences in a person’s life and results in grief and corresponding indicators of mental and physiological distress (Keyes et al., 2014; Holmes and Rahe, 1967). However, for the majority of people acute grief subsides within approximately six months as the bereaved integrates and adapts to the loss (Shear et al., 2011). Most people will also eventually form a new pair bond, which provides a behavioral indicator of loss adaptation (Shear and Shair, 2005). The processes that enable such adaptation remain poorly understood but likely occur via the same neural systems that are uniquely engaged by pair bonding (Blocker and Ophir, 2016).

Socially monogamous prairie voles are a laboratory-amenable species that recapitulates many aspects of human social bonds, making them ideal for interrogating the neurobiology of bonding and loss. Pair bonding in this species results in a shift in sociobehavioral state where specific behaviors are only evident after the transition from living in a same-sex pair (i.e. family group) to an opposite-sex bonded pair. Pair bonded voles will prefer to affiliate with their partner, display selective aggression towards non-partner individuals, and exhibit robust and organized biparental care (Williams, 1992; Getz et al., 1981; Williams et al., 1992; Insel and Young, 2001; Carter et al., 1995; Winslow et al., 1993). Considering that other stable behavioral shifts, such as reproductive status and dominance hierarchies, are underwritten by changes in transcription, we aimed to determine if a mature pair bond—and the process of adapting to loss—are underwritten by stable changes in gene expression in the nucleus accumbens (Cardoso et al., 2015; Zayed and Robinson, 2012; Tripp et al., 2018).

While an extended network of brain regions mediates social processing and decision making, the nucleus accumbens (NAc)—a region important for reward, motivation, and action selection—is a critical hub that is engaged when forming a bond and is implicated in loss processing (Aragona and Wang, 2004; Lim and Young, 2004; Walum and Young, 2018). In humans, holding hands with a romantic partner enhanced blood oxygenation levels (BOLD) in the NAc and successful adaptation to spousal loss is associated with a reduced partner-associated BOLD signal in this region (O’Connor et al., 2008; Kreuder et al., 2017). Thus, the NAc contributes to pair bonding and loss and broad scale changes in this brain region potentially mediate shifts in transcription which may be required to adapt to partner loss. Similarly, in prairie voles, pair bond-associated behavioral changes are underwritten by several known neuromolecular changes within the NAc which maintain and reinforce pair bonds over time (Insel and Young, 2001; Aragona and Wang, 2004; Young and Wang, 2004). Further, when separated from their partner, prairie voles exhibit behavioral and physiological distress that mirrors what is seen upon bond disruption in humans and other species, such as titi monkeys (Carter et al., 1995). These include increased circulating glucocortioids levels, activation of the HPA axis, increased anxiety, decreased pain thresholds, and autonomic dysfunction (McNeal et al., 2014; Grippo et al., 2007; Bosch et al., 2016; Pohl et al., 2019; Osako et al., 2018). However, we have previously shown that if a pair bonded prairie vole loses their partner, given enough time, they will form a new bond that supersedes the original bond (Harbert et al., 2020; Tamarin et al., 1990). As in humans, the ability to form a new bond indicates that prairie voles can adapt to partner loss.

Here, we map the trajectory of the pair bond transcriptional profile by comparing opposite-sex paired males to same-sex sibling male pairs. In the wild, sexually naive male voles can cohabitate with other males, especially siblings, although these relationships do not persist after males form an opposite-sex pair bond and establish a nest/territory with their partner (Getz et al., 1981; Carter et al., 1995). By comparing opposite-sex pair bonded voles to their same-sex paired counterparts before and after partner separation, we have an ethologically relevant means to isolate the unique biology of bonding and loss independent of those that support general affiliative interactions (e.g. peer relationships) or the stressful effects of social isolation more broadly. Thus, we compared behavior and NAc transcriptional profiles of opposite-sex pair bonded and same-sex cohoused naïve male voles to explicitly define the pair bond both behaviorally and transcriptionally. Then we examined how affiliative pair bond characteristics were altered by partner separation to test the hypothesis that pair bond transcriptional signatures and bond-associated behaviors would erode as a function of time since partner separation. We reasoned that these changes represent key components of loss adaptation that, together, may prime the vole to be able to form a new bond.

We found that pairing induced a reliable affiliative preference for a peer or a pair bonded partner, and that this preference is remarkably stable, persisting even after four weeks of separation. We further show that pair bond-associated changes in accumbal gene expression were consistent at two and six weeks post-pairing. However, once opposite-sex pairs are separated, the pair bond transcriptional signature erodes as a function of separation time. To further home in on the transcriptional changes associated with partner separation specifically in NAc neurons, we pioneered translating ribosomal affinity purification in voles (vTRAP). Using vTRAP, we identified clusters of genes associated with dopaminergic signaling, mitochondrial organization, and steroid hormone signaling whose expression patterns are sensitive to acute pair bond disruption and loss adaptation. In sum, our parallel behavioral and transcriptional data suggests that erosion of pair bond transcriptional signatures in the NAc precedes changes in affiliative partner preference, providing insight into time-dependent neuromolecular changes that may contribute to loss adaptation.

Results

We determined how bonding and extended separation affects social behavior and NAc transcription in opposite-sex and same-sex paired males. We employed timepoints that are experimentally validated and ethologically relevant (Figure 1A). We paired all study animals for 2 weeks, a duration that reliably produces mature pair bonds (Scribner et al., 2020; Brusman et al., 2021). For the separation timepoints, the ability to form a new bond after loss serves as a behavioral metric of loss adaptation. Prior work has shown that male voles are able to form a new pair bond 4 weeks post-separation, but not 2 weeks or earlier (Harbert et al., 2020).

Figure 1. Males in either opposite-sex or same-sex pairs retain their partner preferences for at least four weeks following partner separation.

Additional stats including post-hocs provided in Supplementary file 1. (A) Schematic of a partner preference test (PPT) and timelines for behavioral experiments. Opposite-sex (n=8/cohort) and same-sex (n=8/cohort) pairs were paired for 2 weeks prior to a baseline PPT. Pairs were then either in a remain paired or separated cohort. Remain paired animals stayed with their partners throughout the experiment and underwent PPT 48 hours and 4 weeks after baseline. Separated animals were separated from their partners for either 48 hours (short-term) or 4 weeks (long-term) prior to the endpoint PPT. (B) Partner preference scores (% partner huddle/total huddle) from baseline, short-term, and long-term PPTs of opposite-sex (dark green) and same-sex (light green) paired males. Opposite- and same-sex paired males showed a baseline partner preference that remained evident for both groups after short-term separation and long-term separation (one-way t-tests relative to 50%, *** p<0.001, ** p<0.01). Black dotted line at 50% indicates no preference for partner or novel. (C) Partner (left) and novel (right) huddle duration (seconds) for remain paired animals over time during each PPT (two-way RM-ANOVA). (D) Partner huddle for short-term (left) and long-term (right) separated animals between baseline and the endpoint PPT—48 hours or 4 weeks post-baseline, respectively (two-way RM-ANOVA). (E) Novel huddle for short-term (left) and long-term (right) separated animals between baseline and the endpoint PPT—48 hours or 4 weeks post-baseline respectively (two-way RM-ANOVA).

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Partner and novel huddle latency is unchanged in both remain paired and separated opposite- and same-sex paired males.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

Cumulative event plots for partner (A–C) and novel (D–F) huddle latency in opposite-sex (left, dark green) and same-sex (right, light green) paired males showing the first 10 minutes of the partner preference test. (A) There was no difference in latency to huddle after 6 weeks of paring compared to 2 weeks in opposite sex pairs. This was true for both partner and novel huddle. (B) Same-sex paired males huddle significantly more quickly with their partner at baseline compared to 48 hr post-separation (log-rank test: χ2=4.0, p=0.046) while this change is absent in opposite-sex paired males. (C) Males separated for 4 weeks do not have any changes to their partner huddle latency. (D) As in A, same-sex pairs show no different in huddle latency over time. (E) Males separated for 48 hr have no change to their novel huddle latency. (F) Males separated for 4 weeks have no statistically significant differences between their baseline and endpoint novel huddle latencies.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Change scores of partner preference and partner huddle duration for opposite-sex and same-sex paired males.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

Change scores were calculated by subtracting the value of either metric (e.g. partner preference or partner huddle duration) at the endpoint PPT minus the baseline PPT. (A) Change scores of percent partner huddle for opposite-sex (left) and same-sex (right) paired males. (B) Change scores of partner huddle duration. Partner huddle duration significantly increased between 4 weeks post baseline only in males that had remained with their opposite-sex partner (t-test against 0%: p=0.000529). Same-sex paired males continued to increase their partner huddle duration after baseline regardless of separation (t-test against 0%: remain paired 48 hr – base p=0.062; remain paired 4 weeks – base p=0.060; separated 4 weeks – base p=0.046).

Figure 1—figure supplement 3. Pearson’s correlation matrices of partner preference test behaviors across time points.

Figure 1—figure supplement 3.

Left: opposite-sex paired males, Right: same-sex paired males. Correlation of PPT metrics for remain paired males (top 2 rows), males separated for 48 hr (3rd row), and males separated for 4 weeks (4th row). Number and color indicate Pearson’s R-value ***p<0.001, **p<0.01, *p<0.05.

Consistent with our laboratory observations, wild male voles who lose a partner will remain at the nest for ~17 days (Tamarin et al., 1990). Thus, we assessed behavior and transcription in paired voles two days and four weeks after separation—timepoints before and after the animal can form a new bond.

Extended separation disrupts the strengthening of bonds but does not baseline erode partner preference

We measured partner-directed affiliation via a partner preference test (PPT) across time in animals that remained paired and before and after short- or long-term partner separation (Figure 1A). The PPT measures the amount of time spent huddling either with the partner or a novel vole tethered at opposite ends of an arena (Figure 1A, right side). To ensure that the experimental males could not hear, smell, or see their separated partners during separation, all partners were moved to a separate vivarium room. All sample sizes and comprehensive statistical results, including effect size estimates, are reported in Supplementary file 1.

We initially hypothesized that opposite-sex pairs would have a partner preference at baseline and after short-term separation, when the pair bond is still intact, but not after long-term separation (Harbert et al., 2020; Sun et al., 2014). Similarly, we anticipated that same-sex sibling pairs would have a partner preference at baseline due to familiarity but would exhibit a faster and more robust loss of partner preference when separated (Lee et al., 2019).

In contrast to our initial hypothesis, we found that opposite-sex and same-sex paired males demonstrated a partner preference even after extended separation (Figure 1B; Lee et al., 2019; Goodwin et al., 2019). In pairs that remained with their partners throughout the 6-week timeline, both opposite-sex and same-sex pairs form a selective preference for their partner after 2 weeks of pairing (Figure 1B, one sample t-test relative to 50%: opposite-sex p=0.00564, same–sex p=0.00922). The baseline partner preference for both opposite- and same-sex pairs is retained—and even moderately strengthened—after 4 additional weeks of cohabitation (one sample t-test relative to 50% for 48 hr PPT: opposite-sex p=0.0107, same-sex p=0.01478; for 4 weeks PPT: opposite-sex p=5.19 × 10-7, same-sex p=6.49 × 10–7, two-way RM-ANOVA in Supplementary file 1). In separated animals, both opposite-sex and same-sex pairs form a selective preference for their partner after 2 weeks of cohabitation (one sample t-test relative to 50% short-term separated animals: opposite-sex p=3.30 × 10–12, same-sex p=7.72 × 10–5; long-term separated animals: opposite-sex p=1.50 × 10–3, same-sex p=3.04 × 10–5) and retain their partner preference after short-term and long-term separation (one sample t-test relative to 50%: short-term separated animals: opposite-sex p=4.10 × 10–7, same-sex p=3.66 × 10–8; long-term separated animals: opposite-sex p=0.001846, same-sex p=3.14 × 10–8). Additionally, there was no significant difference in partner preference score due to either separation duration or partner type (Figure 1B; two-way RM-ANOVA with repeated measures for timepoint: short-term separation: Main effect of partner (opposite- vs same-sex): F(1,28) = 0.38, p=0.54; Main effect of time: F(1,28) = 0.22, p=0.64; Partner X time: F(1, 28)=1.87, p=0.18; long-term separation: Main effect of partner (opposite- vs same-sex): F(1,26) = 1.70, p=0.20; Main effect of time: F(1,26) = 0.16, p=0.70; Partner X time: F(1, 26)=0.057, p=0.81). Together, these results indicate that prairie voles are capable of forming an affiliative preference for an opposite- or same-sex partner and that preference remains intact despite prolonged separation.

Next, we examined how partner- and novel-directed affiliative behavior, respectively, changed over time. For males that remain paired there were no significant differences in novel huddle over time for either pairing type. Of note, partner huddle increases with extended co-housing in both pairing types indicting a strengthening of partner-directed affiliation (2-way RM-ANOVA: main effect of timepoint (4 wks:base): F(2, 42)=4.92, p=0.012, η=0.170) (Figure 1C). This increase was not observed in opposite-sex or same-sex paired males that had been separated from their partner (Figure 1D and E; two-way RM-ANOVA in Supplementary file 1). This suggests that long-term separation effectively impedes the maturation and strengthening of the pair bond without necessarily dissolving the baseline preference. We also quantified how long it took to begin huddling in the partner preference test but failed to observe any consistent differences for partner or novel huddle latencies over time regardless of experimental condition or pairing type (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A–F, log-rank test in Supplementary file 1).

Additionally, we quantified how much each animal’s partner preference and partner huddle changed between timepoints (endpoint minus baseline; Figure 1—figure supplement 2A, B, stats in Supplementary file 1). These change scores revealed that partner preference and partner huddle were relatively stable between the baseline and short-term timepoint regardless pairing status or pairing type. However, at the long-term timepoint, we observed another indicator that separation impedes ongoing pair bond strengthening: opposite-sex males that remain paired exhibited increased partner huddle duration at 4 weeks relative to baseline, which was not observed in opposite-sex separated males (Figure 1—figure supplement 2B). In contrast, in same sex paired males, ongoing pairing was not required for increased partner huddle at the long-term timepoint (Figure 1—figure supplement 2B).

Finally, we assessed behavioral consistency across PPTs by determining the correlation of partner preference test metrics (huddle times, partner preference score, and chamber times) between baseline to short-term and baseline to long-term timepoints in all cohorts (Figure 1—figure supplement 3). Pairing duration, separation, and partner type did not yield reliable differences in correlated patterns of behavior. Together, these results indicate that prairie voles form an affiliative preference for an opposite- or same-sex partner that remains intact despite prolonged separation. However, separation impedes the normal, strengthening of partner-directed affiliation that occurs over time.

The pair bond transcriptional signature is stable in intact bonds

Changes in behavioral states, such as a shift in dominance or reproductive status, are supported by stable changes in transcription, although this has not been examined in the context of pair bonds (Cardoso et al., 2015; Zayed and Robinson, 2012; Tripp et al., 2018). While prior work has demonstrated that mating and cohabitation in prairie voles result in transcriptional changes within the NAc, the consistency of these changes as long as the bond remains intact has yet to be assessed (Duclot et al., 2020; Tripp et al., 2021; Resendez et al., 2016; Resendez et al., 2012; Aragona et al., 2006). Thus, we compared NAc transcription in opposite- versus same-sex-paired voles following either 2 or 6 weeks of pairing/cohabitation (Figure 2A). By comparing opposite-sex to same-sex-paired animals, we identified transcripts specific to pair bonds compared with those associated with affiliative behavior more generally. For consistency, we limited transcriptional assessment to voles that had a baseline partner preference >50% (Figure 2B; Supplementary file 1; OS n=15; SS n=11. Excluded for PPT <50% OS n=3; SS n=8).

Figure 2. Pair bonding leads to persistent and consistent changes in NAc transcription.

(A) Opposite- and same-sex pairs were paired for 2 weeks prior to a baseline partner preference test. Pairs then remain paired for either 48 hours (short-term; ~2 weeks total pairing) or 4 weeks (long-term; ~6 weeks total pairing) prior to collecting fresh nucleus accumbens tissue (dissection sites in red boxes) for RNA sequencing. (B) Baseline partner preference scores of males included in RNA sequencing for the opposite- and same-sex groups (one-tailed t-test relative to 50%: opposite-sex T14=11.76, p=1.21 X 10–8; same-sex T10=7.78, p=1.50 X 10–5). Black dotted line indicates a 50% partner preference score and the grey dotted line indicates 66%. There were no differences in partner preference score between opposite- and same-sex paired animals used for RNAseq (two-tailed t-test: T21.016 = 1.374, p=0.184). (C) Transcriptional analysis workflow. (D) Gene list from both timepoints ordered from the smallest to largest log2FoldChange after short-term pairing with color indicating up- or down-regulation in opposite- vs same-sex pairs. Expression patterns are strongly correlated across timepoints (Rho = 0.38, p=2.2 X 10–16). (E) Schematic of RRHO analysis. The heatmap is arranged into quadrants of genes upregulated in both lists (up-up: quadrant UU), downregulated in both lists (down-down: quadrant DD), or genes that have opposite regulation (up in list 1-down in list 2: quadrant UD; down in list 1-up in list 2: quadrant DU). Genes that are found in both lists at a similar ranked position result in higher p-values and are represented by a yellow color. (F) RRHO comparing short-term and long-term pair bonding (from 2D) indicates a stable pair bond gene signature over time as evidenced by concordant up- or downregulated genes at the two timepoints. (G) The short-term and long-term time points were pooled for opposite- and same-sex pairs to define the combined pair bond gene signature. (H) We compared GO analysis Mus musculus ontology terms between the combined pair bond DEGs and the RRHO quadrants (from 2 F) with strong correspondence between the two analyses.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Cohort shuffled controls for remain paired cohorts.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Spearman’s Ranked Correlation of gene expression (log2FoldChange) at the short-term pair bond and the long-term pair bond. Black line is the regression line with shading indicating a 95% confidence interval. (B) To ensure that our correlations are not driven by genes with consistent but low differential expression values, we only retained the first and fourth quartile of genes in the short-term pair bond and found a stronger Spearman’s Ranked Correlation between pair bond time points. (C) To test if the correlation in gene expression between remain paired timepoints was likely to be due to chance, we shuffled the Cohort identity for each animal and performed differential expression analysis. Heatmaps show every gene from short-term and long-term pairing ordered from the smallest to largest log2FoldChange after short-term pairing in the observed comparison (top) and a representative Cohort shuffle (bottom). Scale represents the scaled log2FoldChange. (D) Histogram of Rho values for 1000 iterations of (C, shuffled data). Only correlations that did not have a –log10(adjusted pvalue) of infinite were retained (n=636). Vertical dashed line indicates the observed Rho value with the percent of shuffled iteration in which Rho was greater than the observed value to the right (0.0%). (E) Unscaled RRHO plot of the representative Cohort shuffle used in (C) indicates elimination of the concordant signal seen with the observed data.
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Ingenuity Pathway Analysis (IPA) corresponding to GO term analyses.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

Figure is also supplemental data for Figure 3. IPA analyses indicating enriched pathways and upstream regulators for applicable comparisons. Representative pathways and regulators were chosen based on significance, activation pattern, and relationship to GO terms. Full lists can be found in Supplementary file 3. Yellow to blue scales indicate the z-scores of enrichment where yellow is activated and blue is repressed. White to blue scale represents the –log10(p-adjusted) where any non-significant terms are white. (A) IPA analysis for the remain-paired cohorts in Figure 2. The strong similarity of the activation/repression of pathways and regulators from short-term to long-term pairing are well represented by the combined pair bond transcriptional signature. Pathways or regulators of interest are bolded. (B) IPA analysis of the combined pair bond, short-term separation, and long-term separation in Figure 3. Grey boxes indicate that the term is not found in the results for that time point. Terms and regulators of interest are bolded while asterisks denote terms or regulators found in A. (C) IPA analysis of the eroded ST to LT genes for the UU and DD quadrants of Figure 3. Results are denoted as adjusted p-values instead of z-scores because there is no log2FoldChange value associated with the RRHO heatmaps. Pathways or regulators of interest are bolded. (D) IPA analysis of the neuronal enriched clusters of interest from Figure 4. Cluster #1 contained few genes and no significant pathways or regulators. Cluster #2 did not have any significant regulators.

We used DESeq2 to identify transcripts up- or down-regulated in opposite- relative to same-sex paired males after short-term and long-term pairing (Figure 3—figure supplement 2A, B; Love et al., 2014). When ordering transcripts based on their log2FoldChange at the short-term timepoint, the global transcriptional differences for opposite- versus same-sex pairs were strikingly similar following either 2 or 6 weeks of pairing, suggesting stable pair bond associated transcription across these timepoints (Figure 2D; Spearman’s Rho = 0.38, p=2.2 × 10–16). We further determined that the observed correlation across timepoints is stronger than what would be expected by chance. We shuffled each animal’s cohort identity at the long-term timepoint and calculated Rho values of the log2FoldChange of differential gene expression between the observed short-term pair bond and the shuffled long-term pair bond over 1000 iterations (Figure 2—figure supplement 1A–D). The cohort identity is a combination of three variables: pairing type (opposite-sex or same-sex), separation condition (remain paired or separated), and timepoint (short- or long-term). For example, long-term separated opposite-sex animals is one cohort identity of 8 total. This approach effectively randomized timepoint, partner type, and pairing status without disrupting underlying structure that exists within our transcriptional dataset due to correlations in expression across genes. Our observed Rho value was greater than 100% of iterations, indicating that the similarity in gene expression across short and long-term pairing timepoints is greater than would be expected by chance (Figure 2—figure supplement 1D). Next, we asked whether our observed association across timepoints was driven by moderately but consistently expressed genes. Eliminating transcripts whose expression difference fell within the middle two quartiles in the short-term group resulted in a greater Rho value (R=0.44, p<2.2 × 10–16), indicating that the correlation across short- and long-term timepoints is more strongly driven by transcripts with larger expression differences between opposite- and same-sex pairs. To further interrogate the biological function of altered transcripts, we set nominal thresholds of log2FoldChange >0.30 or<–0.30, which are sufficient to identify replicable differences detectable via alternate methods (Walker et al., 2018), along with a p-value threshold of p<0.05 to identify up- and down-regulated gene lists (DEGs). This nominal p-value threshold was used with the aim of identifying gene families regulating molecular pathways responsive to bonding instead of specific gene candidates. Prior studies in animal brain tissue using this p-value have yielded biologically meaningful insights (Walker et al., 2018; Mukamel, 2022; Kronman et al., 2021; Walker et al., 2022; Labonté et al., 2017; Krishnan et al., 2007; Peña et al., 2017; Seney et al., 2018; DEGs based on these thresholds are listed in Supplementary file 2). There was significant overlap in the upregulated (104 shared, Fisher’s Exact Test: χ2=30.24, p=5.36 X 10–31) and downregulated (46 shared, Fisher’s Exact Test: χ2=13.32, p=7.05 X 10–14) transcripts across timepoints (Figure 3—figure supplement 2E; Wang et al., 2015).

To further examine similarity in transcriptional patterns between short-term and long-term pair bonds we employed Rank-Rank Hypergeometric Overlap (RRHO; Figure 2E; Plaisier et al., 2010; Cahill et al., 2018). This threshold free approach allows us to determine if similar global transcription patterns are observed between two comparisons. The resulting RRHO heatmap is arranged into quadrants based on the direction of gene expression and each point represents the significance derived from the number of overlapping genes via the hypergeometric distribution (Figure 2E). We determined how similar global transcription is between short-term and long-term pair bonds by comparing differential gene expression in the opposite- vs same-sex 2-week paired to opposite-sex vs same-sex 6-week paired groups. We observed extensive concordance of transcriptional patterns between 2- and 6-week paired voles, further supporting that the transcriptional profile of pair bonds is stable (Figure 2F). Confirming that our observed RRHO signal was unlikely to be attributable to chance, we randomly shuffled cohort identity (Figure 3—figure supplement 4E) and, separately, shuffled the gene ranking of the combined pair bond genes (Figure 3—figure supplement 4G), both of which ablated the RRHO signal. Since the pair bond transcriptional profiles are concordant at both time points, the 2-week and 6-week animals from each pairing type were pooled to create a single, well-powered opposite-sex- vs same-sex-paired comparison that defines the combined pair bond transcriptional signature (Figure 2G).

To determine which biological pathways might underlie pair bonding we employed Gene Ontology (GO) and Ingenuity Pathway Analysis (IPA) (Yu et al., 2012). Gene lists for GO terms or IPA were generated either from DEG lists or from genes in concordant (UU or DD) RRHO quadrants. GO terms that passed a false discovery rate of p<0.05 were retained while IPA terms that had activation scores of at least –2 or 2 and whose enrichment for genes predicted to be regulated was significant (p-value <0.05) were kept (Supplementary file 3). Combined pair bond GO terms associated with the upregulated DEGs and the quadrant UU genes strongly implicate changes in glial cells and extracellular matrix organization (Figure 2H), which is mirrored by activation of glioblastoma signaling via IPA (Figure 2—figure supplement 2A). IPA additionally identified upregulation of Synaptogenesis, CREB signaling, and Endocannabinoid Neuronal Synapse pathways. All these pathways are consistent with pair bonding as a form of complex learning that is mediated by neuromodulatory signaling (Walum and Young, 2018; Loth and Donaldson, 2021; Modi and Young, 2011; Johnson and Young, 2017). Predicted upstream regulators of these pathways broadly support a role for learning and neuromodulation, including Creb1, Estrogen receptor alpha (Esr1), and various growth-factor and developmental genes (Figure 2—figure supplement 2A). Combined pair bond GO terms associated with the downregulated DEGs and the quadrant DD genes are implicated in nucleoside synthesis and synaptic plasticity (Figure 2H). IPA indicates a suppression of corticotropin releasing hormone signaling, potentially reflecting the strong social buffering of stressors that occurs in pair bonded voles specifically (Bosch et al., 2016; Smith et al., 2013; Bosch et al., 2009; Smith and Wang, 2014; Burkett et al., 2016; Figure 2—figure supplement 2A). Finally, the strong correspondence in the significance of GO terms between the combined pair bond DEGs and the RRHO quadrants further validates that pooling the time points to represent a single pair bond transcriptional signature retains biologically relevant information (Figure 2H).

The pair bond transcriptional signature erodes following prolonged partner separation

We next tested the hypothesis that the pair bond transcriptional signature erodes following long-term partner separation. We paired opposite-sex and same-sex pairs for 2 weeks prior to a baseline partner preference test and then immediately separated males into new cages where they were singly housed for either 48 hours (short-term) or 4 weeks (long-term) prior to harvesting NAc tissue for RNAseq (Figure 3A). As before, we only included animals with a baseline partner preference >50% (Figure 3B; OS n=12; SS n=11. Excluded OS n=4; SS n=5). We determined the effect of separation on the pair bond gene signature by first defining differential gene expression of opposite- vs same-sex animals after short- and long-term separation and then used RRHO to examine genome-wide expression changes relative to the combined pair bond signature (Figure 3C).

Figure 3. Prolonged separation erodes pair bond transcriptional signatures.

(A) Opposite-sex and same-sex pairs were paired for 2 weeks prior to a baseline partner preference test. Pairs were then separated for either 48 hours (short-term) or 4 weeks (long-term) prior to collecting fresh nucleus accumbens tissue for RNAseq. Animals in the Remain Paired group are analyzed in Figure 2 to define the Combined Pair Bond transcriptional signature. (B) Baseline partner preference scores of males included in RNA sequencing for the opposite- and same-sex separation groups (one-tailed t-test relative to 50%: opposite-sex T11=11.56, p=1.71 X 10–7; same-sex T9=6.07, p=1.87 X 10–4). Black dotted line indicates a 50% partner preference score and the grey dotted line indicates 66%. There were no differences in partner preference score between opposite-sex and same-sex separated animals used for RNAseq (two-tailed t-test: T14.33 = 0.33, p=0.75). (C) Transcriptional analysis workflow similar to Figure 2. (D) Heatmap of the scaled log2FoldChange for every gene where short- and long-term separation is compared to the combined pair bond gene signature. (E) RRHO using the combined pair bond, short-term separated, and long-term separated ranked transcript lists show that short-term separated animals retain a gene expression pattern concordant with pair bond transcription while there is a dramatic erosion of pair bond gene expression following long-term separation. (F, G) Upset plot showing overlap of genes in the UU or DD quadrants of 2F and 3E RRHO was determined using a Fisher’s Exact test. The difference between the intersections of the pair-bond:short-term sep. and pair-bond:long-term sep. was used to define the eroded gene lists. (H, I) The RRHO gene lists of eroded pair bond genes were used to filter the DESeq heatmap in 2D. Eroded genes from the UU quadrants show upregulation during pair bonding and short-term separation but downregulation after long-term separation (H) while the eroded genes from the DD quadrants show the opposite pattern (I). (J, K) GO and IPA analysis of RRHO quadrant gene lists and pair bond eroded gene lists. Scale represents the –log10(p-adjusted) where any non-significant terms are white. (J) Glial associated GO terms of the UU quadrants are less significantly represented after separation and are highly significantly represented among the eroded genes. Significant upstream regulators of the eroded genes include Sox2, estrogen receptor, Erbb3, and Ctnnb1. (K) Vesicle docking and synapse organization associated GO terms of the DD quadrants are less significantly represented after separation and are more significantly represented among the pair bond eroded genes. Significant upstream regulators of the eroded genes include Hnf4a, Calmodulin, Hdac4, and Adora2a.

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Cohort shuffled controls for the combined pair bond and separation time points.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

(A) Spearman’s Ranked Correlation of gene expression (log2FoldChange) for the combined pair bond vs short-term separation (left) or the combined pair bond vs long-term separation (right). Black line is the regression line with shading indicating a 95% confidence interval. (B) To ensure that our correlations are not driven by consistent but low differential expression values we only retained the first and fourth quartile of genes in the combined pair bond. We then performed a Spearman’s Ranked Correlation between the combined pair bond and each separation time point and found a greater Rho value for each time point. (C) To test if the correlation in gene expression between timepoints could occur by chance, we shuffled the Cohort identity for each animal and performed differential expression analysis. Heatmaps show every gene from the combined pair bond and separation time points ordered from the smallest to largest log2FoldChange in the combined pair bond for the observed comparison (top) and a representative Cohort shuffle (bottom) with Rho values for each. Scale represents the scaled log2FoldChange. (D) Histogram of Rho values for a 1000 shuffles of the Cohort identity as in C, shuffled. Rho values for the correlation between the combined pair bond vs short-term separation (left) or the combined pair bond vs long-term separation (right) are plotted for each iteration. Only correlations that did not have a –log10(adjusted pvalue) of infinite were retained (short-term separation n = 613; long-term separation n = 617). Vertical dashed line indicates the observed Rho value with the percent of shuffled Rho’s that were greater than the observed value to the right (pair bond:short term 3.26%; pair bond:long term 41%). (E, F) Unscaled RRHO plot of the representative Cohort shuffle used in (C). The combined pair bond vs short-term separation (E) indicates a similar concordant pattern as the observed data though to a much lower degree (note scale maximum). The combined pair bond vs long-term separation (F) shows a highly significant concordant pattern that does not occur in the observed data.
Figure 3—figure supplement 2. Volcano plots of differential expression with Upset plots for overlap of differential expressed genes.

Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

Figure is also supplemental data for Figure 2. (A–D) Volcano plots with associated Upset plots of the differentially expressed genes of the short-term pair bond (A), long-term pair bond (B), short-term partner separation (C), and long-term partner separation (D). Genes that are upregulated in opposite-sex paired males compared to same-sex paired males are to the right (black dots) and genes that are downregulated are to the left (white dots). Dashed lines indicate thresholds of +/-0.30 log2FoldChange (vertical) or 0.05 p-value (horizontal) used to determine differential expression. (E) Upset plot showing overlap of up- and downregulated DEGs from each timepoint. Intersections with no overlapping genes, such as downregulated long-term:upregulated short-term, are not displayed on the graph. Asterisks and shading indicate statistically significant intersections (Fisher’s Exact Test: Upreg ST:LT χ2=30.24, p=5.36 X 10–31; Downreg ST:LT χ2=13.32, p=7.05 X 10–14). (F) Upset plot showing the combined up- and downregulated separation DEGs compared to the combined pair bond DEGs. There are more shared transcripts between the pair bond and short-term separated animals than the pair bond and long-term separated animals. Asterisks and shading indicate statistically significant intersections (Fisher’s exact test: pair-bond:short-term sep χ2=31.86, p=6.78 X 10–19; pair-bond:long-term sep χ2=17.96, p=0.53).
Figure 3—figure supplement 3. Randomly selected subsampled data shows the same differential expression and RRHO patterns as the full dataset.

Figure 3—figure supplement 3.

(A) We selected a random subset of the same number of animals for each opposite-sex (n=6) and same-sex paired (n=5) cohort, and performed differential expression analysis between pairing time points (top) and the combined pair bond vs separation time points (bottom). The heatmap shows genes ordered based on log2FoldChange in the short term Pair bond (tope) or combined pair bond (bottom) with color indicated expression difference between opposite and same-sex paired males. Scale represents the scaled log2FoldChange. (B) RRHO heatmaps of the representative subset used in (A) that use the same scale as the observed data (top) or are unscaled to visually optimize significant signal (bottom).
Figure 3—figure supplement 4. Control analyses for RRHO.

Figure 3—figure supplement 4.

Further examination of RRHO results where A, D, G correspond to Figure 2F (short-term vs long-term remain paired). B, E, F correspond to Figure 3E left (pair bond vs short-term separation). C, F, I correspond to Figure 3E right (pair bond vs long-term separation). (A, B, C) RRHOs showing -log10(p-value) scales optimized for visualization of significant signal. Note the greatly reduced scale (max 16) in C compared with other analyses. (D, E, F) Log-Odds ratio plots for RRHOs of all analyses scaled to a max value of 6. (G–I) Control analyses where the ranked gene lists were randomly shuffled while all other RRHO parameters remain the same.
Figure 3—figure supplement 5. RRHO of short-term vs long-term partner separation reveals a transcriptional signature of partner loss.

Figure 3—figure supplement 5.

(A) RRHO comparing short-term and long-term partner loss. Heatmap indicates that genes that are downregulated after short-term separation remain downregulated after long-term partner separation. (B, C) Gene lists were extracted from the UU and DD quadrants and used to filter the combined pair bond differential expression data of Figure 2. (B) The UU quadrant genes that had a L2FC <–0.30 in the combined pair bond were plotted for their expression in males that stayed with their partner or were separated for 48 hours or 4 weeks. (C) The DD quadrant genes that had a L2FC >0.30 in the combined pair bond were plotted for their expression in males that stayed with their partner or were separated for 48 hr or 4 weeks. (D, E) GO analysis using Mus musculus ontology terms for the UU quadrant filtered genes used in B (D) or the DD quadrant filtered genes used in C (E).

First, we performed differential expression analysis of opposite- vs same-sex-separated males at each separation time point (Figure 3—figure supplement 2C and D). There were significantly more shared DEGs between the combined pair bond group and short-term separation group than would be expected by chance, but this was not observed when comparing the shared genes between the combined pair bond and long-term separation group (Figure 3—figure supplement 2F Fisher’s exact test: pair bond:short-term sep χ2=31.86, p=6.78 X 10–19; pair bond:long-term sep χ2=17.96, p=0.53). The difference in transcription between short- and long-term separation is particularly evident when comparing the global transcriptomes through the lens of pair bond transcription. Specifically, when genes are ordered based on their degree of down- or up-regulation in the pair bond group we found that gene expression was largely indistinguishable in short-term separated animals (Rho = 0.32, p<2.2 × 10–16) and distinct from long-term separated animals (Rho = 0.048, p=1.6 × 10–7), indicating that the pair bond signature is largely eroded after prolonged separation (Figure 3D). We further assessed whether these correlations differed from chance via the previously employed permutation analysis. We found that the pair bond:short-term Rho value indicated a stronger association than would be expected by chance (p=0.0362) while the pair bond:long term separation association value was highly likely to occur by chance (p=0.41). Together, these results suggest that extended separation erodes nucleus accumbens pair bond transcription, a potentially crucial molecular process needed to adapt to partner loss.

To more comprehensively assess genome-wide changes to the pair bond signature following extended partner separation we again employed RRHO (Figure 3E). We compared the ranked gene list of opposite- vs same-sex for the (1) combined pair bond gene signature (Figure 3D, top) versus either (2) short-term partner separation (Figure 3D, middle) or (3) long-term partner separation (Figure 3D, bottom). We found that the pair bond signature remains largely concordant after short-term separation as indicated by significant signal among upregulated genes (quadrant UU), and to a lesser extent, downregulated genes (quadrant DD; Figure 3E). However, following long-term separation, the pair bond gene signature is largely undetectable as indicated by a lack of significant signal throughout the RRHO plot. Plotting the long-term RRHO with an unadjusted p-value scale revealed few overlapping genes in the UU and DD quadrants (scale 1–16 Figure 3—figure supplement 4C rather than 1–300 as in Figure 3D). The low level of overlap indicates that there is a residual, albeit greatly reduced, pair bond signature that is still detectable after long-term separation (Figure 3—figure supplement 4C). Additionally, we used the same control analyses as previously employed (Figure 3—figure supplement 4G–I).

We next examined which genes are consistently present in the UU or DD quadrants between pair bonding and each separation timepoint. Specifically, we compared the UU and DD quadrants (inclusive of all transcripts in each quadrant) of the short-term (2 week) vs long-term (6 week) pair bond from Figure 2F, the combined pair bond vs short-term separation in the left side of Figure 3E, and the combined pair bond vs long-term separation on the right side of Figure 3E. The degree of overlap in the UU quadrants was greatest between the combined pair bond vs short-term separation and least between the combined pair bond vs long-term separation, a result consistent with the most extensive erosion of pair bond transcription occurring after long-term separation (Figure 3F: all stats in Supplementary file 1, genes in Supplementary file 2). The same trend was observed for transcripts in the DD quadrants (Figure 3G, all stats in Supplementary file 1, genes in Supplementary file 2). We next identified genes whose differential expression in pair bonded voles eroded as a function of separation time. We reasoned that such genes are involved in biological pathways that likely regulate adapting to partner loss and prime an animal to form a new pair bond. The genes that were no longer present in the pair bond:long-term separation intersection that were present in the pair bond:short-term separation intersection were deemed eroded genes. These eroded transcripts include 581 UU quadrant transcripts and 1,248 DD quadrant transcripts (Figure 3F and G). We next filtered the genome-wide expression data of Figure 3F by the eroded gene lists (Figure 3H,I). The eroded UU genes are strongly upregulated in a stable pair bond and show dramatic downregulation following long-term separation (Figure 3H). Thus, the eroded UU genes and their associated biological processes may represent key aspects of adapting to partner loss. There was also a corresponding reduction in DD gene overlap between a stable pair bond and following separation, further supporting that bond-related changes in gene expression erode after long-term separation regardless the initial direction of expression during bonding (Figure 3I).

We next determined how separation-induced transcriptional changes affected pair bond associated biological processes. We used the pair bond associated GO terms from Figure 2H as a reference point to determine if these terms were still significantly associated with the underlying transcriptional profile following partner separation (Figure 3J and K). We predicated two outcomes. First, separation would reduce the significance of the pair bond associated GO terms and second, pair bond genes whose expression eroded would be significantly represented by the original pair bond associated terms. The transcripts in the short-term separation UU quadrant were still significantly associated, though to a lesser degree, with the pair bond associated GO terms. After long-term separation, the underlying transcriptional signature is much less significantly associated with the pair bond GO terms. Further, the eroded transcripts are highly associated with the pair bond associated GO terms, including glial-associated terms. A potential role for glia was also implicated in the IPA identification of pathways for glioblastoma multiforme signaling and glioma signaling (Figure 2—figure supplement 2C). Upstream regulators inducing the erosion of these glial-associated genes include Sox2, estrogen receptor, Erbb3, and Ctnnb1 (Figure 3J, Figure 2—figure supplement 2C). We observe a similar trend in the DD quadrants where the pair bond associated GO terms become less representative of the underlying transcriptional signature following long-term separation but are associated with the eroded genes, although to a lesser degree (Figure 3K). IPA pathways activated in a stable pair bond (endocannabinoid signaling, synaptogenesis, and dopamine signaling) are significantly represented among the eroded DD genes (Figure 2—figure supplement 2C). Upstream regulators for the eroded DD transcripts include Hnf4a, Calmodulin, Hdac4, and Adora2a (Figure 3K, Figure 2—figure supplement 2C).

Finally, to gain more nuanced insight into the temporal trajectory of erosion, we also used RRHO to compare short-term to long-term partner separation (Figure 3—figure supplement 5). In contrast to the pattern of consistently upregulated genes between the pair bond and short-term separation, we observed high concordance of downregulated genes between separation timepoints (Figure 3—figure supplement 5A). We then identified transcripts in the UU and DD quadrants of the separation RRHO that exhibited the largest inversion of expression between pair bond and short-term separation (details in Materials and Methods). We reasoned that such transcripts represented potential gene sets that are rapidly eroded following partner separation and which remain eroded after long-term separation, representing a potential first wave transcriptional response to partner loss. We further asked which biological processes are represented by these consistently inverted genes via GO analysis (Figure 3—figure supplement 5B, C). We found that UU pair bond genes that are downregulated following pair bond separation are associated with dopaminergic processes and DD pair bond genes that are upregulated are associated with neuronal rearrangement processes. Thus, this RRHO analysis enriches our interpretation of transcriptional erosion of pair bond signatures following partner separation by revealing temporally complex shifts in gene expression resulting from partner loss.

