Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2023 Apr 30;52(7):1357–1373. doi: 10.1007/s10964-023-01780-z

A Few Close Friends? Adolescent Friendships’ Effect on Internalizing Symptoms Is Serially Mediated by Desire for More Friends and Social Goal Orientation

Reubs J Walsh 1,2,3,, Nikki C Lee 1,2,3,4, Imke L J Lemmers-Jansen 1,2,3,5, Miriam Hollarek 1,2,3, Hester Sijtsma 1,2,3, Mariët van Buuren 1,2,3, Lydia Krabbendam 1,2,3
PMCID: PMC10175381  PMID: 37120791

Abstract

Interpersonal connection is a fundamental human motivation, and the extent to which it is fulfilled is a strong predictor of symptoms of internalizing disorders such as social anxiety and depression, perhaps especially during the “social reorienting” period of adolescence. However, little is known about the contribution to this effect of the individual’s social motivations, which are intensified during adolescence. Furthermore, social goal orientation – an individual’s priorities and intentions in social interactions – is an important predictor of vulnerability to internalizing symptoms. Adolescents spend most of their waking lives in classrooms, bounded social networks with a limited pool of candidates for befriending. This study investigated whether friendships within one’s class protects against internalizing symptoms in part by reducing the desire for more classmate friendships, which may tend to promote maladaptive social goals. Participants were 423 young adolescents (M age = 13.2, sd = 0.52 years; 49.4% girls). As predicted, adolescents’ number of reciprocated classroom friendships had a protective effect on internalizing symptoms which was serially mediated by desire for more such friendships, and social goal orientation. However, only demonstration-avoidance goals significantly predicted internalizing symptoms. Unreciprocated friendship nominations were unexpectedly associated with stronger desire and more social anxiety symptoms. The results suggest that the effect of number of friends is mediated by the individual’s thoughts and feelings about their number of friendships, such that a strong desire for more friendships promotes maladaptive goals, oriented toward social status and consequently less oriented toward the cultivation of interpersonal intimacy with the friends they already have.

Keywords: Early adolescence, Social cognition, Classmate friendships, Social goals, Motivation, Adjustment

Introduction

Adolescence is a sensitive window for the development of the motivation and capacity to navigate social interactions outside of the family. As children enter adolescence, desire to feel included in social groups intensifies, and becomes increasingly oriented towards peer friendships in preference to family. Adolescents’ reward systems are highly attuned to social reward, while their executive functions and social cognition continue gradually to mature (Crone & Dahl, 2012). These changes help motivate and equip the adolescent to complete the developmental tasks of adolescence, such as learning to establish and maintain interdependent peer relationships; becoming independent from family; the construction of a stable and autonomous social identity; and navigating varied social norms and expectations to manage others’ perception of them (Manning, 2002). These developmental tasks are social in nature and therefore depend upon the impact of social experiences on the adolescent’s cognitions and motivations. However, these effects are not always adaptive, and social experiences during this sensitive period dramatically influence individual vulnerability to internalizing symptoms (Rapee et al., 2019), into adulthood (Narr et al., 2019). Indeed, the social support of friendships is a powerful determinant of mental and physical health across the lifespan (Holt-Lunstad et al., 2015), but may be especially important in adolescence (Sebastian et al., 2010), both to facilitate psychosocial development, and because adolescents rejected by their peers may nevertheless withdraw from familial social support, leading to social isolation (Thomas & Bowker, 2015). However, it remains unknown how and how much adolescents’ goals and desires about their social interactions and circumstances contribute to the effect of friendships on internalizing. Additionally, for most adolescents, the majority of their social interactions will take place within the school environment. Many schools are highly structured, constraining which peers an individual may interact with, especially when individuals are assigned to a single class for most or all lessons. This amplifies the importance of classroom friendships to the adolescent. Despite this, the role of the motivations guiding an adolescent’s social cognition and behavior in the association between their classroom friendships and their risk of developing internalizing symptoms, is not yet well understood.

Although many aspects of adolescent friendship experiences have been studied as predictors of mental health outcomes, relatively little is known about the role of social motivation in these associations. Existing evidence suggests that adolescents’ social goals during this sensitive period markedly influence their vulnerability to internalizing symptoms (Kuroda & Sakurai, 2011; Ryan & Shim, 2008). It has been suggested that affect-laden, psychological needs-driven motives or desires regulate the selection and prioritization of goals to pursue (Thrash & Elliot, 2005). Therefore, desire for more friends may influence social goals such as acquiring social skills or status. If so, the effect of number of friendships on internalizing symptoms may arise partly or only because it predicts the extent to which a psychological need for peer-affiliation is satisfied, and so the strength of the desire for more friends. This may in turn drive social goal-pursuit behaviors and cognitions toward core features of internalizing, such as social withdrawal and/or hypervigilance to negative evaluation. Nevertheless, it remains unknown whether and how classroom friendships influence these goals, nor whether such influence contributes to the effect of friendships on the risk of internalizing symptoms.

Adolescent Friendships and Internalizing Symptoms

The number of friends an adolescent has is a particularly powerful predictor of internalizing risk. Adolescents with no friends in their class have high rates of internalizing symptoms and tend to perceive school as socially threatening (Lessard & Juvonen, 2018), and for most adolescents, more reciprocated friendships is associated with fewer internalizing symptoms (Ueno, 2005). However, the mechanism by which a higher number of friends reduces the emergence of internalizing symptoms is unlikely to be (only) a direct effect, and indeed there is substantial evidence that the effect is partially mediated by a variety of constructs, including friendship quality, loneliness, and belonging (Lodder et al., 2017). Moreover, the effect is nonlinear, so that for adolescents with the very highest numbers of friendship ties, additional friendships do not reliably provide additional benefit; on the contrary, such adolescents tend to be more depressed (Falci & McNeely, 2009; c.f. Pachucki et al., 2015) and less socially content (Ferguson & Ryan, 2019) than those with a more moderate number. Nevertheless, the effect of adolescent friendship on internalizing symptoms has many possible mechanisms including those arising from the qualitative attributes of the friendships themselves, (Waldrip et al., 2008) and others arising from friends’ tendency to become more similar over time (Veed et al., 2019), for example.

Number of friends is especially important in the context of the “bounded community” of a classroom. While in unstructured settings, new candidates for friendship may be encountered relatively frequently, in the classroom network, the set of candidates is relatively fixed across time, and it is more difficult to avoid contact with particular individuals. Consequently, adolescents in such classes necessarily have a relationship with all (or most) of their classmates, including those who are not their friend, and it is in the context of this extant non-friend relationship that attempts to initiate friendship would take place. Under these circumstances, the desire for more friends may make non-friend relationships, and, by extension, absolute number of friends, more salient. Meanwhile, perceptions of one’s social competence and the social support available may be calibrated by these classroom experiences. Quantity of reciprocated friendships in the classroom is therefore an important predictor of adolescents’ vulnerability to internalizing symptoms, and its drivers are not necessarily limited to effects occurring at the dyadic level, such as social support, but also include effects at the intra-individual and whole-group (classroom) levels.

Social Goals

Goal orientation (Dweck & Leggett, 1988) was originally developed to study differences in academic motivation and achievement (Elliott & Dweck, 1988), and has since been extended to the social domain (Ryan & Shim, 2006). In both cases, a distinction is drawn between an intrinsic desire to learn (academic “mastery” or social “development” goals) and the extrinsic desire to seem competent (“performance” or “demonstration” goals). Subsequently, demonstration (or performance) goals have been further divided into avoidant and approach dimensions, revealing that the association of demonstration goals with worse outcomes is driven primarily by the avoidant dimension, while demonstration-approach goals may even be beneficial in some cases, in both academic (Elliot & Harackiewicz, 1996) and social (Ryan & Shim, 2006, 2008) domains. However, these beneficial effects are conditional on development (mastery) goals also being endorsed, and on sociodemographic variables (Midgley et al., 2001). This highlights the fact that, at least at the level of goals, these factors are orthogonal, rather than opposing ends of a spectrum or discrete, mutually-exclusive states (Skaalvik, 1997).

Social Goal Orientation (SGO) can be measured with the Social Goals questionnaire (SGQ; Ryan & Shim, 2006, 2008), which asks adolescents about their priorities and intentions in their social interactions and has a three-factor structure. These factors measure (1) intrinsic goals seeking to learn new social skills and deepen existing friendships (“Development”); (2) extrinsic goals seeking to demonstrate social competence and/or (gain) status (“Demonstration-Approach”); and (3) extrinsic goals seeking to avoid demonstrating a lack of social competence and/or (losing) social status (“Demonstration-Avoidance”). The three SGQ subscales correlate differently with psychosocial adjustment variables. Development goals are associated with better adjustment across a range of variables including friendship satisfaction and loneliness (Liem, 2016), and self-acceptance and social worry (Shim et al., 2013). Demonstration-avoidance goals are associated with worse adjustment across variables including loneliness (Mouratidis & Sideridis, 2009), social worry (Shim et al., 2013), depression (Kuroda & Sakurai, 2011), and social anxiety (Shim & Ryan, 2012). Findings regarding demonstration-approach goals vary, similarly to academic performance-approach goals. While some studies found no association with social adjustment variables when the other two SGQ subscales were included in the model (e.g., Ryan & Shim, 2006, 2008), others found that demonstration-approach goals predicted greater belonging, but also greater loneliness, and feeling less accepted by peers (Mouratidis & Sideridis, 2009). The SGQ subscales are often related to closeness-seeking (development subscale), status-seeking (demonstration-approach) and status-preserving (demonstration-avoidance) (e.g., Rodkin et al., 2013; Rudolph et al., 2011; Ryan & Shim, 2008). In light of the associations of SGQ subscales with internalizing symptoms, this characterization is consistent with evidence that adolescents who achieve a status-oriented goal of greater popularity are at greater risk of internalizing symptoms, and that adolescents who achieve a closeness-oriented goal of increased friendship intimacy are at lower risk (Narr et al., 2019).

A previous study found that young adolescent girls with an unfulfilled desire to become friends with one or more peers were lonelier than those who did not, even after controlling for other social network measures (Thomas & Bowker, 2013). This suggests that adolescents’ experience of their social network position can be shaped by their desire to maintain or change that position. Nevertheless, it remains largely unknown what role inter-individual differences in adolescents’ desire to increase their number of classroom friendships plays in these social influences on internalizing symptoms (Rueger et al., 2016). On this basis, one may hypothesize that adolescents with fewer friends in their class are more vulnerable to internalizing symptoms in part because, within the bounded community of the classroom, a desire for more friendships may promote demonstration-oriented social (status) goals. Moreover, acquiring more friends in one’s class requires attending to relationships within the class that are not (yet) friendships. In this context, adolescents’ attention to non-friends may increase their use of metaperception (i.e., perceiving how one is perceived by others) in guiding behavior, as they seek to influence how their peers perceive them.

While adolescents with fewer friends will tend to have a stronger desire for more, an adolescent who strongly desires more friendships would be more vulnerable to internalizing symptoms than an adolescent with the same number of friends in their class, but a milder desire to increase that number. Such a desire could amplify the salience of non-friend relationships, motivating attempts to identify and conceal or alter (likely benign) behaviors or traits (Simone et al., 2018) that the adolescent perceives as hindering them in seeking friendship with those peers – consistent with demonstration-avoidance goals. Meanwhile, an adolescent with a less strong desire for more friends in their class would be less vulnerable to this mechanism, even if they have relatively few classroom friendships. Note, though that they would remain vulnerable to other mechanisms, such as a more limited pool of social support available to draw upon (Aune et al., 2020), amplified effects of conflict in those friendships they do have (Boersma-van Dam et al., 2019), and a diminished sense of belonging (Ueno, 2005).

If this is the case, it may be possible to intervene to reduce this contribution to adolescents’ internalizing risk. A simple, one-off, 30-minute psychoeducational intervention has successfully been used to shift adolescents’ beliefs about personality toward those (personalities can change, i.e., incremental, not entity, beliefs) which promote adaptive goals associated with less risk of internalizing – this intervention improved recovery after a social stressor (Schleider & Weisz, 2016). This has been successfully extended to beliefs about intelligence and self-control (Schleider et al., 2019), and to safeguard against potential side-effects, such as increased self-blame (Perkins et al. (2021)). A further extension of this intervention, also targeting beliefs about social competence and peer relationships (Rudolph, 2010) thought to shape SGO, could further reduce internalizing risk in adolescents (especially those with few friends) by guiding them toward seeking to develop their friendships and social skills by pursuing closeness, instead of seeking to express, obtain and retain social status. This kind of intervention could furthermore produce cultural change within schools. However, there may be changes to the structure of individual schools or whole education systems, that could establish and maintain a culture of social inclusion, in which benign behaviors and traits need not be concealed, non-friend relationships are more amicable, with friendship being defined more by closeness than by alliances and cliques, and peer victimization and exclusion behaviors less routinely rewarded with gains in social status (consider, e.g., Juvonen et al., 2019; Razer et al., 2013).

The Present Study

The number of friends an adolescent has in their school environment is a strong predictor of their risk of developing internalizing symptoms and disorders (Sebastian et al., 2010), and some recent evidence suggests that SGO may also play an important role (e.g., Rodkin et al., 2013). How and to what extent these effects are connected remains unknown, but the theoretical considerations outlined above suggest a desire to increase one’s number of friends would mediate an association between the current number of friends, and SGO. This study sought to shed light on this question by investigating the contribution of desire for more friends in class and social goals to the relationship between adolescents’ number of classroom friendships and internalizing symptoms, focusing on interactions within the internal states of the adolescent, and in particular, their desire to increase their number of friends in their class. Thus, an effect of a comparatively “objective” measure of the school social environment an adolescent experiences (number of friends), on internal motivational states, is hypothesized to depend upon the adolescent’s (subjectively experienced and reported) affective and motivational response to that “objective” number (desire to increase number of friends in their class). To test whether the negative association between number of classroom friendships and internalizing symptoms is partially mediated, in two steps, by the desire to increase one’s number of classroom friendships, and SGO, in a nonclinical community sample of Dutch adolescents in their first and second year of secondary education. The path model arises from two hypotheses. Firstly, that the stronger an adolescent’s desire for more classroom friendships, the more that adolescent would adopt social goals and behaviors that undermine the protective effect of their extant friendships. If this is the case, the intensity of an adolescent’s desire for more classroom friendships should mediate the effect of number of friendships on internalizing symptoms. Secondly, that social goals would be intensified by a strong desire for more friends, and mediate an association between desire and internalizing symptoms by prioritizing either status-seeking (demonstration goals) or closeness-seeking (development goals) at the expense of the other, leading to more internalizing symptoms in demonstration-oriented adolescents. These hypotheses lead to six predictions. First, number of reciprocated and unreciprocated classmate friendships will each be negatively associated with internalizing symptoms, and this effect will be greater for reciprocated friendships. Second, number of reciprocated and, to a lesser extent, unreciprocated classmate friendships will also each be negatively associated with desire for more classmate friendships. Third, desire will mediate the effect of number of friendships on internalizing symptoms and on SGO. Fourth, desire will be positively associated with both demonstration, and to a lesser extent, development SGQ subscales. Fifth, the effect of number of friendships on internalizing symptoms will be serially mediated by desire and SGO. Finally, the indirect effects of number of friendships on internalizing symptoms mediated by demonstration goals will be negative (i.e., more friends leads to less desire, leads to less demonstration goals, leads to less internalizing symptoms), whereas the indirect effect mediated by development goals will be net-positive due to inconsistent mediation (i.e. fewer friends leads to more desire, leads to more development goals, but more development goals leads to less internalizing symptoms, when controlling for the negative direct effect, and the negative indirect effect through desire, of number of friends).

Methods

Participants

Participants were early adolescents enrolled in the first or second year of high-school (M age = 13.2 years, sd = 0.52 years, range: 11.7–14.7 years) in the Netherlands. Participants were recruited through schools participating in the #SOCONNeCT project, in two longitudinal cohorts (see also e.g., Sijtsma et al., 2021). These participants were drawn from academically selective mainstream secondary schools; such schools comprise a total of ~40% of pupils nationally.

A total of 948 adolescents were recruited to the project, 728 of whom had completed, in a single session, all the measures used in the present analyses during their first or second year of high-school. Peer nominations were used to ascertain participants’ number of friends in their class. Incomplete data introduce noise because the participant cannot nominate or be nominated by non-participants in the peer-nomination questionnaire. Therefore, participants from classes with <70% participation were excluded from the current analyses. This threshold was selected because simulation studies (e.g., Smith & Moody, 2013) indicate that 70% participation is adequate for a reliable estimate of the true value of the sociometric variable. Participants in classes excluded due to low classmate participation (N = 305 in 29 classes) did not significantly differ from the included sample on the questionnaire variables used (Beck Youth Inventory Depression Scale (BYI-D; t = −1.624, p = 0.105, d = 0.132), Social Anxiety Scale for Adolescents (SAS-A; t = −0.949, p = 0.343, d = 0.075), SGQ Development (t = 0.550, p = 0.582, d = 0.044), SGQ Demonstration-Avoidance (t = 0.757, p = 0.450, d = 0.059), SGQ Demonstration-Approach (t = 1.599, p = 0.110, d = 0.122)).

A total of 423 participants (across 19 classes with a mean participation rate of 83.3%), 209 of whom were girls (49.4%), were included. Age did not differ by reported sex (t = −0.266, p = 0.791, d = 0.042).

Measures

The Cronbach’s alpha values reported in this section were calculated for the present sample. The internalizing symptoms studied as outcome measures were those associated with social anxiety and depression, as these are closely associated with both inter- and intrapersonal social risk factors (such as low peer acceptance and negative self-image, respectively), and are relatively common in adolescent samples (Rapee et al., 2019).

Peer nominations

In response to nine typical sociometric nomination questions (Cillessen & Marks, 2017), participants nominated classmates by selecting their names from a list, presented in a grid on a tablet screen. The list was composed of all the names of participating classmates, and excluded those who did not consent to participate in the study. For the planned analyses, the responses to the “Who on this list are your friends (maximum 15)?” item were used; this was the only item for which the number of nominations was limited. The nomination lists include only participating classmates, so participation rates below 100% do not affect the ability to determine which friendship nominations are reciprocated, as participants were only able to nominate classmates who could potentially return the nomination. This allows one to distinguish a more “objective” measure of the participant’s friendships from a more “subjective” measure that reflects the participant’s perception of their friendships. References to “reciprocal” friendships will hereafter be used to mean (the number of) friendships that were reported by both the participant and the friend, and “unreciprocated” friendships, will be used to mean (the number of) friendship nominations by the participant where the nominated friend did not nominate the participant. The items “With whom on this list do you talk about your feelings?”, “Who on this list is popular?”, and “Who on this list is likable?” were used in the post-hoc analyses.

Social network perception

Participants were also asked to report their perception of their network and, in particular, to rate statements that they desired to 1. have more friends in their class, 2. be more popular with classmates, and 3. be liked more by classmates. Responses to each question were on a five-point scale between “not at all” and “very much so”. The response to the first item is referred to hereafter as “desire” (i.e., desire to have more friends). Due to the single-item nature of each of these measures, the resultant variables are treated as ordinal.

Social goals questionnaire (SGQ)

Ryan and Shim (2006) developed and validated a three-factor social achievement goals questionnaire in college students, which assesses the participant’s approach to social interactions based on an earlier study (Erdley et al., 1997). This questionnaire was adapted for use with children (Rudolph et al., 2011). A Dutch-language version of this questionnaire was administered to participants after in-house translation. Items with a factor-loading greater than 0.50 were translated by a natively bilingual speaker of Dutch and English and back-translated into English to confirm the accuracy of the translation. The three-factor structure comprises development (6 items, α = 0.86), demonstration-approach (5 items, α = 0.86) and demonstration-avoidance (7 items, α = 0.84) subscales. Responses are on a five-point scale between “never” (1) and “always” (5). Subscale scores were calculated as the mean of the responses on the items in that subscale, with higher scores indicating greater endorsement of the corresponding goal-orientation factor.

Social anxiety scale for adolescents (SAS-A)

This questionnaire evaluates the extent of social anxiety symptomatology in adolescents (for validation see; Inderbitzen-Nolan & Walters, 2010; La Greca & Lopez, 1998). A Dutch-language translation of SAS-A with consistently high Cronbach’s alpha was used (Blöte, et al., 2010; Blöte & Westenberg, 2007; Miers, et al., 2013). It is comprised of three factors: fear of negative evaluation (8 items, α = 0.92), novelty-related social avoidance and distress (6 items, α = 0.84) and general social avoidance and distress (4 items, α = 0.78). Responses to each item were on a five-point scale between “not at all” and “very much so”. Scores were calculated as the unweighted sum of the responses on all 18 items (α = 0.93), with higher scores indicating that the participant reports experiencing more social anxiety symptoms.

Beck youth inventory depression scale (BDI-Y)

The BDI-Y evaluates the extent of depressive symptomatology in children and adolescents aged 7–18 and has 20 items in a single factor (α = 0.93). A validated Dutch translation of this validated questionnaire was used (Beck et al., 2005). Responses are on a four-point scale between “never” and “always”. Scores were calculated as the unweighted sum of the responses on all 20 items, with higher scores indicating that the participant reports experiencing more depression symptoms.

Procedure

Both the participant and their parent(s) or guardian(s) provided written informed consent to participate in the #SOCONNeCT project following a letter and an information evening to inform them of the protocol and aims of the study and their rights as participants. Participation took place under exam conditions in the classroom setting, supervised by members of the research team, and lasted approximately 90 min (plus breaks) including explanation and administration of all behavioral measures for the wave, including several which are not used in the present analysis. Throughout data collection participants were able to ask questions to the researchers. Each participant completed the questionnaires individually on an iPad provided by the researchers. All materials and the means of administration (i.e., iPads) were tested and validated in focus groups, which were also used to ensure that the information provided was sufficient and to compile prepared answers to frequent queries. Payment of €7.50 per participant was made to the school to be spent on a class activity. The #SOCONNeCT project was approved by the Scientific and Ethical Review Board of the Faculty of Behavioral and Movement Sciences of the Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam.

Data Preprocessing

Missing data were handled by listwise deletion; peer nomination data from classes with below 70% participation were considered unreliable, and therefore treated as missing (i.e., all data from such classes were excluded from the analyses). Peer nomination data were corrected for effects of class size and participation rate using a method adapted from a previous study (Velásquez et al., 2013). This method mitigates the effect of the missing peer nomination data that results from less than 100% participation within a given class. Questionnaire and peer nomination data were then normalized and centered. Please see the supplementary materials for more details on each of these steps.

Planned Path Analyses

Planned analyses for the present study consisted of two unnested path models to separately examine effects of reciprocated friendships (model 1A) and unreciprocated friendships (model 1B), as shown in Fig. 1. This analysis used R (R Core Team, 2020) and the lavaan package (Rosseel, 2012) in RStudio (RStudio Team (2020)). Means, errors and variances were freely estimated, as were the covariances of the three SGQ subscales, and the covariance of the SAS-A (Social Anxiety) and BDI-Y (Depression).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Models for (a) reciprocated and (b) unreciprocated friendships. Straight lines are freely-estimated regressions, curved lines are freely-estimated covariances. Regressions with an estimate <1 are bold lines, and those which failed to reach significance (i.e., p < 0.05) are dashed lines

The following model-test statistics were used: Comparative Fit Index (CFI): values >0.94 indicate good model fit; Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA): values <0.08 indicate acceptable, and <0.06 good fit of the residuals (Hu & Bentler, 1999). Throughout, RMSEA values are accompanied by a p-value representing the probability that the true value of RMSEA lies below 0.05. Values above p = 0.05 are typically interpreted as indicating that the RMSEA distribution does not recommend model rejection. Due to the use of a robust DWLS (diagonally-weighted least squares) estimation, information criteria cannot be calculated. In all cases, the DWLS-robust estimate of the fit statistic is reported. These planned analyses were followed up with univariate regressions examining the estimated regression paths individually; a linear regression examining the association of demonstration goals with desires to be popular and to be liked, as measures of participants’ desire for social status; and linear regressions examining the associations of desire for more friends, depression, and social anxiety (as the dependent variables) with numbers of reciprocated and unreciprocated friendship ties.

Results

Friendship Effects’ Path to Psychopathology

Planned models

Descriptive statistics and correlations of all path-model variables are given in Table 1. Both models converged normally, and are depicted in Fig. 1. Table 2 shows the parameter estimates for these models. Table 3 gives Sobel estimates of the total effect, indirect effects, and the direct effect, of number of (un/reciprocated) friendship nominations on social anxiety symptoms and on depressive symptoms, including the ratio of each (in)direct effect to the total effect. Model 1A (in which the corrected estimate of number of reciprocated friendship nominations was the “friends” variable), performed well (CFI = 0.993, RMSEA = 0.059 (0.314)). Model 1B (in which the corrected estimate of number of unreciprocated friendship nominations was the “Friends” variable) performed less well (CFI = 0.987, RMSEA = 0.084 (0.130)). Non-nested models cannot be formally compared. In both cases, the effect of friends on depression and social anxiety was mediated in two steps; friends predicted desire, which predicted SGQ subscale scores, one of which (demonstration-avoidance) significantly predicted social anxiety and depression symptoms. This effect amounted to between ~18 and 53% of the total effect of number of (un/reciprocated) friendship nominations on social anxiety and depression symptoms. All other indirect effects were nonsignificant. The total effects of reciprocated friendships on social anxiety and depression symptoms were significant, as was the total effect of unreciprocated friendships on social anxiety symptoms. However, the total effect of unreciprocated friendship nominations on depression symptoms was nonsignificant. Moreover, the direct effects of reciprocated friendship nominations on depression symptoms, and of unreciprocated friendship nominations on social anxiety symptoms and on depression symptoms, were all nonsignificant, while the direct effect of reciprocated friendship nominations on social anxiety symptoms was significant, amounting to ~61% of the total effect.

Table 1.

Means, standard deviations, and correlations for all model variables

Mean SD 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. 7. 8.
1. Desire 2.118 1.162 3.622*** 8.427*** 3.592*** 8.296*** 7.134*** −6.662*** 3.866***
2. Depression Symptoms 7.953 7.956 3.860*** 11.911*** 1.658 4.630*** 2.859** −2.307* 1.385
3. Social Anxiety Symptoms 38.612 11.81 8.438*** 12.327*** 4.124*** 14.759*** 6.523*** −4.999*** 2.341*
4. Development 3.148 0.850 3.701*** 2.247* 4.803*** 6.747*** 3.179** −0.149 2.718**
5. Demonstration-Avoidance 2.990 0.810 8.033*** 4.947*** 15.154*** 7.000*** 9.162*** −3.977*** 1.373
6. Demonstration-Approach 2.194 0.809 7.167*** 3.133** 7.207*** 3.635*** 9.420*** −0.651 2.779**
7. Reciprocated Friendships 4.111 2.201 −7.194*** −2.202* −5.356*** −0.074 −3.945*** −0.721 1.468
8. Unreciprocated Friendships 2.466 2.196 3.859*** 1.643 2.114* 2.722** 1.335 2.717** 1.462

Correlations between the untransformed variables are given above the diagonal, and their correlations after the square-root transformation are given below the diagonal

*p ≤ 0.05; **p ≤ 0.01; ***p ≤ 0.001

Table 2.

Regression weights and covariance estimates per path for Models 1A & B

Path Model 1A Model 1 B
Estimate (SE) CSS Estimate (SE) Estimate (SE) CSS Estimate (SE)
Friends Desire −0.581 (0.069)*** −0.348 (0.042)*** 0.358 (0.084)*** 0.198 (0.047)***
Friends Social Anxiety −0.240 (0.074)** −0.156 (0.049)** −0.049 (0.077) −0.027 (0.042)
Friends Depression −0.138 (0.119) −0.057 (0.049) 0.046 (0.129) 0.017 (0.048)
Desire Social Anxiety 0.087 (0.052) 0.094 (0.055) 0.360 (0.202) 0.358 (0.179)*
Desire Depression 0.101 (0.090) 0.070 (0.062) 0.318 (0.204) 0.215 (0.136)
Desire Development 0.037 (0.018)* 0.149 (0.070)* 0.088 (0.030)** 0.344 (0.109)**
Desire Demonstration-Approach 0.073 (0.025)** 0.267 (0.089)** 0.159 (0.050)** 0.547 (0.146)***
Desire Demonstration-Avoidance 0.082 (0.027)** 0.339 (0.103)*** 0.152 (0.048)** 0.581 (0.153)***
Demonstration-Approach Social Anxiety 0.250 (0.133) 0.074 (0.039) 0.029 (0.264) 0.008 (0.076)
Demonstration-Approach Depression 0.245 (0.263) 0.046 (0.049) 0.046 (0.351) 0.009 (0.069)
Demonstration-Avoidance Social Anxiety 1.889 (0.161)*** 0.494 (0.039)*** 1.685 (0.323)*** 0.439 (0.086)***
Demonstration-Avoidance Depression 1.001 (0.320)** 0.167 (0.053)** 0.821 (0.394)* 0.145 (0.067)*
Development Social Anxiety 0.109 (0.151) 0.029 (0.041) −0.002 (0.188) 0.000 (0.048)
Development Depression 0.174 (0.298) 0.030 (0.052) 0.073 (0.323) 0.013 (0.056)
Development Demonstration-Approach 0.009 (0.003)* 0.132 (0.051)** 0.003 (0.005) 0.053 (0.087)
Development Demonstration-Avoid 0.016 (0.003)*** 0.284 (0.046)*** 0.011 (0.005)* 0.221 (0.074)**
Demonstration -Approach Demonstration-Avoid 0.020 (0.004)*** 0.344 (0.048)*** 0.013 (0.008) 0.246 (0.119)*
Depression Social Anxiety 0.472 (0.053)*** 0.469 (0.036)*** 0.452 (0.067)*** 0.459 (0.044)***

→ represents regressions and ↔ represents covariances

SE standard error; CSS completely standardized solution

*p ≤ 0.05; **p ≤ 0.01; ***p ≤ 0.001

Table 3.

Indirect, direct and total effect estimates (sobel) of friends on internalizing for Models 1A & B

DV Effect (Mediators) Model 1A Model 1B
Estimate (SE) CSS Estimate (SE) Ratio Estimate (SE) CSS Estimate (SE) Ratio
Social Anxiety Indirect (Desire) −0.050 (0.031) −0.033 (0.020) 0.128 0.129 (0.071) 0.071 (0.035)* 0.745
Indirect (Desire, Demonstration-Avoidance) −0.090 (0.032)** −0.058 (0.020)** 0.228 0.092 (0.024)*** 0.050 (0.012)*** 0.530
Indirect (Desire, Demonstration-Approach) −0.011 (0.007) −0.007 (0.004) 0.027 0.002 (0.015) 0.001 (0.008) 0.010
Indirect (Desire, Development) −0.002 (0.003) −0.002 (0.002) 0.006 0.000 (0.006) 0.000 (0.003) 0.000
Direct −0.240 (0.074)** −0.156 (0.049** 0.611 –0.049 (0.077) −0.027 (0.042) −0.284
Total Effect −0.393 (0.071)*** −0.255 (0.043*** 0.173 (0.079)* 0.095 (0.042)*
Depression Indirect (Desire) −0.059 (0.053) −0.024 (0.022) 0.226 0.114 (0.072) 0.043 (0.026) 0.543
Indirect (Desire, Demonstration-Avoidance) −0.048 (0.022)* −0.020 (0.009)* 0.184 0.045 (0.021)* 0.017 (0.008)* 0.213
Indirect (Desire, Demonstration-Approach) −0.010 (0.012) −0.004 (0.005) 0.040 0.003 (0.020) 0.001 (0.007) 0.012
Indirect (Desire, Development) −0.004 (0.007) −0.002 (0.003) 0.015 0.002 (0.010) 0.001 (0.004) 0.011
Direct −0.138 (0.119) −0.057 (0.049) 0.535 0.046 (0.129) 0.017 (0.048) 0.221
Total Effect −0.259 (0.108)* −0.107 (0.044)* 0.210 (0.126) 0.078 (0.047)

Ratio is the (in)direct effect divided by the total effect

SE standard error; CSS completely standardized solution

*p ≤ 0.05; **p ≤ 0.01; ***p ≤ 0.001

Respecified models

The presence of very small, statistically-insignificant regression estimates for several paths in Models 1A and 1B suggested the need to respecify the model. In particular, only one SGQ subscale– demonstration-avoidance – appeared to be predictive of internalizing symptoms. All regression weights for paths with the other two subscales as the predictor were consequently fixed to 0, and the resultant models 2A and 2B (see Fig. 2) were compared with models 1A and 1B, respectively. Both models improved (A: ∆χ2 = 0.467, (p = 0.977), CFI > 0.999, RMSEA < 0.008 (0.855); B: ∆χ2 = 3.296, (p = 0.510), CFI = 0.987, RMSEA = 0.050 (0.445)), and are depicted in Fig. 2. Table 4 shows the parameter estimates for these models. Table 5 gives Sobel estimates of the total effect, indirect effects, and the direct effect, of number of (un/reciprocated) friendship nominations on social anxiety symptoms and on depressive symptoms, including the ratio of each (in)direct effect to the total effect. The two-step mediations observed in models 1A and 1B were also observed in models 2A and 2B, amounting to between ~22 and 53% of the total effect. In addition, as in models 1A and 1B, the total effect of unreciprocated friendship nominations on depression symptoms was the only instance of a nonsignificant total effect in these models, and the direct effect of reciprocated friendship nominations on social anxiety was the only instance of a significant direct effect in these models, amounting to ~59% of the total effect. In addition, as in models 1A and 1B, the total effects of reciprocated friendship nominations on depression and social anxiety symptoms were significant, as was the total effect of unreciprocated friendship nominations on social anxiety symptoms. However, as in model 1B, the total effect of unreciprocated friendship nominations on depression symptoms was nonsignificant. Also as in models 1A and 1B, the direct effects of reciprocated friendship nominations on depression symptoms, and of unreciprocated friendship nominations on social anxiety symptoms and on depression symptoms, were all nonsignificant, while the direct effect of reciprocated friendship nominations on social anxiety symptoms was significant, amounting to ~59% of the total effect. Unlike model 1B, however, model 2B revealed significant one-step mediation effects of unreciprocated friendship nominations on social anxiety and depression symptoms, mediated by desire, amounting to ~77% and ~59% of the total effects, respectively.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Respecified models for (a) reciprocated and (b) unreciprocated friendships. Straight lines are freely- estimated regressions, curved lines are freely-estimated covariances. Regressions with an estimate >1 are bold lines, and those which failed to reach significance (i.e., p > 0.05) are dashed lines

Table 4.

Regression weights and covariance estimates per path for Models 2A & B

Path Model 2A Model 2B
Estimate (SE) CSS Estimate (SE) Estimate (SE) CSS Estimate (SE)
Friends Desire −0.575 (0.069)*** −0.344 (0.043)*** 0.354 (0.082)*** 0.195 (0.046)***
Friends Social Anxiety −0.232 (0.075)** −0.150 (0.049)** −0.053 (0.070) −0.029 (0.038)
Friends Depression −0.129 (0.120) −0.054 (0.050) 0.041 (0.126) 0.015 (0.047)
Desire Social Anxiety 0.099 (0.055) 0.107 (0.058) 0.379 (0.107) 0.374 (0.093)***
Desire Depression 0.116 (0.092) 0.080 (0.063) 0.346 (0.126)** 0.234 (0.083)**
Desire Development 0.040 (0.018)* 0.158 (0.070)* 0.091 (0.020)*** 0.354 (0.071)***
Desire Demonstration-Approach 0.077 (0.025)** 0.284 (0.089)** 0.164 (0.030)*** 0.560 (0.085)***
Desire Demonstration-Avoidance 0.085 (0.026)** 0.355 (0.102)*** 0.156 (0.031)*** 0.591 (0.097)***
Demonstration-Avoidance Social Anxiety 2.159 (0.179)*** 0.558 (0.036)*** 1.672 (0.282)*** 0.434 (0.073)***
Demonstration-Avoidance Depression 1.281 (0.327)*** 0.211 (0.052)*** 0.840 (0.375)* 0.149 (0.065)*
Development Demonstration-Approach 0.008 (0.003)* 0.129 (0.051)* 0.003 (0.004) 0.048 (0.064)
Development Demonstration-Avoid 0.016 (0.003)*** 0.297 (0.051)*** 0.011 (0.004)** 0.218 (0.062)***
Demonstration-Approach Demonstration-Avoid 0.022 (0.004)*** 0.377 (0.051)*** 0.012 (0.006)* 0.242 (0.089)**
Depression Social Anxiety 0.453 (0.055)*** 0.462 (0.038)*** 0.446 (0.061)*** 0.455 (0.041)***

→ represents regressions and ↔ represents covariances

SE standard error; CSS completely standardized solution

*p ≤ 0.05; **p ≤ 0.01; ***p ≤ 0.001

Table 5.

Indirect, direct and total effect estimates (sobel) of friends on internalizing for Models 2A & B

DV Effect (Mediators) Model 2A Model 2B
Estimate (SE) CSS Estimate (SE) Ratio Estimate (SE) CSS Estimate (SE) Ratio
Social Anxiety Indirect (Desire) −0.057 (0.033) −0.037 (0.021) 0.144 0.134 (0.047)** 0.073 (0.024)** 0.774
Indirect (Desire, Demonstration-Avoidance) −0.105 (0.035)** −0.068 (0.021)** 0.267 0.092 (0.023)*** 0.050 (0.012)*** 0.532
Direct −0.232 (0.075)** −0.150 (0.049)** 0.589 −0.053 (0.070) −0.029 (0.038) −0.306
Total Effect −0.393 (0.071)*** −0.255 (0.043)*** 0.173 (0.079)* 0.094 (0.042)*
Depression Indirect (Desire) −0.067 (0.054) −0.028 (0.022) 0.259 0.122 (0.050)* 0.046 (0.019)* 0.584
Indirect (Desire, Demonstration-Avoidance) −0.062 (0.024)** −0.026 (0.010)** 0.241 0.046 (0.021)* 0.017 (0.008)* 0.221
Direct −0.129 (0.120) −0.054 (0.050) 0.500 0.041 (0.126) 0.015 (0.047) 0.195
Total Effect −0.259 (0.108)* −0.107 (0.044)* 0.210 (0.126) 0.078 (0.047)

Ratio is the (in)direct effect divided by the total effect

SE standard error; CSS completely standardized solution

*p ≤ 0.05; **p ≤ 0.005; ***p ≤ 0.001

Post-hoc Analyses

These analyses used R (R Core Team, 2020) and the lme4 (Bates et al., 2015) and QuantPsyc (Fletcher, 2012) packages in RStudio (RStudio Team (2020)).

Univariate regressions

To facilitate comparison with the model-based estimates, univariate analyses were run, corresponding to each freely-estimated regression (i.e., path) in any model. The results of these analyses are shown in Table 6. Note the inverted associations with other variables when comparing reciprocated and unreciprocated friendships.

Table 6.

Univariate regressions of variables sharing a path in any model

Path Estimate (ß) t-statistic R2 F-statistic
Reciprocated Friends Desire −0.331*** −7.194 0.109 51.75
Reciprocated Friends Social Anxiety −0.253*** −5.356 0.064 28.69
Reciprocated Friends Depression −0.107* −2.202 0.011 4.847
Unreciprocated Friends Desire 0.185*** 3.859 0.034 14.89
Unreciprocated Friends Social Anxiety 0.103* 2.114 0.011 4.471
Unreciprocated Friends Depression 0.080 1.643 0.006 2.699
Desire Social Anxiety 0.380*** 8.438 0.145 71.20
Desire Depression 0.185*** 3.860 0.034 14.90
Desire Development 0.178*** 3.701 0.032 13.70
Desire Demonstration-Approach 0.330*** 7.167 0.109 51.36
Desire Demonstration-Avoidance 0.365*** 8.033 0.133 64.54
Demonstration-Approach Social Anxiety 0.331*** 7.207 0.110 51.94
Demonstration-Approach Depression 0.151** 3.133 0.023 9.817
Demonstration-Avoidance Social Anxiety 0.594*** 15.15 0.353 229.6
Demonstration-Avoidance Depression 0.234*** 4.947 0.055 24.48
Development Social Anxiety 0.228*** 4.803 0.052 23.07
Development Depression 0.109* 2.247 0.012 5.047

*p ≤ 0.05; **p ≤ 0.005; ***p ≤ 0.001

Demonstration as status-seeking

Demonstration goals overlap conceptually with status-seeking behaviors, and this overlap was an important factor in the formulation of the hypotheses investigated here. To test whether this relationship held for this sample, a linear regression with (total, α = 0.86) demonstration goals as the dependent variable, and self-report desire for popularity and to be liked as the predictors, was performed. This regression was statistically significant (F(2,420) = 105.1, p < 0.001, adjusted R2 = 0.330). The main effects of desire for popularity (t = 9.398, p < 0.001, β = 0.454), and desire to be liked (t = 3.793, p < 0.001, β = 0.183) on demonstration goals were also significant. Note that the coefficient of desire for popularity exceeds double that of desire to be liked. This is consistent with the idea that demonstration goals overlap with status-orientation.

Comparison of reciprocated and unreciprocated friendships

As mentioned above, associations between friendships and other variables were in opposite directions depending on reciprocity. It is not possible to formally compare models A and B because the underlying data for the friendship variable are different, and the models are consequently unnested. Therefore, to compare the contributions of the two different measures of friendship in the social network, the corrected estimates of reciprocated and unreciprocated friendships were entered as independent variables into linear regressions predicting the desire, depression and social anxiety variables.

The linear regression with desire as the dependent variable was statistically significant (F(2,416) = 35.25, p < 0.001, adjusted R2 = 0.141). The main effects of number of reciprocated friends (t = −7.509, p < 0.001, β = −0.342), and number of unreciprocated friends (t = 4.500, p < 0.001, β = 0.205) on desire were significant. Note that contrary to expectations (P2b), an increase in the number of reported unreciprocated friendships was associated with an increase in the desire for more friendships, while reciprocated friendships was associated with a decrease in desire.

The linear regression with depression as the dependent variable was statistically significant (F(2,416) = 3.571, p = 0.029, adjusted R2 = 0.012). The main effect of reciprocated friends (t = −2.324, p = 0.021, β = −0.116), was significant, but the effect of unreciprocated friends was not (t = 1.548, p = 0.122, β = 0.076). Participants with more reciprocated friendships reported fewer depressive symptoms, but any association that may exist with unreciprocated nominations was too small to detect here. It may still be worth noting, however, that the coefficient estimates for the reciprocated and unreciprocated terms in this regression were once again in opposite directions.

The linear regression with social anxiety as the dependent variable was statistically significant (F(2,416) = 17.17, p < 0.001, adjusted R2 = 0.072). The main effects of reciprocated friends (t = −599, p < 0.001, β = −0.265), and unreciprocated friends (t = 2.291, p = 0.023, β = 0.109) were significant. Note that once again the influence of unreciprocated friendships runs counter to that of reciprocated friendships. Fewer reciprocated friendship nominations was associated with higher levels of social anxiety but, contrary to prediction, fewer unreciprocated nominations was associated with lower levels of social anxiety.

Discussion

Adolescence is characterized by a dramatic increase in sensitivity to social reward, which motivates adolescents to pursue social acceptance, especially from peers (Crone & Dahl, 2012). This motivation serves the developmental tasks of adolescence, which depend on learning from social experience (Manning, 2002). However, adolescents’ intense social motivation also contributes to their marked vulnerability to internalizing symptoms (Rapee et al., 2019). Moreover, the specific goals an adolescent’s social motivation drives them to pursue – their social goal orientation (SGO) – predict differences in vulnerability to internalizing symptoms (Ryan & Shim, 2008). Similarly, adolescents with larger numbers of friendships have lower rates of internalizing symptoms and disorders including social anxiety (Rapee et al., 2019) and depression (Rueger et al., 2016). This study investigated whether friendships within one’s class may influence internalizing symptoms in part by reducing the desire for more classmate friendships, which may promote maladaptive social goals. A pair of path analyses were performed to test the effects of number of friendships on internalizing symptoms, using (A) only reciprocated and (B) only unreciprocated friendships. The results of path analysis (A) provide evidence that the effect of the number of reciprocated friends on internalizing symptoms is serially mediated by the desire for more friends, and SGO; specifically, demonstration-avoidance goals.

In the path models (A), as predicted, an association between numbers of reciprocated friendships and internalizing symptoms was mediated in two steps; number of reciprocated friendships negatively predicted desire, which in turn predicted more demonstration-avoidance goals, which in turn predicted more symptoms of depression and social anxiety. This was the only path to significantly predict depressive symptoms, and the only indirect path to significantly predict social anxiety symptoms (a significant direct effect of number of friends on social anxiety symptoms remained after controlling for desire and SGO). This confirms the prediction that internalizing symptoms would be positively associated with demonstration goals, but the predicted negative association with development goals was not found. Nevertheless, that the data supported this model suggests that the association between number of friends and internalizing symptoms is indeed driven in part by a tendency for adolescents who strongly desire more friendships to pursue maladaptive social goals, perhaps by encouraging a tendency to evaluate one’s social success in terms of status. One mechanism that could contribute to this (re)orientation toward status is that, from the perspective of low-status adolescents, their high-status peers have more friendships, and the quality (and sincerity) of those friendships cannot be evaluated from outside. Thus, for those adolescents who feel that they have too few friends, their social status seems to be synonymous with social inclusion and the avoidance of social exclusion. This is consistent with the observation here that desire was positively associated with both the demonstration-approach and demonstration-avoid goals subscales, which have been associated with status-seeking and -preserving in several past studies (e.g., Rodkin et al., 2013; Rudolph et al., 2011; Ryan & Shim, 2008). Moreover, a post-hoc analysis showed that demonstration goals were tightly, positively associated with the desire to be popular, and less strongly associated with the desire to be liked, which supports the suggestion that demonstration goals reflect a pursuit of social status in preference over interpersonal connection and closeness. One might therefore expect that demonstration-avoidance (status-preserving) goals would predispose the adolescent to differences in information processing conceptually similar to those associated with social anxiety. By drawing their attention to the possibility that aspects of one’s social behavior could be evaluated negatively by peers (Vassilopoulos & Banerjee, 2008), status-preserving goals may promote attentional bias toward “negative self-related social information” (Mellings & Alden, 2000, p. 254), setting them on a developmental pathway towards social anxiety disorder.

The finding that demonstration-avoidance goals were the only goals subscale to significantly positively predict internalizing symptoms is congruent with the goal orientation literature, which shows the most consistent harmful effects are driven by demonstration-avoidance goals (e.g. Kuroda & Sakurai, 2011; Ryan & Shim, 2008), while demonstration-approach goals often have a mixture of beneficial and harmful effects, and are highly contingent on individuals’ traits and circumstances (e.g. Mouratidis & Sideridis, 2009). However, while development goals have generally been shown to have protective or ameliorative effects (e.g., Kuroda & Sakurai, 2011; Shim et al., 2013), the present results indicate a positive association with internalizing symptoms, which was statistically significant only at the univariate level. One possible interpretation of this is that development goals are more likely to be endorsed by individuals who feel that their social skills are lacking, and thus an unmodeled, positive path from social anxiety to development goals (i.e., in the opposite direction to the path included in the model) could have concealed a protective effect of development goals. Related to this, in the academic domain, a similar approach/avoidance division for development goals has been suggested, to distinguish the pursuit of increased skill from the avoidance of decreases in skill (Lou, Masuda & Li, 2017); if such a distinction is justified, the combination of both approach and avoidance within one SGQ subscale may have obscured positive effects of one with negative effects of the other.

Unexpectedly, in the path models (B), which used unreciprocated ties, the numbers of such ties were positively associated with both desire and depression symptoms, and thus these effects were opposite in sign depending on whether reciprocated or unreciprocated friendships were used; adolescents who made more unreciprocated friendship nominations were more likely to have a stronger desire to increase their number of friends, and to report more depression symptoms. Similarly, significant univariate regressions of reciprocated or unreciprocated friendships on desire and on social anxiety were also in opposite directions, such that decreasing number of reciprocated friendships or increasing number of unreciprocated friendships both predicted stronger desire and greater symptoms of social anxiety. While this confirms the prediction that the association with desire would be less negative for unreciprocated than reciprocated friendships, both associations were predicted to be negative. The unexpected finding that the association of unreciprocated friendships with desire was positive also conflicts with previous work suggesting that unreciprocated friendship nominations reflect a looser tie, but qualitatively similar effects (Lin & Weinberg, 2014). One possible explanation for this finding is that desire may reflect the perceived (in)adequacy of current ties, and reciprocated ties contribute to the perception of adequacy (thereby lowering desire) because of the social support offered to those perceived as friends. If so, unreciprocated ties may imply that the adolescent perceives as friends individuals who do not offer such support, which may lower the expected benefit of each additional friendship, and therefore increase the extent to which an increase in numbers of friends appears to be required for a given amount of benefit. Alternatively, the direction of causation may be inverted for unreciprocated nominations: desire may influence unreciprocated friendship nominations through a kind of “wishful thinking” wherein the desire for more friendships lowers the threshold when selecting who to nominate (see e.g., Scholte et al., 2009). Future studies of adolescent social networks examining the differences and similarities between reciprocated and unreciprocated ties would allow further interpretation of this finding.

Limitations

This study employed a path model analysis of cross-sectional data. This statistical method allows one to perform regression analyses on a complex, multi-step equation, but is unable to distinguish between causal and merely correlational effects using cross-sectional data. A future study using a longitudinal design would provide more robust support for possible causal relations between these constructs. Furthermore, substantial evidence shows bidirectional effects between internalizing symptoms and social inclusion (e.g., Jacobson & Newman, 2016). For example, anxious withdrawal and social exclusion are mutually reinforcing (Gazelle & Ladd, 2003). Similarly, social anxiety entails information-processing biases that can hamper accurate detection of social threat (Blöte et al., 2010), and depression is associated with shyness (Murberg, 2009) and other difficulties in social interactions (Segrin & Flora, 2000; see also Hames, et al., 2013). A longitudinal (repeated measures) design would also permit future studies to differentiate between the effects on internalizing symptoms of; friendship networks, desire, and SGO; and the inverse effects. Furthermore, the use of a single item to measure desire prevents estimation of Chronbach’s alpha (the standard statistical estimate of the reliability of a psychometric, which assumes multiple items administered concurrently) and reduces construct validity since differences between participants’ interpretation of the item cannot be “averaged out” by summing multiple items. Moreover, while we treated the desire variable as ordinal, and not continuous, in recognition of the low sensitivity of a five-point single-item measure, the low sensitivity remains.

There are also limitations to our approach to estimating effects of friendships. Firstly, we estimated these effects based on number of reciprocated nominations alone. While the number of friends is of direct importance to the theorized relationships through desire, future studies could improve on our design by accounting for differences in the strength of friendships, as well as their duration (e.g., asking whether a friendship pre-dates the transition to secondary school), as a moderator or similar. Another limitation is that while self- and peer-report measures facilitate the collection of data from larger samples, the resulting data provide a less direct insight into how participants actually behave in social contexts than would be provided by directly observing them interacting. Furthermore, social goals and behavior may vary across different contexts (Erdley et al., 1997) whereas the peer nomination data used here were specific to the classroom, and so the measures of social relationships were focused on those peer relationships. However, in contrast with depressive symptoms’ negative association with number of friends at school, adolescents who report having more friends outside school also report more depressive symptoms (Ueno, 2005), suggesting friendships outside school cannot compensate for a lack of school friends. Additionally, parent-child relationships are also clearly important contributors to vulnerability or resilience to internalizing symptoms (Pössel et al., 2018). The present findings thus describe an observation about academically-able Dutch early adolescents in the specific context of classroom relationships with peers (see also; Henrich et al., 2010), which can be generalized to similar groups and, in concert with future work in other cultures and contexts and with different samples, may subsequently be found to generalize outside of western European classrooms.

Directions for Future Research

As discussed above, future research should employ longitudinal designs and examine these dynamics also in other contexts (e.g., the family, or in another cultural context). Peer nomination studies comparing reciprocated and unreciprocated ties are also needed. Furthermore, responses to failures and successes have been studied in relation to goal orientation, with demonstration goals predicting faster withdrawal of effort and persistence (Sideridis & Kaplan, 2011), and increased helplessness (Erdley et al., 1997), and negative interpersonal experiences exaggerate the effects of social goals on adjustment (Kuroda & Sakurai, 2011). Future research should investigate the influence of experiences of social “failures” on the interplay between social goals, perceptions of status and closeness, self-disclosure and silencing, and adjustment. It may be important for studies of social “failure” experiences to examine the possibility of a division of the development goals concept into goals of increasing skill and goals of avoiding reducing skill (Lou, Masuda & Li, 2017; see also Larsen et al. (2012); and Pearson & Rose, 2021). Finally, the present results support the proposition that status-oriented goals would compete with closeness-oriented goals for priority in guiding behavior. Future research should investigate whether status-oriented goals reduce closeness-seeking behaviors such as self-disclosure. Indeed, self-silencing (the antonym of self-disclosure) in friendships has been found to mediate the deleterious effect of rejection sensitivity on self-reported feelings of friendship support (Thomas & Bowker, 2015).

Conclusion

While the protective effects of friendships have been extensively studied in relation to adolescents’ vulnerability to internalizing symptoms, the possibility that qualitative differences in social motivation could play a role in producing or amplifying these effects has received little attention. The path models tested here rest on the hypothesis that a strong desire for more friends in the bounded community of a classroom would promote demonstration (and thus status) -oriented social goals, which would in turn promote internalizing symptoms. As predicted, the link between numbers of friendships and internalizing symptoms was mediated in two steps. First, adolescents with fewer reciprocated classmate friendships expressed a stronger desire for more classmate friendships. Second, adolescents who expressed a stronger desire for more friendships endorsed more demonstration-avoidance goals, which in turn predicted more internalizing symptoms. This may reflect a tendency toward status-oriented social behavior in adolescents with a strong desire for more friends, and increased attention to social status may come at the expense of cultivating interpersonal intimacy in extant friendships, and promote psychosocially maladaptive attention allocation in social situations. However, not all predictions were supported; no beneficial effect of development goals was observed, and number of unreciprocated friendship nominations was positively associated with desire for more friendships and social anxiety. Future research should seek to examine bidirectional or cyclical relationships in this domain using a longitudinal design.

Supplementary Information

Supplementary Materials (17.8KB, docx)

Acknowledgments

Authors’ Contributions

R.J.W. conceived of the study, participated in its design and coordination, performed the statistical analyses and drafted the manuscript; N.C.L. conceived of the study, participated in its design and coordination, and in the interpretation of data, and helped to draft the manuscript; I.L.J.L.J. participated in statistical analysis, and helped with the literature search and drafting of the manuscript; M.H. participated in the design and coordination of the study, and collected data; H.S. participated in the design and coordination of the study, and collected data; MvB participated in the design and coordination of the study; L.K. procured funding for the project, conceived of the study, participated in its design and coordination, and in the interpretation of data, and helped to draft the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by European Research Council Consolidator Grant (no. 648082) awarded to L.K.

Data Sharing Declaration

The datasets analyzed during the current study are not publicly available but are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Biographies

Reubs J. Walsh

is a PhD candidate in Clinical, Neuro- and Developmental Psychology at Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam. Their research interests include typical and atypical social cognition, interoception and affect, and biosocial and developmental determinants of neuropsychiatric health.

Nikki C. Lee

is an assistant professor in Developmental Psychology at Utrecht University, Amsterdam. Her research focuses on social-emotional development during adolescence and how this enables adolescents to flexibly navigate their social environments.

Imke L.J. Lemmers-Jansen

is an assistant professor in Clinical, Neuro- and Developmental Psychology at Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam. Her focus of research is psychosis, social interactions and the translation to real life social behavior in friendships.

Miriam Hollarek

is a PhD candidate in Clinical, Neuro- and Developmental Psychology at Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam. Her research interests include the influence of the social environment on the development of social cognition from infancy to adolescence and neural mechanisms associated with these processes.

Hester Sijtsma

is a postdoctoral researcher at Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam at the department of Clinical, Neuro- and Developmental Psychology. Her research is focused on the interactions between social behavior, social networks, and the brain during adolescence.

Mariët van Buuren

is an assistant professor in Clinical, Neuro- and Developmental Psychology at Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam. Her research is focused on the development of social cognition, underlying brain mechanisms and social functioning in adolescence.

Lydia Krabbendam

is professor of Developmental Neuropsychology and head of the Dept Clinical, Neuro- and Developmental Psychology at Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam. Her research focuses on the development of self-regulation and social cognition in adolescence.

Compliance with Ethical Standards

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare no competing interests.

Ethical Approval

This study was granted ethical approval as part of a larger project approved by the Scientific and Ethical Review Board (VCWE) of the Faculty of Behavior and Movement Sciences, Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam, and was performed in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments.

Informed Consent

Both the participant and their parent(s) or guardian(s) provided written informed consent to participate in the project following a letter and an information evening to inform them of the protocol and aims of the study and their rights as participants.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Change history

10/28/2023

A Correction to this paper has been published: 10.1007/s10964-023-01889-1

Supplementary information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s10964-023-01780-z.

References

  1. Aune, T., Marie, E., Juul, L., Beidel, D. C., Nordahl, H. M., & Dvorak, R. D. (2020). Mitigating adolescent social anxiety symptoms: the effects of social support and social self ‑ efficacy in findings from the Young ‑ HUNT 3 study. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 10.1007/s00787-020-01529-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  2. Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker BM, Walker SC. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software. 2015;67(1):1–48. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. Beck, J. S., Beck, A. T., Jolly, J. B., & Steer, R. A. (2005). BYI-2-NL|Beck Youth Inventories 2nd Edition – Nederlandstalige bewerking (2nd ed.). Pearson Clinical. Retrieved from https://www.pearsonclinical.nl/byi-2-nl.
  4. Blöte AW, Duvekot J, Schalk RDF, Tuinenburg EM, Westenberg PM. Nervousness and performance characteristics as predictors of peer behavior towards socially anxious adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2010;39(12):1498–1507. doi: 10.1007/s10964-009-9463-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Blöte AW, Westenberg PM. Socially anxious adolescents’ perception of treatment by classmates. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2007;45(2):189–198. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Boersma-van Dam E, Hale B, Koot H, Meeus W, Branje S. Adolescents’ and best friend’s depressive symptoms and conflict management: intraindividual and interpersonal processes over time. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2019;48(2):203–217. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2016.1253017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cillessen, A. H. N., & Marks, P. E. L. (2017). Methodological choices in peer nomination research. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2017(157), 21–44. 10.1002/cad.20206. [DOI] [PubMed]
  8. Crone EA, Dahl RE. Understanding adolescence as a period of social-affective engagement and goal flexibility. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2012;13(9):636–650. doi: 10.1038/nrn3313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Dweck CS, Leggett EL. A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review. 1988;95(2):256–273. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.95.2.256. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  10. Elliot AJ, Harackiewicz JM. Approach and avoidance achievement goals and intrinsic motivation: a mediational analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1996;70(3):461–475. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.70.3.461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Elliott ES, Dweck CS. Goals: an approach to motivation and achievement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1988;54(1):5–12. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.1.5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Erdley CA, Cain KM, Loomis CC, Dumas-Hines F, Dweck CS. Relations among children’s social goals, implicit personality theories, and responses to social failure. Developmental Psychology. 1997;33(2):263–272. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.33.2.263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Falci C, McNeely C. Too many friends: Social integration, network cohesion and adolescent depressive symptoms. Social Forces. 2009;87(4):2031–2062. doi: 10.1353/sof.0.0189. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  14. Ferguson SM, Ryan AM. It’s lonely at the top: adolescent students’ peer-perceived popularity and self-perceived social contentment. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2019;48(2):341–358. doi: 10.1007/s10964-018-0970-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Fletcher, T. D. (2012). QuantPsyc: Quantitative Psychology Tools.
  16. Gazelle H, Ladd GW. Anxious solitude and peer exclusion: a diathesis-stress model of internalizing trajectories in childhood. Child Development. 2003;74(1):257–278. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hames JL, Hagan CR, Joiner TE. Interpersonal processes in depression. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2013;9(1):355–377. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185553. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Henrich J, Heine SJ, Norenzayan A. The weirdest people in the world? The Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2010;33(2–3):61–83. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0999152X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Holt-Lunstad J, Smith TB, Baker M, Harris T, Stephenson D. Loneliness and social isolation as risk factors for mortality: a meta-analytic review. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2015;10(2):227–237. doi: 10.1177/1745691614568352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Hu LT, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling. 1999;6(1):1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  21. Inderbitzen-Nolan HM, Walters KS. Social anxiety scale for adolescents: normative data and further evidence of construct validity. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology. 2010;29(3):360–371. doi: 10.1207/S15374424JCCP2903_7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Jacobson NC, Newman MG. Perceptions of close and group relationships mediate the relationship between anxiety and depression over a decade later. Depression and Anxiety. 2016;33(1):66–74. doi: 10.1002/da.22402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Juvonen J, Lessard LM, Rastogi R, Schacter HL, Smith DS. Promoting social inclusion in educational settings: challenges and opportunities. Educational Psychologist. 2019;54(4):250–270. doi: 10.1080/00461520.2019.1655645. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kuroda Y, Sakurai S. Social goal orientations, interpersonal stress, and depressive symptoms among early adolescents in Japan: a test of the diathesis-stress model using the trichotomous framework of social goal orientations. Journal of Early Adolescence. 2011;31(2):300–322. doi: 10.1177/0272431610363158. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. La Greca AM, Lopez N. Social anxiety among adolescents: linkages with peer relations and friendships. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 1998;26(2):83–94. doi: 10.1023/A:1022684520514. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Larsen JK, Vermulst AA, Eisinga R, English T, Gross JJ, Hofman E, Engels RCME. Social coping by masking? Parental support and peer victimization as mediators of the relationship between depressive symptoms and expressive suppression in adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2012;41(12):1628–1642. doi: 10.1007/s10964-012-9782-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Lessard LM, Juvonen J. Friendless adolescents: do perceptions of social threat account for their internalizing difficulties and continued friendlessness. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2018;28(2):277–283. doi: 10.1111/jora.12388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Liem GAD. Academic and social achievement goals: their additive, interactive, and specialized effects on school functioning. British Journal of Educational Psychology. 2016;86(1):37–56. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12085. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Lin X, Weinberg BA. Unrequited friendship? How reciprocity mediates adolescent peer effects. Regional Science and Urban Economics. 2014;48:144–153. doi: 10.1016/j.regsciurbeco.2014.06.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lodder GMA, Scholte RHJ, Goossens L, Verhagen M. Loneliness in early adolescence: friendship quantity, friendship quality, and dyadic processes. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 2017;46(5):709–720. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2015.1070352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Lou NM, Masuda T, Li LMW. Decremental mindsets and prevention-focused motivation: an extended framework of implicit theories of intelligence. Learning and Individual Differences. 2017;59(February):96–106. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2017.08.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  32. Manning ML. Havighurst’s developmental tasks, young adolescents, and diversity. The Clearing House: A Journal of Educational Strategies, Issues and Ideas. 2002;76(2):75–78. doi: 10.1080/00098650209604953. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  33. Mellings TMB, Alden LE. Cognitive processes in social anxiety: the effects of self-focus, rumination and anticipatory processing. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2000;38(3):243–257. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00040-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Midgley C, Kaplan A, Middleton M. Performance-approach goals: good for what, for whom, under what circumstances, and at what cost. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2001;93(1):77–86. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.93.1.77. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  35. Miers AC, Blöte AW, De Rooij M, Bokhorst CL, Westenberg PM. Trajectories of social anxiety during adolescence and relations with cognition, social competence, and temperament. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 2013;41(1):97–110. doi: 10.1007/s10802-012-9651-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Mouratidis A, Sideridis G. On social achievement goals: their relations with peer acceptance, classroom belongingness, and perceptions of loneliness. Journal of Experimental Education. 2009;77(3):285–308. doi: 10.3200/JEXE.77.3.285-308. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  37. Murberg TA. Shyness predicts depressive symptoms among adolescents: a prospective study. School Psychology International. 2009;30(5):507–519. doi: 10.1177/0143034309107065. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  38. Narr RK, Allen JP, Tan JS, Loeb EL. Close friendship strength and broader peer group desirability as differential predictors of adult mental health. Child Development. 2019;90(1):298–313. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12905. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Pachucki MC, Ozer EJ, Barrat A, Cattuto C. Mental health and social networks in early adolescence: A dynamic study of objectively-measured social interaction behaviors. Social Science and Medicine. 2015;125:40–50. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.04.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Pearson A, Rose K. A conceptual analysis of autistic masking: understanding the narrative of stigma and the illusion of choice. Autism in Adulthood, aut. 2021;2020:0043. doi: 10.1089/aut.2020.0043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Perkins AM, Bowers G, Cassidy J, Meiser-Stedman R, Pass L. An enhanced psychological mindset intervention to promote adolescent wellbeing within educational settings: a feasibility randomized controlled trial. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2021;77(4):946–967. doi: 10.1002/jclp.23104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Pössel P, Burton SM, Cauley B, Sawyer MG, Spence SH, Sheffield J. Associations between social support from family, friends, and teachers and depressive symptoms in adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2018;47(2):398–412. doi: 10.1007/s10964-017-0712-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. R Core Team. (2020). R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from https://www.r-project.org/.
  44. Rapee RM, Oar EL, Johnco CJ, Forbes MK, Fardouly J, Magson NR, Richardson CE. Adolescent development and risk for the onset of social-emotional disorders: a review and conceptual model. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2019;123:103501. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2019.103501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Razer M, Friedman VJ, Warshofsky B. Schools as agents of social exclusion and inclusion. International Journal of Inclusive Education. 2013;17(11):1152–1170. doi: 10.1080/13603116.2012.742145. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  46. Rodkin PC, Ryan AM, Jamison R, Wilson T. Social goals, social behavior, and social status in middle childhood. Developmental Psychology. 2013;49(6):1139–1150. doi: 10.1037/a0029389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Rosseel, Y. (2012). Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(2), 1–36. Retrieved from http://hdl.handle.net/1854/LU-3099674.
  48. RStudio Team. (2020). RStudio: Integrated Development Environment for R. Boston, MA, USA. Retrieved from http://www.rstudio.com/
  49. Rudolph KD. Implicit theories of peer relationships. Social Development. 2010;19(1):113–129. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00534.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Rudolph KD, Abaied JL, Flynn M, Sugimura N, Agoston AM. Developing relationships, being cool, and not looking like a loser: social goal orientation predicts children’s responses to peer aggression. Child Development. 2011;82(5):1518–1530. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01631.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Rueger SY, Malecki CK, Pyun Y, Aycock C, Coyle S. A meta-analytic review of the association between perceived social support and depression in childhood and adolescence. Psychological Bulletin. 2016;142(10):1017–1067. doi: 10.1037/bul0000058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Ryan AM, Shim SS. Social achievement goals: The nature and consequences of different orientations toward social competence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2006;32(9):1246–1263. doi: 10.1177/0146167206289345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Ryan AM, Shim SS. An exploration of young adolescents’ social achievement goals and social adjustment in middle school. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2008;100(3):672–687. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.100.3.672. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  54. Schleider, J. L., Burnette, J. L., Widman, L., Hoyt, C., & Prinstein, M. J. (2019). Randomized trial of a single‐session growth mindset intervention for rural adolescents’ internalizing and externalizing problems. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 10.1080/15374416.2019.1622123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  55. Schleider JL, Weisz JR. Reducing risk for anxiety and depression in adolescents: effects of a single-session intervention teaching that personality can change. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2016;87:170–181. doi: 10.1016/J.BRAT.2016.09.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Scholte RHJ, Overbeek G, Ten Brink G, Rommes E, De Kemp RAT, Goossens L, Engels RCME. The significance of reciprocal and unilateral friendships for peer victimization in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2009;38(1):89–100. doi: 10.1007/s10964-008-9287-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Sebastian, C., Viding, E., Williams, K. D., & Blakemore, S. J. (2010, February). Social brain development and the affective consequences of ostracism in adolescence. Brain and Cognition. 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.06.008 [DOI] [PubMed]
  58. Segrin C, Flora J. Poor social skills are a vulnerability factor in the development of psychosocial problems. Human Communication Research. 2000;26(3):489–514. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-2958.2000.tb00766.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  59. Shim SS, Cho YJ, Wang C. Classroom goal structures, social achievement goals, and adjustment in middle school. Learning and Instruction. 2013;23(1):69–77. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.05.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  60. Shim SS, Ryan AM. What do students want socially when they arrive at college? Implications of social achievement goals for social behaviors and adjustment during the first semester of college. Motivation and Emotion. 2012;36(4):504–515. doi: 10.1007/s11031-011-9272-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  61. Sideridis GD, Kaplan A. Achievement goals and persistence across tasks: the roles of failure and success. Journal of Experimental Education. 2011;79(4):429–451. doi: 10.1080/00220973.2010.539634. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  62. Sijtsma, H., Lee, N. C., Hollarek, M., Walsh, R. J., Buuren, M. Van, Braams, B. R., & Krabbendam, L. (2021). Social Cognition and Friendships in Adolescents With Autistic-Like Experiences and Psychotic-Like Experiences, 11(January), 1–11. 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.589824. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  63. Simone M, Long E, Lockhart G. The dynamic relationship between unhealthy weight control and adolescent friendships: a social network approach. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2018;47(7):1373–1384. doi: 10.1007/s10964-017-0796-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Skaalvik EM. Self-enhancing and self-defeating ego orientation: relations with task and avoidance orientation, achievement, self-perceptions, and anxiety. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1997;89(1):71–81. doi: 10.1037//0022-0663.89.1.71. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  65. Smith JA, Moody J. Structural effects of network sampling coverage I: nodes missing at random. Social Networks. 2013;35(4):652–668. doi: 10.1016/j.socnet.2013.09.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Thomas KK, Bowker JC. An investigation of desired friendships during early adolescence. The Journal of Early Adolescence. 2013;33(6):867–890. doi: 10.1177/0272431612469725. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  67. Thomas KK, Bowker JC. Rejection sensitivity and adjustment during adolescence: do friendship self-silencing and parent support matter. Journal of Child and Family Studies. 2015;24(3):608–616. doi: 10.1007/s10826-013-9871-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  68. Thrash, T. M., & Elliot, A. J. (2005). Delimiting and integrating achievement motive and goal constructs. Trends and Prospects in Motivation Research, 3–21. 10.1007/0-306-47676-2_1.
  69. Ueno K. The effects of friendship networks on adolescent depressive symptoms. Social Science Research. 2005;34(3):484–510. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2004.03.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  70. Vassilopoulos SP, Banerjee R. Interpretations and judgments regarding positive and negative social scenarios in childhood social anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2008;46(7):870–876. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Veed GJ, McGinley M, Crockett LJ. Friendship network influence on the development of internalizing symptoms during adolescence. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2019;60:157–165. doi: 10.1016/J.APPDEV.2018.09.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  72. Velásquez AM, Bukowski WM, Saldarriaga LM. Adjusting for group size effects in peer nomination data. Social Development. 2013;22(4):845–863. doi: 10.1111/sode.12029. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  73. Waldrip AM, Malcolm KT, Jensen-Campbell LA. With a little help from your friends: the importance of high-quality friendships on early adolescent adjustment. Social Development. 2008;17(4):832–852. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2008.00476.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Materials (17.8KB, docx)

Articles from Journal of Youth and Adolescence are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES