Abstract
Shared time is a centrally important component of relationship maintenance, and over the past few decades, couples have reported spending increasingly more time together. However, over this same time period divorce rates have risen much higher for lower-income couples compared to higher-income couples. One theorized explanation for the disparity in divorce rates between lower- and higher-income couples is a difference across the socioeconomic strata in the quantity and quality of time couples spend together. This theory argues that lower-income couples may experience a time deficit because they face a greater number of stressors that take up time, diminishing the quantity of time they have available to spend together. They may also need to use the time they do have available to deal with stressors rather than engaging in more enjoyable activities together, diminishing the quality of time they share. Using a sample of N = 14,788 respondents from the American Time Use Survey, the current study examined whether household income was associated with the quantity and quality of time married couples spend together. Consistent with predictions, lower-income couples spent less alone time together, but this was moderated by weekday vs weekend and presence of children. Lower-income couples also reported higher levels of stress during time spent with their spouse than higher-income couples, and this association was moderated by hours worked by the couple. Results support the theory, indicating that quantity and quality of time may be important factors in understanding differences in relationship outcomes between lower- and higher-income couples.
Keywords: time use, household income, couples, marriage, socioeconomic status
Lower-income couples are more likely to divorce compared to their higher earning counterparts, but the factors contributing to this disparity remain unclear (Trail & Karney, 2012). One theorized explanation for the socioeconomic disparity in divorce rates is a higher degree of “constraint on time use” experienced by lower-income couples. This theory, dubbed the “Two Route Model of Stress Effects on Marriage” suggests that the constraints that result from external stressors decrease the quantity of time that low-income couples have to spend together, and diminish the quality of the shared time they do have (Karney & Neff, 2013; Neff & Karney, 2017). Shared time between spouses is central to relationship maintenance because it provides the opportunity to engage in pro-relationship activities together. Thus, if shared spousal time differs by household income, then lack of shared quality time may be a factor contributing to the disproportionately high rate of divorce for lower-income couples. This theory is backed by suggestive evidence which indicates that couples who are facing greater demands outside the home share less quality time together in activities such as leisure and sex (Bodenmann et al., 2007; Crouter et al., 2001). However, the contention that quantity and quality of shared spousal time differs by household income has not been rigorously tested. Thus, the current study aims to examine whether there is support for the hypothesis put forth by the “Two Route Model of Stress” that lower-income couples have less quantity and quality of shared spousal time compared to higher-income couples.
Sharing time together is vitally important for romantic relationships because it allows for opportunities to engage in relationship maintenance behaviors such as sharing positive moments, providing social support, and building intimacy (Ogolsky & Bowers, 2013). Accordingly, shared spousal time is linked with relationship well-being concurrently and prospectively. Spouses who spend more time together have more positive relationship outcomes, including higher levels of relationship satisfaction, lower levels of conflict, and greater marital stability (Claxton & Perry-Jenkins, 2008; Girme et al., 2014; M. D. Johnson & Anderson, 2013). Shared time between spouses has been increasing over the past few decades, and shared leisure time in particular has increased, which should be a net positive for relationship well-being (Genadek et al., 2016; Voorpostel et al., 2010). However, the literature has been equivocal on whether shared spousal time differs across socioeconomic status: some studies indicate that higher-income couples have more shared time (e.g., Sevilla et al., 2012), whereas others indicate that higher-income couples spend less time together (e.g., Genadek et al., 2020).
Absent clear data indicating that lower-income couples face a shortage of time together, the “Two Route Model of Stress” makes this prediction based upon suggestive evidence that lower-income couples face a greater number of daily stressors external to their relationship that take up time (Roy et al., 2004). For example, low-income individuals face greater wait times for basic services such as shopping or medical care, and experience a disproportionate administrative paperwork burden compared to higher income individuals (Holt & Vinopal, 2021; Schanzenbach, 2009). They are also more likely to work nonstandard schedules, which leads to less time spent with their families in comparison to those with standard-schedule jobs because they are often at work when other family members have time off from work or school (Enchautegui, 2013). In addition, they are often asked to work shifts with little notice, which disrupts the time that they were going to spend with family members and often requires a stressful and time-consuming search for available childcare (Carrillo et al., 2017; Lambert, 2008). Low-income individuals are also more likely to use public transportation than higher earning individuals and have longer commutes to work (Anderson, 2016; Murphy et al., 2022; Roy et al., 2004). The time that low-income individuals spend dealing with these demands and stressors is time that is no longer available to spend with their spouse. Indeed, low-income couples report having difficulty in finding time to spend together (Trail & Karney, 2012), suggesting that the demands for their time outside of the relationship are negatively affecting the time they have together.
Higher earning couples appear to be at an advantage in their ability to spend time together; not only do they not face these stressors that eat away at their time, they are also more likely to be able to synchronize their schedules (Hamermesh, 2000) and more likely to have the ability to gain more time by making time-saving purchases (e.g., grocery delivery, laundry service). For example, women who hire household help spend less time on housework and experience less time pressure compared to women who do not outsource housework (Craig et al., 2016), and time saving purchases promote greater relationship satisfaction by enabling couples to spend more time together (Whillans et al., 2018). Thus, higher-income couples appear to be able to overcome demands on their time by outsourcing undesirable tasks to others, which frees up time to spend together.
The evidence reviewed thus far suggests that higher-income couples are likely to have more time to spend with their spouse than lower-income couples, consistent with the “Two Route Model of Stress” (Karney & Neff, 2013; Neff & Karney, 2017). However, there is also some evidence to suggest that the opposite might be true; it is possible that lower-income couples may be spending more time together than higher-income couples. For example, low-income individuals are more likely than higher-income individuals to work part-time or be unemployed (U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2021), and are less able to afford outside childcare, which often requires one parent to stay home (Mattingly et al., 2016). These factors lead to less time spent at work, and therefore potentially more time spent at home with family. In contrast, higher earning individuals tend to have occupations that require more time spent at work, and greater job demands (i.e., task pressure, job intensity, time urgency), all of which result in less time spent at home with family (Solomon et al., 2022). Thus, it is possible that the relatively longer hours worked by higher-income individuals may lead to a greater deficit in shared time among these couples, compared to lower earning couples.
Although the availability of shared spousal time is a necessary component of relationship well-being, it is not sufficient on its own. The quality of the time spent together, including whether it is experienced as satisfying, positive, and enjoyable, is highly influential. Quality time often involves activities such as shared leisure activities or relationship-enhancing activities that promote closeness between partners (Sullivan, 1996), and the more couples engage in these types of activities together, the greater their relationship quality (Crawford et al., 2002; Girme et al., 2014; H. A. Johnson et al., 2006). Although leisure activities that are more engaging or novel tend to increase an individual’s feelings of closeness towards their partner more than routine leisure tasks (Graham, 2008), partners’ satisfaction with their time together seems to be of greatest importance: satisfaction with shared spousal time is more predictive of marital satisfaction than the amount of time spent in shared activities (Ward et al., 2014).
However, the ability to spend time engaging in enjoyable activities with one’s spouse (i.e., quality time) may not be equally accessible to all. One factor that differentiates lower- and higher-income couples is the amount of daily external stressors that lower-income couples face, such as housework, informal caregiving, and managing finances (Cash et al., 2005; Jackson et al., 2016; Williams et al., 2003). Because lower-income couples face more challenges and stressors, they may be forced to spend more of their time engaged in activities such as housework and care work, rather than leisure activities, and they may experience their shared time as more stressful and less happy, regardless of the amount of time they have together (Saxbe et al., 2011).
In sum, one theory for the socioeconomic disparity in marital outcomes is that lower-income couples experience a higher degree of “constraint on time use” than higher-income couples. This constraint is theorized to manifest in a reduction in the quantity of time they have to spend together and in a decrease in the quality of the shared time they do have (Karney & Neff, 2013; Neff & Karney, 2017). To test this hypothesis, the present study uses time diary data from a large, nationally representative sample of married individuals gathered through the American Time Use Survey to examine whether the quantity and quality of time couples spend together differs by household income. Specifically, we test three research questions: 1) Does the amount of time spouses spend alone together differ by household income? 2) Do feelings of happiness and stress during time spent alone with one’s spouse differ by household income? and 3) Do factors known to be related to shared spousal time, including day of the week, presence of children in the home, and work hours, moderate the effect of income on quantity and quality of shared spousal alone time?
The “constraint on time use” theory does not specify whether exclusive spousal time (i.e., time spent with one’s spouse and no one else) or total spousal time (i.e., all time spent with one’s spouse, including exclusive time and time when others are present) is theorized to be the active mechanism. This is likely because the existing literature has not been clear about which version of spousal time is more important in determining relationship outcomes, and in fact, many studies are not even clear about which version they are measuring. Absent clear guidance from the literature, we have chosen to focus on exclusive spousal time because couples are most likely to be able to engage in important relationship maintenance activities, such as shared leisure time and sexual activity when they are alone together, and feelings of happiness and stress during time shared with others may be due to some other influence, such as engaging in parenting activities (Kahneman & Krueger, 2006; Negraia & Augustine, 2020).
Empirically testing the theory that low-income couples spend less time together, and that time spent together is of lower quality compared to higher earning couples, will help elucidate whether this is a potentially viable mechanism for explaining the disparity in divorce rates between lower- and higher-income couples.
Method
Sample
Data were drawn from the American Time Use Survey (ATUS), a nationally representative cross-sectional survey administered each year to a subset of respondents completing the Current Population Survey (CPS). For the current study, data were obtained using the IPUMS data extract system (Flood et al., 2022). Respondents reported on their activities for one 24-hour period, referred to as the diary day, via a semi-structured interview administered over the phone. The diary day began at 4:00 a.m. the prior day and ended at 4:00 a.m. on the day of the interview. Respondents reported on what activities they were doing, how long each activity lasted, where they were during the activity, and who was present for the activity. Analyses were limited to individuals who reported that they were married and that their spouse was present in the household at the time of the diary (i.e., not incarcerated, away on military service, etc.). Additionally, individuals reporting that they were retired were excluded from analyses because retired couples may have greater opportunities to spend time together regardless of household income status. In the years 2010, 2012, and 2013, an ancillary Well-Being Module was fielded to collect additional information about health and well-being. These additional questions (detailed below) are required to answer our research questions about quality of time. Thus, we use the combined sample of ATUS participants from 2010, 2012, and 2013 (N = 14,788) to test our hypotheses.
Measures
Income.
Household income was collected at the final Current Population Survey interview, which took place approximately 2–5 months prior to the time diary. Respondents were asked to report the combined income of all household members from the past 12 months. In order to account for differences in household sizes which result in different levels of effective income per person, we combine household income and household size to calculate the respondent’s income to poverty ratio. First, the federal poverty threshold for the household is determined using the number of people in the household and the year of data collection (Office of the Assistant Secretary for Planning and Evaluation, 2021), then the household income is divided by the poverty threshold. An income to poverty ratio of <1 means that the household’s income is below 100% of the federal poverty level meaning that the household is in poverty. A ratio of between 1 and 2 means that household’s income is between 100% and 200% of the federal poverty level, which is typically considered “low-income.” Finally, a higher ratio, such as 4 for example, means that the household’s income is 4 times the poverty level.
Quantity of time with spouse.
The total time, in minutes, respondents reported spending alone with their spouse during the 24-hour period was calculated by summing the number of minutes spent in each activity in which the spouse was the only other person present. The presence of others is not asked for times during which respondents reported sleeping, grooming, or engaging in personal/private activities, thus the total time with spouse is exclusive of these activities.
Quantity of time with spouse in leisure and housework activities.
The ATUS has a standard coding system that categorizes each reported activity into one of eighteen categories. Two activity groups representing activities of primary interest in the current study were created using existing procedures for combining the activity categories (e.g., Pepin et al., 2018). Leisure includes eating and drinking; socializing, relaxing, and leisure; sports, exercise, and recreation; religious and spiritual activities; and volunteer activities. Housework Activities includes household activities (e.g., cooking, cleaning); consumer purchases (e.g., shopping); professional and personal care services (e.g., legal, medical services); household services (e.g., hiring a plumber); and government services and civic obligations (e.g., filling out forms to receive compensation). Activities that ATUS classified into categories ‘telephone calls’ and ‘traveling’ were divided into the three activity categories according to the type of phone call/travel reported. For example, telephone calls to/from household service providers were included in the Housework Activities category.
Quality of time with spouse.
As part of the Well-Being Module, respondents were asked to rate the extent to which they experienced various emotions during three randomly selected activities on their diary day. The possible range of ratings was from 0 (not at all) to 6 (very much). To be selected, activities had to be at least 5 minutes long and could not be activities related to sleeping, grooming, or personal care. We used ratings of happiness and stress for activities that occurred with one’s spouse as a measure of the quality of time spent together.
Household children.
The ATUS provides a variable in which each respondent was coded Yes or No for whether they have children under age 18 residing in their household.
Total work hours.
Respondents reported the typical number of hours they and their spouse work each week at paid employment. These two numbers were summed to determine the total number of hours spent at work by the couple.
Transparency and Openness
We report how we determined our sample size, all data exclusions, all manipulations, and all measures in the study, and we follow JARS. Data and materials are available through IPUMS (https://www.atusdata.org/atus). Analysis code is available at https://osf.io/aq3tw/. Data were analyzed using Stata v17 (StataCorp, 2021). This study’s design and its analysis were not preregistered.
Analytic Plan
A series of ordinary least squares regression models were estimated, with person-level weights incorporated in all analyses to account for the complex sampling procedures of the ATUS and to make the estimates nationally representative (Flood et al., 2022). Calculations of quantity and quality of time also incorporated the well-being activity weights to account for additional sampling design procedures that determine selection of activities for the well-being module and account for non-response to the well-being module. In line with other studies using the ATUS dataset (e.g., Pepin et al., 2018), we also control for weekday, which is the day of the week that the time diary was completed, to account for differences in the amount of time spouses may spend together during the week versus weekend. A dichotomous measure with weekday coded as 1 and weekend coded as 0 was included in all analyses.
All models were initially fitted with a linear and squared term for household income in the model, to test for curvilinear effects of income. If the quadratic effect was non-significant it was dropped from the model and only a linear model was fitted.
Results
Descriptive statistics for sample demographics as well as the quantity of time and quality of time variables are provided in Table 1. The sample was restricted to couples in which neither partner was retired, thus participants were primarily in their 30s, 40s, and 50s (M = 45 years, SD = 12 years). The majority of participants were employed (80%), with 4% reporting that they were unemployed and looking for work, and 16% reporting that they were not in the labor force. Couples worked a cumulative 63 hours per week on average (SD = 27 hrs).
Table 1.
Descriptive Statistics of Analytic Sample
Variable | Mean (SD) or % |
---|---|
| |
Age (years) | 45.36 (11.68) |
Sex | 51% Male, 49% Female |
Race/Ethnicity | |
White (non-Hispanic) | 71.2% |
Hispanic | 15.7% |
Black | 6.8% |
Asian or Pacific Islander | 4.8% |
Multiracial | 0.8% |
Native American | 0.7% |
Education | |
Less than HS degree | 9.4% |
HS degree | 28.2% |
Some college or AA degree | 24.1% |
College degree | 24.0% |
Graduate degree | 14.3% |
Employment Status | |
Employed | 79.7% |
Unemployed | 4.2% |
Not in labor force | 16.1% |
Child in the household | 45.8% No, 54.2% Yes |
Total weekly hours worked by the couple | 62.9 (27.3) |
Income to poverty ratio | 3.4 (2.1) |
Total time spent alone together (minutes) | 151.8 (183.5) |
Leisure time spent alone together (minutes) | 105.0 (135.7) |
Housework time spent alone together (minutes) | 33.0 (76.0) |
Happiness during time spent alone with spouse | 4.5 (1.3) |
Stress during time spent alone with spouse | 1.1 (1.5) |
Note. Estimates incorporate survey weights.
N = 14,788.
The majority of participants were White non-Hispanic (71%), followed by Hispanic (16%), Black (7%), Asian or Pacific Islander (5%), Multiracial (<1%), and Native American (<1%). Participants had a broad range of formal education; 9% had less than a high school degree, 28% had a high school degree, 24% had some college or an associate’s degree, 24% had a college degree, and 14% had a graduate degree. Slightly more than half (54%) of couples had children in the home. The average income to poverty ratio was 3.4 (SD = 2.1), which indicates that the household income is 340% of the federal poverty level for their household size.
Quantity of time estimates are based on one 24-hour day or 1440 minutes. On average, respondents spent 152 minutes alone with their spouse, which is consistent with past estimates for exclusive spousal time which range from 63 minutes to 271 minutes (Genadek et al., 2016, 2020; Voorpostel et al., 2010). Of their alone time, couples spent 105 minutes on leisure activities, whereas 33 minutes were spent on housework. Ratings of happiness indicated that people were fairly happy when spending time alone with their spouse, with an average happiness rating of 4.5 (on a 0 – 6 scale). Ratings of stress indicated that people experienced relatively low levels of stress when spending time alone with their spouse, with an average level of 1.1 (on a 0 – 6 scale).
Associations Between Income and Quantity of Time
Table 2 presents results from the regression models testing the main effect of household income on the quantity of time spent with spouse, as well as the three moderation models. For the main effect of household income on total time with spouse, there was a significant quadratic effect of income, such that as household income increased, the amount of shared spousal time increased exponentially, as seen in Panel A of Figure 1. For households with an income to poverty ratio of 1 (that is, those living at the poverty line), spouses spent 144 minutes together. When the household income to poverty ratio increases to 5 (that is, those whose income is five times the poverty level), shared spousal time increases only a small amount, to 155 minutes, but when the household income to poverty ratio increases to 10 (that is, those whose income is ten times the poverty level), shared spousal time increases to 217 minutes.
Table 2.
Results of Regression Analyses for Quantity of Time Alone with Spouse
Main effect | Moderated by Day of Week | Moderated by Work Hours | Moderated by Child in Household | |
---|---|---|---|---|
|
||||
Income | −3.961 | 15.610*** | 5.666* | −5.293*** |
Income2 | 1.096** | -- | -- | -- |
Weekday | −68.780*** | −20.625** | −70.783*** | −70.479*** |
Weekday X Income | -- | −14.183*** | -- | -- |
Work Hours | -- | -- | −1.285*** | -- |
Work Hours X Income | -- | -- | 0.070 | -- |
Child | -- | -- | -- | −170.491*** |
Child X Income | -- | -- | -- | 6.287*** |
Constant | 196.059*** | 147.736*** | 246.752*** | 302.544*** |
R2 | 0.031 | 0.036 | 0.055 | 0.174 |
Note. Test statistics are from OLS regressions which incorporate survey weights.
N = 14,788
p < 0.05
p < 0.01
p < 0.001
Figure 1.
Results of Regression Analyses for Quantity of Time Alone with Spouse
Next, we tested whether the effect of income was moderated by weekday vs. weekend day, total hours worked by the couple, and the presence of children in the household. First, the interaction between income and work hours was not significant, indicating that the association between income and quantity of time was not moderated by the total hours worked by the couple.
Second, the interaction between income and day of week was significant, such that the association between income and quantity of time differs on weekdays compared to weekends. As shown in Panel B of Figure 1, the quantity of time spouses spend together on weekdays does not differ by level of income. However, the quantity of time spouses spend together on weekends significantly differs by income, such that higher-income couples spend significantly more time together on the weekend than lower-income couples. For households with an income to poverty ratio of 1, spouses spent 163 minutes together on weekend days. When the household income to poverty ratio increases to 5, shared spousal time increases to 226 minutes on weekend days, and when the household income to poverty ratio increases to 10, shared spousal time increases to 300 minutes on weekend days.
Finally, the interaction between income and presence of children was significant, such that the association between income and quantity of time differs depending on whether there is a child in the household. As shown in Panel C of Figure 1, for couples with children, the quantity of time spouses spend together does not differ by level of income. However, the quantity of time couples without children spend alone significantly differs by income, such that higher-income couples spend significantly less time together than lower-income couples. For households with an income to poverty ratio of 1, spouses with no children spent 247 minutes together. When the household income to poverty ratio increases to 5, shared spousal time for spouses with no children decreases to 226 minutes, and when the household income to poverty ratio increases to 10, shared spousal time for spouses with no children decreases to 199 minutes.
We also examined the main effect of household income on the quantity of time spouses spend alone together in leisure and housework. For leisure activities, there was a significant quadratic effect of income (coef = .68, p = .007) such that as household income increased, the amount of shared leisure time increased exponentially. For households with an income to poverty ratio of 1, spouses spent 100 minutes together in leisure. When the household income to poverty ratio increases to 5, shared spousal time in leisure increases only a small amount, to 107 minutes, but when the household income to poverty ratio increases to 10, shared spousal time in leisure increases to 147 minutes.
For housework, the quadratic effect of income was non-significant, but there was a significant linear effect of income (coef = .99, p = .011), such that shared housework time increased as income increased. For households with an income to poverty ratio of 1, spouses spent 31 minutes together in housework. When the household income to poverty ratio increases to 5, shared spousal time in housework increases to 35 minutes, and when the household income to poverty ratio increases to 10, shared spousal time in housework increases to 40 minutes.
Associations Between Income and Quality of Time
Table 3 presents the results from the regression models testing the association between household income and ratings of happiness during time spent alone with one’s spouse, as well as the three moderation models. Curvilinear effects of income were tested and were found to be nonsignificant for all models predicting level of happiness. In addition, there was no linear main effect of income on happiness during time with one’s spouse, and the three moderators were all nonsignificant, indicating that spouses were equally happy while spending time together, across all of these dimensions
Table 3.
Results of Regression Analyses for Happiness During Time Alone with Spouse
Main effect | Moderated by Day of Week | Moderated by Work Hours | Moderated by Child in Household | |
---|---|---|---|---|
|
||||
Income | −0.019 | −0.026* | 0.004 | −0.015 |
Weekday | −0.107* | −0.146 | −0.116* | −0.114* |
Weekday X Income | -- | 0.011 | -- | -- |
Work Hours | -- | -- | 0.004* | -- |
Work Hours X Income | -- | -- | −0.001 | -- |
Child | -- | -- | -- | 0.005 |
Child X Income | -- | -- | -- | −0.046 |
Constant | 4.665*** | 4.691*** | 4.457*** | 4.693*** |
R2 | 0.002 | 0.002 | 0.004 | 0.005 |
Note. Test statistics are from OLS regressions which incorporate survey weights.
N = 14,788
p < 0.05
p < 0.01
p < 0.001
Table 4 presents the results from the regression models testing the association between household income and ratings of stress during time spent alone with one’s spouse, as well as the three moderation models. Curvilinear effects of income were tested and were found to be nonsignificant for all models predicting level of stress. For the main effect of household income on total time with spouse, there was a significant linear effect of income such that stress decreased as income increased, as seen in Panel A of Figure 2.
Table 4.
Results of Regression Analyses for Stress During Time Alone with Spouse
Main effect | Moderated by Day of Week | Moderated by Work Hours | Moderated by Child in Household | |
---|---|---|---|---|
|
||||
Income | −0.045*** | −0.058*** | −0.150*** | −0.042** |
Weekday | 0.220*** | 0.146 | 0.206*** | 0.229*** |
Weekday X Income | -- | 0.020 | -- | -- |
Work Hours | -- | -- | −0.006** | -- |
Work Hours X Income | -- | -- | 0.002*** | -- |
Child | -- | -- | -- | 0.129 |
Child X Income | -- | -- | -- | 0.028 |
Constant | 1.153*** | 1.202*** | 1.531*** | 1.071*** |
R2 | 0.009 | 0.009 | 0.015 | 0.013 |
Note. Test statistics are from OLS regressions which incorporate survey weights.
N = 14,788
p < 0.05
p < 0.01
p < 0.001
Figure 2.
Results of Regression Analyses for Stress During Time Alone with Spouse
Next, we tested whether the effect of income was moderated by weekday vs. weekend day, total hours worked by the couple, and the presence of children in the household. First, the interactions between income and day of the week, and income and presence of children in the household were not significant, indicating that the association between income and stress during shared spousal time was not moderated by either of these factors.
Second, the interaction between income and total work hours was significant, such that the association between income and stress differs depending on the cumulative hours worked by the couple. As shown in Panel B of Figure 2, couples who worked a cumulative 80 hours per week experienced the same level of stress during time with their spouse, regardless of income (slope = −.02, p = .170). However, couples with fewer cumulative work hours experienced higher levels of stress at lower levels of income and lower levels of stress at higher levels of income (simple slope for 40 cumulative work hours = −.09, p < .001; simple slope for 0 cumulative work hours = −.15, p < .001).
Discussion
Shared spousal time is important for relationship wellbeing, and evidence suggests that overall couples are spending more time together than in decades past. However, the increase in shared spousal time may not have reached all couples across socioeconomic strata equally; one theorized explanation for the higher rate of divorce observed in lower-income couples is a “constraint on time use,” in which external stressors diminish the quantity and quality of shared spousal time for lower-income couples (Karney & Neff, 2013). Utilizing a nationally representative sample of respondents from the American Time Use Survey, the current study provided a direct test of the viability of this hypothesized mechanism by testing whether the quantity and quality of time spouses spend together differs by couples’ household income status.
The theory that lower-income couples suffer from greater constraints on their time use posits that these external constraints have two pathways through which they undermine marital well-being; “reducing opportunities for activities that promote and nourish the relationship, while simultaneously increasing opportunities for conflicts and tensions to arise” (Neff & Karney, 2017, p. 107). In regards to the first hypothesized pathway, that lower-income couples have reduced opportunities for activities that promote and nourish the relationship, we found evidence mostly in support of this claim. Overall, lower-income couples spent less time alone together than higher-income couples, and importantly, this was also true of leisure time spent alone with their spouse, which is a key opportunity for pro-relationship behavior and is consistently linked with positive relationship outcomes (Crawford et al., 2002; H. A. Johnson et al., 2006).
There were also important moderators of this pattern, including day of the week: the income differential in shared spousal time was only evident on weekends and not on weekdays. Lower-income couples had relatively limited alone time together regardless of whether it was a weekday or weekend, whereas higher-income couples had limited alone time together on weekdays, but much more plentiful alone time together on weekends. This is consistent with past research which indicates that higher-income couples have more control over their schedules and are better able to coordinate in order to spend time together (Chenu & Robinson, 2002; van Klaveren & van den Brink, 2007).
The effect of presence of children in the household was also tested, and consistent with past research there was a main effect on time, such that couples with children spent much less time alone together than couples without children (Dew, 2009; Flood & Genadek, 2016). We extend this past work by examining whether presence of children also moderated the effect of income. We found that couples with a child in the household had equal amounts of alone time together regardless of their income level, consistent with past research which also documented no differences in alone time together for lower-SES vs higher-SES parents (Fein, 2009). However, for couples without children, there was an income differential, such that lower-income couples had significantly more time together than higher-income couples. This result is not consistent with the theoretical model and indicates the need for more research specifically on lower-income couples without children, to better understand this group who are often overlooked in favor of research on low-income parents.
In regards to the second hypothesized pathway, that lower-income couples experience increased opportunities for conflicts and tensions to arise, we also found evidence tentatively in support of this claim. Our results indicate that lower-income couples experience the same amount of happiness together as higher-income couples, but they experience significantly higher levels of stress than higher-income couples. Couples who are experiencing high levels of stress are more likely to engage in more negative and less positive and effective communication, suggesting that spending stressful alone time together could create the opportunity for increased tension and conflict (Williamson et al., 2013). Further research is needed to understand what is transpiring during lower-income couples’ time together. Additionally, levels of stress were moderated by hours worked, such that lower-income couples who work fewer hours experienced more stress during time with their spouse, whereas those who work 80 cumulative hours experienced the same amount of stress as higher-income couples. This suggests that being unemployed or under-employed may play a key role in the stress experienced by lower-income couples.
Overall, results indicate that constraints on time use may be an important factor impacting the relationships of lower-income couples, which has important implications for interventions targeting these couples. These interventions have traditionally focused on improving the behaviors exchanged between partners (Hawkins & Erickson, 2015), but if partners are limited in the time that they have to spend together, then they have few opportunities to implement these behavioral strategies. Thus, an intervention that focuses on increasing the amount of time that partners have to spend together, by aligning work schedules, or increasing the resources available to make time-saving purchases, may be a more effective upstream strategy (Giurge et al., 2020; Whillans et al., 2017, 2018).
Despite the novel contributions of this work, it is not without limitations. A strength of the ATUS is that it provides highly precise estimates of time usage. However, the ATUS data is limited in its ability to inform our understanding of spousal interactions because it only asks respondents to report on who was present during each activity, but does not specify whether the individuals were engaged in the activity with the respondent or whether they were simply present in the room. More naturalistic observations of couples are needed to better understand the specific ways that lower-income couples experience their time together during their day-to-day lives (e.g., Saxbe et al., 2011). Additionally, the ATUS does not collect any data pertaining to relationship quality, precluding the ability to directly test whether the observed differences in quantity and quality of time contribute to relationship outcomes, or whether differences in relationship quality contribute to differences in quantity and quality of time. For example, it is conceivable that less satisfied couples may choose to spend less time together, or feel more stressed when alone with their partner. Furthermore, our sample consists of married individuals who are primarily middle-aged and employed, with many also raising children. This mid-life period is characterized by the need to balance multiple roles, such as caring for children and aging parents while investing in career success, which can make this a particularly busy and constrained period of the life course (Infurna et al., 2020). Thus, the results for shared spousal time obtained from our sample may not generalize to those in other periods of life, such as retirement or early adulthood. Finally, we chose to focus on exclusive shared spousal time as the aspect of shared time that is most relevant to the “constraints on time use” theory. However, other aspects of time shared between partners, such as family mealtime and seeing one’s partner being a good parent, are also important for family and relationship outcomes (Fiese, 2021; Fiese et al., 2006; Halpern-Meekin, 2019). Although it was beyond the scope of the current manuscript, it will be important to examine systematic differences across socioeconomic status in these types of shared time as well.
Notwithstanding these limitations, this study provides an empirical test of the theory that “constraints on time use” contribute to poor relationship outcomes among lower-income couples using a nationally representative sample of married individuals. Consistent with the theory, results show that lower-income couples spend less time alone with their spouse than higher-income couples, indicating that lack of time together is a potentially viable explanation for population level differences in relationship outcomes across SES. In addition, lower-income individuals reported experiencing higher levels of stress during time with their spouse compared to higher income individuals. Thus, it will be important moving forward to further examine the role of quantity and quality of time, including the effects of stress on shared spousal time, as a potential factor contributing to the disparity in divorce rates between lower- and higher-income couples.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by grant, P2CHD042849, Population Research Center, awarded to the Population Research Center at The University of Texas at Austin by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.
Footnotes
The authors have no known conflicts of interest to disclose. Data and materials are available through IPUMS (https://www.atusdata.org/atus/). Analysis code is available at https://osf.io/aq3tw. This study’s design and its analysis were not preregistered.
References
- Anderson M (2016). Who relies on public transit in the U.S. Pew Research Center. https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2016/04/07/who-relies-on-public-transit-in-the-u-s/
- Bodenmann G, Ledermann T, & Bradbury TN (2007). Stress, sex, and satisfaction in marriage. Personal Relationships, 14(4), 551–569. 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2007.00171.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Carrillo D, Harknett K, Logan A, Luhr S, & Schneider D (2017). Instability of work and care: How work schedules shape child-care arrangements for parents working in the service sector. Social Service Review, 91(3), 422–455. 10.1086/693750 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cash SJ, Sellers SL, & Claps M (2005). Money equals time: Influence of poverty status on hours spent doing housework. Journal of Poverty, 9(2), 89–109. 10.1300/J134v09n02_05 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Chenu A, & Robinson JP (2002). Synchronicity in the work schedules of working couples. Monthly Labor Review, 125(4), 55–63. https://www.jstor.org/stable/41845355 [Google Scholar]
- Claxton A, & Perry-Jenkins M (2008). No fun anymore: Leisure and marital quality across the transition to parenthood. Journal of Marriage and Family, 70(1), 28–43. 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2007.00459.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craig L, Perales F, Vidal S, & Baxter J (2016). Domestic outsourcing, housework time, and subjective time pressure: New insights from longitudinal data. Journal of Marriage and Family, 78(5), 1224–1236. 10.1111/jomf.12321 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Crawford DW, Houts RM, Huston TL, & George LJ (2002). Compatibility, leisure, and satisfaction in marital relationships. Journal of Marriage and Family, 64(2), 433–449. 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2002.00433.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Crouter AC, Bumpus MF, Head MR, & McHale SM (2001). Implications of overwork and overload for the quality of men’s family relationships. Journal of Marriage and Family, 63(2), 404–416. 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2001.00404.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dew J (2009). Has the marital time cost of parenting changed over time? Social Forces, 88(2), 519–542. 10.1353/sof.0.0273 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Enchautegui ME (2013). Nonstandard work schedules and the well-being of low-income families (Paper 26; Low-Income Working Families). Urban Institute. https://www.urban.org/research/publication/nonstandard-work-schedules-and-well-being-low-income-families [Google Scholar]
- Fein DJ (2009). Spending time together: Time use estimates for economically disadvantaged and nondisadvantaged married couples in the United States [Working Paper]. Abt Associates. https://www.acf.hhs.gov/opre/report/spending-time-together-time-use-estimates-economically-disadvantaged-and
- Fiese BH (2021). Family Mealtimes: Promoting Health and Well-being. In Francis LA, McHale SM, King V, & Glick JE (Eds.), Families, Food, and Parenting: Integrating Research, Practice and Policy (pp. 77–94). Springer International Publishing. 10.1007/978-3-030-56458-2_4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fiese BH, Foley KP, & Spagnola M (2006). Routine and ritual elements in family mealtimes: Contexts for child well-being and family identity. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2006(111), 67–89. 10.1002/cd.156 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flood SM, & Genadek KR (2016). Time for each other: Work and family constraints among couples. Journal of Marriage and Family, 78(1), 142–164. 10.1111/jomf.12255 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flood SM, Sayer LC, & Backman D (2022). American Time Use Survey Data Extract Builder: Version 3.1 [dataset]. College Park, MD: University of Maryland and Minneapolis, MN: IPUMS. 10.18128/D060.V3.1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Genadek KR, Flood SM, & Roman JG (2016). Trends in spouses’ shared time in the United States, 1965–2012. Demography, 53(6), 1801–1820. 10.1007/s13524-016-0512-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Genadek KR, Flood SM, & Roman JG (2020). Same-sex couples’ shared time in the United States. Demography, 57(2), 475–500. 10.1007/s13524-020-00861-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Girme YU, Overall NC, & Faingataa S (2014). “Date nights” take two: The maintenance function of shared relationship activities. Personal Relationships, 21(1), 125–149. 10.1111/pere.12020 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Giurge LM, Whillans AV, & West C (2020). Why time poverty matters for individuals, organisations and nations. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(10), 993–1003. 10.1038/s41562-020-0920-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Graham JM (2008). Self-expansion and flow in couples’ momentary experiences: An experience sampling study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(3), 679–694. 10.1037/0022-3514.95.3.679 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halpern-Meekin S (2019). Social Poverty: Low-Income Parents and the Struggle for Family and Community Ties. NYU Press. https://nyupress.org/9781479891214/social-poverty [Google Scholar]
- Hamermesh D (2000). Togetherness: Spouses’ synchronous leisure, and the impact of children (No. w7455). National Bureau of Economic Research. 10.3386/w7455 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hawkins AJ, & Erickson SE (2015). Is couple and relationship education effective for lower income participants? A meta-analytic study. Journal of Family Psychology, 29(1), 59–68. 10.1037/fam0000045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holt SB, & Vinopal K (2021). It’s about time: Examining the inequality in the time cost of waiting. Preprint. 10.31235/osf.io/jbk3x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Infurna FJ, Gerstorf D, & Lachman ME (2020). Midlife in the 2020s: Opportunities and challenges. American Psychologist, 75(4), 470–485. 10.1037/amp0000591 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jackson GL, Trail TE, Kennedy DP, Williamson HC, Bradbury TN, & Karney BR (2016). The salience and severity of relationship problems among low-income couples. Journal of Family Psychology, 30(1), 2–11. 10.1037/fam0000158 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson HA, Zabriskie RB, & Hill B (2006). The contribution of couple leisure involvement, leisure time, and leisure satisfaction to marital satisfaction. Marriage & Family Review, 40(1), 69–91. 10.1300/J002v40n01_05 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson MD, & Anderson JR (2013). The longitudinal association of marital confidence, time spent together, and marital satisfaction. Family Process, 52(2), 244–256. 10.1111/j.1545-5300.2012.01417.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kahneman D, & Krueger AB (2006). Developments in the measurement of subjective well-being. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 20(1), 3–24. 10.1257/089533006776526030 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Karney BR, & Neff LA (2013). Couples and stress: How demands outside a relationship affect intimacy within the relationship. In Simpson JA & Campbell L (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of close relationships (pp. 664–684). Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Lambert SJ (2008). Passing the buck: Labor flexibility practices that transfer risk onto hourly workers. Human Relations, 61(9), 1203–1227. 10.1177/0018726708094910 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mattingly M, Schaefer A, & Carson J (2016). Child care costs exceed 10 percent of family income for one in four families. The Carsey School of Public Policy at the Scholars’ Repository. 10.34051/p/2020.277 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy AK, McDonald-Lopez K, Pilkauskas N, & Gould-Werth A (2022). Transportation insecurity in the United States: A descriptive portrait. Socius, 8, 1–12. 10.1177/23780231221121060 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Neff LA, & Karney BR (2017). Acknowledging the elephant in the room: How stressful environmental contexts shape relationship dynamics. Current Opinion in Psychology, 13, 107–110. 10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.05.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Negraia DV, & Augustine JM (2020). Unpacking the parenting well-being gap: The role of dynamic features of daily life across broader social contexts. Social Psychology Quarterly, 83(3), 207–228. 10.1177/0190272520902453 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Office of the Assistant Secretary for Planning and Evaluation. (2021, March 16). Prior HHS Poverty Guidelines and Federal Register References. https://aspe.hhs.gov/prior-hhs-poverty-guidelines-and-federal-register-references
- Ogolsky BG, & Bowers JR (2013). A meta-analytic review of relationship maintenance and its correlates. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 30(3), 343–367. 10.1177/0265407512463338 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pepin JR, Sayer LC, & Casper LM (2018). Marital status and mothers’ time use: Childcare, housework, leisure, and sleep. Demography, 55(1), 107–133. 10.1007/s13524-018-0647-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roy KM, Tubbs CY, & Burton LM (2004). Don’t have no time: Daily rhythms and the organization of time for low-income families. Family Relations, 53(2), 168–178. 10.1111/j.0022-2445.2004.00007.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Saxbe DE, Repetti RL, & Graesch AP (2011). Time spent in housework and leisure: Links with parents’ physiological recovery from work. Journal of Family Psychology, 25(2), 271–281. 10.1037/a0023048 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schanzenbach DW (2009). Experimental estimates of the barriers to food stamp enrollment (No. 1367–09; Discussion Paper). Institute for Research on Poverty. https://www.irp.wisc.edu/resource/experimental-estimates-of-the-barriers-to-food-stamp-enrollment/ [Google Scholar]
- Sevilla A, Gimenez-Nadal JI, & Gershuny J (2012). Leisure inequality in the United States: 1965—2003. Demography, 49(3), 939–964. 10.1007/s13524-012-0100-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Solomon BC, Nikolaev BN, & Shepherd DA (2022). Does educational attainment promote job satisfaction? The bittersweet trade-offs between job resources, demands, and stress. Journal of Applied Psychology, 107(7), 1227–1241. 10.1037/apl0000904 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- StataCorp. (2021). Stata Statistical Software: Release 17. StataCorp LLC. [Google Scholar]
- Sullivan O (1996). Time co-ordination, the domestic division of labour and affective relations: Time use and the enjoyment of activities within couples. Sociology, 30(1), 79–100. 10.1177/0038038596030001006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Trail TE, & Karney BR (2012). What’s (not) wrong with low-income marriages. Journal of Marriage and Family, 74(3), 413–427. 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2012.00977.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2021). A profile of the working poor, 2019 (Report Number 1093; BLS Reports). https://www.bls.gov/opub/reports/working-poor/2019/pdf/home.pdf
- van Klaveren C, & van den Brink HM (2007). Intra-household work time synchronization. Social Indicators Research, 84(1), 39. 10.1007/s11205-006-9072-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voorpostel M, van der Lippe T, & Gershuny J (2010). Spending time together—Changes over four decades in leisure time spent with a spouse. Journal of Leisure Research, 42(2), 243–265. 10.1080/00222216.2010.11950204 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ward PJ, Barney KW, Lundberg NR, & Zabriskie RB (2014). A critical examination of couple leisure and the application of the core and balance model. Journal of Leisure Research, 46(5), 593–611. 10.1080/00222216.2014.11950344 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Whillans AV, Dunn EW, Smeets P, Bekkers R, & Norton MI (2017). Buying time promotes happiness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(32), 8523–8527. 10.1073/pnas.1706541114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whillans AV, Pow J, & Norton MI (2018). Buying time promotes relationship satisfaction (No. 18–072; Harvard Business School Working Paper; ). Harvard Business School. [Google Scholar]
- Williams A, Forbes D, Mitchell J, & Corbett B (2003). The influence of income on the experience of informal caregiving: Policy implications. Health Care for Women International, 24(4), 280–291. 10.1080/07399330390183606 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williamson HC, Karney BR, & Bradbury TN (2013). Financial strain and stressful events predict newlyweds’ negative communication independent of relationship satisfaction. Journal of Family Psychology, 27(1), 65–75. 10.1037/a0031104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]