Neuronal-specific interrogation reveals gene clusters associated with pair bond disruption and loss adaptation

Our tissue-level analysis of gene expression strongly implicated a role for glia in bond formation and loss, and we reasoned that relevant neuronal transcriptional changes may have been masked by these prominent glial transcriptional signals. Thus, we specifically examined separation-induced neuronal transcriptional changes by pioneering translating ribosome affinity purification in voles (vTRAP) (Heiman et al., 2014; Heiman et al., 2008). The vTRAP construct, DIO-eGFP-pvRPL10a, is a double-floxed inverse orientation eGFP-tagged ribosomal subunit that uses the prairie vole RPL10a gene sequence (Figure 4A; Figure 4—figure supplement 1A). The vTRAP construct is paired with Cre-recombinase (Cre) for inducible, cell-type specific expression. AAV-mediated delivery of hSyn-Cre, which drives neuron-specific Cre-recombinase expression, and hSyn-DIO-vTRAP vectors results in neuronal expression of GFP-tagged ribosomes. Subsequent immunoprecipitation of the tagged ribosomes provides a means to isolate neuron-specific, actively-translating mRNAs (Heiman et al., 2014; Heiman et al., 2008). vTRAP is Cre-dependent as confirmed by bilateral NAc injections of +/-Cre (Figure 4—figure supplement 1A). We performed vTRAP immunoprecipitation of 3 cohorts: (1) opposite-sex long-term separated, (2) opposite-sex long-term remain paired, and (3) same-sex long-term separated.

Figure 4. vTRAP elucidates separation-induced transcriptional changes specifically in neurons.

(A) Schematic of the vTRAP system. AAV-mediated delivery of hSyn-Cre, which drives neuron-specific Cre-recombinase expression, and DIO-vTRAP vectors results in neuronal expression of GFP-tagged ribosomes. Subsequent immunoprecipitation of the tagged ribosomes isolates neuron-specific, actively-translating mRNAs. (B) Transcripts enriched in the immunoprecipitation pulldowns compared to the input fractions. Differential expression skews towards depletion. (C) WGCNA to identify gene modules correlated with the input (negative correlation) or pulldown (positive correlation) fractions. The grey60 module contains neuronal transcripts of interest such as Drd1a, Fosb, Oprk1, and Pdyn while the blue module contains Drd2. (D) Neuronal enriched genes—2,365 genes—were defined as the intersection of the Input v Pulldown enriched DEGs and the WGCNA pulldown-correlated modules (modules grey60, blue, greenyellow, brown, green, royalblue, and cyan). When using Enrichr to search for cell types associated with the neuronal enriched gene list, nucleus accumbens is the most prominent and is highly significant followed by dorsal striatum. Scale represents –log10(p-adjusted). (E) Heatmap showing expression of identified neuronally enriched genes in a single-nuclei RNAseq dataset from the nucleus accumbens of adult male rats. The scale indicates expression of each gene in the snRNAseq dataset. (F) Hierarchical clustering using one minus Pearson correlation with complete linkage of bulk tissue-level sequencing for all genes (top heatmap) and neuronal enriched genes (bottom heatmap) for the combined pair bond, short-term separation, and long-term separation. The scale indicates scaled log2FoldChange. The mapping of neuronal clusters to the all genes clusters is visualized via a Sankey plot. Neuronal clusters of interest are highlighted by dark grey links. (G) Gene Ontology analysis using mouse terms for each neuronal cluster of interest. Genes of interest that occur in multiple terms are referred to as prominent genes. Scale represents –log10(p-adjusted).

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Alignment of mouse (Mus musculus) and prairie vole (Microtus ochrogaster) ribosomal protein L10a (RPL10a) cDNA sequences and amino acid sequences.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(A) The mouse RPL10a mRNA sequence (GenBank BC083346.1) was aligned to the prairie vole RPL10a cDNA sequence (XP_005360358.1) using BLASTN 2.8.0+33. Asterisks indicate matching nucleotides. The mouse and vole cDNA sequences are 93% identical, with an expected value of 0.0. (B) The mouse RPL10a amino acid sequence was deduced from the mouse RPL10a cDNA sequence (GenBank BC083346.1) and aligned to the prairie vole RPL10a amino acid sequence (XP_005360415.1) using BLASTX 2.8.0+33. Asterisks indicate matching amino acids. The mouse and vole amino acid sequences are 100% identical, with an expected value of 3 X 10-145.
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. Validation of expression and analyses for vole optimized Translating Ribosome Affinity Purification.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

(A) Vole optimized GFP-RPL10a vector is Cre-dependent as confirmed by bilateral NAc injections +/- Cre. ac = anterior commissure. (B) The input and pulldown samples from vTRAP cohorts were used to construct gene co-expression networks using WGCNA. The resulting co-expression networks are clustered in a dendrogram and each gene module is assigned a unique color. (C) Gene Ontology analysis using mouse ontology terms indicates that transcripts associated with the mitochondria and cellular respiration are enriched while transcripts associated with gliogenesis and myelination are depleted, supporting neuronal targeting of RPL10-GFP. (D) The neuronal enriched genes from Figure 4E were used to filter the input and pulldown samples of the opposite-sex 4-week separated cohort. The high correspondence in the scaled log2FoldChange of the Morpheus heatmaps validates that, for neuronal enriched genes, their expression in bulk sequencing is largely representative of their expression in neurons.

We defined a set of neuronally enriched genes by using the intersection of threshold and threshold-free based analyses. We first identified neuronally-enriched transcripts by comparing the transcriptional profiles of the input fraction (equivalent to bulk RNA-seq) and pulldown fraction of the animals from the TRAP groups specified above. We predicted that the pulldown fraction would be enriched for neuronal markers and depleted in glial-associated transcripts. Differential expression analysis using the previously defined thresholds revealed that most transcripts are depleted in the pulldown fraction as expected (Figure 4B). GO terms support that hSyn-vTRAP successfully isolates neuronally-enriched transcripts and filters out genes associated with gliogenesis (Figure 4—figure supplement 2C). Second, we used weighted correlation network analysis (WGCNA) to identify gene modules significantly correlated with the input (negative correlation) or pulldown (positive correlation) fractions (Figure 4C, Figure 4—figure supplement 2B; Langfelder and Horvath, 2008). Of note, two of the most significantly positively correlated modules, blue and grey60, contain multiple neuronal genes of interest such as Drd1a, Drd2, Oprk1, Fosb, and Pdyn, which have previously been implicated in pair bonding and reward learning (Resendez et al., 2016; Resendez et al., 2012; Pitchers et al., 2010). Enrichr analysis for cell type also indicated that our identified neuronal enriched genes are predominantly associated with the nucleus accumbens and dorsal striatum (Figure 4D; Chen et al., 2013; Kuleshov et al., 2016; Xie et al., 2021). Finally, we validated that the gene list at the intersection of the DEGs and the WGCNA modules was predominately expressed in neurons and not glia using a publically available single nuclei NAc gene expression dataset generated from adult male rats (Savell et al., 2020). We filtered the dataset by our neuronal enriched gene list and plotted those gene’s expression in the rat nucleus accumbens, which confirmed that our genes are predominately expressed in neuronal cell types (Figure 4E).

We next queried potential neuronal transcriptional changes in our tissue-level RNAseq data. To ensure that the tissue-level expression is representative of neuron-specific expression we compared the expression patterns of the neuronal enriched genes in the input and pulldown fractions of opposite- vs same-sex long-term separated males (Figure 4—figure supplement 2D). The strong similarity in expression validates that bulk sequencing expression can be used to infer the neuronal expression of those same genes (Figure 4—figure supplement 2D). As such, we filtered the tissue-level RNAseq dataset from Figure 3D by the neuronal enriched gene list of Figure 4D to track neuronal gene expression throughout partner loss. We then performed unsupervised clustering of all genes (Figure 4F, top) and neuronal enriched genes (Figure 4F, bottom). We identified three neuronal-enriched gene clusters that had particularly interesting expression patterns. Cluster 1 consists of genes that were upregulated in opposite-sex paired males during pair bonding and short-term separation and downregulated following long-term separation. GO terms associated with these genes include adult walking behavior, a term that contains the autism-associated gene Shank3 (Figure 4G; Gong et al., 2015; Durand et al., 2007). Cluster 2 transcripts are downregulated in pair bonded males and then robustly upregulated after short-term partner loss and, to a lesser extent, at the long-term timepoint, indicating an acute transcriptional response to pair bond disruption. The GO terms associated with these genes suggest an involvement of dopaminergic systems (Drd1a and Drd2) and learning processes potentially engaged to adapt to loss (Figure 4G). Finally, Cluster 3 contains genes that were downregulated in pair bonded males and became upregulated only after long-term opposite-sex partner separation, representing transcripts that may help prime the vole to form a new bond. The GO terms associated with this cluster are primarily implicated in cellular metabolism (Figure 4G). IPA revealed that neuronal clusters 2 and 3 were also enriched for estrogen receptor signaling and oxidative phosphorylation pathways with cluster 2 also implicating opioid signaling pathways (Figure 2—figure supplement 2D). Steroid and opioid signaling have been extensively implicated in pair bonding (Resendez et al., 2016; Resendez et al., 2012; Loth and Donaldson, 2021; Inoue et al., 2013; Stetzik et al., 2018; Carter and Perkeybile, 2018; Lei et al., 2010; Cushing et al., 2008).

Discussion

Our data provide the first comprehensive assessment of social behavior and accumbal transcription in pair bonded male prairie voles before and after partner loss, providing novel insight into the biological processes that may enable loss adaptation. Prairie voles, unlike laboratory mice and rats, form exclusive lifelong pair bonds that are distinct from affiliative same-sex relationships, making them ideal for studying the unique neurobiology of bonding and loss (Getz et al., 1981; Carter et al., 1995; Pohl et al., 2019; Osako et al., 2018; Lee et al., 2019; Goodwin et al., 2019; Lee and Beery, 2021). Additionally, the direct comparison of opposite- versus same-sex paired animals provides an important control for the effects of social context – whether living with another animal or alone (Arzate-Mejía et al., 2020; Wallace et al., 2009). By leveraging this comparison across our experiments, we demonstrated that pair bonding leads to consistent and persistent shifts in NAc transcription, which erode as a function of long-term partner separation. This suggests that substantial changes in accumbal gene expression may contribute to priming the vole to form a new bond but are not sufficient to erase an existing bond as measured by partner preference. In addition, we demonstrate the utility of vTRAP for interrogating neuron-specific responses to bonding and separation that are obscured when analyzing bulk sequencing results. Together, these results expand the utility of monogamous prairie voles for future behavioral and molecular-genetic studies of partner loss and loss adaptation.

Separation disrupts the continued strengthening of pair bonds but does not diminish baseline partner preference

We observed overall similarity and persistence in social behaviors exhibited by opposite-sex and same-sex paired male prairie voles where both types displayed a partner preference despite prolonged separation. The partner preference test, as employed in our study, may be insufficient to capture the uniquely rewarding aspect of pair bonds compared with other affiliative relationships such as sibling relationships (Goodwin et al., 2019). In part, this may reflect the coarse but commonly employed metric of proximity to the partner used to measure preference, without analyzing the valence or content of specific social interactions. A more nuanced assessment that includes naturalistic behaviors and/or captures the selectively motivating aspects of a pair bond would further elucidate differences in partner-directed behavior as a function of separation (Brusman et al., 2021; Goodwin et al., 2019; Beery et al., 2021; Vahaba et al., 2022).

However, the metrics employed here revealed that opposite-sex animals that remained paired exhibited increased partner huddle duration between baseline and long-term timepoints while the separated animals do not. Thus, while partner separation does not erase an established bond, it does disrupt the observed increase in partner huddle duration of remain paired males.

Prior work demonstrates the dissolution of partner preference in male voles after four weeks of separation (Sun et al., 2014). Yet the apparent contradiction we observed in our experiments may be due to differences in study design. In particular, we paired animals for two weeks before separation while prior studies had much shorter periods of cohabitation pre-separation (Carter et al., 1995; Sun et al., 2014). As demonstrated here and in other studies, pair bonds strengthen and mature over extended cohabitation periods, which may contribute to the observed perseverance of partner preference for weeks after separation (Scribner et al., 2020; Brusman et al., 2021). In addition, our females were tubally ligated in order to prevent pregnancy without disrupting other hormonal signaling. There is no consensus regarding the effects of pregnancy on male pair bonding behaviors (Aragona and Wang, 2004; Brusman et al., 2021; DeVries et al., 1997). However, partner preference in males is not affected by female ovariectomy (Curtis, 2010). Regardless, our results suggest that male voles remember and express selective affiliation despite prolonged isolation, though partner separation impedes the ongoing bond strengthening.

Pair bonding results in consistent glia-associated transcriptional changes that erode following prolonged separation

Our studies begin to interrogate how dramatic shifts in socio-emotional context alter transcription in a mesolimbic hub—the NAc. We first asked whether bonding-induced gene expression is stable as long as pairs remain cohoused (Resendez et al., 2016; Resendez et al., 2012; Aragona et al., 2006; Aragona et al., 2003). Using three orthogonal analytical methods, we found remarkable consistency in the transcriptional pattern of opposite- vs same-sex pairs co-housed for 2 or 6 weeks. Prior work on social dominance suggests that such stable transcriptional changes are critical to maintain different behavioral states (Cardoso et al., 2015; Zayed and Robinson, 2012; Tripp et al., 2018). However, to our knowledge, this is the first demonstration that a similar conceptual underpinning may maintain mature pair bonds.

Consistently upregulated transcripts in pair bonded males are associated with glia and extracellular matrix organization while downregulated transcripts are associated with nucleoside processes, synaptic plasticity, and vesicle trafficking. Together, these terms implicate processes central to neuronal remodeling and synaptic plasticity. Of note, while our analyses do identify a number of mitochondrial GO terms, these terms are not as strongly implicated in our analyses as in previous reports (Duclot et al., 2020). Despite this, our study confirms a potential role for mitochondrial dynamics in the NAc in male prairie vole pair bonding. This is consistent with a model in which pair bonding and separation induce neuronal plasticity and synapse re-organization, which require large amounts of energy and induce mitochondrial redistribution within neurons (Ülgen et al., 2023).

We next found that pair bond transcriptional signatures erode as a function of separation duration. RRHO analyses demonstrated that there is considerable reduction in concordant pair bond gene expression following long-term but not short-term separation. When we asked which transcripts and associated biological processes erode over this timeframe, we found that they largely corresponded with those evident in the pair bond transcriptional signature. Specifically, transcripts associated with glia, extracellular matrix organization, synapse organization, and vesicle docking are stably expressed in pair bonded males but eroded following long-term separation. The erosion of the pair bond transcriptional signature over time in opposite-sex separated males may be central to adapting to partner loss in order to form a new pair bond if given the opportunity. It remains unclear though, how these changes may reflect or alter the component processes of pair bonding, such as partner memory, reward association, and/or motivation.

Our analyses strongly indicate a role for glia in regulating pair bonding and the response to partner separation. Transcripts associated with gliogenesis, glial cell differentiation, and myelination are upregulated in stable pair bonds, potentially reflecting the importance of glial cells in sculpting and refining neural circuits in response to social experience (Loth and Donaldson, 2021; Kopec et al., 2018; Pohl et al., 2021). Similar GO terms—with the addition of oligodendrocyte differentiation—are associated with eroded pair bond transcripts after long-term partner separation. Hypomyelination and oligodendrocyte dysfunction are seen in mice and rats after chronic social defeat or chronic social isolation and in individuals with depression (Bonnefil et al., 2019; Lehmann et al., 2017; Zhou et al., 2021). This appears reversible as social reintroduction rescued hypomyelination in mice (Liu et al., 2012). One mechanism for social deprivation-mediated hypomyelination and impaired oligodendrocyte maturation is by reduced flux through Erbb3-based signaling pathways (Liu et al., 2012; Makinodan et al., 2012). Of note, Erbb3 is one of 24 genes whose differential expression is conserved in taxonomically diverse monogamous species when compared to closely related non-monogamous relatives (Young et al., 2019). We find Erbb3 upregulated during pair bonding through short-term separation but it is no longer upregulated following long-term separation. Further, IPA analysis indicates that Erbb3 is an upstream regulator of the eroded genes. Thus, it is possible that long-term separation is downregulating Erbb3-associated pathways resulting in reduced myelination and disrupting oligodendrocytes specifically in opposite-sex-separated animals. An important future direction is whether bonding with a new partner re-engages these glial processes.

Neurons have distinct transcriptional responses to bonding and loss

While there is an emerging appreciation for the role of glia in mediating behavior (Thornton and Hughes, 2020; Bacmeister et al., 2022; Auguste et al., 2022; Hughes et al., 2018; Duncan and Emery, 2023; Kim et al., 2023), these effects are largely due to modulation of neuronal activity, and we reasoned that the prominence of glial-related terms may have masked important neuronal transcriptional changes in our dataset. To home in on neuronal transcriptional changes specifically, we developed vole-optimized translating ribosome affinity purification in prairie voles (vTRAP). We used DESeq, WGCNA, and a publicly available snRNAseq dataset to identify neuronal enriched genes, which show a 1.2–8.8 fold enrichment in neuronal pulldowns compared to the tissue-level sequencing data. We then queried expression changes specifically among these genes as a function of pair bonding and partner separation. With this enhanced sensitivity, we were able to identify three neuronal gene expression patterns that are differentially sensitive to partner loss. Notably, dopamine associated genes in cluster #2 are robustly upregulated in neurons after short-term partner loss, possibly reflecting a withdrawal-like state (Potretzke and Ryabinin, 2019; Young et al., 2008; Lieberwirth and Wang, 2016). This finding also underscores the discovery value of vTRAP because GO terms such as ‘response to amphetamine’, which indicates changes to dopaminergic genes, was a top 10 result, while the tissue level gene cluster predicts this GO term as the 34th result. Genes associated with anxiety and energy production in cluster #3 are upregulated only after long-term partner loss, possibly to facilitate seeking a new partner. Thus, together with our tissue level sequencing implicating the role of glia, vTRAP provides a more nuanced understanding of how specifically the pair bond neuronal transcriptional signature responds to partner separation over time.

Limitations and future directions

Pair bonding and loss are complex and multifaceted processes, engaging diverse neural circuits in temporal- and context-specific ways. Yet transcriptomics provides a single snapshot of gene expression at one point in time in one context. So while tissue-level transcriptional analysis is an excellent tool to capture an averaged biological state, one limitation of our approach is that it is not ideal for examining more temporally-dynamic processes, such as the rapid information-processing that occurs during social decision making.

A notable aspect of our results is that the substantial erosion of pair bond transcription in the NAc after long-term separation is not matched by a similar reduction in partner-directed affiliative behaviors, despite evidence that changes in gene expression in this brain region help maintain bonds over time (Aragona and Wang, 2004; Aragona et al., 2006; Aragona et al., 2003). This apparent mismatch between behavior and transcription has a few possible biological explanations. First, while our focus on the NAc is well justified, other brain regions are also important for bonding (Lim and Young, 2004; Tripp et al., 2021; Bosch et al., 2009; Gobrogge et al., 2009). Specifically, the ventral tegmental area, medial pre-frontal cortex, and the hippocampus innervate the NAc and have known roles in social behavior. Transcriptional dynamics may differ within these regions, potentially contributing to bond persistence (Okuyama et al., 2016; Smith et al., 2016; Kogan et al., 2000; Amadei et al., 2017). In addition, pair bond transcription may induce other long-term biological changes that erode more slowly. These include long-lasting epigenetic memory as well as proteomic and/or structural changes that may contribute to behavioral persistence (Tuesta and Zhang, 2014; Nestler et al., 2016). Alternatively, transcriptional changes in a key subset of cells, which may be undetectable in tissue-level RNAseq, could drive bond persistence. More refined approaches, such as single-nucleus RNAseq, could greatly enhance our understanding of the cell types regulating complex behavioral persistence. All three of these hypothesized mechanisms for bond persistence represent important future lines of inquiry.

Previous work from our lab has shown that, when given the opportunity, male voles can form a new, stable pair bond that supplants the original bond if first isolated from their previous partner for at least four weeks (Harbert et al., 2020). However, in the current study design, male voles did not have an opportunity to find a new partner or form a new bond. Under such conditions, it may be advantageous to maintain behaviors that would enable the male to resume a bond if reunited with an absent partner. Speculatively, this framework optimizes reproductive opportunities by dually enabling a new bond to form or resuming an existing bond. It is likewise tempting to extrapolate this result into the context of human experience. We do not forget our previous bonds or their emotional valence; rather we integrate the loss in order to continue on with life.

Finally, oxytocin signaling in the NAc is required for pair bond formation and maintenance in male prairie voles (Johnson et al., 2016). While nonapeptide systems were not the focus of the current study, an intriguing follow-up question is how much of the pair bond transcription and its maintenance is attributable to nonapeptide dynamics. Recent publication of pair bond formation in Oxtr null voles provides a unique opportunity to identify oxytocin-dependent and independent transcriptional changes that support pair bonds (Berendzen and Manoli, 2022).

Conclusions

In sum, we provide additional insights into the neurogenomic processes that promote and maintain pair bonds and leverage these findings to gain a unique understanding of how the brain adapts to loss. We show that, in the absence of new bonding opportunities, the original pair bond will persist for at least 4 weeks post-partner-separation. However, prior work indicates that there is still an adaptive process occurring within this time frame that primes a vole to form a new bond if available (Harbert et al., 2020). Our work suggests that this adaptation is mediated at least in part by an erosion of bond-related transcription in mesolimbic systems that largely precedes bond dissolution at the behavioral level. Together, this work and future studies will help shed light on the pain associated with loss, as well as how we integrate grief in order to meaningfully reengage with life.

Materials and methods

Animals

Sexually naive adult prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster, N=219: 161 M, 58 F, P60 - P168 at experiment start) were bred in-house in a colony originating from a cross between voles obtained from colonies at Emory University and University of California Davis, both of which were established from wild animals collected in Illinois. Animals were weaned at 21 days and housed in same-sex groups of two to four animals in standard static rodent cages (7.5 x 11.75 x 5 in.) with ad libitum water, rabbit chow (5326–3 by PMI Lab Diet) supplemented with alfalfa cubes, sunflower seeds, cotton nestlets, and igloos for enrichment until initiation of the experiment. All voles were aged between post-natal day 60 and ~180 at the start of the experiment. Throughout the experiment, animals were housed in smaller static rodent cages (11.0 x 8.0 x 6.5 in.) with ad libitum water, rabbit chow (5326–3 by PMI Lab Diet), and cotton nestlets. They were kept at 23–26°C with a 10:14 dark:light cycle to facilitate breeding. All procedures were performed in accordance with standard ethical guidelines (National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals) and approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) at the University of Colorado Boulder (protocol #2435).

Tubal ligations

All females in opposite-sex pairs were tubally ligated to avoid confounds of pregnancy, but to keep the ovaries intact as to not impact hormonal function. Females were anaesthetized with a mixture of 2% Isoflurane and O2 gas and depth of anesthetize was monitored by toe pinch throughout surgery. Prior to surgery, animals were weighed and given subcutaneous injections of the analgesic Meloxicam SR (4.0 mg/kg, Zoopharm) and saline (1 mL, Nurse Assist). Each fallopian tube was cauterized and bisected through lumbar inscitions on each side of the body using standard sterile practices. Animals were monitored once a day, for three days and staples were removed one week post-surgery. Females were given 2 weeks to recover before pairing with a male.

Pairing

For the RNAseq portion of the experiment, opposite-sex, non-sibling pairs ranged in age between P98 and P168 (average ~P122) and same-sex, sibling pairs ranged in age between P60 to P163 (average ~P111) at pairing. For the behavioral experiments, all animals ranged in age between P85 and P168 (average ~121) at the time of pairing. For RNAseq and behavior testing, at pairing, pairs were transferred into smaller static rodent cages (11.0 in. x 8.0 in. x 6.5 in.) with ad libitum water and rabbit chow (5326–3 by PMI Lab Diet), a cotton nestlet, and an igloo. Female partners had been previously tubally ligated and were not induced prior to pairing. Partners cohabitated undisturbed, except for weekly cage changing performed by the same member of animal care staff, for 2 weeks prior to the baseline partner preference test (PPT). Same-sex pairings, sibling pairs from the same homecage were moved to smaller static rodent cages in the same way as the opposite-sex pairs. After the baseline PPT, RNAseq and behavior experiment animals that were either returned to their partner (Remain Paired condition) or were separated into individual cages with no opportunity to see or smell their partner (Separated conditions).

Separation for behavioral tests

Immediately following the baseline PPT, each pair was separated into fresh small static rodent cages (11.0 in. x 8.0 in. x 6.5 in.) with ad libitum water and rabbit chow (5326–3 by PMI Lab Diet), a cotton nestlet, and an igloo.

Separated animals were placed in an adjacent vole vivarium to eliminate visual and olfactory cues of the partner during the separation period. Each animal in the pair remained in their individual cages undisturbed for either 48 hours (short-term separation) or 4 weeks (long-term separation) except for weekly cage changes done by the same animal care staff member. At each separation time point, experimental animals performed a partner preference test (PPT) starting between 9:00 and 10:00 AM each day.

Partner preference test (PPT)

PPT was carried out as described by Ahern et al. to assess selective partner affiliation prior to partner separation and were performed between the hours of 9:00 and 10:00 AM each testing day (Ahern et al., 2009). Each 3 hr test was recorded by overhead cameras (Panasonic WVCP304) that film two boxes simultaneously. The test animal is placed in the middle chamber for 10 minutes with the cage dividers still in place. The test begins when the cage dividers are removed and the test animal is allowed free range of all three chambers for the duration of the test. The movement of the test animal was tracked post-hoc using Topscan High-Throughput software (v3.0, Cleversys Inc) and frame by frame behavioral data was analyzed via a custom Python based script (https://github.com/donaldsonlab/separationRNAseq; copy archived at Sadino, 2023) to calculate the time spent huddling with each tethered animal. This data was then used to generate a number of behavior metrics including the partner preference score (Partner Huddle/Partner +Novel Huddle) (https://Figureshare.com/authors/zoe_donaldson_colorado_edu_Donaldson/6883910).

Statistical analysis for behavioral experiments

All statistical analyses were performed in R (v4.0.4) using the base t.test package, base aov package, and the survival package (v3.2–7). Visualization was done using ggplot2 (v3.3.3), ggpubr (v0.4.0), and Adobe Illustrator (25.2.3). All statistical results can be found in Supplementary file 1. For PPT, we first determined if partner preference significantly changed from 50% at each time point (baseline, ST, or LT) and for each pairing type (OS or SS) by using an unpaired, one-sided Student’s t-test against a null value of 50. We also performed a 2-way repeated measures ANOVA with Tukey’s post-hoc test to determine main effects of, and interactions between, the pairing type and separation duration. Next, we calculated partner preference change scores for all conditions and compared the variance between pairing types using a Levene’s test—the non-parametric equivalent of a F-test of variance. Finally, we determined changes to partner and novel huddling latency using the built in logRank test of the survminer package (v0.4.8).

Generation of vole optimized hSyn-DIO-eGFP-RPL10a construct for vTRAP

Prior to generating the vTRAP viral construct, the nucleotide and amino acid sequences for RPL10a in mice and prairie voles were compared. To assess nucleotide homology, the mouse RPL10a cDNA sequence was aligned to the vole RPL10a cDNA sequence using BLASTN (v2.8.033) with a percent identity of 93% and an E-value of 0.0 (Figure 4—figure supplement 1A). The mouse RPL10a cDNA sequence was input into BLASTX (v2.8.033), and the resulting protein alignment against the prairie vole RPL10a amino acid sequence was 100% identical with an E-value of 3E–145 (Figure 4—figure supplement 1B). Although the amino acid sequences are identical between mice and prairie voles, we generated the vTRAP construct using the prairie vole RPL10a cDNA sequence to account for possible species-specific differences in aminoacyl-tRNA concentrations.

To generate the TRAP construct, the DNA fragment containing the vole RPL10a sequence was inserted into the pAAV-hSyn-DIO-eGFP (Addgene plasmid #50457) backbone. The RPL10a fragment was designed for cloning using the NEBuilder HiFi Assembly method and consisted of a 5’ homology arm sequence, a linker sequence coding for SGRTQISSSSFEF, the vole RPL10a sequence (GenBank XM_005360358.1), and a 3’ homology arm sequence (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). pAAV-hSyn-DIO-eGFP was digested using restriction endonucleases Asc1 (NEB) and BsrG1-HF (NEB), and the linearized plasmid was purified using the Zymogen Gel DNA Recovery Kit. The cloned plasmid, hereafter referred to as pAAV-hSyn-DIO-eGFPpvRPL10a (vTRAP),was generated using the NEBuilder HiFi DNA Assembly Cloning kit and subsequently transfected into 5-alpha F'Iq Competent E. coli (NEB). Transfected bacteria were selected for using carbenicillin (100 µg/ml) and harvested using the EZDNA Plasmid Mini Kit (Omega) or HiSpeed Plasmid Maxi Kit (Qiagen). The sequence of vTRAP was verified using Sanger sequencing (QuintaraBio) and restriction digest before being sent for viral packaging in an AAV1 serotype (Stanford Neuroscience Gene Vector and Virus Core).

Viral injections

For stereotaxic surgery, adult prairie voles were anesthetized using isoflurane (3% induction, 1–2.5% maintenance) and depth of anesthesia was monitored by breathing and toe pinch reflex. After induction, animals were placed on a heating pad and given an analgesic (2 mg/kg Meloxicam SR, subcutaneous, Zoopharm) and sterile saline (1 mL, subcutaneous). Standard sterile surgical procedures were followed to expose and drill through the skull. Nucleus accumbens injection coordinates were adapted from the Allen Mouse Brain Atlas and validated prior to the experiment (AP: –1.7, ML:±1.0, DV: −4.7,–4.6, −4.5,–4.4, –4.3). For each DV coordinate, 100 nL of virus was dispensed at 0.1 nL/s for a total of 500 nL using a Nanoject II (Drummond Scientific). After the last injection, the needle remained in place for 10 minutes to allow for diffusion. The glass needle was then removed and the skin was sutured closed (vicryl sutures, eSutures), coasted with lidocaine and antibacterial ointment, and animals recovered in a cage on a heating pad with continued monitoring. The animal’s health was monitored for 3 days following surgery and they were allowed to recover in their homecage for 2 weeks prior to the experiment start.

Verification of TRAP expression

Cre-dependent expression of the vole optimized eGFP-RPL10a custom virus was validated in adult male prairie voles (n=3) using a within animal design. The stereotaxic injection coordinates for the NAc were adapted from the Paxinios and Franklin mouse brain atlas (the compact third edition) and were:+1.7 AP,+/-1 ML, –4.7 to –4.4 DV. The left NAc was injected with a viral cocktail of AAV1-DIO-eGFP-RPL10a (2 µL; 5.55E11 vg/mL) and AAV1-hSyn-iCre (2 µL; 3.9E12 vg/mL) while the right NAc only received the AAV1-DIO-eGFP-RPL10a virus (600 nL; 2.22E+12 vg/mL). Following 2 weeks of recovery to allow for robust viral expression the animal was perfused. The animal received a lethal injection of 0.2 mL 1:2 ketamine/xylazine and was perfused with 1 X PBS followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (in 1 X PBS, Electron Microscopy Sciences) and kept in 4% PFA overnight. The brain was then placed in 5 mL of 30% sucrose for 3 days prior to sectioning on a microtome (Leica JungSM2000R, 50 microns/slice). Sections were wet mounted onto Superfrost Plus glass slides (Thermo Fisher Scientific), cover slipped (ProLong Gold; Invitrogen), and allowed to dry overnight prior to imaging. Prepared slides were then sealed with nail polish (Electron Microscopy Sciences) and imaged using a Yokogawa CV1000 Confocal Scanner System (University of Colorado Light Microscopy Core Facility). Stitched images were produced using ImageJ (v1.51).

Separation for RNAseq experiments

Immediately following the baseline PPT, each pair was moved to fresh small static rodent cages (11.0 in. x 8.0 in. x 6.5 in.) with ad libitum water and rabbit chow (5326–3 by PMI Lab Diet), a cotton nestlet, and an igloo. Remain Paired animals were kept with their partner while Separated animals were separated from their partner and moved to a fresh cage while their partner was immediately sacrificed to remove confounds of visual or olfactory cues. All animals remained in their individual cages undisturbed for either 48 hours (short-term separation) or 4 weeks (long-term separation) except for weekly cage changes done by the experimenter. After the appropriate separation time point, experimental animals were removed directly from their home cage for rapid decapitation and tissue harvesting.

RNA preparation and sequencing

Tissue lysis and RNA isolation was adapted from Heiman and performed under RNase free conditions (Heiman et al., 2014). On each tissue collection day, 2 animals from each experimental cohort were processed, with the exception of same-sex 4-week paired animals who were all collected on the same day. Animals were removed from home cages, rapid decapped, and both hemispheres of the nucleus accumbens were hand dissected using sterile, RNase free razor blades and pooled. The NAc coordinates for dissections were:+1.7 AP,+/-1 ML, –4.7 to –4.4 DV with the TRAP dissections additionally guided by GFP fluorescence visualized with a fluorescent dissecting scope. During dissection, tissue was kept wet with dissection buffer (HEPES KOH, pH 7.3 20 mM, 1 X HBSS, Glucose 35 mM, NaHCO3 4 mM, CHX 10 uL of 1000 X stock, DEPC-treated water up to 10 mL). Tissue was added to 1 mL of ice-cold lysis buffer (DEPC-treated water, HEPES 20 mM, KCl 150 mM, MgCl2 (10 mM), DTT 5 µL of 1 M stock, CHX 10 µL of 1000 X stock, 1 protease inhibitor, RNasin 400 Units, and Superasin 800 Units), immediately homogenized using a Scilogex homogenizer, and kept on wet ice while the remaining animals for the day were processed. The homogenizer tip was thoroughly cleaned in between animals with DEPC-treated water and 70% ethanol to eliminate cross contamination between samples. Homogenized samples were centrifuged at 4 °C, for 10 min at ~3000 Xg and supernatent was transferred to new, pre-chilled tubes. 10% NP-40 (AG Scientific: #P-1505) and 300 mM DHPC (Avanti Polar Lipids, Inc: 850306 P-200 mg) were added to the supernatent and incubated on ice (4 °C) for 5 min. Samples were then centrifuged at 4 °C for 10 min at ~16,000 Xg and supernatent was transferred to new, pre-chilled tubes. RNA was then extracted and cleaned from the lysate supernatent using the Norgen Total RNA micro kit (cat. #) according to the manufacturer’s instructions. RNA integrity was measured using an Agilent Tapestation prior to library preparation. RINs for all samples were >7.3 (average 8.8).

For TRAP samples, dissections and Input fraction RNA were prepared as above with the addition of GFP-guided dissections using a fluorescent dissecting scope. To perform immunoprecipitation pulldowns, affinity matrices were prepared fresh day-of using 300 µL of 10 mg/mL Streptavidin MyOne T1 Dynabeads (Invitrogen), 150 µL of 1 µg/µL Biotinylated Protein L (Fisher Scientific), and 50 µg each of eGFP antibodies Htz-19C8 and Htz-19F7 (Memorial-Sloan Kettering Monoclonal Antibody Facility) 2 hr prior to adding Input RNA sample. A small sample of Input RNA was saved for next day clean up (~250 µL/sample) and frozen at –80 °C overnight. The remaining Input RNA (~1 mL) was added to the GFP-conjugated beads and rotated overnight (16–18 hr) at 4 °C. The next day, the flowthrough was saved and stored at –80 °C and bound mRNA was released from the beads using a series of high salt buffer washes (4 washes; 20 mM HEPES, 150 mM KCl, 10 mM MgCl2, 10% NP-40, RNase-free water, protease inhibitor tablets, 0.5 mM DTT, 100 µg/µl cycloheximide). The beads were then incubated with the Norgen Total RNA micro kit SKP buffer (300 µL) for 10 min at room temperature. The Pulldown supernatant was then saved and processed as above using the kit manufacturer’s instructions.

Library preparation for sequencing was done using the KAPA mRNA HyperPrep kit with polyA enrichment according to the manufacturer’s instructions for a 100–200 bp insert size and using 1.5 µM TruSeq3-SE adapters. 71 samples were pooled and 3 runs of NextSeq V2 high output 75 cycle (1X75) sequencing produced a total read depth of ~20 M reads/sample. The Illumina sequencing was performed at the BioFrontiers Institute Next-Gen Sequencing Core Facility of University of Colorado Boulder. RNA quantity, RIN, and RNA amount used for library preparation can be found in Supplementary file 1.

Sequence mapping and counting

For quality control, FastQC (0.11.8) and Trimmomatic-Src (v0.38) were used to analyze read quality and to trim the TruSeq3-SE adapters from all samples (Bolger et al., 2014). Additionally, each sequencing run was correlated to each other to ensure that there was not a significant difference in run composition. Reads were aligned to the prairie vole genome (Microtus_ochrogaster.MicOch1.0.dna.toplevel, released Feb 2017; accession GCA_000317375) using HiSat2 (2.1.0) with default options and individual runs for each animal were merged into a single sample using SAMtools (1.9) (Kim et al., 2015; Danecek et al., 2021). Reads were then counted using HTseq (0.11.2) with default options using the annotated prairie vole genome (Microtus_ochrogaster.MicOch1.0.95.gtf, released Feb 2017; accession GCA_000317375) (Anders et al., 2015). Genes with less than 10 read counts were filtered from downstream analysis and, where necessary, were normalized using transcripts per million (TPM). Where possible, all Ensembl IDs were converted to prairie vole gene IDs using BioMart. If no gene ID was found, the original Ensembl ID was retained. Only genes with greater than 10 counts were included in subsequent analyses (total genes = 21152, genes >10 counts = 12128).

Data analysis and visualization

All sequencing analysis was performed using R (v4.0.4) (R:The R project for statistical computing, 2022). The packages used and their specific parameters are described in the relevant sections below. Plots were made using base plot, ggplot2 (v3.3.3), EnhancedVolcano (1.8.0), RRHO2 (v1.0), clusterProfiler (v3.18.1), and Adobe Illustrator (25.2.3) (Cahill et al., 2018; Yu et al., 2012; Wickham, 2016; Blighe, 2021).

DESeq2

Differential gene expression was analyzed with the R package DESeq2 (v1.30.1) following the standard workflow (Love et al., 2014). To determine differential gene expression between cohorts a DESeq dataset of all animals was made with the design ~ParentCode + AgeAtPairing + Cohort to correct for the effect of parentage and age at initial pairing. Other batch effects (processing day, processing order, RIN, etc.) did not have sufficient influence on the model to include. Each result was a pair-wise contrast between the appropriate cohorts with the opposite-sex condition as the experimental and the same-sex condition as the reference. Differential gene expression was defined as >0.30 and<−0.30 log2FoldChange and p-value <0.05. This log2FoldChange threshold was used for two reasons. (1) A threshold of +/-0.20 can be successfully validated by qPCR (Walker et al., 2018) but (2) we chose a slightly higher threshold as it allows for smaller gene lists that appear more biologically relevant by Gene Ontology analysis. Differential gene expression was visualized using EnhancedVolcano. To determine if there was statistically significant overlap between differentially expressed gene lists compared to all genes (n=12,128) we performed a Fisher’s exact test using the SuperExactTest (v1.0.7) package (Wang et al., 2015). Additionally, we used Morpheus (https://software.broadinstitute.org/morpheus) heatmaps to compare the expression profiles of each DESeq comparison using scaled log2FoldChange values. For each heatmap, a reference dataset ordered the remaining datasets such that each column is the same gene and the color represents the scaled log2FoldChange in each DESeq comparison. In Figure 2F the short-term pair bond was set as the reference and in Figure 3F, J and L the combined pair bond was set as the reference. Gene ontology and pathway analysis of relevant gene lists were performed using the R package enrichGO with Mus musculus ontology terms (Yu et al., 2012). Representative terms with an adjusted p-value of <0.05 were hand selected. Results were visualized using base R plots modified in Illustrator. All DESeq result data frames can be found in Supplementary file 2.

Rank-rank hypergeometric overlap (RRHO)

RRHO is a threshold free approach used to identify genome-wide expression patterns between two ranked gene lists. RRHO analysis was performed using the RRHO2 (v1.0) package with parameters of stepsize = 100 and boundary = 0.02. First, transcripts from each DESeq comparison are ranked by their p-value and effect size direction:

rankValue=1log10(pvalue)sign(Log2FoldChange)

This results in a ranked list where the most significantly upregulated genes are at the top of the list and the most significantly downregulated genes are at the bottom of the list with each gene appearing only once. Next, two ranked gene lists are compared to each other using a hypergeometric distribution to determine the significance of the number of overlapping genes within defined sample populations. Sample populations are independently defined for each list. Each population consists of all of the genes that are ranked higher than a specified threshold. Thresholds successively move down a list at a user specified step size. For computation convenience, here we define our step size as 100 genes. However, the smallest possible step size is 1 gene and the largest possible step size is the number of genes found in the smallest list. The hypergeometric p-values resulting from comparing the List 1 genes (all genes above threshold x) to List 2 (all genes above threshold y) then populate a square matrix. As a result, the furthest bottom-left position of the matrix is the p-value of the overlap between genes ranked above the 1st threshold in each list and the furthest top-right position is the overlap between genes ranked above the last threshold in each list. As another example, the furthest bottom-right position is the p-value comparing all genes above the last threshold of List 1 to all genes above the 1st threshold of List 2. The final matrix of hypergeometric p-values is visualized as a heatmap where List 1 is along the x-axis and List 2 is along the y-axis and each point represents a p-value. The axes are arranged such that genes that are upregulated in both lists are in the bottom-left quadrant (quadrant UU) and genes that are downregulated in both lists are in the top-right quadrant (quadrant DD). Genes with opposite regulation in each condition, up in one list but down in the other, are along the opposite diagonal (quadrants UD and DU) (Figure 2E). To visualize RRHO our heatmaps with a consistent p-value scale we set the scale maximum = 300 and the minimum = 1.

The genes in each quadrant were extracted to form UU, DD, UD, and DU gene lists for further analysis. Using these gene lists, the significant overlap between quadrants were determined using the SuperExactTest package. Gene ontology and pathway analysis of these gene lists were performed using the R package enrichGO with Mus musculus ontology terms. Representative terms with an adjusted p-value of <0.05 were hand selected. For the short-term vs long-term separation RRHO only UU and DD quadrant genes were used to filter the DESeq2 results of the pair bond, short-term separation, and long-term separation. We then further filtered the pair bond data by a log2FoldChange <–0.30 for the UU genes and >0.30 for the DD genes.

Additional information for RRHOs are in Figure S8. Unscaled p-value RRHOs can be found in Figure S8A-C. Since the p-value of the hypergeometirc distribution is sensitive to the number of genes, RRHO also plots a log-Odds ratio for all comparisons. Log-Odds ratio plots can be found in Figure S8D-F where the maximum scale was changed from 8 to 6 on all plots to better visualize values. Finally, to ensure that the signal we see in the RRHOs is not due to chance, we shuffled the ranked order of List 1 in each RRHO (Figure S8G-I). The resulting lack of signal throughout the RRHOs support that the signal we see in our experimental RRHOs is not due to chance.

Pathway and upstream regulator analysis with ingenuity pathway analysis (IPA)

Ingenuity pathway analysis (Qiagen, Germantown, MD), was used to determine potential biological pathways and upstream regulators of DEGs and co-regulated genes identified using RRHO. Core expression analysis was applied for each gene list followed by comparison analysis for related lists to help identify common pathways and regulators across groups. For both, outcomes were filtered to include only those that reached an activation z-score of +/-2 and a corrected p-value of <0.05 (corrected by Benjamini Hochberg). All predicted biological pathways and upstream regulators are presented in Supplementary file 3.

Weighted correlation network analysis (WGCNA)

A weighted gene co-expression network analysis was conducted using the R package WGCNA (v1.70–3) (Langfelder and Horvath, 2008). WGCNA analysis was done using the Input and Pulldown sequencing data from only animals in the vTRAP cohorts (opposite-sex remain paired 4 weeks, opposite-sex separated 4 weeks, and same-sex separated 4 weeks). A signed co-expression network was constructed using a power of 9 and minimum module size of 30 genes according to the author’s standard module-trait relationship workflow. No samples were designated as outliers using complete clustering. Gene modules were then correlated to either the trait Input or Pulldown to determine the gene clusters associated with each sample type. To determine which correlation direction (positive or negative) is associated with which sample type, the significantly positively or negatively correlated modules were grouped and analyzed by Enrichr for cell types (Chen et al., 2013; Kuleshov et al., 2016; Xie et al., 2021). The top term for the positively correlated modules was nucleus accumbens while the top term for the negatively correlated modules was spinal cord. Therefore, the positively correlated modules represent the Pulldown samples, and by extension, neuronally enriched genes.

Neuronal enriched genes throughout separation

To robustly define neuronally enriched genes we found the intersection of the vTRAP Input vs Pulldown enriched DEGs (Figure 4B) and the genes from the significantly, positively correlated WGCNA modules (Figure 4D). The resulting intersection—2365 genes— is referred to as ‘neuronal enriched genes’ and includes almost all of the vTRAP enriched DEGs. To further validate the neuronal enriched genes we analyzed this gene list using Enrichr for cell types. The top two terms are nucleus accumbens and dorsal striatum confirming the specificity of this gene list.

We next wanted to follow the expression of these neuronal enriched genes throughout separation. First, since we only had vTRAP samples from specific cohorts in our experiment, we ensured that neuronal gene expression can be inferred from bulk sequencing of the same genes. To validate this, we filtered the Input and Pulldown samples of the opposite-sex 4-week separated cohort and compared the expression of the neuronal enriched genes using Morpheus (Figure 4—figure supplement 2D). The strong similarity in expression of the neuronal enriched genes using two different collection methods validated that bulk sequencing expression can be used to infer the neuron specific expression of those same genes. Second, to compare the expression patterns of the neuronal enriched genes to the bulk sequencing data we filtered the bulk data for only the neuronal genes. We then clustered the unfiltered bulk sequencing data, from 3F, and the neuronal enriched genes only bulk sequencing data using one minus Pearson correlation with complete linkage. For each neuronal cluster, we then found the corresponding cluster in the unfiltered expression data with the correspondence links visualized by a Sankey plot using the R package networkD3 (version 0.4)(Allaire et al., 2017). Third, we identified three clusters in the neuronal enriched data that had differential expression based on separation duration. We analyzed each gene list using Gene Ontology as previously described and highlighted specific genes associated with top terms. Which clusters were spotlighted in Figure 4F was determined by a combination of transcriptional patterns due to separation, which genes are in the cluster, and the resulting GO terms for each cluster. We identified clusters that represented three interesting patterns: (1.) down only after long-term pair bond separation (cluster 1) (2.) up due to acute pair separation (cluster 2) and (3.) up only after long-term pair bond separation (cluster 3).

Acknowledgements

We thank Robin Dowell, Mary Allen, Ryan Logan, Xiangning Xue, and Gracie Sapp for their assistance in designing the experiments and analysis. We also thank Cayla Jo Paulson and Jessica Abazaris of the animal care staff at the University of Colorado Boulder. We thank the rest of the Donaldson lab for their feedback and support and the voles for their sacrifice. For vTRAP images we used the Molecular, Cellular, and Developmental Biology Light Microscopy Core at the University of Colorado Boulder with James Orth’s assistance. This work was supported by NIH awards DP2OD026143, R01MH125423, and funds from the Whitehall Foundation and the Dana Foundation (to ZRD) and NIH award T32 GM008759-17/18 (to JMS).

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Zoe R Donaldson, Email: zoe.donaldson@colorado.edu.

Margaret M McCarthy, University of Maryland School of Medicine, United States.

Kate M Wassum, University of California, Los Angeles, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Institutes of Health T32 GM008759-17/18 to Julie M Sadino.

  • Dana Foundation to Zoe R Donaldson.

  • Whitehall Foundation to Zoe R Donaldson.

  • National Institutes of Health DP2OD026143 to Zoe R Donaldson.

  • National Institutes of Health R01MH125423 to Zoe R Donaldson.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Supervision, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Project administration, Writing - review and editing.

Investigation.

Investigation.

Data curation, Formal analysis, Visualization, Writing - review and editing.

Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Visualization, Writing - review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Visualization, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Project administration, Writing - review and editing.

Ethics

All procedures were performed in accordance with standard ethical guidelines (National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals) and approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) at the University of Colorado Boulder (protocol #2435).

Additional files

Supplementary file 1. Comprehensive statistical analyses and individual animal information.

10.6084/m9.figshare.19911541. Statistical analyses for behavior and transcription experiments including applicable post-hocs and version numbers of analysis packages used. Also includes relevant metrics for all experimental animals including information about excluded animals.

elife-80517-supp1.xlsx (120.2KB, xlsx)
Supplementary file 2. DESeq, RRHO, and Fisher’s Exact Test intersection gene lists.

10.6084/m9.figshare.19911577. Workbook includes all gene lists used in analysis. For DESeq, all results dataframes and differentially expressed genes are included with all output metrics. RRHO tabs include genes in each list used to generate heatmaps and overlapping genes for each quadrant as well as genes in the Fisher’s Exact Test intersections. Descriptions of all column names, the associated figure, and versions of packages used are in the last three tabs.

elife-80517-supp2.xlsx (8.1MB, xlsx)
Supplementary file 3. Ingenuity Pathway Analysis data.

10.6084/m9.figshare.19911583. Comprehensive output from IPA analyses for both Canonical Pathways and Upstream Regulators. Includes all terms with those that are statistically significant highlighted in red.

elife-80517-supp3.xlsx (86.9KB, xlsx)
MDAR checklist

Data availability

All sequencing data is available on GEO (GSE192661). All lab specific code is available on the Donaldson Lab GitHub (https://github.com/donaldsonlab/separationRNAseq; copy archived at Sadino, 2023) and all metadata including comprehensive statistical analyses, animal information, differential gene expression, gene sets, and IPA analyses are available on the Donaldson lab Figshare. The vole optimized DIO-eGFP-pvRPL10a vector will be made available on Addgene.

The following datasets were generated:

Sadino JM. 2022. Supplemental Data 1. figshare.

Sadino JM. 2022. Supplemental Data 2. figshare.

Sadino JM. 2022. Supplemental Data 3. figshare.

Sadino JM. 2023. RNAseq data from: Prolonged partner separation erodes nucleus accumbens transcriptional signatures of pair bonding in male prairie voles. NCBI Gene Expression Omnibus. GSE192661

References

  1. Ahern TH, Modi ME, Burkett JP, Young LJ. Evaluation of two automated metrics for analyzing partner preference tests. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2009;182:180–188. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2009.06.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Allaire JJ, Gandrud C, Russell K, Yetman CJ. NetworkD3: D3 javascript network graphs from R. 0.4CRAN. 2017 https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=networkD3
  3. Amadei EA, Johnson ZV, Jun Kwon Y, Shpiner AC, Saravanan V, Mays WD, Ryan SJ, Walum H, Rainnie DG, Young LJ, Liu RC. Dynamic corticostriatal activity biases social bonding in monogamous female prairie voles. Nature. 2017;546:297–301. doi: 10.1038/nature22381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Anders S, Pyl PT, Huber W. HTSeq -- a python framework to work with high-throughput sequencing data. Bioinformatics. 2015;31:166–169. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btu638. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Aragona BJ, Liu Y, Curtis JT, Stephan FK, Wang Z. A critical role for nucleus accumbens dopamine in partner-preference formation in male prairie voles. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23:3483–3490. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-08-03483.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Aragona BJ, Wang Z. The prairie vole (Microtus ochrogaster): an animal model for behavioral neuroendocrine research on pair bonding. ILAR Journal. 2004;45:35–45. doi: 10.1093/ilar.45.1.35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Aragona BJ, Liu Y, Yu YJ, Curtis JT, Detwiler JM, Insel TR, Wang Z. Nucleus accumbens dopamine differentially mediates the formation and maintenance of monogamous pair bonds. Nature Neuroscience. 2006;9:133–139. doi: 10.1038/nn1613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Arzate-Mejía RG, Lottenbach Z, Schindler V, Jawaid A, Mansuy IM. Long-Term impact of social isolation and molecular underpinnings. Frontiers in Genetics. 2020;11:589621. doi: 10.3389/fgene.2020.589621. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Auguste YSS, Ferro A, Kahng JA, Xavier AM, Dixon JR, Vrudhula U, Nichitiu AS, Rosado D, Wee TL, Pedmale UV, Cheadle L. Oligodendrocyte precursor cells engulf synapses during circuit remodeling in mice. Nature Neuroscience. 2022;25:1273–1278. doi: 10.1038/s41593-022-01170-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bacmeister CM, Huang R, Osso LA, Thornton MA, Conant L, Chavez AR, Poleg-Polsky A, Hughes EG. Motor learning drives dynamic patterns of intermittent myelination on learning-activated axons. Nature Neuroscience. 2022;25:1300–1313. doi: 10.1038/s41593-022-01169-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Beery AK, Lopez SA, Blandino KL, Lee NS, Bourdon NS. Social selectivity and social motivation in voles. eLife. 2021;10:e72684. doi: 10.7554/eLife.72684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Berendzen KM, Manoli DS. Rethinking the architecture of attachment: new insights into the role for oxytocin signaling. Affective Science. 2022;3:734–748. doi: 10.1007/s42761-022-00142-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Blighe K. Kevinblighe/enhancedvolcano. 7abca28Github. 2021 https://github.com/kevinblighe/EnhancedVolcano
  14. Blocker TD, Ophir AG. A preference to bond? male prairie voles form pair bonds even in the presence of multiple receptive females. Animal Behaviour. 2016;122:89–97. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.10.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bolger AM, Lohse M, Usadel B. Trimmomatic: a flexible trimmer for illumina sequence data. Bioinformatics. 2014;30:2114–2120. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btu170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bonnefil V, Dietz K, Amatruda M, Wentling M, Aubry AV, Dupree JL, Temple G, Park HJ, Burghardt NS, Casaccia P, Liu J. Region-Specific myelin differences define behavioral consequences of chronic social defeat stress in mice. eLife. 2019;8:e40855. doi: 10.7554/eLife.40855. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bosch OJ, Nair HP, Ahern TH, Neumann ID, Young LJ. The CRF system mediates increased passive stress-coping behavior following the loss of a bonded partner in a monogamous rodent. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34:1406–1415. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Bosch OJ, Dabrowska J, Modi ME, Johnson ZV, Keebaugh AC, Barrett CE, Ahern TH, Guo J, Grinevich V, Rainnie DG, Neumann ID, Young LJ. Oxytocin in the nucleus accumbens shell reverses CRFR2-evoked passive stress-coping after partner loss in monogamous male prairie voles. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2016;64:66–78. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.11.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Brusman LE, Protter DSW, Fultz AC, Paulson MU, Chapel GD, Elges IO, Cameron RT, Beery AK, Donaldson ZR. Emergent Intra-Pair Sex Differences and Behavioral Coordination in Pair Bonded Prairie Voles. bioRxiv. 2021 doi: 10.1101/2021.09.03.458892. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  20. Burkett JP, Andari E, Johnson ZV, Curry DC, de Waal FBM, Young LJ. Oxytocin-Dependent consolation behavior in rodents. Science. 2016;351:375–378. doi: 10.1126/science.aac4785. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Cahill KM, Huo Z, Tseng GC, Logan RW, Seney ML. Improved identification of concordant and discordant gene expression signatures using an updated rank-rank hypergeometric overlap approach. Scientific Reports. 2018;8:9588. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-27903-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Cardoso SD, Teles MC, Oliveira RF. Neurogenomic mechanisms of social plasticity. The Journal of Experimental Biology. 2015;218:140–149. doi: 10.1242/jeb.106997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Carter CS, DeVries AC, Getz LL. Physiological substrates of mammalian monogamy: the prairie vole model. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 1995;19:303–314. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(94)00070-h. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Carter CS, Perkeybile AM. The monogamy paradox: what do love and sex have to do with it? Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution. 2018;6:202. doi: 10.3389/fevo.2018.00202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Chen EY, Tan CM, Kou Y, Duan Q, Wang Z, Meirelles GV, Clark NR, Ma’ayan A. Enrichr: interactive and collaborative HTML5 gene list enrichment analysis tool. BMC Bioinformatics. 2013;14:128. doi: 10.1186/1471-2105-14-128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Curtis JT. Does fertility Trump monogamy? Animal Behaviour. 2010;80:319–328. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.05.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Cushing BS, Perry A, Musatov S, Ogawa S, Papademetriou E. Estrogen receptors in the medial amygdala inhibit the expression of male prosocial behavior. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:10399–10403. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1928-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Danecek P, Bonfield JK, Liddle J, Marshall J, Ohan V, Pollard MO, Whitwham A, Keane T, McCarthy SA, Davies RM, Li H. Twelve years of samtools and bcftools. GigaScience. 2021;10:giab008. doi: 10.1093/gigascience/giab008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. DeVries AC, Johnson CL, Carter CS. Familiarity and gender influence social preferences in prairie voles ( microtus ochrogaster ) Canadian Journal of Zoology. 1997;75:295–301. doi: 10.1139/z97-037. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. Duclot F, Sailer L, Koutakis P, Wang Z, Kabbaj M. Transcriptomic regulations underlying pair-bond formation and maintenance in the socially monogamous male and female prairie vole. Biological Psychiatry. 2020;1:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.11.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Duncan GJ, Emery B. End of the road: astrocyte endfeet regulate OPC migration and myelination. Neuron. 2023;111:139–141. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2022.12.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Durand CM, Betancur C, Boeckers TM, Bockmann J, Chaste P, Fauchereau F, Nygren G, Rastam M, Gillberg IC, Anckarsäter H, Sponheim E, Goubran-Botros H, Delorme R, Chabane N, Mouren-Simeoni M-C, de Mas P, Bieth E, Rogé B, Héron D, Burglen L, Gillberg C, Leboyer M, Bourgeron T. Mutations in the gene encoding the synaptic scaffolding protein SHANK3 are associated with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Genetics. 2007;39:25–27. doi: 10.1038/ng1933. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Getz LL, Carter CS, Gavish L. The mating system of the prairie vole, Microtus ochrogaster: field and laboratory evidence for pair-bonding. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 1981;8:189–194. doi: 10.1007/BF00299829. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  34. Gobrogge KL, Liu Y, Young LJ, Wang Z. Anterior hypothalamic vasopressin regulates pair-bonding and drug-induced aggression in a monogamous rodent. PNAS. 2009;106:19144–19149. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908620106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Gong P, Liu J, Blue PR, Li S, Zhou X. Serotonin receptor gene (HTR2A) T102C polymorphism modulates individuals’ perspective taking ability and autistic-like traits. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2015;9:575. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Goodwin NL, Lopez SA, Lee NS, Beery AK. Comparative role of reward in long-term peer and mate relationships in voles. Hormones and Behavior. 2019;111:70–77. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2018.10.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Grippo AJ, Cushing BS, Carter CS. Depression-Like behavior and stressor-induced neuroendocrine activation in female prairie voles exposed to chronic social isolation. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2007;69:149–157. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31802f054b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Harbert KJ, Pellegrini M, Gordon KM, Donaldson ZR. How prior pair-bonding experience affects future bonding behavior in monogamous prairie voles. Hormones and Behavior. 2020;126:104847. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2020.104847. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Heiman M, Schaefer A, Gong S, Peterson JD, Day M, Ramsey KE, Suárez-Fariñas M, Schwarz C, Stephan DA, Surmeier DJ, Greengard P, Heintz N. A translational profiling approach for the molecular characterization of CNS cell types. Cell. 2008;135:738–748. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2008.10.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Heiman M, Kulicke R, Fenster RJ, Greengard P, Heintz N. Cell type-specific mRNA purification by translating ribosome affinity purification (TRAP) Nature Protocols. 2014;9:1282–1291. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2014.085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Holmes TH, Rahe RH. The social readjustment rating scale. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 1967;11:213–218. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(67)90010-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Hughes EG, Orthmann-Murphy JL, Langseth AJ, Bergles DE. Myelin remodeling through experience-dependent oligodendrogenesis in the adult somatosensory cortex. Nature Neuroscience. 2018;21:696–706. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0121-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Inoue K, Burkett JP, Young LJ. Neuroanatomical distribution of μ-opioid receptor mrna and binding in monogamous prairie voles (microtus ochrogaster) and non-monogamous meadow voles (microtus pennsylvanicus) Neuroscience. 2013;244:122–133. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.03.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Insel TR, Young LJ. The neurobiology of attachment. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2001;2:129–136. doi: 10.1038/35053579. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Johnson ZV, Walum H, Jamal YA, Xiao Y, Keebaugh AC, Inoue K, Young LJ. Central oxytocin receptors mediate mating-induced partner preferences and enhance correlated activation across forebrain nuclei in male prairie voles. Hormones and Behavior. 2016;79:8–17. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.11.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Johnson ZV, Young LJ. Oxytocin and vasopressin neural networks: implications for social behavioral diversity and translational neuroscience. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2017;76:87–98. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Keyes KM, Pratt C, Galea S, McLaughlin KA, Koenen KC, Shear MK. The burden of loss: unexpected death of a loved one and psychiatric disorders across the life course in a national study. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 2014;171:864–871. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13081132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Kim D, Langmead B, Salzberg SL. HISAT: a fast spliced aligner with low memory requirements. Nature Methods. 2015;12:357–360. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.3317. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Kim J, Sung Y, Park H, Choi DI, Kim J, Lee H, Jung MK, Noh S, Ye S, Lee J, Islam MA, Chun H, Mun JY, Kaang BK. Astrocytic Connection to Engram Neurons Increased after Learning. bioRxiv. 2023 doi: 10.1101/2023.01.25.525617. [DOI]
  50. Kogan JH, Frankland PW, Silva AJ. Long-Term memory underlying hippocampus-dependent social recognition in mice. Hippocampus. 2000;10:47–56. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(2000)10:1&#x0003c;47::AID-HIPO5&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Kopec AM, Smith CJ, Ayre NR, Sweat SC, Bilbo SD. Microglial dopamine receptor elimination defines sex-specific nucleus accumbens development and social behavior in adolescent rats. Nature Communications. 2018;9:3769. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-06118-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Kreuder AK, Scheele D, Wassermann L, Wollseifer M, Stoffel-Wagner B, Lee MR, Hennig J, Maier W, Hurlemann R. How the brain codes intimacy: the neurobiological substrates of romantic touch. Human Brain Mapping. 2017;38:4525–4534. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23679. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Krishnan V, Han M-H, Graham DL, Berton O, Renthal W, Russo SJ, Laplant Q, Graham A, Lutter M, Lagace DC, Ghose S, Reister R, Tannous P, Green TA, Neve RL, Chakravarty S, Kumar A, Eisch AJ, Self DW, Lee FS, Tamminga CA, Cooper DC, Gershenfeld HK, Nestler EJ. Molecular adaptations underlying susceptibility and resistance to social defeat in brain reward regions. Cell. 2007;131:391–404. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2007.09.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Kronman H, Torres-Berrío A, Sidoli S, Issler O, Godino A, Ramakrishnan A, Mews P, Lardner CK, Parise EM, Walker DM, van der Zee YY, Browne CJ, Boyce BF, Neve R, Garcia BA, Shen L, Peña CJ, Nestler EJ. Long-Term behavioral and cell-type-specific molecular effects of early life stress are mediated by h3k79me2 dynamics in medium spiny neurons. Nature Neuroscience. 2021;24:667–676. doi: 10.1038/s41593-021-00814-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kuleshov MV, Jones MR, Rouillard AD, Fernandez NF, Duan Q, Wang Z, Koplev S, Jenkins SL, Jagodnik KM, Lachmann A, McDermott MG, Monteiro CD, Gundersen GW, Ma’ayan A. Enrichr: a comprehensive gene set enrichment analysis web server 2016 update. Nucleic Acids Research. 2016;44:W90–W97. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkw377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Labonté B, Engmann O, Purushothaman I, Menard C, Wang J, Tan C, Scarpa JR, Moy G, Loh YHE, Cahill M, Lorsch ZS, Hamilton PJ, Calipari ES, Hodes GE, Issler O, Kronman H, Pfau M, Obradovic ALJ, Dong Y, Neve RL, Russo S, Kazarskis A, Tamminga C, Mechawar N, Turecki G, Zhang B, Shen L, Nestler EJ. Sex-Specific transcriptional signatures in human depression. Nature Medicine. 2017;23:1102–1111. doi: 10.1038/nm.4386. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Langfelder P, Horvath S. WGCNA: an R package for weighted correlation network analysis. BMC Bioinformatics. 2008;9:559. doi: 10.1186/1471-2105-9-559. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Lee NS, Goodwin NL, Freitas KE, Beery AK. Affiliation, aggression, and selectivity of peer relationships in meadow and prairie voles. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2019;13:52. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Lee NS, Beery AK. The role of dopamine signaling in prairie vole peer relationships. Hormones and Behavior. 2021;127:104876. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2020.104876. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Lehmann ML, Weigel TK, Elkahloun AG, Herkenham M. Chronic social defeat reduces myelination in the mouse medial prefrontal cortex. Scientific Reports. 2017;7:46548. doi: 10.1038/srep46548. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Lei K, Cushing BS, Musatov S, Ogawa S, Kramer KM. Estrogen receptor-alpha in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis regulates social affiliation in male prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) PLOS ONE. 2010;5:e8931. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0008931. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Lieberwirth C, Wang Z. The neurobiology of pair bond formation, bond disruption, and social buffering. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2016;40:8–13. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2016.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Lim MM, Young LJ. Vasopressin-Dependent neural circuits underlying pair bond formation in the monogamous prairie vole. Neuroscience. 2004;125:35–45. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2003.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Liu J, Dietz K, DeLoyht JM, Pedre X, Kelkar D, Kaur J, Vialou V, Lobo MK, Dietz DM, Nestler EJ, Dupree J, Casaccia P. Impaired adult myelination in the prefrontal cortex of socially isolated mice. Nature Neuroscience. 2012;15:1621–1623. doi: 10.1038/nn.3263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Loth MK, Donaldson ZR. Oxytocin, dopamine, and opioid interactions underlying pair bonding: highlighting a potential role for microglia. Endocrinology. 2021;162:bqaa223. doi: 10.1210/endocr/bqaa223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Love MI, Huber W, Anders S. Moderated estimation of fold change and dispersion for RNA-seq data with deseq2. Genome Biology. 2014;15:550. doi: 10.1186/s13059-014-0550-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Makinodan M, Rosen KM, Ito S, Corfas G. A critical period for social experience-dependent oligodendrocyte maturation and myelination. Science. 2012;337:1357–1360. doi: 10.1126/science.1220845. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. McNeal N, Scotti M-AL, Wardwell J, Chandler DL, Bates SL, Larocca M, Trahanas DM, Grippo AJ. Disruption of social bonds induces behavioral and physiological dysregulation in male and female prairie voles. Autonomic Neuroscience. 2014;180:9–16. doi: 10.1016/j.autneu.2013.10.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Modi ME, Young LJ. D-Cycloserine facilitates socially reinforced learning in an animal model relevant to autism spectrum disorders. Biological Psychiatry. 2011;70:298–304. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.01.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Mukamel EA. Multiple comparisons and inappropriate statistical testing lead to spurious sex differences in gene expression. Biological Psychiatry. 2022;91:e1–e2. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2021.06.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Nestler EJ, Peña CJ, Kundakovic M, Mitchell A, Akbarian S. Epigenetic basis of mental illness. The Neuroscientist. 2016;22:447–463. doi: 10.1177/1073858415608147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. O’Connor M-F, Wellisch DK, Stanton AL, Eisenberger NI, Irwin MR, Lieberman MD. Craving love? enduring grief activates brain’s reward center. NeuroImage. 2008;42:969–972. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Okuyama T, Kitamura T, Roy DS, Itohara S, Tonegawa S. Ventral CA1 neurons store social memory. Science. 2016;353:1536–1541. doi: 10.1126/science.aaf7003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Osako Y, Nobuhara R, Arai YCP, Tanaka K, Young LJ, Nishihara M, Mitsui S, Yuri K. Partner loss in monogamous rodents: modulation of pain and emotional behavior in male prairie voles. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2018;80:62–68. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000524. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Peña CJ, Kronman HG, Walker DM, Cates HM, Bagot RC, Purushothaman I, Issler O, Loh YHE, Leong T, Kiraly DD, Goodman E, Neve RL, Shen L, Nestler EJ. Early life stress confers lifelong stress susceptibility in mice via ventral tegmental area Otx2. Science. 2017;356:1185–1188. doi: 10.1126/science.aan4491. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Pitchers KK, Frohmader KS, Vialou V, Mouzon E, Nestler EJ, Lehman MN, Coolen LM. ΔFosB in the nucleus accumbens is critical for reinforcing effects of sexual reward. Genes, Brain, and Behavior. 2010;9:831–840. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2010.00621.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Plaisier SB, Taschereau R, Wong JA, Graeber TG. Rank–rank hypergeometric overlap: identification of statistically significant overlap between gene-expression signatures. Nucleic Acids Research. 2010;38:e169. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkq636. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Pohl TT, Young LJ, Bosch OJ. Lost connections: oxytocin and the neural, physiological, and behavioral consequences of disrupted relationships. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2019;136:54–63. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2017.12.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Pohl TT, Jung O, Di Benedetto B, Young LJ, Bosch OJ. Microglia react to partner loss in a sex- and brain site-specific manner in prairie voles. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 2021;96:168–186. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2021.05.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Potretzke S, Ryabinin AE. The prairie vole model of pair-bonding and its sensitivity to addictive substances. Frontiers in Psychology. 2019;10:2477. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02477. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. R:The R project for statistical computing The R project for statistical computing. R Project. 2022 https://www.r-project.org/
  82. Resendez SL, Kuhnmuench M, Krzywosinski T, Aragona BJ. Κ-Opioid receptors within the nucleus accumbens shell mediate pair bond maintenance. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:6771–6784. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5779-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Resendez SL, Keyes PC, Day JJ, Hambro C, Austin CJ, Maina FK, Eidson LN, Porter-Stransky KA, Nevárez N, McLean JW, Kuhnmuench MA, Murphy AZ, Mathews TA, Aragona BJ. Dopamine and opioid systems interact within the nucleus accumbens to maintain monogamous pair bonds. eLife. 2016;5:e15325. doi: 10.7554/eLife.15325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Sadino J. SeparationRNAseq. swh:1:rev:45c0386e0655cc52f89e575668bf3f00ed907d7fSoftware Heritage. 2023 https://archive.softwareheritage.org/swh:1:dir:6aa42dbc34e9f11413e2eac6a5d26885b39ee0d0;origin=https://github.com/donaldsonlab/separationRNAseq;visit=swh:1:snp:b1f68d9e450e91713f4c797b3dda12c89d68ca23;anchor=swh:1:rev:45c0386e0655cc52f89e575668bf3f00ed907d7f
  85. Savell KE, Tuscher JJ, Zipperly ME, Duke CG, Phillips RA, Bauman AJ, Thukral S, Sultan FA, Goska NA, Ianov L, Day JJ. A dopamine-induced gene expression signature regulates neuronal function and cocaine response. Science Advances. 2020;6:eaba4221. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aba4221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Scribner JL, Vance EA, Protter DSW, Sheeran WM, Saslow E, Cameron RT, Klein EM, Jimenez JC, Kheirbek MA, Donaldson ZR. A neuronal signature for monogamous reunion. PNAS. 2020;117:11076–11084. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1917287117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Seney ML, Huo Z, Cahill K, French L, Puralewski R, Zhang J, Logan RW, Tseng G, Lewis DA, Sibille E. Opposite molecular signatures of depression in men and women. Biological Psychiatry. 2018;84:18–27. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.01.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Shear K, Shair H. Attachment, loss, and complicated grief. Developmental Psychobiology. 2005;47:253–267. doi: 10.1002/dev.20091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Shear MK, Simon N, Wall M, Zisook S, Neimeyer R, Duan N, Reynolds C, Lebowitz B, Sung S, Ghesquiere A, Gorscak B, Clayton P, Ito M, Nakajima S, Konishi T, Melhem N, Meert K, Schiff M, O’Connor M-F, First M, Sareen J, Bolton J, Skritskaya N, Mancini AD, Keshaviah A. Complicated grief and related bereavement issues for DSM-5. Depression and Anxiety. 2011;28:103–117. doi: 10.1002/da.20780. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Smith AS, Lieberwirth C, Wang Z. Behavioral and physiological responses of female prairie voles (microtus ochrogaster) to various stressful conditions. Stress. 2013;16:531–539. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2013.794449. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Smith AS, Wang Z. Hypothalamic oxytocin mediates social buffering of the stress response. Biological Psychiatry. 2014;76:281–288. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.09.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Smith AS, Williams Avram SK, Cymerblit-Sabba A, Song J, Young WS. Targeted activation of the hippocampal Ca2 area strongly enhances social memory. Molecular Psychiatry. 2016;21:1137–1144. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Stetzik L, Ganshevsky D, Lende MN, Roache LE, Musatov S, Cushing BS. Inhibiting ERα expression in the medial amygdala increases prosocial behavior in male meadow voles (microtus pennsylvanicus) Behavioural Brain Research. 2018;351:42–48. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.05.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Sun P, Smith AS, Lei K, Liu Y, Wang Z. Breaking bonds in male prairie vole: long-term effects on emotional and social behavior, physiology, and neurochemistry. Behavioural Brain Research. 2014;265:22–31. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.02.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Tamarin RH, Ostfeld RS, Pugh SR, Bujalska G. Social Systems and Population Cycles in Voles. Springer; 1990. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  96. Thornton MA, Hughes EG. Neuron-oligodendroglia interactions: activity-dependent regulation of cellular signaling. Neuroscience Letters. 2020;727:134916. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2020.134916. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Tripp JA, Feng NY, Bass AH. Behavioural tactic predicts preoptic-hypothalamic gene expression more strongly than developmental morph in fish with alternative reproductive tactics. Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 2018;285:20172742. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2017.2742. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Tripp JA, Berrio A, McGraw LA, Matz MV, Davis JK, Inoue K, Thomas JW, Young LJ, Phelps SM. Comparative neurotranscriptomics reveal widespread species differences associated with bonding. BMC Genomics. 2021;22:399. doi: 10.1186/s12864-021-07720-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Tuesta LM, Zhang Y. Mechanisms of epigenetic memory and addiction. The EMBO Journal. 2014;33:1091–1103. doi: 10.1002/embj.201488106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Ülgen DH, Ruigrok SR, Sandi C. Powering the social brain: mitochondria in social behaviour. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2023;79:102675. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2022.102675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Vahaba DM, Halstead ER, Donaldson ZR, Ahern TH, Beery AK. Sex differences in the reward value of familiar mates in prairie voles. Genes, Brain, and Behavior. 2022;21:e12790. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12790. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Walker DM, Cates HM, Loh YHE, Purushothaman I, Ramakrishnan A, Cahill KM, Lardner CK, Godino A, Kronman HG, Rabkin J, Lorsch ZS, Mews P, Doyle MA, Feng J, Labonté B, Koo JW, Bagot RC, Logan RW, Seney ML, Calipari ES, Shen L, Nestler EJ. Cocaine self-administration alters transcriptome-wide responses in the brain’s reward circuitry. Biological Psychiatry. 2018;84:867–880. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.04.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Walker DM, Zhou X, Cunningham AM, Lipschultz AP, Ramakrishnan A, Cates HM, Bagot RC, Shen L, Zhang B, Nestler EJ. Sex-Specific transcriptional changes in response to adolescent social stress in the brain’s reward circuitry. Biological Psychiatry. 2022;91:118–128. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2021.02.964. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Wallace DL, Han M-H, Graham DL, Green TA, Vialou V, Iñiguez SD, Cao J-L, Kirk A, Chakravarty S, Kumar A, Krishnan V, Neve RL, Cooper DC, Bolaños CA, Barrot M, McClung CA, Nestler EJ. Creb regulation of nucleus accumbens excitability mediates social isolation-induced behavioral deficits. Nature Neuroscience. 2009;12:200–209. doi: 10.1038/nn.2257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Walum H, Young LJ. The neural mechanisms and circuitry of the pair bond. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2018;19:643–654. doi: 10.1038/s41583-018-0072-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Wang M, Zhao Y, Zhang B. Efficient test and visualization of multi-set intersections. Scientific Reports. 2015;5:16923. doi: 10.1038/srep16923. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Wickham H. Ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. Springer International Publishing; 2016. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  108. Williams J. Development of partner preferences in female prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster): the role of social and sexual experience. Hormones and Behavior. 1992;26:339–349. doi: 10.1016/0018-506X(92)90004-F. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Williams JR, Carter CS, Insel T. Partner preference development in female prairie voles is facilitated by mating or the central infusion of oxytocin. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1992;652:487–489. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1992.tb34393.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Winslow JT, Hastings N, Carter CS, Harbaugh CR, Insel TR. A role for central vasopressin in pair bonding in monogamous prairie voles. Nature. 1993;365:545–548. doi: 10.1038/365545a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Xie Z, Bailey A, Kuleshov MV, Clarke DJB, Evangelista JE, Jenkins SL, Lachmann A, Wojciechowicz ML, Kropiwnicki E, Jagodnik KM, Jeon M, Ma’ayan A. Gene set knowledge discovery with enrichr. Current Protocols. 2021;1:e90. doi: 10.1002/cpz1.90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Young LJ, Wang Z. The neurobiology of pair bonding. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7:1048–1054. doi: 10.1038/nn1327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Young KA, Liu Y, Wang Z. The neurobiology of social attachment: A comparative approach to behavioral, neuroanatomical, and neurochemical studies. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology. Toxicology & Pharmacology. 2008;148:401–410. doi: 10.1016/j.cbpc.2008.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Young RL, Ferkin MH, Ockendon-Powell NF, Orr VN, Phelps SM, Pogány Á, Richards-Zawacki CL, Summers K, Székely T, Trainor BC, Urrutia AO, Zachar G, O’Connell LA, Hofmann HA. Conserved transcriptomic profiles underpin monogamy across vertebrates. PNAS. 2019;116:1331–1336. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1813775116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Yu G, Wang L-G, Han Y, He Q-Y. ClusterProfiler: an R package for comparing biological themes among gene clusters. OMICS. 2012;16:284–287. doi: 10.1089/omi.2011.0118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Zayed A, Robinson GE. Understanding the relationship between brain gene expression and social behavior: lessons from the honey bee. Annual Review of Genetics. 2012;46:591–615. doi: 10.1146/annurev-genet-110711-155517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Zhou B, Zhu Z, Ransom BR, Tong X. Oligodendrocyte lineage cells and depression. Molecular Psychiatry. 2021;26:103–117. doi: 10.1038/s41380-020-00930-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Editor's evaluation

Margaret M McCarthy 1

This study focuses on an important but understudied topic, the biological basis of pair bonding and associated stress when the bond is lost. The authors present convincing evidence that transcriptional changes associated with the formation of a pair bond in voles degrade during prolonged separation but do not precisely mirror behavioral responses. Gene expression changes were associated with glia, steroid hormone signaling, and myelination, among others, providing critical guideposts for future investigations. This work will be of interest to behavioral neuroscientists and/or those interested in social behavior.

Decision letter

Editor: Margaret M McCarthy1
Reviewed by: Margaret M McCarthy2

Our editorial process produces two outputs: (i) public reviews designed to be posted alongside the preprint for the benefit of readers; (ii) feedback on the manuscript for the authors, including requests for revisions, shown below. We also include an acceptance summary that explains what the editors found interesting or important about the work.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Prolonged partner separation erodes nucleus accumbens transcriptional signatures of pair bonding in male prairie voles" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by 3 peer reviewers, including Margaret M McCarthy as Reviewing Editor and Reviewer #1, and the evaluation has been overseen by Catherine Dulac as the Senior Editor.

The reviewers have discussed their reviews with one another, and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this to help you prepare a revised submission.

Essential revisions:

1) The disconnect between the behavioral responses in terms of endurance of the pair-bond, and the transcriptomics was of concern to the Reviewers. It was noted that there are subtle but perhaps important differences in the separation protocol used for the behavioral studies versus the transcriptomics. This combined with the behavioral data here not being consistent with the published literature undermines the current conclusions. The Reviewers felt that repeating the behavior but with the precise protocol used for the transcriptomics would allow for more direct comparison and strengthen the conclusions.

2) The value added by the use of TRAP was questionable. This approach should either be further validated or more strongly justified if it is to remain in the manuscript.

3) Some conclusions were overly anthropomorphized and an extensive literature on the roles of oxytocin and vasopressin in pair-bonding in this species was neglected. Changes in wording and emphasis combined with some new material in the Discussion could easily address this.

Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

1) Prior work has implicated both the oxytocin and vasopressin systems in pair bonding, albeit in complicated ways, but there was both no discussion of that here nor an opportunity to provide a positive control since the nucleus accumbens does not include these systems. Perhaps the authors could discuss this as among the shortcomings.

2) Presumably the authors plan future studies using viral vectors to either over-express or repress candidate genes for manipulating the endurance of pair bonds. It is very unfortunate that the behavior did not correspond as expected. The authors' explanations for why this might be are legitimate but it does leave one wondering what to do next and highlights the challenge of such large sequencing studies.

Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

This study was clearly a ton of work, and the authors marshall an impressive amount of data to address an important and understudied question. The combination of bioinformatics + bench work to get methods going in an emerging model is impressive.

It could help make the article more accessible to a wider readership if the authors could explain a little more clearly what vTRAP tells us that RNAseq doesn't. Why do we care about neuronal vs non-neuronal processes?

Some of the language about the transcriptomic results implies within-individual change although that can't be possible given that sampling is terminal, e.g. "…stably maintained while animals remained paired but eroded…" (abstract).

Interpretation of the transcriptional differences: clearly the transcriptional differences are not causal to the behavior that happened in the past. The decoupling of behavior and transcription offers some insights – the behavior remains the same (still shows behavioral signs of bonding) but the gene expression diverges; authors infer it has to do with the ability to establish a new bond but it's likely to be more heterogeneous? What about recovery from the loss, for example? Doesn't it seem likely that some of the DEGs reflect the ability to establish a new bond while others are probably about other processes, e.g. recovery?

Top left page 2: provide refs to support statements like "considering that other stable behavioral shifts…are underwritten by changes in transcription"

Why did the authors focus on males rather than females? The experimental design would be even better if they were brothers.

The sample size is ok with n=15, 16 pairs, but it would be helpful if the authors could provide some explicit acknowledgement of power (Figure 1).

R column, top of page 3: "latency to query"?

L column, page 3: need to explain why expected animals to have a preference for same-sex partners sooner before introducing the hypothesis.

L column, page 5: How to interpret opposite sex males initiated tumble fighting more quickly?

Page 5: please clearly state n for each treatment /time point group in the RNAseq analysis; presumably it's 50% of the n=15, 16 used in behavioral experiments?

Page 5, R column: which variables are being correlated here? "we calculated Rho values between the observed short-term pair bond and the shuffled…"

Page 5: how many individuals were excluded because they did not have baseline partner preference >50%?

Page 7: need to know experimental design for sequencing earlier, L column "on 8 groups of animals" – please remind us n for each group.

The major comparison is between same vs opposite sex housed pairs; therefore differences between treatment groups could reflect pair bonding OR they could reflect something about the sex of the partner. What about an additional control? They don't just differ in whether or not pair-bonded, but also in whether or not pair-bonded/housed/interacted with a male or a female

How did the authors choose which male member of the same-sex pair was used as the focal?

Page 8 L column: could this be correlated with the interesting behavioral results at the pair/individual level, i.e. pairs that were more bonded showed more of an effect than pairs that were less bonded?

Page 8, L column "the combined pair bond signature remains intact after short-term separation…" implies within-individual sampling.

Page 8, R column: this is a creative way to analyze RNAseq data!

vTRAP: Page 12, L column: It's neat that the authors were able to filter out genes so they could focus on genes expressed in neurons, but it seems like a lot of trouble to do all this work with cre and viruses – did they really learn that much more?

Page 12, R column: "the strong similarity in expression validates that bulk sequencing…" right – so did you really need to go to all the trouble of vTRAP?

Discussion on page 13 focuses on usual suspects/candidate genes: so why go to all this trouble to do this genomic approach if what really interests you are genes we already know are important? why not just have started with those?

Page 13: text here emphasizes that vTRAP didn't teach us anything particularly new we couldn't have figured out from the bulk RNASeq data alone. Here the authors emphasize that it's included in this paper to illustrate the utility of the method and make it available to the wider community of vole researchers, which is great but…. Does it belong in this paper?

Page 13 L column: the direct comparison of opposite versus same-sex paired animals provides a control for the effects of social context (alone vs with a pair) but it introduces another confound, namely the sex of the partner.

Page 13, R column: "correlation across partner preference behaviors…" seems like there could also be an opportunity to look for correlations between gene expression and behavior.

Page 14, L column: "transcripts associated with glia…" so can the effects of the experience of pair bonding vs pair bonding itself be teased apart? The experience of pair bonding could influence subsequent behavior via e.g learning -- there could be a transcriptional signal of that. There could also be a transcription signature of remembering that particular partner – can you differentiate between these two?

Sometimes the language goes a little too far "parsimonious with the human experience" page 15.

Methods: Tell us a little more about the colonies and genetic variation within them; how inbred are they?

Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

1) The methods state that prairie voles were treated differently in the behavioral and transcriptomics studies. This might be why the authors do not see a strong behavioral degradation of pair bond-related behavior after long-term separation but do see a strong transcriptional signature. This reviewer understands that re-doing the behavioral study may not be feasible, this flaw should be addressed in the manuscript as a possible reason why the behavioral and transcriptomics results are not consistent. Further, the conclusions should be edited to fit this limitation.

2) While RRHO is helpful to assess overall patterns of transcriptional signatures across datasets, its utility for determining the exact transcripts is limited. For instance, even though the signal in the up-up quadrant in Figure 3F is stronger than the signal in the down-down quadrant in Figure 3G, there are more overlapping transcripts identified for a down-down. This is because of how RRHO determines the overlapping transcripts for its Venn diagram feature (by taking the point where the p-value is most significant and taking the list to the outside corner of that quadrant). It might be more helpful to use the RRHO quadrant information as a guide and then use traditional p-value cutoffs to look at overlapping transcripts. This would also reduce the number of transcripts fitting the desired pattern for follow-up experiments. The authors do this in the supplemental information which would be more helpful than the RRHO-identified overlapping transcripts presented in the main text. I suggest swapping these two approaches.

3) For the neuronal cluster analysis in Figure 4, the cluster 1 signal appears to be fairly weak, both in terms of the clustering in the neuronal enriched gene heatmap and the associated pathways. Why was this cluster selected? Further, it would be helpful to the reader if the authors included a description of each neuronal cluster in the figure (e.g., up in pair bond and ST separation, down in LT separation).

4) What is the signal for RRHO of short vs. long-term separation? Did I miss this? I feel that this is an important comparison to show.

5) In the volcano plots, it would be helpful to indicate the number of transcripts that are up or down-regulated on the figures.

6) Please include the coordinates for the NAc dissections.

7) The TRAP expression was verified in only one animal, which is not sufficient.

eLife. 2023 Feb 28;12:e80517. doi: 10.7554/eLife.80517.sa2

Author response


Essential revisions:

1) The disconnect between the behavioral responses in terms of endurance of the pair-bond, and the transcriptomics was of concern to the Reviewers. It was noted that there are subtle but perhaps important differences in the separation protocol used for the behavioral studies versus the transcriptomics. This combined with the behavioral data here not being consistent with the published literature undermines the current conclusions. The Reviewers felt that repeating the behavior but with the precise protocol used for the transcriptomics would allow for more direct comparison and strengthen the conclusions.

2) The value added by the use of TRAP was questionable. This approach should either be further validated or more strongly justified if it is to remain in the manuscript.

3) Some conclusions were overly anthropomorphized and an extensive literature on the roles of oxytocin and vasopressin in pair-bonding in this species was neglected. Changes in wording and emphasis combined with some new material in the Discussion could easily address this.

We thank the reviewers for their thoughtful and constructive comments which have helped us improve our manuscript. In our revised manuscript, have respond to three main weaknesses:

1. We addressed the inconsistency in the experimental design across the behavior and the transcription experiments by repeating the behavior with an experimental timeline that more exactly matches that of the animals used in transcriptional studies and in which separated animals were housed in separate colony rooms. Briefly, we replicated our initial results and found that same- and opposite-sex pairs display a partner preference even after prolonged partner separation. However, by including an additional control group that was never separated, we also found that although partner directed huddle increased in animals that remain paired from two to six weeks, this was not true for animals that were separated, suggesting that separation disrupts normal bond strengthening. Thus while the behavior seems surprisingly robust in light of the observed transcriptional changes, we are confident that our core finding – that voles can maintain a preference despite prolonged separation – is indeed a reliable finding.

2. We further validated and justified our use of TRAP and our focus on the NAc as the sole brain region of investigation as articulated in more detail in the manuscript and below in our response to reviewers.

3. We revised the language throughout the manuscript, especially in the discussion, to reduce anthropomorphizing our results and interpretations. Below we have provided responses to specific concerns articulated by each reviewer.

Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

1) Prior work has implicated both the oxytocin and vasopressin systems in pair bonding, albeit in complicated ways, but there was both no discussion of that here nor an opportunity to provide a positive control since the nucleus accumbens does not include these systems. Perhaps the authors could discuss this as among the shortcomings.

A primary goal of our approach was to expand work in prairie voles beyond the narrow focus on nonapeptide systems. Justifying our approach to use RNA sequencing for an unbiased investigation, it was recently shown that the oxytocin receptor is not genetically required for pair bonding thus suggesting OXT-independent mechanisms of attachment in voles (20). We agree that an interesting area of future research will be to determine the contribution of nonapeptides to the observed pair bond transcriptional signature, but it was not a primary goal of the current study.

2) Presumably the authors plan future studies using viral vectors to either over-express or repress candidate genes for manipulating the endurance of pair bonds. It is very unfortunate that the behavior did not correspond as expected. The authors' explanations for why this might be are legitimate but it does leave one wondering what to do next and highlights the challenge of such large sequencing studies.

The opportunity to examine large biological datasets provides both challenges and opportunities. It this case, we have gained valuable timing information regarding the stability of pair bond transcription and the erosion of transcriptional signatures and robustness of partner preference following loss. Yet we are studying a

complicated system where we need additional information before confidently choosing and manipulating targets. In addition by repeating our behavioral experiments by including animals that remain paired we now know that separation impedes the strengthening of bonds providing us with an opportunity to potentially manipulate bond maturation. Such work is currently underway in our lab but beyond the scope of the current study.

Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

This study was clearly a ton of work, and the authors marshall an impressive amount of data to address an important and understudied question. The combination of bioinformatics + bench work to get methods going in an emerging model is impressive.

Thank you

It could help make the article more accessible to a wider readership if the authors could explain a little more clearly what vTRAP tells us that RNAseq doesn't. Why do we care about neuronal vs non-neuronal processes?

We feel that inclusion of TRAP adds substantially to this manuscript and to our understanding of the neuromolecular underpinnings of bonding and loss in the NAc. The value of this experiment is twofold. As noted by Reviewer 3, “the TRAP approach in prairie voles is novel and will provide a great resource to the research community.” The prairie vole community has just developed its first transgenic Cre lines, which could be paired with vTRAP to query bond-associated gene expression changes exclusively in Cre-expressing neurons (12). Second, we noticed a puzzle in our tissue-level data. The majority of cells in the NAc are neurons (13, 14), and the vast majority of pair bonding studies of this region have focused on neuronal phenotypes, but our transcriptional signatures were linked to changes in glial populations. Ultimately, changes in glia are likely to act via their interactions with neurons, and vTRAP enables us to query specifically the neuronal transcriptional changes within our data. Supporting that this provides novel insights into our datasets, when we cluster transcripts based on their expression profiles following short and long-term separation, we predict different GO terms from the tissue level and neuronally-enriched gene sets. For instance, the GO terms resulting from cluster 2 for neuronal genes (Fig 4) includes “response to amphetamine” within the top 10 results, but the same cluster of genes from tissue level sequencing predicts this GO term as the 34th result.

Some of the language about the transcriptomic results implies within-individual change although that can't be possible given that sampling is terminal, e.g. "…stably maintained while animals remained paired but eroded…" (abstract).

Thank you. It was not our intent to imply within animal changes but rather to clarify potential transcriptional signatures that characterize bonding and loss where some language signifying continuity is unavoidable (e.g. you cannot experience loss without a bond to lose). We have updated the verbiage throughout to clarify that our measurements are not evidence of within-animal change.

Interpretation of the transcriptional differences: clearly the transcriptional differences are not causal to the behavior that happened in the past. The decoupling of behavior and transcription offers some insights – the behavior remains the same (still shows behavioral signs of bonding) but the gene expression diverges; authors infer it has to do with the ability to establish a new bond but it's likely to be more heterogeneous? What about recovery from the loss, for example? Doesn't it seem likely that some of the DEGs reflect the ability to establish a new bond while others are probably about other processes, e.g. recovery?

We agree that there appears to be a mismatch between the behavioral and transcriptional results. Because of this we repeated the behavior (please see public response to Reviewer 1) with separate cohorts of animals for each separation timepoint and the addition of a remain paired cohort that stayed with their partner throughout the six weeks of the experiment. When we compared opposite-sex paired males that remained with their partner to those that were separated from their partner we observed that separation impedes the continual strengthening of the pair bond. We interpreted this result to mean that upon separation the original pair bond is not lost per-say it is not being maintained in the way that it would be if the animals had stayed paired. Thus, the behavior now more closely resembles the transcriptional result of a residual pair bond signature that erodes over the course of separation. Future experiments will focus on a more nuanced understanding of how transcription can influence behavior to remain pliable in multiple social contexts. We addressed this comment in the discussion as follows:

“Pair bonding and loss are complex and multifaceted processes, engaging diverse neural circuits in temporal- and context-specific ways. Yet transcriptomics provides a single snapshot of gene expression at one point in time in one context. So while tissue-level transcriptional analysis is an excellent tool to capture an averaged biological state, one limitation of our approach is that it is not ideal for examining more temporally-dynamic processes, such as the rapid information-processing that occurs during social decision making.”

Top left page 2: provide refs to support statements like "considering that other stable behavioral shifts…are underwritten by changes in transcription"

Thank you for bringing this oversight to our attention. Three references have been added to this statement.

Why did the authors focus on males rather than females? The experimental design would be even better if they were brothers.

The methods state that the same-sex pairs were indeed siblings though we updated the language in the introduction to explicitly state that the same-sex pairs were siblings: “Here we map the trajectory of the pair bond transcriptional profile by comparing opposite-sex paired male to same-sex sibling pairs.” We plan to carry out similar studies in females, but colony size consideration and the complexity of the current dataset contributed to our decision to focus on males initially.

The sample size is ok with n=15, 16 pairs, but it would be helpful if the authors could provide some explicit acknowledgement of power (Figure 1).

We performed a power analysis prior to undertaking the study in order to estimate the group size needed to obtain significant partner preference at (α, Β blah blah). In general, it is not recommended to do posthoc power estimation. If a study was underpowered to detect the true effect size, a post-hoc power analysis of significant (p < 0.05) data is likely to subsequently overestimate power. Mathematically, the power at the observed effect is a function of the p-value. Instead, we have provided effect sizes for significant results, reported in the supplementary statistics table.

R column, top of page 3: "latency to query"?

We changed “query” to “determine” for clearer language.

L column, page 3: need to explain why expected animals to have a preference for same-sex partners sooner before introducing the hypothesis.

Thank you for pointing out our oversight in not including this information. We have added the following clause to the text:

“Similarly, we anticipated that same-sex sibling pairs would have a partner preference at baseline due to familiarity but would exhibit a faster and more robust loss of partner preference when separated.”

L column, page 5: How to interpret opposite sex males initiated tumble fighting more quickly?

We believed that repeating the partner preference test data was a crucial revision, and we correspondingly chose to avoid any potential confounds. Thus we did not to measure resident intruder due to concerns of that aggressive homecage interactions when the partner was removed could have unintended effects on affiliative behavior. All aggression data has been removed from the manuscript.

Page 5: please clearly state n for each treatment /time point group in the RNAseq analysis; presumably it's 50% of the n=15, 16 used in behavioral experiments?

Page 5: how many individuals were excluded because they did not have baseline partner preference >50%?

Group size and excluded animals for all transcription experiments are now clearly stated in the results of the manuscript. Additionally, cohort numbers can be found in the partner preference graphs of Figures 2 and 3.

In addition to listing the excluded animals in Supplemental Data S1, we have added the numbers of excluded animals that had a baseline partner preference <50% in the text as well. For the combined pair bond analysis the verbiage now reads as:

“For consistency, we limited transcriptional assessment to voles that had a baseline partner preference >50% (Figure 2B; Table S1) (OS n = 15; SS n = 11. Excluded for PPT <50% OS n = 3; SS n = 8).” For the separation analysis the text now reads “As before, we only included animals with a baseline partner preference >50% (Figure 3B) (OS n = 12; SS n = 11. Excluded for PPT <50% OS n = 4; SS n = 5).”

Page 5, R column: which variables are being correlated here? "we calculated Rho values between the observed short-term pair bond and the shuffled…"

The variable used to calculate the Rho values was the log2FoldChange as calculated by DESeq2 during differential gene expression analysis. We have revised the sentence to include this information:

“We shuffled each animal’s cohort identity at the long-term timepoint and calculated Rho values of the log2FoldChange of differential gene expression between the observed short-term pair bond and the shuffled long-term pair bond over 1000 iterations”

Page 7: need to know experimental design for sequencing earlier, L column "on 8 groups of animals" – please remind us n for each group.

The experimental design is described in the first paragraph of the Results section and is graphically represented in Figures 2A and 3A. The verbiage in question was changed to “on all animals in this study (n = 49).”

The major comparison is between same vs opposite sex housed pairs; therefore differences between treatment groups could reflect pair bonding OR they could reflect something about the sex of the partner. What about an additional control? They don't just differ in whether or not pair-bonded, but also in whether or not pair-bonded/housed/interacted with a male or a female

Page 13 L column: the direct comparison of opposite versus same-sex paired animals provides a control for the effects of social context (alone vs with a pair) but it introduces another confound, namely the sex of the partner.

Please see our response to your request (point 1) in the public response.

How did the authors choose which male member of the same-sex pair was used as the focal?

The following sentence was added to methods under section RNA preparation and sequencing:

“The focal animal was chosen at random from each pair prior to the beginning of the experiment.”

Page 8 L column: could this be correlated with the interesting behavioral results at the pair/individual level, i.e. pairs that were more bonded showed more of an effect than pairs that were less bonded?

Page 13, R column: "correlation across partner preference behaviors…" seems like there could also be an opportunity to look for correlations between gene expression and behavior.

Please see our public response point R2.1.

Page 8, L column "the combined pair bond signature remains intact after short-term separation…" implies within-individual sampling.

We have updated the verbiage to make it clearer that we are examining across groups and not within animals. The text now reads:

“Specifically, when genes are ordered based on their degree of down- or up-regulation in the pair bond group we found that gene expression was largely indistinguishable in short-term separated animals but the pair bond signature(Rho = 0.32, p < 2.2X10-16) is largely eroded in long-term separated animals (Rho = 0.048, p = 1.6X10-7) (Figure 3D).”

Page 8, R column: this is a creative way to analyze RNAseq data!

Thank you!

vTRAP: Page 12, L column: It's neat that the authors were able to filter out genes so they could focus on genes expressed in neurons, but it seems like a lot of trouble to do all this work with cre and viruses – did they really learn that much more?

Page 12, R column: "the strong similarity in expression validates that bulk sequencing…" right – so did you really need to go to all the trouble of vTRAP?

Page 13: text here emphasizes that vTRAP didn't teach us anything particularly new we couldn't have figured out from the bulk RNASeq data alone. Here the authors emphasize that it's included in this paper to illustrate the utility of the method and make it available to the wider community of vole researchers, which is great but…. Does it belong in this paper?

We feel strongly that the vTRAP data adds substantially although we agree that this could have been more clearly articulated that it was in the prior version of the manuscript. The following is copied verbatim from our public response and is now included in the Discussion section: We feel that inclusion of TRAP adds substantially to this manuscript and to our understanding of the neuromolecular underpinnings of bonding and loss in the NAc. The value of this experiment is twofold. As noted by Reviewer 3, “the TRAP approach in prairie voles is novel and will provide a great resource to the research community.” The prairie vole community has just developed its first transgenic Cre lines, which could be paired with vTRAP to query bond-associated gene expression changes exclusively in Cre-expressing neurons (15). Second, we noticed a puzzle in our tissue-level data. The majority of cells in the NAc are neurons (16, 17), and the vast majority of pair bonding studies of this region have focused on neuronal phenotypes, but our transcriptional signatures were linked to changes in glial populations. Ultimately, changes in glia are likely to act via their interactions with neurons, and vTRAP enables us to query specifically the neuronal transcriptional changes within our data. Supporting that this provides novel insights into our datasets, when we cluster transcripts based on their expression profiles following short and long-term separation, we predict different GO terms from the tissue level and neuronally enriched gene sets. For instance, the GO terms resulting from cluster 2 for neuronal genes (Figure 4) includes “response to amphetamine” within the top 10 results, but the same cluster of genes from tissue level sequencing predicts this GO term as the 34th result.

Discussion on page 13 focuses on usual suspects/candidate genes: so why go to all this trouble to do this genomic approach if what really interests you are genes we already know are important? why not just have started with those?

We take a slightly different perspective. Our identification of such “usual suspects” demonstrates that our results are consistent with the known biology of pair bonding. This dually validates prior work and provides opportunities to expand our understanding by looking at other features of our dataset. In particular, our genomic approach has expanded our understanding via: (1) identification of transcriptional patterns important for regulating behavioral states, (2) identification of upstream regulators that modulate such multi-gene expression patterns, and (3) insight into potential biological processes not previously implicated in pair bonding, such as oligodendrogenesis/myelination, which would be unlikely to result from focusing on candidate genes alone.

Page 14, L column: "transcripts associated with glia…" so can the effects of the experience of pair bonding vs pair bonding itself be teased apart? The experience of pair bonding could influence subsequent behavior via e.g learning -- there could be a transcriptional signal of that. There could also be a transcription signature of remembering that particular partner – can you differentiate between these two?

We interpret this to comment to mean that (1) pair bond formation is distinct from bond maintenance, with the former likely reflecting complex learning processes and the latter evoking homeostatic mechanisms that reinforce the bond. And (2) bonding requires multiple component processes (social recognition memory, motivation, reward association, etc). Towards the former, we are currently investigating the hypothesis that gliogenesis/myelination may be required for bond formation/maturation, consistent with its role in other complex forms of learning and long-term memory (2123). These results will be presented in a separate manuscript. Towards the latter, the behavioral test we employed (the partner preference test) is the gold standard to assessing pair bonds, but it cannot dissociate the different component processes that contribute to bonding. An important area of follow-up research will be to ask how transcriptional or other neurobiological changes contribute to the component processes and their integration in bonding. However, we feel that this question is beyond the scope of the current experiment.

Sometimes the language goes a little too far "parsimonious with the human experience" page 15.

We thank the reviewer for encouraging us to remain objective with our interpretations. We have addressed anthropomorphic wording in the manuscript with the following revision:

We changed “Such an explanation is parsimonious with the human experience.“ to

“Speculatively, this framework optimizes reproductive opportunities by dually enabling a new bond to form or resuming an existing bond. It is likewise tempting to extrapolate this result into the context of human experience. We do not forget our previous bonds or their emotional valence; rather we integrate the loss in order to continue on with life.”

Methods: Tell us a little more about the colonies and genetic variation within them; how inbred are they?

The current methods state that Sexually naive adult prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster, N = 219: 161M, 58F, P60 – P168 at experiment start) were bred in-house in a colony originating from a cross between voles obtained from colonies at Emory University and University of California Davis, both of which were established from wild animals collected in Illinois.

We are careful to maintain an outbred colony. In particular, we maintain pedigrees, and we routinely breed in new, wild-derived voles when animals approach ~20% shared background among non-first-degree relations (approximately every 2 years). However, we have not performed genome sequencing to calculate their degree of relatedness.

Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

1) The methods state that prairie voles were treated differently in the behavioral and transcriptomics studies. This might be why the authors do not see a strong behavioral degradation of pair bond-related behavior after long-term separation but do see a strong transcriptional signature. This reviewer understands that re-doing the behavioral study may not be feasible, this flaw should be addressed in the manuscript as a possible reason why the behavioral and transcriptomics results are not consistent. Further, the conclusions should be edited to fit this limitation.

The following is modified from the public response: The experimental designs employed to assess behavior and transcription differed, which may have contributed to the apparent mismatch in our results when comparing these two levels of biology. Thus we repeated our behavioral assessment using a design that directly matches the one we used in our transcriptional assessment. Specifically, we included a set of “remain paired” controls, separated partners were kept in separate colony rooms to ensure no possible access to partner-associated sensory cues (visual, auditory, olfactory), and separate cohorts of animals were used for short- and long-term separation. This design avoided partner re-introduction during the short-term partner preference test. In short, we replicated our initial findings; prairie voles will display a partner preference for same- or opposite-sex partner even after prolonged separation. Of note, animals that remained paired showed an increase in partner-directed affiliation (huddle time), which was not observed in separated animals suggesting that separation impairs the normal strengthening of bonds with time. Results associated with Figure 1 and the relevant discussion points have been updated in the manuscript.

2) While RRHO is helpful to assess overall patterns of transcriptional signatures across datasets, its utility for determining the exact transcripts is limited. For instance, even though the signal in the up-up quadrant in Figure 3F is stronger than the signal in the down-down quadrant in Figure 3G, there are more overlapping transcripts identified for a down-down. This is because of how RRHO determines the overlapping transcripts for its Venn diagram feature (by taking the point where the p-value is most significant and taking the list to the outside corner of that quadrant). It might be more helpful to use the RRHO quadrant information as a guide and then use traditional p-value cutoffs to look at overlapping transcripts. This would also reduce the number of transcripts fitting the desired pattern for follow-up experiments. The authors do this in the supplemental information which would be more helpful than the RRHO-identified overlapping transcripts presented in the main text. I suggest swapping these two approaches.

Please see our public response point R3.2.

3) For the neuronal cluster analysis in Figure 4, the cluster 1 signal appears to be fairly weak, both in terms of the clustering in the neuronal enriched gene heatmap and the associated pathways. Why was this cluster selected? Further, it would be helpful to the reader if the authors included a description of each neuronal cluster in the figure (e.g., up in pair bond and ST separation, down in LT separation).

Which clusters were spotlighted in Figure 4F was determined by a combination of transcriptional patterns due to separation, which genes are in the cluster, and the resulting GO terms for each cluster. We identified clusters that represented three interesting patterns: (1.) down only after long-term pair bond separation (cluster 1) (2.) up due to acute pair separation (cluster 2) and (3.) up only after long-term pair bond separation (cluster 3). While other clusters could have been used, we felt that these were important transcriptional patterns to highlight in the context of the rest of our results. Further, we noted that cluster 1 contains Shank3, a gene that has been linked to autism and for which mice containing the null allele show social behavior deficits (24). We reasoned that the expression of Shank3, and other genes with a similar transcriptional pattern after partner separation, may be of interest to a broad audience. Finally, we ran GO analysis on each cluster. Unfortunately, at this last step, none of the GO terms for cluster 1 were significant though this is likely due to the cluster’s small size. This is indicated by the light shading, but we felt it was worth including for the sake of transparency.

We have added the following description of cluster selection taken from this response has been added to the “Neuronal enriched genes throughout separation” methods:

“Which clusters were spotlighted in Figure 4F was determined by a combination of transcriptional patterns due to separation, which genes are in the cluster, and the resulting GO terms for each cluster. We identified clusters that represented three interesting patterns: 1.) down only after long-term pair bond separation (cluster

1) 2.) up due to acute pair separation (cluster 2) and 3.) up only after long-term pair bond separation (cluster 3).”

The following description of Cluster 1 is provided in the text:

“Cluster 1 consists of genes that were upregulated in opposite-sex paired males during pair bonding and short-term separation and downregulated following longterm separation.”

The following description of Cluster 2 is provided in the text:

“Cluster 2 transcripts are downregulated in pair bonded males and then robustly upregulated after short-term partner loss and, to a lesser extent, at the longterm timepoint indicating an acute transcriptional response to pair bond disruption.”

The following description of Cluster 3 is provided in the text:

“Finally, Cluster 3 contains genes that were downregulated in pair bonded males and became upregulated only after long-term opposite-sex partner separation, representing transcripts that may help prime the vole to form a new bond.”

Additionally, we updated cluster labels in Figure 4 to be more descriptive.

4) What is the signal for RRHO of short vs. long-term separation? Did I miss this? I feel that this is an important comparison to show.

We thank the reviewer for pointing this out. While we had looked at this in some of our initial analyses, we ultimately chose to focus on the question of what happens to the pair bond signature upon short- or long-term separation, thus omitting this secondary analysis. Based on the above question, we revisited this RRHO, which is now provided in Figure 3 —figure supplement 5. Notably, we see substantial overlap in the UU and DD quadrants in the short-term to long-term separation comparison. This pattern is particularly enriched for overlapping DD transcripts, which is the inverse of what we see in the pair bond::short term RRHO. One likely explanation for this potential overlap, especially in the DD quadrant, is that we are picking up on genes that have already eroded by the short-term separation timepoint and remain eroded after long-term separation. Supporting this interpretation, we took the UU and DD quadrant gene lists (referred to as stable separation genes) and filtered the DESeq2 results of the pair bond, short-term separation, and long-term separation (the same process as Figure 3H and I) for these genes. We identified an interesting pattern in that the genes that were most highly up- or downregulated in the pair bond showed a reversal of expression after short-term separation, correspondingly falling in the opposite UU or DD quadrant post-separation. Thus we filtered the stable separation genes for each condition by a log2FoldChange < -0.30 in the UU quadrant and > 0.30 in the DD quadrant and assessed the transcriptional state of these genes in the pair bond. Figure 3 —figure supplement 5B and C show that the genes that were most downregulated in the UU quadrant show a sharp inversion of expression after short-term separation that is maintained through long-term separation and vice-versa for the DD quadrant genes. Thus, there are subsets of transcripts that are up- or downregulated in pair bonded animals that are already exhibiting erosion after short-term separation. We further asked which biological processes are represented by the genes in Figure 3 —figure supplement 5D and E to investigate which processes are sensitive specifically to short-term separation. We found that UU genes that are then downregulated are associated with dopaminergic processes and DD genes that are then upregulated are associated with neuronal rearrangement processes. Thus, this RRHO analysis enriches our interpretation of transcriptional erosion of pair bond signatures following partner separation. Also related to point R3.2, overall we observed only slight to moderate erosion at the short-term timepoint, which was masked when relying exclusively on a strict p-value cutoff as in Supplemental Figure 6F. Rather, our rigorous experimental design, which has been noted as a strength of this manuscript, allows for more interrogation of the temporal complexity of these transcriptional signatures via threshold-free approaches.

5) In the volcano plots, it would be helpful to indicate the number of transcripts that are up or down-regulated on the figures.

We have added the number of upregulated and downregulated genes to each volcano plot. Additionally, gene lists and the associated differential expression values can be found in Supplemental Data 2.

6) Please include the coordinates for the NAc dissections.

The section “RNA preparation and sequencing” now includes the following description: “The NAc coordinates for dissections were: +1.7 AP, +/- 1 ML, -4.7 to -4.4 DV with the TRAP dissections additionally guided by GFP fluorescence visualized with a fluorescent dissecting scope.”

7) The TRAP expression was verified in only one animal, which is not sufficient.

We interpret this to mean that there are concerns that we only confirmed the Cre-dependence of the TRAP construct in a single animal as all other aspects of the TRAP work involved significantly more animals. We note that most labs rarely confirm Cre-dependence of vectors in more than one or two animals as the results, including those shown in Figure 4 —figure supplemental 2A, are typically definitive (i.e. no expression in the absence of Cre, expression in the presence of Cre). In addition to the images shown in Figure 4 —figure supplemental 2A, we used fluorescent guided dissection to harvest tissue/mRNA, serving as an additional visual confirmation of RPL10-GFP expression in the animals used to generate Figure 4. Additionally, a collaborating lab also confirmed that this vector also expresses in rats when Cre-recombinase is present. Finally, we performed two additional surgeries to confirm that TRAP is only expressed in the presence of Crerecombinase since the prior submission.

References

1. J. L. Scribner, E. A. Vance, D. S. W. Protter, W. M. Sheeran, E. Saslow, R. T. Cameron, E. M. Klein, J. C. Jimenez, M. A. Kheirbek, Z. R. Donaldson, A neuronal signature for monogamous reunion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 117, 11076–11084 (2020).

2. A. M. Borie, S. Agezo, P. Lunsford, A. J. Boender, J.-D. Guo, H. Zhu, G. J. Berman, L. J. Young, R. C. Liu, Social experience alters oxytocinergic modulation in the nucleus accumbens of female prairie voles. Curr Biol. 32, 1026-1037.e4 (2022).

3. in vivo imaging identifies temporal signature of D1 and D2 medium spiny neurons in cocaine reward | PNAS, (available at https://www.pnas.org/doi/10.1073/pnas.1521238113).

4. M.-F. O’Connor, D. K. Wellisch, A. L. Stanton, N. I. Eisenberger, M. R. Irwin, M. D. Lieberman, Craving love? Enduring grief activates brain’s reward center. NeuroImage. 42, 969–972 (2008).

5. T. Hikida, S. Yao, T. Macpherson, A. Fukakusa, M. Morita, H. Kimura, K. Hirai, T. Ando, H. Toyoshiba, A. Sawa, Nucleus accumbens pathways control cell-specific gene expression in the medial prefrontal cortex. Sci Rep. 10, 1838 (2020).

6. C. Baimel, L. M. McGarry, A. G. Carter, The Projection Targets of Medium Spiny Neurons Govern Cocaine-Evoked Synaptic Plasticity in the Nucleus Accumbens. Cell Reports. 28, 2256-2263.e3 (2019).

7. N. S. Lee, N. L. Goodwin, K. E. Freitas, A. K. Beery, Affiliation, aggression, and selectivity of peer relationships in meadow and prairie voles. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 13 (2019), doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00052.

8. O. J. Bosch, H. P. Nair, T. H. Ahern, I. D. Neumann, L. J. Young, The CRF System Mediates Increased Passive Stress-Coping Behavior Following the Loss of a Bonded Partner in a Monogamous Rodent. Neuropsychopharmacology. 34, 1406–1415 (2009).

9. O. J. Bosch, J. Dabrowska, M. E. Modi, Z. V. Johnson, A. C. Keebaugh, C. E. Barrett, T. H. Ahern, J. Guo, V. Grinevich, D. G. Rainnie, I. D. Neumann, L. J. Young, Oxytocin in the nucleus accumbens shell reverses CRFR2-evoked passive stress-coping after partner loss in monogamous male prairie voles. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 64, 66–78 (2016).

10. A. J. Grippo, B. S. Cushing, C. S. Carter, Depression-like behavior and stressor-induced neuroendocrine activation in female prairie voles exposed to chronic social isolation. Psychosomatic Medicine. 69, 149– 157 (2007).

11. A. J. Grippo, D. Gerena, J. Huang, N. Kumar, M. Shah, R. Ughreja, C. Sue Carter, Social isolation induces behavioral and neuroendocrine disturbances relevant to depression in female and male prairie voles. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2007), doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.07.004.

12. K. Horie, K. Inoue, S. Suzuki, S. Adachi, S. Yada, T. Hirayama, S. Hidema, L. J. Young, K. Nishimori, Oxytocin receptor knockout prairie voles generated by CRISPR/Cas9 editing show reduced preference for social novelty and exaggerated repetitive behaviors. Horm Behav. 111, 60–69 (2019).

13. K. E. Savell, J. J. Tuscher, M. E. Zipperly, C. G. Duke, R. A. Phillips, A. J. Bauman, S. Thukral, F. A. Sultan, N. A. Goska, L. Ianov, J. J. Day, A dopamine-induced gene expression signature regulates neuronal function and cocaine response. Science Advances. 6, eaba4221 (2020).

14. Single-Cell RNA-Seq Uncovers a Robust Transcriptional Response to Morphine by Glia: Cell Reports, (available at https://www.cell.com/cell-reports/fulltext/S2211-1247(18)31384-6?_returnURL=https%3A%2F%2Flinkinghub.elsevier.com%2Fretrieve%2Fpii%2FS2211124718313846%3 Fshowall%3Dtrue).

15. S. L. Fulton, S. Mitra, A. E. Lepack, J. A. Martin, A. F. Stewart, J. Converse, M. Hochstetler, D. M. Dietz, I. Maze, Histone H3 dopaminylation in ventral tegmental area underlies heroin-induced transcriptional and behavioral plasticity in male rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 47, 1776 (2022).

16. S. G. Caradonna, T.-Y. Zhang, N. O’Toole, M.-J. Shen, H. Khalil, N. R. Einhorn, X. Wen, C. Parent, F. S. Lee, H. Akil, M. J. Meaney, B. S. McEwen, J. Marrocco, Genomic modules and intramodular network concordance in susceptible and resilient male mice across models of stress. Neuropsychopharmacol. 47, 987–999 (2022).

17. J. S. Wang, T. Kamath, C. M. Mazur, F. Mirzamohammadi, D. Rotter, H. Hojo, C. D. Castro, N. Tokavanich, R. Patel, N. Govea, T. Enishi, Y. Wu, J. da Silva Martins, M. Bruce, D. J. Brooks, M. L. Bouxsein, D. Tokarz, C. P. Lin, A. Abdul, E. Z. Macosko, M. Fiscaletti, C. F. Munns, P. Ryder, M. KostAlimova, P. Byrne, B. Cimini, M. Fujiwara, H. M. Kronenberg, M. N. Wein, Control of osteocyte dendrite formation by Sp7 and its target gene osteocrin. Nat Commun. 12, 6271 (2021).

18. D. A. Gallegos, M. Minto, F. Liu, M. F. Hazlett, S. Aryana Yousefzadeh, L. C. Bartelt, A. E. West, Cell-type specific transcriptional adaptations of nucleus accumbens interneurons to amphetamine. Mol Psychiatry, 1–15 (2022).

19. B. J. Hilton, A. Husch, B. Schaffran, T. Lin, E. R. Burnside, S. Dupraz, M. Schelski, J. Kim, J. A. Müller, S. Schoch, C. Imig, N. Brose, F. Bradke, An active vesicle priming machinery suppresses axon regeneration upon adult CNS injury. Neuron. 110, 51-69.e7 (2022).

20. K. M. Berendzen, D. S. Manoli, Rethinking the Architecture of Attachment: New Insights into the Role for Oxytocin Signaling. Affect Sci. 3, 734–748 (2022).

21. C. M. Bacmeister, R. Huang, L. A. Osso, M. A. Thornton, L. Conant, A. R. Chavez, A. Poleg-Polsky, E. G. Hughes, Motor learning drives dynamic patterns of intermittent myelination on learning-activated axons. Nat Neurosci. 25, 1300–1313 (2022).

22. E. G. Hughes, J. L. Orthmann-Murphy, A. J. Langseth, D. E. Bergles, Myelin remodeling through experience-dependent oligodendrogenesis in the adult somatosensory cortex. Nat Neurosci. 21, 696–706 (2018).

23. M. A. Thornton, E. G. Hughes, Neuron-oligodendroglia interactions: Activity-dependent regulation of cellular signaling. Neurosci Lett. 727, 134916 (2020).

24. C. M. Durand, C. Betancur, T. M. Boeckers, J. Bockmann, P. Chaste, F. Fauchereau, G. Nygren, M. Rastam, I. C. Gillberg, H. Anckarsäter, E. Sponheim, H. Goubran-Botros, R. Delorme, N. Chabane, M.-C. Mouren-Simeoni, P. de Mas, E. Bieth, B. Rogé, D. Héron, L. Burglen, C. Gillberg, M. Leboyer, T. Bourgeron, Mutations in the gene encoding the synaptic scaffolding protein SHANK3 are associated with autism spectrum disorders. Nat Genet. 39, 25–27 (2007).

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Sadino JM. 2022. Supplemental Data 1. figshare. [DOI]
    2. Sadino JM. 2022. Supplemental Data 2. figshare. [DOI]
    3. Sadino JM. 2022. Supplemental Data 3. figshare. [DOI]
    4. Sadino JM. 2023. RNAseq data from: Prolonged partner separation erodes nucleus accumbens transcriptional signatures of pair bonding in male prairie voles. NCBI Gene Expression Omnibus. GSE192661 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

    Supplementary Materials

    Supplementary file 1. Comprehensive statistical analyses and individual animal information.

    10.6084/m9.figshare.19911541. Statistical analyses for behavior and transcription experiments including applicable post-hocs and version numbers of analysis packages used. Also includes relevant metrics for all experimental animals including information about excluded animals.

    elife-80517-supp1.xlsx (120.2KB, xlsx)
    Supplementary file 2. DESeq, RRHO, and Fisher’s Exact Test intersection gene lists.

    10.6084/m9.figshare.19911577. Workbook includes all gene lists used in analysis. For DESeq, all results dataframes and differentially expressed genes are included with all output metrics. RRHO tabs include genes in each list used to generate heatmaps and overlapping genes for each quadrant as well as genes in the Fisher’s Exact Test intersections. Descriptions of all column names, the associated figure, and versions of packages used are in the last three tabs.

    elife-80517-supp2.xlsx (8.1MB, xlsx)
    Supplementary file 3. Ingenuity Pathway Analysis data.

    10.6084/m9.figshare.19911583. Comprehensive output from IPA analyses for both Canonical Pathways and Upstream Regulators. Includes all terms with those that are statistically significant highlighted in red.

    elife-80517-supp3.xlsx (86.9KB, xlsx)
    MDAR checklist

    Data Availability Statement

    All sequencing data is available on GEO (GSE192661). All lab specific code is available on the Donaldson Lab GitHub (https://github.com/donaldsonlab/separationRNAseq; copy archived at Sadino, 2023) and all metadata including comprehensive statistical analyses, animal information, differential gene expression, gene sets, and IPA analyses are available on the Donaldson lab Figshare. The vole optimized DIO-eGFP-pvRPL10a vector will be made available on Addgene.

    The following datasets were generated:

    Sadino JM. 2022. Supplemental Data 1. figshare.

    Sadino JM. 2022. Supplemental Data 2. figshare.

    Sadino JM. 2022. Supplemental Data 3. figshare.

    Sadino JM. 2023. RNAseq data from: Prolonged partner separation erodes nucleus accumbens transcriptional signatures of pair bonding in male prairie voles. NCBI Gene Expression Omnibus. GSE192661


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES