Abstract
Background:
Poor air quality has been linked to cognitive deficits in children, but this relationship has not been examined in the first year of life when brain growth is at its peak.
Methods:
We measured in-home air quality focusing on particulate matter with diameter of <2.5 μm (PM2.5) and infants’ cognition longitudinally in a sample of families from rural India.
Results:
Air quality was poorer in homes that used solid cooking materials. Infants from homes with poorer air quality showed lower visual working memory scores at 6 and 9 months of age and slower visual processing speed from 6 to 21 months when controlling for family socio-economic status.
Conclusions:
Thus, poor air quality is associated with impaired visual cognition in the first two years of life, consistent with animal studies of early brain development. We demonstrate for the first time an association between air quality and cognition in the first year of life using direct measures of in-home air quality and looking-based measures of cognition. Because indoor air quality was linked to cooking materials in the home, our findings suggest that efforts to reduce cooking emissions should be a key target for intervention.
Funding:
Bill & Melinda Gates Foundation grant OPP1164153.
Research organism: Human
Introduction
The impact of poor air quality on neurocognitive health is a global concern. This impact was recently quantified by the Global Burden of Disease Study in India, attributing 1.67 million deaths in 2019 to air pollution-related causes with an overall national economic loss of $36.8 billion (Pandey et al., 2021). Such economic losses are compounded by evidence that poor air quality impacts neurocognitive health in childhood as economic losses accumulate over years of reduced productivity (Heckman and Mosso, 2014). Studies have reported that poor air quality and proximity to roadways is associated with reduced general cognitive functioning in childhood (Freire et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2009; Harris et al., 2015; Suglia et al., 2008) and slower growth in working memory (Sunyer et al., 2015). Exposure to poor air quality is also a risk factor for child emotional and behavioural problems which can have severe impacts on families (Midouhas et al., 2019).
But how early do these effects emerge? This is an important question as animal studies show profound impacts on the brain in early development, with inhalation of diesel exhaust and ultrafine particles resulting in elevated cytokine expression and oxidative stress in the brain (Gerlofs-Nijland et al., 2010; Bos et al., 2012), as well as altered neurogenesis (Patten et al., 2020). Very small particulate fragments (particulate matter with diameter of <2.5 μm; PM2.5) are of major concern as they can move from the respiratory tract into the circulatory system reaching the brain. The brain may also be particularly sensitive during infancy due an immature detoxification response (Grandjean and Landrigan, 2014). Several large-scale studies have looked at the effects of prenatal exposure to nitrous oxide and PM2.5 on human cognition early in development (Guxens et al., 2012; Guxens et al., 2014). Results show reduced psychomotor functioning at 1–6 years of age, but no associations with early cognition, although three studies have reported slower cognitive growth and emotional / conduct problems in children exposed to poorer indoor air quality as assessed via questionnaires (Midouhas et al., 2019; Gonzalez-Casanova et al., 2018; Vrijheid et al., 2012).
Critically, no studies have looked at the relationship between poor air quality and cognition in the first year of life when brain size doubles and may be particularly sensitive to toxins. This may reflect the challenge of assessing cognition in infancy. While standardized measures exist (e.g. Mullen Scales of Early Learning), these tools may not generalize to non-Western cultures where air quality is poorest. An alternative is to measure infant cognition using specially-designed looking-based tasks. Multiple aspects of visual cognition that are predictive of later cognitive abilities (Rose et al., 2012) can be measured reliably in the first year across cultures (Rose, 1994; Wijeakumar et al., 2019). For instance, visual processing speed measured using looking-based tasks in both infancy and toddlerhood is significantly predictive of working memory and executive function scores at 11 years (Rose et al., 2012). A second challenge is that infants spend much of the first year indoors. Consequently, data from outdoor monitoring stations may not accurately reflect air quality exposure critical for early brain development, particularly in contexts where use of, for instance, solid fuels in cooking may lead to differences between indoor and outdoor air quality. Recent advances in air quality monitoring allow for the measurement of PM2.5 directly in homes; this may be critical as PM2.5 is possibly the most neurotoxic component of residential air quality (Block and Calderón-Garcidueñas, 2009).
Here, we examined the relationship between poor air quality and cognition in infancy using looking-based measures of cognition and in-home measures of PM2.5. To assess visual cognition, we used a specially-designed, transculturally-relevant visual cognition task which assessed infants’ visual working memory and processing speed (Figure 1A and B; see Ross-Sheehy et al., 2003), capitalizing on infants’ tendency to look away from visual familiarity and toward visual novelty. This task has been examined in several prior studies (Oakes et al., 2006; Oakes et al., 2009), but this is the first study to use this task longitudinally with a sample of non-western infants. Infants visually explored two displays that blinked ‘on’ and ‘off’. On one side – the ‘no change’ side – the same colored squares were always presented; on the other side, one randomly-selected square changed color after each blink. If infants begin looking at the ‘no change’ side and they can remember the colors in working memory, they should lose interest in this display, releasing fixation to visually explore the ‘changing’ display. Here, they should detect the novel color and sustain looking to this display, leading to a strong change preference – a high proportion of looking to the changing side. This change preference is modulated by visual working memory capacity. When the number of items on the screen is small (i.e. the memory load is low), infants can detect the difference between the ‘no change’ and ‘change’ displays, and they show a higher change preference. With increasing memory load (and increasing difficulty), infants may falsely detect changes on the ‘no change side’ and have difficulty releasing fixation, leading to a lower change preference (Perone et al., 2011). Thus, the change preference score yields a quantifiable measure of infants’ visual working memory abilities. Note that if infants begin looking at the ‘change’ side, they should remain looking at this display with a consistently high change preference score (see Methods for further discussion).
Our study was conducted in Shivgarh, India, a rural community in Uttar Pradesh, one of the states in India that has been most strongly impacted by poor air quality (Pandey et al., 2021). We report data from 215 families from a range of socio-economic backgrounds (see Table 1). Infants were enrolled at 6 months (N=108) or 9 months of age (N=107) at which time they completed an in-lab assessment of visual cognition (see Figure 1A) as well as standardized psychomotor and cognitive assessments. A similar assessment was repeated a year later when the same infants were 18 or 21 months of age (see Methods). We examined these two cohorts based on evidence that there is an improvement in visual working memory capacity between 6 and 8 months. In particular, Ross-Sheehy and colleagues (Ross-Sheehy et al., 2003) showed that 6.5-month-old infants demonstrate greater-than-chance change preference scores with a memory load of one item, while 10- and 13-month-old infants showed greater-than-chance change preference scores for memory loads of two and three items. Similarly, 6.5-month-old infants could remember one spatial location, while 8- and 12.5-month-old infants showed evidence of remembering multiple locations (Oakes et al., 2011). By assessing infants in rural India on either side of this transition, we hoped to either replicate a similar transition or, alternatively, to reveal a delay in this transition across cultures.
Table 1. Proportional distribution of key demographic indices for families classified as high versus low SES based on a median split of family SES measured using the modified Kuppuswamy scale.
Electricity? | Income* | Cooking Fuel | High SES | Low SES |
---|---|---|---|---|
No | Low | Cow dung | 0.02 | 0.06 |
Wood | 0.02 | 0.25 | ||
LPG | 0.01 | 0.01 | ||
Medium | Cow dung | 0.01 | 0.02 | |
Wood | 0.04 | 0.19 | ||
LPG | 0.00 | 0.00 | ||
High | Cow dung | 0.00 | 0.00 | |
Wood | 0.03 | 0.08 | ||
LPG | 0.01 | 0.00 | ||
Yes | Low | Cow dung | 0.04 | 0.01 |
Wood | 0.08 | 0.10 | ||
LPG | 0.04 | 0.01 | ||
Medium | Cow dung | 0.03 | 0.02 | |
Wood | 0.17 | 0.17 | ||
LPG | 0.02 | 0.00 | ||
High | Cow dung | 0.02 | 0.01 | |
Wood | 0.22 | 0.06 | ||
LPG | 0.26 | 0.02 |
Incomes ranged from ₹8000 to ₹480,000 with tertile divisions at ₹45,000 and ₹72,720.
Methods
Participants
Infants born to mothers from Shivgarh, Uttar Pradesh, India and who were aged 6 months±15 days or 9 months±15 days were eligible for participation. Infants were initially screened as belonging to either ‘high socioeconomic status (SES)’ (both parents having >10 years of education) or ‘low SES’ families (both parents have ≤ 5 years of education; see Ahmed et al., 2018 for a similar screening approach). Infants born to parents screened with colour vision deficits (due to the nature of the VWM task), or with any congenital problems, or gestational age <26 weeks at birth, were excluded from the study.
We brought 257 families to the lab for the VWM and cognitive assessment; however, 17 families (10 6-month-old infants; 7 9-month-old infants) did not complete the assessment and were dropped from the study. The remaining 240 families were followed up for the duration of the study which spanned two years and included the following: (1) a laboratory assessment in year 1 at 6 or 9 months of age; (2) a home visit every three months thereafter for the remainder of year 1 (e.g. at 9, 12, and 15 months of age for the 6 month cohort); (3) a laboratory assessment in year 2 at 18 or 21 months of age; (4) a home visit every three months thereafter for the remainder of year 2 (e.g. at 21, 24, and 27 months of age for the 6-month cohort). Enrollment was distributed over 4 waves separated by 3 months, such that we enrolled approximately 60 infants per wave of data collection (for a full list of data collected in the study, see Supplementary Materials). The study was approved by the Community Empowerment Lab Institutional Ethics Committee (Ref. No: CEL/2018005). Participants’ caregivers provided written informed consent; where caregivers were illiterate, a witness gave signed consent accompanied by a thumb impression of the caregiver in place of a signature. At the end of each laboratory session, families received a small token of appreciation.
Air quality data were collected for a total of 219 children; of those, 215 had data that survived initial data quality checks (described below). 213 children also had data from the visual working memory task and cognitive assessment during either the first laboratory assessment (when infants were 6 or 9 months of age) or the second laboratory assessment (when infants were 18 or 21 months of age). In particular, 204 participants contributed visual working memory data in year 1 when they were 6 months of age (N=109; N girls = 50; M age = 182.4 days, SD age = 14.9 days) or 9 months of age (N=95; N girls = 51; M age = 267.2 days, SD age = 14.2 days). In year 2, 181 participants contributed visual working memory data when the infants were 18 months of age (N=90; N girls = 45; M age = 546.2 days, SD age = 19.2 days) or 21 months of age (N=91; N girls = 45; M age = 627.4 days, SD age = 27.7 days).
As part of the first laboratory visit, caregivers were interviewed to obtain demographic indices and family SES data using the modified Kuppuswamy Scale (Mohd Saleem, 2020). This scale classifies SES using occupation, education, and family income. Table 1 shows how high and low SES families (using a median split on the Kuppuswamy SES Score) were distributed along multiple demographic indices.
Materials
Visual cognition task
We used a preferential looking change detection task (Ross-Sheehy et al., 2003) with a 10 s trial duration (see Wijeakumar et al., 2019; Delgado Reyes et al., 2020). A 42-inch LCD monitor that was connected to a PC running Experiment Builder was used to display the stimuli. Looking data were collected at 500 Hz using an Eyelink 1000 Plus eye-tracker (SR Research). Where eye-tracking data were not available, looking data were collected with a webcam and hand-coded at 30 frames-per-second using Datavyu, 2014.
Infants sat on their mother’s lap 100 cm from the screen. A target sticker was placed on the infant’s forehead so the eye-tracking system could track head movement. The stimuli consisted of two side-by-side flickering displays composed of colored squares (Figure 1A). One side contained the ‘change’ display and the other contained the ‘no-change’ display. Each stimulus display area was 29.5 cm in width and 21 cm in height, with a 21 cm gap between the display on the left and right (each coloured square was approximately 5cm x 5 cm). The displays had a solid grey background. The colours of the squares presented on each display were selected from a set of nine colours: green (RGB: 0, 153, 0), brown (128, 64, 32), black (0, 0, 0), violet (128, 0, 128), cyan (128, 255, 255), yellow (255, 255, 0), blue (0, 0, 255), white (255, 255, 255), and red (255, 0, 0). The set size (number of items) was the same between the two displays and remained constant during the 10 s trials. The colors on a display were always different from each other but colors could be repeated between the displays.
Standardized assessments
We collected standardized psychomotor and cognitive assessments in year 1 using the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL). In year 2, we used the Ages and Stages Questionnaire (3rd edition; ASQ). We switched to the ASQ in year 2 because preliminary testing with the MSEL revealed many questions that were not relevant to the rural setting in Shivgarh.
Air quality monitoring
Air quality data were acquired using the Air Visual Node monitor (model B01MF6X1 YK, Atlanta Healthcare, Inc). We selected this device based on validation data comparing the device to a reference Beta Attenuation Monitor (BAM) in Beijing, China during June of 2015 (Molenar, 2016). Daily concentration estimates from the two devices correlated highly (R2 =0.96). We purchased 20 devices.
Procedure
Visual cognition task
Children were tested individually. The task began with a 5-point calibration sequence. Next, the visual cognition task began (see Figure 1A). The squares simultaneously appeared for 500ms and disappeared for 250ms during the 10 s trials. For the ‘no-change’ display, the colors of the squares remained constant throughout the trial. For the ‘change’ display, one of the squares changed color after each disappearance. The changing square was randomly selected, and its color was derived from the set of colors not currently present in that display. Consistent with prior studies, memory load was varied between 1, 2, and 3 items on each side for 6- and 9-month-old infants and 2, 4, and 6 items on each side for 18- and 21-month-old infants. Infants were presented with 36 total trials in six blocks of six trials. Each block contained three trials (one for each load) for each change side. Participants could take breaks between blocks. Participants completed on average 20.99 trials in year 1 (SD = 9.72) and 26.25 trials in year 2 (SD = 9.66).
Standardized assessments
Standardized assessments were conducted in a quiet room with trained staff. The MSEL was administered in Year 1, while the ASQ was administered in Year 2. The ASQ questionnaire for each infant was selected using the online ASQ calculator (https://agesandstages.com/free-resources/asq-calculator/). We adapted the ASQ administration to improve the reliability of the data. Specifically, a trained assessor administered the ASQ in collaboration with the parent. In cases where ASQ questions asked about behaviors that could be elicited (e.g. ‘When you ask your child to, does he go into another room to find a familiar toy or object?’), these tasks were completed ‘live’, ensuring that the child was given ample time. If a question was not amenable to live assessment, the mother’s verbal report was taken as the response.
Use of the Air Visual Node device
During each in-home assessment period, air quality data were collected across 3 days with the monitor placed at roughly head height in the room where the child typically slept or spent most of their time (see Figure 2A). The monitor was inaccessible to children. Air quality was monitored in 3-monthly intervals in-between the Year 1 and Year 2 laboratory visits for a maximum of 6 collection periods for each household. Data were collected from the AVN device every 10 s. Households had to have at least 5 hr of recorded data in any given round for that round to be included in analysis.
Data from the air quality monitors were downloaded after each 3-day period and quality checked. After 8–10 months of use, a few of the devices were returning very high or very low values. Henceforth, devices were regularly cleaned. After cleaning, multiple devices were tested in the same room to ensure they were returning the same air quality readings. Any devices returning erroneous values were retired. By the end of the 2-year study, 11 devices had been retired.
Methods of analysis
Visual cognition measures
The eye-tracking data were exported on a frame-by-frame basis using SR research Data Viewer. The area of interest around the two objects was increased to match video coded data which coded looking to the left, right, or away. Where there was no recorded eye-tracking data, the hand-coded video data were used instead. Of the 9956 total trials used to calculate the scores, 3047 trials (30.6%) were manually coded. We re-coded 17% of the data to check reliabilities. Reliabilities were very good with a mean Kappa for the 6-month cohort of 0.73 and a mean Kappa for the 9-month cohort of 0.83.
The looking data were read into R and pre-processed using the R package eyetrackingR (Dink and Ferguson, 2021). For the change preference measure, trials where more than 75% of the data was recorded as not looking at the screen were excluded. Initial analyses of these change preference scores revealed that the change preference measure was not robust longitudinally, that is, year 1 change preference scores did not predict year 2 change preference scores (note: a full correlation table for key measures used in the present study can be found in the supplementary materials; see Supplementary file 1). This was the case for the present sample as well as a longitudinal sample from urban UK infants collected as part of a complementary study (see S. Forbes, K. Jackson, K. Mee, J. McCarthy, L. Delgado-Reyes, M. Tiwari, V. Singh, A. Kumar, J. P. Spencer, A longitudinal assessment of the development of visual working memory accross cultures. manuscript in preparation for details). Additional analyses from a forthcoming paper (Forbes et al., manuscript in preparation) revealed that sorting trials based on where infants are looking at the start of the first ‘change’ display (i.e. 1000ms) yields two measure that are stable longitudinally – a ‘first-look change’ score and a ‘first-look no-change’ score. This makes sense as prior to the first change on the screen participants have no way of knowing which is the ‘no-change’ side and which is the ‘change’ side. Furthermore, the demands placed on visual cognition differ depending on the starting location. If infants start on the ‘no-change’ side, they should notice the ‘sameness’ if the number of items is within their visual working memory capacity and release fixation due to a lack of visual novelty. Thus, ‘first-look no-change’ trials might be particularly sensitive to individual differences in visual working memory capacity (for discussion, see Forbes et al., manuscript in preparation). By contrast, if infants start on the ‘change’ side, they should readily notice the novelty and remain fixated on that display. Note that sorting trials based on where infants are looking at a key moment in time is a standard approach in the infant eye-tracking literature (e.g. see Fernald et al., 1998).
Infants’ looking data were sorted into two types of trials (first-look no-change, first-look change) based on where they were looking at the onset of the first change (1000ms after trial start). Unfortunately, 1030 trials (13.1% of the total trials) had missing data within the time window from 1000 to 1100ms (e.g. due to a failure to track the eye in this window). Thus, we allowed the ‘first-look’ classification to be determined based on the first frame of non-missing eye-tracking data up to 2500ms (which spanned from the onset of the first change display [1000ms] through two display +delay periods). This allowed us to classify an additional 613 trials, yielding a total trial loss of 417 trials (5.3%) due to a failure to classify the ‘first-look’ status. The change preference analysis focused on the analysis period from 1750ms to 6750ms. We trimmed the last few seconds of data from each trial as the number of eye-tracking samples diminished as attention waned (see Forbes et al., manuscript in preparation for a detailed analysis justifying this time window).
The shift rate measure was taken from the full length of any trial. This measure counted the number of switches participants made from one side of the screen to the other divided by the number of seconds that participants were looking at the display, resulting in shifts per second.
The baseline model for each change preference measure was a linear mixed-effect model with year (1 or 2), working memory load (low, medium or high), SES (Kuppuswamy SES score), and age cohort (6 or 9 months) as independent variables. We also included the mean proportion looking to the first fixated item in the first time window (1–750ms) before any visual changes had been introduced to get an initial measure of visual dynamics for each infant (LookingWindow1). Year and age cohort were difference-coded, SES was centered, and load was input as a factor. To allow for individual differences across year and load, a random intercept for each participant was included. To arrive at a minimal baseline model, we began with a model that only included main effects. We then introduced two-way, three-way, and four-way interactions, only including interactive effects that showed evidence of improving the model fit. For the ‘first-look no-change’ measure, the final baseline model included all main effects and the following interactions: Year x SES, Year x LookingWindow1, SES x LookingWindow1, and Year x SES x LookingWindow1 (see Table 2). For the ‘first-look change’ measure, the final baseline model included all main effects (see Table 10).
Table 2. Baseline model for the change preference scores.
Variable | Estimate | Std. Error | DF | t value | Pr(>|t|) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(Intercept) | 0.488 | 0.054 | 534.793 | 9.062 | <0.001 |
Year | –0.044 | 0.107 | 742.433 | –0.411 | 0.681 |
Load1 | 0.029 | 0.009 | 857.116 | 3.130 | 0.002 |
Load2 | –0.001 | 0.009 | 854.106 | –0.114 | 0.909 |
SES | –0.006 | 0.013 | 527.033 | –0.417 | 0.677 |
LookingWindow1 | –0.067 | 0.065 | 548.916 | –1.038 | 0.300 |
Age | 0.027 | 0.014 | 183.400 | 1.913 | 0.057 |
Year:SES | 0.036 | 0.027 | 715.570 | 1.350 | 0.177 |
Year:LookingWindow1 | 0.075 | 0.129 | 731.071 | 0.582 | 0.561 |
SES:LookingWindow1 | 0.006 | 0.016 | 532.948 | 0.381 | 0.704 |
Year:SES:LookingWindow1 | –0.054 | 0.033 | 710.951 | –1.665 | 0.096 |
All models were assessed for fit based on a Q-Q plot of the residuals and the R package DHARMa (Hartig, 2020). Analyses reported in the tables use the p values calculated using Satterthwaite’s method from the package lmerTest (Kuznetsova et al., 2017) to enable the reader to glean the direction of all effects. In addition, the contribution of each significant effect to a model was assessed using type 3 Wald Chi-squared tests, with effect sizes reported in text using the effectsize package in R (Ben-Shachar et al., 2020). In all models we aimed to theoretically motivate the inclusion of all effects and limit spurious correlations by only adding effects where they were thought to contribute a priori. In the case of interactions, models were tested to see whether interactions improved model fit through formal model comparison, and interactions were not included if they did not contribute to model fit.
The baseline model for shift rate was arrived at using a similar procedure. The final baseline model included all main effects and a Year x SES interaction (see Table 3).
Table 3. Model parameters for linear mixed effect model assessing the impact of year, load, SES and age cohort on the shift rate (baseline visual processing speed model).
Variable | Estimate | Std. Error | DF | t value | Pr(>|t|) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(Intercept) | 0.631 | 0.014 | 196.30 | 45.260 | <0.001 |
Year | 0.016 | 0.017 | 893.40 | 0.963 | 0.336 |
Load1 | 0.040 | 0.011 | 812.80 | 3.485 | <0.001 |
Load2 | 0.001 | 0.011 | 811.70 | 0.071 | 0.943 |
SES | 0.002 | 0.003 | 207.00 | 0.545 | 0.587 |
Age | 0.012 | 0.028 | 195.80 | 0.429 | 0.668 |
Year:SES | –0.007 | 0.004 | 928.20 | –1.553 | 0.121 |
Standardized assessments
We used non-standardized measures from the MSEL and ASQ as there are no standardized scores for rural India. From the MSEL, we use the composite standard T-score as a general measure of cognition. The baseline model for this measure was a linear model with age cohort and SES included as main effects (see Table 4). For the ASQ, we used the problem-solving measure. The baseline model was a linear model with the ASQ measure as the dependent variable and age cohort and SES as independent variables (see Table 4).
Table 4. Model parameters for linear models describing effects of age cohort and SES on standardized cognitive scores (baseline standardized cognitive models).
Measure | Variable | Estimate | Std. Error | t value | Pr(>|t|) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mullen Composite | (Intercept) | 101.299 | 0.960 | 105.562 | <0.001 |
Age | –0.345 | 1.920 | –0.180 | 0.857 | |
SES | 0.769 | 0.232 | 3.308 | 0.001 | |
ASQ Problem Solving | (Intercept) | 30.581 | 0.955 | 32.032 | <0.001 |
Age | –5.549 | 1.909 | –2.907 | 0.004 | |
SES | 0.923 | 0.231 | 3.998 | <0.001 |
Data filtering and analysis of air quality data
The air quality data were collated and read into R. Data were inspected to understand the cyclical patterns across days and months. The air quality data were filtered to remove data from any defective devices (see above), and then down-sampled so that we had one observation per hour. This smoothed the data and removed temporal autocorrelation. The down-sampled data were then fit with a generalized additive mixed model (GAMM) in R using the ‘bam’ function from the package mgcv (Wood, 2011). The model included air quality as the dependent variable and a tensor smooth of time of day (in hours) and date, with a cubic regression spline of 11 dimensions for each as independent variables. The model also allowed a smoothed random effect of participant. To reduce autocorrelation, the model was fit with a Rho of 0.6. Temporal autocorrelation was assessed using the itsadug package in R (van Rij et al., 2020). The model provided an excellent fit, capturing annual fluctuations by date (see Figure 2B) and daily fluctuations by hour (see Figure 2C).
The participant-level random intercept was extracted from the model, to act as a single data measurement to represent air quality in each home. This measurement was highly correlated with the mean air quality score for each participant, t(213) = 21.348, p<0.001. This individual level score was centered around 0, where a positive score indicated a higher air quality score (i.e. poorer air quality) on average than the grand mean (i.e. 185.824). For visualization purposes, Figures 2 and 3 were adjusted by adding the grand mean to the individual score. Note that the participant-level random intercept provides a measurement that is more robust than the mean air quality value as time of day and seasonal effects are included in the model. This was particularly evident in cases where a participant contributed only a small number of samples (in which case, the sample values can be biased due to time of day).
We modelled individual estimates of air quality using a linear model with the AQI random intercept value as the dependent variable and age cohort and cooking fuel type as independent variables (see Table 5). A second model for air quality was run but using the SES score (centered) as a replacement for cooking fuel (also in Table 5). These models were comparable, showing the strong relationship between cooking fuel and SES (see also, Table 1).
Table 5. Model parameters for linear models assessing the baseline variability of SES, cooking fuel, and age cohort on the air quality reading (baseline air quality models).
Model | Variable | Estimate | Std. Error | t value | Pr(>|t|) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Cooking Fuel | (Intercept) | 1.461 | 2.767 | 0.528 | 0.598 |
Cooking Fuel1 | 10.866 | 4.517 | 2.405 | 0.017 | |
Cooking Fuel2 | –1.519 | 3.119 | –0.487 | 0.627 | |
Age | –0.548 | 4.213 | –0.13 | 0.897 | |
SES | (Intercept) | –0.004 | 2.099 | –0.002 | 0.999 |
SES | –1.152 | 0.511 | –2.255 | 0.025 | |
Age | –1.714 | 4.198 | –0.408 | 0.683 |
Analysis of air quality associations with cognition
To assess the association of air quality with visual cognition, we added air quality to the baseline model for each dependent variable, that is, for the ‘first-look no-change’ change preference score (Table 6), shift rate (Table 7), and the ‘first-look change’ change preference score (Table 11). An Air Quality x Year interaction was also included in the ‘first-look no-change’ change preference model as this improved the model fit, assessed using the anova function in R, Χ2 (1)=4.824, p=0.028, η2 (partial)=0.005.
Table 6. Model parameters for linear mixed-effect model assessing the impact of air quality (AQI) on the baseline change preference score model which included Year, Load, SES, Age Cohort, and LookingWindow1 as predictors (see Table 2).
Variable | Estimate | Std. Error | DF | t value | Pr(>|t|) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(Intercept) | 0.494 | 0.054 | 535.80 | 9.186 | <0.001 |
Year | –0.044 | 0.107 | 741.90 | –0.411 | 0.681 |
Load1 | 0.029 | 0.009 | 858.10 | 3.135 | 0.002 |
Load2 | –0.001 | 0.009 | 855.20 | –0.124 | 0.901 |
SES | –0.009 | 0.014 | 524.70 | –0.663 | 0.508 |
LookingWindow1 | –0.075 | 0.065 | 549.90 | –1.155 | 0.249 |
Age | 0.026 | 0.014 | 184.70 | 1.810 | 0.072 |
AQI | 0.000 | 0.000 | 181.80 | –0.654 | 0.514 |
Year:SES | 0.040 | 0.027 | 720.10 | 1.471 | 0.142 |
Year:LookingWindow1 | 0.075 | 0.128 | 730.70 | 0.585 | 0.559 |
SES:LookingWindow1 | 0.010 | 0.016 | 530.80 | 0.607 | 0.544 |
Year:AQI | 0.001 | 0.000 | 965.10 | 2.431 | 0.015 |
Year:SES:LookingWindow1 | –0.057 | 0.033 | 714.90 | –1.750 | 0.080 |
Table 7. Model parameters for linear mixed-effect model assessing the impact of air quality (AQI) on the baseline visual processing speed (shift rate) model which included Year, Load, SES, and Age Cohort as predictors (see Table 3).
Variable | Estimate | Std. Error | DF | t value | Pr(>|t|) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(Intercept) | 0.631 | 0.014 | 195.40 | 45.655 | <0.001 |
Year | 0.016 | 0.017 | 894.50 | 0.977 | 0.329 |
Load1 | 0.040 | 0.011 | 813.20 | 3.501 | <0.001 |
Load2 | 0.001 | 0.011 | 812.10 | 0.045 | 0.964 |
SES | 0.001 | 0.003 | 205.10 | 0.225 | 0.823 |
Age | 0.009 | 0.028 | 195.30 | 0.318 | 0.750 |
AQI | –0.001 | 0.000 | 192.10 | –2.166 | 0.032 |
Year:SES | –0.006 | 0.004 | 928.60 | –1.493 | 0.136 |
To assess the association of air quality with standardized cognitive scores, we added air quality to the baseline model for the MSEL scores (Table 8) and the ASQ problem-solving scores (Table 9). In addition, following findings from Guxens et al., 2014, we conducted analyses on the ASQ fine and gross motor scores to examine whether these measures were associated with air quality (see Table 9).
Table 8. Model parameters for linear model assessing the impact of air quality (AQI) on the baseline Mullen model (Composite T-Score) which included Age Cohort and SES as predictors (see Table 4).
Variable | Estimate | Std. Error | t value | Pr(>|t|) |
---|---|---|---|---|
(Intercept) | 101.301 | 0.961 | 105.398 | <0.001 |
Age | –0.377 | 1.923 | –0.196 | 0.845 |
SES | 0.746 | 0.236 | 3.163 | 0.002 |
AQI | –0.019 | 0.032 | –0.596 | 0.552 |
Table 9. Model parameters for linear models assessing the impact of air quality (AQI) on the baseline ASQ model which included Age Cohort and SES as predictors (see Table 4).
Measure | Variable | Estimate | Std. Error | t value | Pr(>|t|) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
ASQ Problem Solving | (Intercept) | 30.563 | 0.955 | 32.004 | <0.001 |
Age | –5.617 | 1.910 | –2.941 | 0.004 | |
SES | 0.890 | 0.233 | 3.817 | <0.001 | |
AQI | –0.030 | 0.031 | –0.983 | 0.327 | |
ASQ Fine Motor | (Intercept) | 34.224 | 1.039 | 32.944 | <0.001 |
Age | –5.655 | 2.078 | –2.721 | 0.007 | |
SES | 0.815 | 0.254 | 3.214 | 0.002 | |
AQI | –0.025 | 0.033 | –0.748 | 0.455 | |
ASQ Gross Motor | (Intercept) | 36.849 | 1.061 | 34.733 | <0.001 |
Age | –8.221 | 2.122 | –3.874 | <0.001 | |
SES | 0.571 | 0.259 | 2.206 | 0.029 | |
AQI | –0.054 | 0.034 | –1.582 | 0.115 |
Results
We focused on three primary measures from the preferential looking task: (1) the shift rate, that is, the number of looks back and forth (per second), (2) the change preference score for trials where infants’ ‘first look’ was to the ‘no change’ side, and (3) the change preference score for trials where infants’ ‘first look’ was to the ‘change’ side. Use of the shift rate measure was motivated by prior work showing that visual processing speed in infancy is predictive of longer-term cognitive outcomes (Rose et al., 2012). The latter two measures are an adaptation of the standard ‘change preference’ looking measure from this task. Note that because the ‘first-look no-change’ measure starts at 0 (i.e. looking to no change) and the ‘first-look change’ measure starts at 1 (i.e. looking to change), chance performance is no longer anchored at 0.5. Although this differs from prior work, the change was motivated by evidence that the standard measure is not predictive longitudinally, while the adapted measures are (see Methods for discussion). Note further that we focus on the ‘first look no-change’ measure below (referred to as the ‘change preference’ score for simplicity), as the only significant finding for the ‘first look change’ measure was a decrease in the change preference score from year 1 to year 2 (see Table 10 and Table 11).
Table 10. Model parameters from the baseline mixed-effects model assessing the effects of Year, Load, SES, LookingWindow1 and Age cohort on the first look change measure.
Variable | Estimate | Std. Error | DF | t value | Pr(>|t|) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(Intercept) | 0.592 | 0.054 | 613.50 | 11.009 | <0.001 |
Year | –0.030 | 0.013 | 966.10 | –2.353 | 0.019 |
Load1 | 0.000 | 0.009 | 846.40 | 0.024 | 0.981 |
Load2 | 0.002 | 0.009 | 847.40 | 0.175 | 0.861 |
SES | 0.002 | 0.002 | 205.30 | 0.874 | 0.383 |
LookingWindow1 | 0.057 | 0.065 | 628.30 | 0.882 | 0.378 |
Age | –0.019 | 0.015 | 188.80 | –1.331 | 0.185 |
Table 11. Model parameters assessing the impact of air quality (AQI) on the baseline mixed-effects model assessing the effects of Year, Load, SES, LookingWindow1 and Age cohort on the first look change measure.
Variable | Estimate | Std. Error | DF | t value | Pr(>|t|) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(Intercept) | 0.589 | 0.054 | 612.30 | 10.944 | <0.001 |
Year | –0.030 | 0.013 | 967.10 | –2.395 | 0.017 |
Load1 | 0.000 | 0.009 | 847.40 | –0.008 | 0.994 |
Load2 | 0.002 | 0.009 | 848.30 | 0.182 | 0.855 |
SES | 0.002 | 0.002 | 205.30 | 1.105 | 0.270 |
LookingWindow1 | 0.062 | 0.065 | 626.90 | 0.952 | 0.341 |
Age | –0.018 | 0.014 | 189.50 | –1.275 | 0.204 |
AQI | 0.000 | 0.000 | 185.80 | 1.263 | 0.208 |
Year:AQI | –0.001 | 0.000 | 968.20 | –1.402 | 0.161 |
Six-month-old infants showed lower change preference scores than 9-month-old infants (Figure 1C), Χ2 (1)=3.66, p=0.056 ηp 2 = 0.02 (for full results, see Table 2). In addition, change preference scores decreased as the memory load increased, Χ2 (2)=12.53, p=0.002 ηp 2 = 0.01 (see Figure 1D). Similar effects were observed in an analysis of infants’ rate of looking back and forth between the displays (i.e. shift rate; see Table 3). Infants’ shift rate decreased as the memory load increased, Χ2 (2)=16.52, p<0.001 ηp 2 = 0.02 (see Figure 1E). In addition, infants’ standardized cognitive scores in year 1, F(1) = 10.94, p=0.001 ηp 2 = 0.050, and year 2, F(1) = 15.99, p<0.001 ηp 2 = 0.080, were consistently lower for low SES infants (see Figure 1F and G and Table 4).
We recorded in-home air quality using a laser particle sensor (Air Visual Node, Atlanta Healthcare, Inc) placed in the home (see Figure 2A) for 3 continuous days during each assessment period. Field workers were instructed to place the sensor in the room where infants slept or spent most of their time. We re-assessed air quality up to six times for each family (every three months in-between lab visits; M visits = 4, SD = 1.16). These data were modelled and a participant-level score which adjusted for season and time of day was extracted for use in the analyses (see Methods). We focused on PM2.5 concentrations expressed as US Air Quality Index (AQI) values. This index maps PM2.5 concentrations measured in μg/m3 to a more intuitive categorical scale where AQI values under 50 are good, values from 51 to 100 indicate moderate air quality, values 101–150 indicate air that is unhealthy for sensitive groups, and values higher than 151 are considered unhealthy extending up to hazardous (>301). Note that AQI values can be readily converted to μg/m3 using on-line calculators (e.g. https://www.airnow.gov/aqi/aqi-calculator/).
As can be seen in Figure 2B, air quality in Shivgarh was quite poor, with AQI values often higher than 151, with an overall mean of 207. This is comparable to recent AQI data for the northern states in India that ranged from 186 to 267 (Pandey et al., 2021). We also compared in-home air quality to daily outdoor air quality from the nearest monitoring station in Lucknow (red line in Figure 2B; see Central Pollution Control Board in India: https://cpcb.nic.in). Annual fluctuations in in-home air quality generally mirrored fluctuations in outdoor air quality in Lucknow, although with a lower peak in the winter of 2018–2019 (for similar seasonal variations in indoor and outdoor air quality, see Rohra and Taneja, 2016). We note that this lower peak occurred during a pause in our indoor air quality data collection; thus, we cannot evaluate if this is a mismatch or simply a result of limited data during this period in the indoor air quality model.
In-home air quality (PM2.5) was strongly influenced by daily meal preparation, with peaks in poor air quality occurring during meal preparation times (see Figure 2C; see Mukhopadhyay et al., 2012 for similar meal preparation findings). Air quality was poorest in households using cow dung for fuel and best in households using LPG, F(2) = 3.23, p=0.042 ηp 2 = 0.030 (see Figure 2D; Table 5). Note that we were not able to look at the impact of cooking fuel independently from SES as most families that used LPG fuel fell into the high SES tertile (see Table 1).
Next, we explored the association between air quality (PM2.5) and cognitive measures while controlling for both age cohort and SES (see Methods). Critically, infants living in homes with poor air quality had poorer visual cognitive performance. Infants from households with poorer air quality had lower change preference scores in year 1, an effect that was attenuated in year 2, Χ2 (1)=5.91, p=0.015 ηp 2 = 0.006 (see Figure 3A and Year x Air Quality interaction in Table 6). We also found a strong negative association of air quality with visual processing speed (shift rate), Χ2 (1)=4.69, p=0.03 ηp 2 = 0.02 (see Air Quality main effect in Table 7). As can be seen in Figure 3B, infants from households with poor air quality showed slower rates of visual processing. Note that both effects shown in Figure 3 were robust in models that controlled for SES using the modified Kuppuswamy Scale which aggregates effects of family occupation, education, and income (see Mohd Saleem, 2020). Moreover, in both cases, models that included air quality captured a significant proportion of variance above and beyond the baseline cognitive models: models comparing the baseline change preference model (Table 2) to a model that included air quality (Table 6) showed that the air quality model captured a greater proportion of variance in the change preference scores, Χ2 (2)=6.35, p=0.04; similarly, comparing the baseline visual processing speed model (Table 3) to a model that included air quality (Table 7) showed that the air quality model captured a greater proportion of variance in processing speed, Χ2 (1)=4.74, p=0.03.
One concern with these findings is that change preference and shift rate are measured in the same task and are correlated. Thus, it is possible the findings above reflect some shared variance rather than separate effects. To examine this, we re-ran the change preference / air quality analysis, adding shift rate as a predictor of the change preference score. This analysis revealed a main effect of shift rate, Χ2 (1)=17.36, p<0.001 ηp 2 = 0.02, confirming the relationship with the change preference score; however, the Load and Year x Air Quality interaction remained significant (see Supplementary file 2). Thus, the effects shown in Figure 3A are statistically robust, even when shift rate is included in the change preference model. We also re-ran the shift rate / air quality model, including the change preference score as a predictor. This analysis revealed a main effect of change preference score, Χ2 (1)=23.02, p<0.001 ηp 2 = 0.02. Critically, however, the Load and Air Quality main effects remained significant even when the change preference score was included as a predictor (see Supplementary file 3).
Another key question is why the change preference measure only showed a robust association with air quality in the first year. As discussed in the Methods, we modulated the working memory load in year 2 to make the task more challenging and age-appropriate; thus, it is possible we made the task too hard for some infants, dampening our ability to detect individual differences in working memory. To explore this possibility, we re-ran the change preference / air quality analysis, only using data from load 2 trials (i.e. the ‘medium’ load in year 1 and the ‘low’ load in year 2; see Methods). In this analysis with identical task stimuli, we still found a significant Year x Air Quality interaction, F(1,340) = 6.83, p=0.009 ηp 2 = 0.02 (see Supplementary file 4).
Finally, we note that air quality did not significantly impact standardized cognitive scores in year 1 (measured using the Mullen Scales) or year 2 (measured using the ASQ; see Tables 8 and 9). This may indicate that the effects of air quality are specific to the visual cognitive system rather than impacting the more general aspects of cognition and psychomotor function assessed by these measures.
Discussion
The present findings indicate that poor air quality (PM2.5) is associated with slower visual processing speed in the first two years of life and poorer visual working memory scores in year 1. These negative impacts were evident only for looking-based measures of cognition. Our results contrast with findings from prior studies that have failed to show an association between outdoor air quality and cognition in early development. It is possible that this difference reflects the broader range of PM2.5 exposure in our sample. For instance, Guxens and colleagues (Guxens et al., 2014) reported no systematic relationships between air quality and cognitive measures across six European birth cohorts (although they did find effects on psychomotor function); however, air quality ranged from AQI values of 53–72. Our mean AQI value (207) was three to four times higher. Thus, infants in the present report were exposed to much poorer air quality which might explain the strong relationship with visual cognition.
It is also possible that indoor levels of PM2.5 are critical to cognition in infancy. Most prior studies have looked at outdoor air quality, although several studies have reported a negative impact of poor indoor air quality on cognition as assessed via questionnaires (Midouhas et al., 2019; Gonzalez-Casanova et al., 2018; Vrijheid et al., 2012; Midouhas et al., 2018). Interestingly, Vrijheid et al., 2012 found that use of a gas cooker in the home showed a stronger negative association with standardized cognitive scores after 14 months. Our findings extend this work by also showing an association between poor indoor air quality and visual cognition as early as 6 months of age.
One question raised by our findings is why visual working memory effects were isolated to year 1. We investigated one possibility – that the working memory task was too difficult in year 2. Results suggested that this was not the case: when we compared identical conditions across years 1 and 2 we still found an inverse relationship between change preference scores and air quality in year 1 but not in year 2. Another possibility is that the impact of indoor air quality on visual cognition wanes in year 2. Our findings for shift rate argue against this possibility as these findings were robust across both years. The robustness of the shift rate effect may suggest that visual processing speed is a particularly sensitive measure in infancy, consistent with other work showing that visual processing speed in infancy is predictive of schooling outcomes 11 years later (Rose et al., 2012). A third possibility for why the impact of air quality on change preference scores wanes in year 2 is that other factors which are not correlated with air quality have a stronger impact on visual working memory in year 2. For instance, we are currently examining how interactions with caregivers impact infants’ visual working memory abilities. We suspect these interactions have a strong influence on visual working memory development, particularly in year 2 after extended interactions accumulate over time. Interestingly, infant-led interactions – which have been shown to support working memory development (Landry et al., 2006; Perone and Spencer, 2013) – appear to be quite frequent in low SES families in our sample. It is possible such positive influences counter the impact of poor air quality in some families.
Another interesting result from the present study was the specificity of our findings to the visual cognition task. In particular, although results from the Mullen and Ages and Stages Questionnaire showed robust relationships with SES suggesting good sensitivity, these measures showed no associations with air quality. Thus, air quality may have specific effects in infancy, targeting early emerging cognitive systems. Future work will be needed to determine if this is the case. For instance, it would be interesting to examine if air quality has an impact on auditory and/or statistical learning processes in the first two years of life (Saffran et al., 1996).
Strengths of the current study include our use of multiple measures to assess infant cognition including looking-based measures, the longitudinal design, inclusion of a large sample, and a high density of air quality measurements for each household. We also included a continuous range of SES families from the same micro-cultural context, allowing us to tease apart influences of SES while holding culture relatively constant.
Regarding limitations, we note that effect sizes were relatively low in the present study. In addition, there was some instability in the air quality devices over time (see Methods). Note that although laser particle sensors have shown robust correlations with beta attenuation monitors in laboratory and field tests (Zhang and Srinivasan, 2020), it would be ideal in future work to use personal air quality monitors such as gravimetric devices that correct light scatter and use weighted PM2.5. This would enable a direct, localized measure of the air each child is exposed to.
Another limitation of the present study was the absence of more detailed information about cooking practices in the home. Although we identified the primary cooking fuel used in each household, it is likely some households used multiple cooking methods, and other fuels for warming, smoking, and so on (Rohra and Taneja, 2016; Mukhopadhyay et al., 2012). Future work will also be needed to carefully examine whether poor air quality has a causal influence on cognition in early development. Given recent data on the impact of poor air quality on the developing brain in animals, future work using neuroimaging tools might be particularly useful to clarify mechanistic pathways in infancy.
Our data suggest that global efforts to improve air quality could have benefits to infants’ emerging cognitive abilities. This, in turn, could have a cascade of positive impacts, including positive impacts on families as well as economic consequences as improved cognition can lead to improved economic productivity longer-term and reduce the burden on healthcare and mental health systems (Moffitt et al., 2011). Our results showing links between indoor air quality and cooking materials also suggest that efforts to reduce cooking emissions in homes should be a key target for intervention. This requires both increased availability of clean technologies and uptake of such technologies in rural households where traditional methods of meal preparation might be a barrier (see Mukhopadhyay et al., 2012). Our findings can motivate both policymakers and families to improve air quality as this should positively boost the neurocognitive health of young infants.
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to the community of Shivgarh, especially the mothers and babies who participated. We thank the 1000 Dreams Team at CEL – Tarawati Chachi, Hariprasad, Matadeen, Ram Kishore, Mukesh, Arjun, Ashok Kumar Pandey, Shivendra, Raj Kishore, Sunil Shukla, Satya Prakash Singh, Vineeta Maurya, Babita Singh, Saroj, Ritu Singh, Pooja Verma, Beenu Pandey, Jitendra Patel, Amit Patel, Dileep Verma, Saurabh Verma, Ankit Bajpayee, Jyoti Singh, Jay Prakash, Shreya Singhal, Dr. Ravi Keshava Lele, Rajesh Kumar, and Abhishek Singh who was the Operations Lead for the study. We also thank Ranjit Kumar for his administrative and logistical support, and Mohammad Amir, Sudhanshu Srivastav, Deepak Sahu, Vishwadeep Mukherjee, and Amit Tandon for their technological support. Our gratitude to Dr. Vishwajeet Kumar and Mr. Rakesh Pratap for their support and guidance. Outdoor air quality data from Lucknow were downloaded from https://www.kaggle.com/rohanrao/air-quality-data-in-india. We thank Dan Pope, Elisa Puzzolo, and Prerna Aneja for useful discussions. Bill & Melinda Gates Foundation grant OPP1164153 (JPS).
Funding Statement
The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.
Contributor Information
John P Spencer, Email: j.spencer@uea.ac.uk.
Charlotte Cecil, Erasmus MC, Netherlands.
Chris I Baker, National Institute of Mental Health, United States.
Funding Information
This paper was supported by the following grant:
Bill and Melinda Gates Foundation OPP1164153 to John P Spencer.
Additional information
Competing interests
No competing interests declared.
No competing interests declared.
Sean Deoni has grants or contracts, received consulting fees from and has patents planned, issued or pending with Nestle Nutrition and has received Payment or honoraria for lectures, presentations, speakers bureaus, manuscript writing or educational events from Wyeth Nutrition and Mead Johnson Nutrition. The author has no other competing interests to declare.
Author contributions
Conceptualization, Resources, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Project administration, Writing – review and editing.
Resources, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Visualization, Writing – review and editing.
Data curation, Formal analysis.
Resources, Data curation, Supervision, Methodology, Project administration.
Data curation, Formal analysis.
Conceptualization, Data curation, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review and editing.
Data curation, Supervision, Methodology, Writing – review and editing.
Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review and editing.
Ethics
The study was approved by the Community Empowerment Lab Institutional Ethics Committee (Ref. No: CEL/2018005). Participants' caregivers provided written informed consent; where caregivers were illiterate, a witness gave signed consent accompanied by a thumb impression of the caregiver in place of a signature.
Additional files
Data availability
All data and code are available at https://osf.io/fspzb/.
The following dataset was generated:
Spencer JP, Forbes S. 2023. Air quality and visual working memory. Open Science Framework. fspzb
References
- Ahmed I, Ali SM, Amenga-Etego S, Ariff S, Bahl R, Baqui AH, Begum N, Bhandari N, Bhatia K, Bhutta ZA, Biemba G, Deb S, Dhingra U, Dube B, Dutta A, Edmond K, Esamai F, Fawzi W, Ghosh AK, Gisore P, Grogan C, Hamer DH, Herlihy J, Hurt L, Ilyas M, Jehan F, Kalonji M, Kaur J, Khanam R, Kirkwood B, Kumar A, Kumar A, Kumar V, Manu A, Marete I, Masanja H, Mazumder S, Mehmood U, Mishra S, Mitra DK, Mlay E, Mohan SB, Moin MI, Muhammad K, Muhihi A, Newton S, Ngaima S, Nguwo A, Nisar I, O’Leary M, Otomba J, Patil P, Quaiyum MA, Rahman MH, Sazawal S, Semrau KE, Shannon C, Smith ER, Soofi S, Soremekun S, Sunday V, Taneja S, Tshefu A, Wasan Y, Yeboah-Antwi K, Yoshida S, Zaidi A. Population-Based rates, timing, and causes of maternal deaths, stillbirths, and neonatal deaths in South Asia and sub-Saharan Africa: a multi-country prospective cohort study. The Lancet. Global Health. 2018;6:e1297–e1308. doi: 10.1016/S2214-109X(18)30385-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ben-Shachar M, Lüdecke D, Makowski D. Effectsize: estimation of effect size indices and standardized parameters. Journal of Open Source Software. 2020;5:2815. doi: 10.21105/joss.02815. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Block ML, Calderón-Garcidueñas L. Air pollution: mechanisms of neuroinflammation and CNS disease. Trends in Neurosciences. 2009;32:506–516. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.05.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bos I, De Boever P, Int Panis L, Sarre S, Meeusen R. Negative effects of ultrafine particle exposure during forced exercise on the expression of brain-derived neurotrophic factor in the hippocampus of rats. Neuroscience. 2012;223:131–139. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.07.057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Datavyu Datavyu: A video coding tool. Datavyu. 2014 http://datavyu.org
- Delgado Reyes L, Wijeakumar S, Magnotta VA, Forbes SH, Spencer JP. The functional brain networks that underlie visual working memory in the first two years of life. NeuroImage. 2020;219:116971. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116971. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dink J, Ferguson B. EyetrackingR. version 0.2.0R Package. 2021 http://www.eyetracking-r.com/
- Fernald A, Pinto JP, Swingley D, Weinberg A, McRoberts GW. Rapid gains in speed of verbal processing by infants in the 2nd year. Psychological Science. 1998;9:228–231. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00044. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Freire C, Ramos R, Puertas R, Lopez-Espinosa MJ, Julvez J, Aguilera I, Cruz F, Fernandez MF, Sunyer J, Olea N. Association of traffic-related air pollution with cognitive development in children. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2010;64:223–228. doi: 10.1136/jech.2008.084574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerlofs-Nijland ME, van Berlo D, Cassee FR, Schins RPF, Wang K, Campbell A. Effect of prolonged exposure to diesel engine exhaust on proinflammatory markers in different regions of the rat brain. Particle and Fibre Toxicology. 2010;7:1–10. doi: 10.1186/1743-8977-7-12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gonzalez-Casanova I, Stein AD, Barraza-Villarreal A, Feregrino RG, DiGirolamo A, Hernandez-Cadena L, Rivera JA, Romieu I, Ramakrishnan U. Prenatal exposure to environmental pollutants and child development trajectories through 7 years. International Journal of Hygiene and Environmental Health. 2018;221:616–622. doi: 10.1016/j.ijheh.2018.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grandjean P, Landrigan PJ. Neurobehavioural effects of developmental toxicity. The Lancet. Neurology. 2014;13:330–338. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(13)70278-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guxens M, Aguilera I, Ballester F, Estarlich M, Fernández-Somoano A, Lertxundi A, Lertxundi N, Mendez MA, Tardón A, Vrijheid M, Sunyer J, INMA Project Prenatal exposure to residential air pollution and infant mental development: modulation by antioxidants and detoxification factors. Environmental Health Perspectives. 2012;120:144–149. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1103469. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guxens M, Garcia-Esteban R, Giorgis-Allemand L, Forns J, Badaloni C, Ballester F, Beelen R, Cesaroni G, Chatzi L, de Agostini M, de Nazelle A, Eeftens M, Fernandez MF, Fernández-Somoano A, Forastiere F, Gehring U, Ghassabian A, Heude B, Jaddoe VWV, Klümper C, Kogevinas M, Krämer U, Larroque B, Lertxundi A, Lertxuni N, Murcia M, Navel V, Nieuwenhuijsen M, Porta D, Ramos R, Roumeliotaki T, Slama R, Sørensen M, Stephanou EG, Sugiri D, Tardón A, Tiemeier H, Tiesler CMT, Verhulst FC, Vrijkotte T, Wilhelm M, Brunekreef B, Pershagen G, Sunyer J. Air pollution during pregnancy and childhood cognitive and psychomotor development: six European birth cohorts. Epidemiology. 2014;25:636–647. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0000000000000133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris MH, Gold DR, Rifas-Shiman SL, Melly SJ, Zanobetti A, Coull BA, Schwartz JD, Gryparis A, Kloog I, Koutrakis P, Bellinger DC, White RF, Sagiv SK, Oken E. Prenatal and childhood traffic-related pollution exposure and childhood cognition in the project viva cohort (Massachusetts, USA) Environmental Health Perspectives. 2015;123:1072–1078. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1408803. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartig F. DHARMa: residual diagnostics for hierarchical (multi-level / mixed) regression models. R Packages. 2020 https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/DHARMa/vignettes/DHARMa.html
- Heckman JJ, Mosso S. The economics of human development and social mobility. Annual Review of Economics. 2014;6:689–733. doi: 10.1146/annurev-economics-080213-040753. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuznetsova A, Brockhoff PB, Christensen RHB. lmertest package: tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software. 2017;82:1–26. doi: 10.18637/jss.v082.i13. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Landry SH, Smith KE, Swank PR. Responsive parenting: establishing early foundations for social, communication, and independent problem-solving skills. Developmental Psychology. 2006;42:627–642. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.42.4.627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Midouhas E, Kokosi T, Flouri E. Outdoor and indoor air quality and cognitive ability in young children. Environmental Research. 2018;161:321–328. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2017.11.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Midouhas E, Kokosi T, Flouri E. The quality of air outside and inside the home: associations with emotional and behavioural problem scores in early childhood. BMC Public Health. 2019;19:406. doi: 10.1186/s12889-019-6733-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, Belsky D, Dickson N, Hancox RJ, Harrington H, Houts R, Poulton R, Roberts BW, Ross S, Sears MR, Thomson WM, Caspi A. A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. PNAS. 2011;108:2693–2698. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010076108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mohd Saleem S. Modified kuppuswamy socioeconomic scale updated for the year 2020. Indian Journal of Forensic and Community Medicine. 2020;7:1–3. doi: 10.18231/j.ijfcm.2020.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Molenar JV. Comparison of PM2.5 Measurements Using the AirVisual Monitor and Beta Attenuation Monitor. 2016. [May 10, 2017]. https://www.airvisual.com/air-pollution-information/research/comparison-of-pm2-5-measurements-using-the-airvisual-monitor-and-beta-attenuation-monitor)
- Mukhopadhyay R, Sambandam S, Pillarisetti A, Jack D, Mukhopadhyay K, Balakrishnan K, Vaswani M, Bates MN, Kinney PL, Arora N, Smith KR. Cooking practices, air quality, and the acceptability of advanced cookstoves in Haryana, India: an exploratory study to inform large-scale interventions. Global Health Action. 2012;5:1–13. doi: 10.3402/gha.v5i0.19016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oakes LM, Ross-Sheehy S, Luck SJ. Rapid development of feature binding in visual short-term memory. Psychological Science. 2006;17:781–787. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01782.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oakes LM, Messenger IM, Ross-Sheehy S, Luck SJ. New evidence for rapid development of color-location binding in infants’ visual short-term memory. Visual Cognition. 2009;17:67–82. doi: 10.1080/13506280802151480. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oakes LM, Hurley KB, Ross-Sheehy S, Luck SJ. Developmental changes in infants’ visual short-term memory for location. Cognition. 2011;118:293–305. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.11.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pandey A, Brauer M, Cropper ML, Balakrishnan K, Mathur P, Dey S, Turkgulu B, Kumar GA, Khare M, Beig G, Gupta T, Krishnankutty RP, Causey K, Cohen AJ, Bhargava S, Aggarwal AN, Agrawal A, Awasthi S, Bennitt F, Bhagwat S, Bhanumati P, Burkart K, Chakma JK, Chiles TC, Chowdhury S, Christopher DJ, Dey S, Fisher S, Fraumeni B, Fuller R, Ghoshal AG, Golechha MJ, Gupta PC, Gupta R, Gupta R, Gupta S, Guttikunda S, Hanrahan D, Harikrishnan S, Jeemon P, Joshi TK, Kant R, Kant S, Kaur T, Koul PA, Kumar P, Kumar R, Larson SL, Lodha R, Madhipatla KK, Mahesh PA, Malhotra R, Managi S, Martin K, Mathai M, Mathew JL, Mehrotra R, Mohan BVM, Mohan V, Mukhopadhyay S, Mutreja P, Naik N, Nair S, Pandian JD, Pant P, Perianayagam A, Prabhakaran D, Prabhakaran P, Rath GK, Ravi S, Roy A, Sabde YD, Salvi S, Sambandam S, Sharma B, Sharma M, Sharma S, Sharma RS, Shrivastava A, Singh S, Singh V, Smith R. Health and economic impact of air pollution in the states of India: the global burden of disease study 2019. The Lancet. Planetary Health. 2021;5:e25–e38. doi: 10.1016/S2542-5196(20)30298-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patten KT, González EA, Valenzuela A, Berg E, Wallis C, Garbow JR, Silverman JL, Bein KJ, Wexler AS, Lein PJ. Effects of early life exposure to traffic-related air pollution on brain development in juvenile Sprague-Dawley rats. Translational Psychiatry. 2020;10:166. doi: 10.1038/s41398-020-0845-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perone S, Simmering VR, Spencer JP. Stronger neural dynamics capture changes in infants’ visual working memory capacity over development. Developmental Science. 2011;14:1379–1392. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01083.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perone S, Spencer JP. Autonomous visual exploration creates developmental change in familiarity and novelty seeking behaviors. Frontiers in Psychology. 2013;4:648. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rohra H, Taneja A. Indoor air quality scenario in india—an outline of household fuel combustion. Atmospheric Environment. 2016;129:243–255. doi: 10.1016/j.atmosenv.2016.01.038. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rose SA. Relation between physical growth and information processing in infants born in India. Child Development. 1994;65:889–902. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rose SA, Feldman JF, Jankowski JJ. Implications of infant cognition for executive functions at age 11. Psychological Science. 2012;23:1345–1355. doi: 10.1177/0956797612444902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ross-Sheehy S, Oakes LM, Luck SJ. The development of visual short-term memory capacity in infants. Child Development. 2003;74:1807–1822. doi: 10.1046/j.1467-8624.2003.00639.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saffran JR, Aslin RN, Newport EL. Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science. 1996;274:1926–1928. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5294.1926. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suglia SF, Gryparis A, Wright RO, Schwartz J, Wright RJ. Association of black carbon with cognition among children in a prospective birth cohort study. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2008;167:280–286. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sunyer J, Esnaola M, Alvarez-Pedrerol M, Forns J, Rivas I, López-Vicente M, Suades-González E, Foraster M, Garcia-Esteban R, Basagaña X, Viana M, Cirach M, Moreno T, Alastuey A, Sebastian-Galles N, Nieuwenhuijsen M, Querol X. Association between traffic-related air pollution in schools and cognitive development in primary school children: a prospective cohort study. PLOS Medicine. 2015;12:e1001792. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001792. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Rij J, Wieling M, Baayen R, van Rijn H. Itsadug: interpreting time series and autocorrelated data using gamms. version 2.4.1R Package. 2020 https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/itsadug/citation.html
- Vrijheid M, Martinez D, Aguilera I, Bustamante M, Ballester F, Estarlich M, Fernandez-Somoano A, Guxens M, Lertxundi N, Martinez MD, Tardon A, Sunyer J, INMA Project Indoor air pollution from gas cooking and infant neurodevelopment. Epidemiology. 2012;23:23–32. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0b013e31823a4023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang S, Zhang J, Zeng X, Zeng Y, Wang S, Chen S. Association of traffic-related air pollution with children’s neurobehavioral functions in quanzhou, China. Environmental Health Perspectives. 2009;117:1612–1618. doi: 10.1289/ehp.0800023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wijeakumar S, Kumar A, Delgado Reyes LM, Tiwari M, Spencer JP. Early adversity in rural India impacts the brain networks underlying visual working memory. Developmental Science. 2019;22:e12822. doi: 10.1111/desc.12822. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wood SN. Fast stable restricted maximum likelihood and marginal likelihood estimation of semiparametric generalized linear models. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B. 2011;73:3–36. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9868.2010.00749.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang H, Srinivasan R. A systematic review of air quality sensors, guidelines, and measurement studies for indoor air quality management. Sustainability. 2020;12:9045. doi: 10.3390/su12219045. [DOI] [Google Scholar]