Abstract
Objective:
Mild traumatic brain injury (mTBI) and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) commonly occur among military Service Members and Veterans and have heterogenous, but also overlapping symptom presentations, which often complicate the diagnoses of underlying impairments and development of effective treatment plans. Thus, we sought to examine whether the combination of whole brain gray matter (GM) and white matter (WM) structural measures with neuropsychological performance can aid in the classification of military personnel with mTBI and PTSD.
Method:
Active-Duty U.S. Service Members (n = 156; 87.8% male) with a history of mTBI, PTSD, combined mTBI+PTSD, or orthopedic injury completed a neuropsychological battery and T1- and diffusion-weighted structural neuroimaging. Cortical, subcortical, ventricular, and WM volumes and whole brain fractional anisotropy (FA), mean diffusivity (MD), radial diffusivity (RD), and axial diffusivity (AD) were calculated. Latent profile analyses were performed to determine how the GM and WM indicators, together with neuropsychological indicators, classified individuals.
Results:
For both GM and WM respectively, a four-profile model was the best fit. The GM model identified greater ventricular volumes in Service Members with cognitive symptoms, including those with a diagnosis of mTBI, either alone or with PTSD. The WM model identified reduced FA and elevated RD in those with psychological symptoms, including those with PTSD or mTBI and comorbid PTSD. However, contrary to expectation, a global neural signature unique to those with comorbid mTBI and PTSD was not identified.
Conclusion:
The findings demonstrate that neuropsychological performance alone is more robust in differentiating Active-Duty Service Members with mTBI and PTSD, whereas global neuroimaging measures do not reliably differentiate between these groups.
Keywords: Combat, Military, Traumatic brain injury, PTSD, Brain volumetric measures, Diffusion tensor imaging, Cognitive/Psychological Functioning, Latent profile analysis
Introduction
Up to 20% of U.S. Service Members report traumatic brain injury (TBI) or probable TBI1-4, with ~80% or more being mild TBI (mTBI)5. MTBI often results in varied symptoms including headache, decreased processing speed, memory and attention performance, and psychosocial symptoms2,6,7. Many military personnel with mTBI will recover to normal function, but a significant proportion experience chronic cognitive complaints2,8. Volumetric cortical and subcortical gray matter (GM) abnormalities are typically associated with moderate and severe TBI, but subtle changes are noted in the cingulate, hippocampus, amygdala, and basal ganglia after mTBI, although location and extent of volume loss are inconsistent across studies9. Altered GM shape characteristics have also been found in the basal ganglia and thalamus showing unique associations with attention, processing speed, memory, and subjective symptoms in Service Members with mTBI10-12.
However, white matter (WM) microstructure is more vulnerable in mTBI as the injury causes stretching and shearing of axons, resulting in traumatic axonal injury (e.g., damage to cytoskeletal structure and axonal membranes), which disrupts cell function and impairs neurotransmission13. Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) can be used to detect axonal injury by measuring alterations in global WM microstructure14,15. Fractional anisotropy (FA), a commonly assessed scalar DTI metric, indicates the directional preference of diffusion where higher values indicate greater diffusion along the axon than across it, reflecting more intact axonal structure14,16. Military personnel with mTBI show heterogenous and spatially distributed patterns of reduced FA in regions including the corpus callosum (CC), corona radiata (CR), superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF), internal capsule (IC), and posterior thalamic radiation (PTR)17-19 which are associated with declines in verbal memory and executive functioning17,20. Other scalar diffusivity metrics can provide further WM microstructure characterization. Specifically, mean diffusivity (MD) represents the overall level of diffusion while radial diffusivity (RD) and axial diffusivity (AD) reflect diffusion perpendicular and parallel to the axon, respectively14,16. However, the direction of change of diffusion metrics and WM regions across studies are inconsistent, potentially due to injury heterogeneity (e.g., injury mechanism, pathophysiological processes, clinical symptoms)13,21.
Understanding the effects of mTBI in military personnel is further complicated by the presence of comorbid psychiatric conditions such as post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Up to half of Veterans from Operation Enduring Freedom and Operation Iraqi Freedom (OEF/OIF) with combat-related mTBI meet criteria for PTSD3,22, and deployment-related mTBI increases the risk of PTSD4,22. PTSD alone contributes to cognitive compromise2,23 and reduced health-related quality of life24. MTBI co-morbid with PTSD presents challenges for diagnosis and treatment due to the high degree of symptom overlap (e.g., fatigue, mood disturbances, memory dysfunction etc.)25,26. Moreover, the presence of mTBI and PTSD together may exacerbate psychological symptoms leading to more severe impairments in cognitive and psychological functioning27-29. Similar brain structures are implicated in mTBI and PTSD and may contribute to these impairments. Limbic structures including the hippocampus, amygdala, cingulate, and their WM connections are vulnerable in both mTBI and PTSD as these regions are located in areas particularly susceptive to biomechanical forces and are involved in affective and cognitive processing28,30,31. Greater GM and WM microstructure abnormalities in limbic regions are associated with more severe PTSD symptoms in comorbid mTBI/PTSD31-33.
We previously used latent profile analysis (LPA) to characterize cognitive and psychological performance in U.S. Active-Duty Service Members with mTBI, PTSD, and mTBI with comorbid PTSD34. The neuropsychological indicators identified five classes based on symptom presentation: high functioning, normal functioning, cognitive symptoms, psychological symptoms, and combined cognitive and psychological symptoms. These symptom clusters closely aligned with the diagnostic classifications, but also demonstrated heterogeneity within diagnostic groups. Critically, those with mTBI with comorbid PTSD were almost split evenly among the cognitive symptoms, psychological symptoms, combined cognitive and psychological symptoms, and normal functioning groups. Here, we extend these analyses to include GM and WM measures to determine whether the addition of these metrics can aid classification of participants. Given the heterogeneity of mTBI, PTSD, and comorbid mTBI and PTSD, focusing on the individual patient’s specific symptoms and corresponding GM/WM metrics, rather than diagnosis, may be a more useful guide for selecting and evaluating treatment strategies. We focused on whole brain, global measures of GM and WM brain structures as these measures of brain health are easier for clinicians to integrate into their assessments.
Materials and Methods
Participants
Participants (N = 156; 87.8% male) were drawn from a large sample of U.S. Active-Duty Service Members recruited from a military treatment facility, as previously described34. Participants were included in the current study if they had completed a neuroimaging assessment. The study consisted of four groups including: 1) mTBI only, 2) mTBI with PTSD symptoms, 3) PTSD only, and 4) orthopedic controls (Table 1). The study protocol was approved by the local hospital IRB and the Human Research Protection Office at the U.S. Army Medical Research and Materiel Command. All participants provided informed consent prior to participation.
Table 1.
Demographic Characteristics.
All | OI Control | PTSD | mTBI | mTBI+PTSD | Significance | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N | 156 | 60 | 22 | 30 | 44 | |
Age | 35.3 (8.4) | 37.4 (7.3) | 37.6 (6.2) | 29.6 (6.5) | 35.2 (10.3) |
F = 7.07, p < .001 (mTBI group different) |
Sex (Male/Female) | 137 / 19 | 48 / 12 | 20 / 2 | 29 / 1 | 40 / 4 | χ2 = 6.21, p = .10 |
Race | ||||||
White | 106 [67.9%] | 38 [63.3%] | 13 [59.1%] | 23 [76.7%] | 32 [72.7%] |
χ2 = 13.70, p = .033 (Bonferroni corrected post-hocs not significant, but OI group had more Black/African-American and less Other race) |
Black or African-American | 29 [18.6%] | 18 [30%] | 3 [13.6%] | 2 [6.7%] | 6 [13.6%] | |
Other race | 21 [13.5%] | 4 [6.7%] | 6 [27.3%] | 5 [16.7%] | 6 [13.6%] | |
Ethnicity | ||||||
Hispanic or Latino | 44 [28.2%] | 13 [21.7%] | 10 [45.5%] | 9 [30%] | 12 [27.3%] | χ2 = 4.57, p = .21 |
Not Hispanic or Latino | 112 [71.8%] | 47 [78.3%] | 12 [54.5%] | 21 [70%] | 32 [72.7%] | |
Education | ||||||
High School / GED | 68 [43.6%] | 15 [25.0%] | 8 [36.4%] | 22 [73.3%] | 23 [52.3%] |
χ2 = 24.23, p < .001 (mTBI group had more high school and less college and post graduate degrees) |
College (Associates or Bachelor’s) | 63 [40.4%] | 29 [48.3%] | 10 [45.5%] | 8 [26.7%] | 16 [36.4%] | |
Post Graduate Degree | 25 [16%] | 16 [26.7%] | 4 [18.2%] | 0 [0%] | 5 [11.4%] | |
Years of Military Service | 13.2 (7.6) | 15.7 (6.7) | 14.5 (5.9) | 7.7 (5.7) | 13.1 (8.9) |
F = 8.81, p <.001 (mTBI group different) |
Number of Deployments | 2 (1) | 2 (0.9) | 2.1 (1.3) | 1.9 (0.9) | 2.1 (1.1) | F = 0.28, p = .84 |
PTSD Checklist Military Version | 42.1 (18.5) | 27.2 (12.4) | 60.6 (11.7) | 33.6 (7.1) | 58.9 (10.2) |
F = 100.09, p < .001 (All groups differed except PTSD and mTBI+PTSD) |
Mechanism of Injury | ||||||
Blast | - | - | - | 21 [70%] | 22 [50%] | χ2 = 3.75, p = .29 |
Vehicular | - | - | - | 1 [3.3%] | 6 [13.6%] | |
Fall | - | - | - | 3 [10%] | 6 [13.6%] | |
Other | - | - | - | 5 [16.7%] | 10 [22.7%] | |
Number of Prior TBI Reported | - | - | - | 2.4 (4.2) | 3.3 (4.4) | t = 0.83, p = .41 |
Time Since Injury (Months) | - | - | - | 10.3 (5.6) | 10.1 (6) | t = 0.10, p = .92 |
Loss of Consciousness | - | - | - | 11 [36.7%] | 20 [45.5%] | χ2 = 1.40, p = .50 |
Post-Traumatic Amnesia | - | - | - | 1 [3.3%] | 6 [13.6%] | χ2 = 2.21, p = .14 |
Note: Mean with standard deviation in parentheses. Frequency with percentage in square brackets. One OI control was missing data for deployments. Two participants (1 mTBI and 1 mTBI+PTSD) had missing data for number of prior TBI. One mTBI+PTSD had missing data for loss of consciousness.
mTBI Participants:
mTBI participants (n=74; 69 males, 5 females) were Active-Duty Service Members who took part in a large cognitive rehabilitation clinical trial study (Study of Cognitive Rehabilitation Effectiveness (SCORE!)35. The diagnosis of mTBI was made using Veteran Affairs (VA)/Department of Defense Clinical Practice Guidelines6 following a screening interview and medical record review by experienced TBI and Concussion Center medical staff. Furthermore, each participant was required to have persistent cognitive symptoms which was defined as having a Neurobehavioral Symptom Inventory (NSI) score of 3 or higher on any of the four cognitive symptoms35. Participants were included if they were 18 to 55 years old, sustained a closed head injury during deployment (OEF/OIF/Operation New Dawn [OND]) activities 3 to 24 months prior to recruitment, and were able to understand and communicate in English. Given that mTBI often coexists with PTSD, we further classified our mTBI group into mTBI with potential comorbid PTSD using the PTSD Checklist - Military Version (PCL-M) cut-off score of >4436-38. This resulted in a mTBI-only group (n=30; 29 male) and a mTBI with PTSD group (n=44; 40 male).
PTSD Participants:
PTSD participants (n=22; 20 males, 2 females) included Active-Duty Service Members recruited through the Department of Behavioral Health Clinic. Potential participants underwent a screening interview and medical record review and were excluded if they had a previous closed head injury. PTSD control participants were required to have a deployment related Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS; DSM-IV criteria) confirmed diagnosis of PTSD. In addition, PTSD participants were selected using similar inclusion criteria including age range, deployment history, and English communication skills.
Orthopedic Participants:
Orthopedic injury controls (n=60; 48 males, 12 females) were Active-Duty Service Members recruited through the Orthopedic Clinic. Potential participants underwent a screening interview and medical record review and were excluded if they had a previous closed head injury and/or a PTSD diagnosis according to the standardized CAPS. In addition, orthopedic injury control participants were selected using similar inclusion criteria including age range, deployment history, and English communication skills.
Participants were excluded (regardless of diagnostic group) if they had neurologic comorbidities (i.e., seizures, psychosis), history of moderate/severe TBI, spinal cord injury, were on scheduled narcotic pain medications, unable to use dominant hand, or had abnormal MRI findings. Attempts were made to include patients in each group such that the groups would be similar in age, rank, and sex distribution.
Measures
Demographic, Clinical, and Cognitive Variables.
Demographic information (e.g., age, sex, education, number of deployments, years in service) and injury history (e.g., time since injury, loss/alteration of consciousness, post-traumatic amnesia, mechanism of injury, number of prior injuries) were collected. Participants completed self-report psychological questionnaires including the Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test (AUDIT), Neurobehavioral Symptom Inventory (NSI), PCL-M, and the Depression and Anxiety subscales from the Symptom Checklist 90 (SCL-90). Technician-administered assessments were used to assess cognitive performance: memory: Total Recall, Short Delay Free Recall, Long Delay Free Recall and Recognition Hits from the California Verbal Learning Test-II (CVLT-II); verbal fluency: Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS Category and Letter Fluency subtests); attention: Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT); executive function: time to complete Trail Making Test Part B minus Part A (TMT B-A); working memory and processing speed: Working Memory Index (Digit Span and Letter-Number Sequencing) and Processing Speed Index (Coding and Symbol Search) from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale 4th edition (WAIS-IV). Mean self-report scores or mean performance are reported in Supplemental Table 1.
Neuroimaging Acquisition.
Multimodal neuroimaging assessments were collected on a single 3T Siemens Verio Syngo scanner with 32-channel head coil. MRI and cognitive assessments were collected concurrently (same week, mode=1 day). T1-weighted and DTI sequences were analyzed in the current study.
The T1-weighted sequence was acquired with the following parameters: field of view (FOV)=256 mm, repetition time (TR)=2300 ms, echo time (TE)=2.98 ms, flip angle=9°, and slice thickness=1 mm. The DTI sequence was collected using a clinical TBI protocol with the following acquisition parameters: 64 noncollinear directions, baseline acquisition sequences at b=0 s/mm with diffusion-weighting sequences at b=1000 s/mm, 1.8 x 1.8mm in-plane resolution with slice thickness of 4mm, TR=6300 ms, TE=96 ms, FOV=203 mm. T1-weighted data were available from 156 participants and DTI data were available from 144 participants.
Volumetric Image Processing and Analysis.
The T1-weighted images were visually inspected for artifacts including motion, complete anatomy coverage, and inhomogeneities, and there were no observable lesions on the images. The raw DICOM images were processed in Freesurfer v6.0.0. (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/) with the recon-all command, resulting in fully segmented and labeled images. The processed images were visually inspected for any errors (e.g., skull stripping, segmentation, labeling) and no manual editing was necessary. Volumetric data were extracted from the aseg.stats file and the variables SubCorGrayVol, CortexVol and CerebralWhiteMatterVol were used to estimate volumes of subcortical GM, cortical GM, and WM, respectively, based on the standard Freesurfer atlas 39. Total ventricular volume was calculated by summing the Freesurfer variables representing the lateral, inferior lateral, third and fourth ventricles. All volumes were corrected for total intracranial volume, and thus all values inputted for analysis were a ratio.
DTI Processing and Analysis.
Enhancing NeuroImaging Genetics through Meta-Analysis (ENIGMA)-DTI protocols were implemented for processing and extraction of scalar metrics from diffusion-weighted images 40. For each participant, diffusion images underwent standardized preprocessing steps including eddy current correction, brain extraction and masking, and tensor fitting41,42. Data were then processed using fMRI Software Library (FSL) tract-based spatial statistics41,42 tools though predefined ENIGMA templates and atlases were used in the registration and region of interest (ROI) mapping steps. The ENIGMA-ROI extraction protocol uses the Johns Hopkins University WM atlas43 to extract ROI values as well as average scalar metrics (i.e., FA, MD, AD, and RD) across the entire WM skeleton. Only the average whole brain metrics were used in the main analyses.
Statistical Approach
LPA was used to examine the sample based on neuroimaging and neuropsychological indicators. Model 1 included the GM metrics (subcortical GM, cortical GM, ventricular volume) and 15 cognitive and psychological indicators. Ventricular volume was included in the GM model as ventricular enlargement can be an indirect measure of cerebral atrophy more generally44. Model 2 included the WM metrics (WM volume, average FA, MD, RD, AD) and the 15 cognitive and psychological indicators. Models were fitted in MPlus Version 8.6. The final models were selected based on the model fit statistics (Akaike Information Criterion [AIC], Bayesian Information Criterion [BIC], and sample-size adjusted BIC values [SABIC]45, Lo-Mendell-Rubin Likelihood [LMR]46, Bootstrapped Likelihood Ratio Test [BLRT]47, and entropy45) and conceptual meaning (for full description of model fitting procedures, see Supplemental Material and Esopenko et al.34). GM/WM and neuropsychological indicators are presented so higher values indicate higher volumes/scalar metrics and better performance, respectively.
Post-estimation provided information on the proportion of mTBI, PTSD, mTBI+PTSD and orthopedic controls for each of the identified classes. For each LPA model, one-way ANOVAs with class membership as the between-subjects variable were used to assess separation between classes. Significant ANOVAs were followed with Tukey’s HSD post-hoc tests to determine significant differences between classes (p < 0.05). Semantic labels for class profiles were assigned based on previous findings in the larger sample34. Labels were determined by two neuropsychologists who were blinded to participant representation (i.e., clinical diagnosis group) and based on performance across neuropsychological indicators within each class. For example, normal cognitive function with high psychological symptoms was labelled Psychological Symptoms class.
Exploratory post-hoc analyses with GM and WM ROIs were performed to further explore the whole brain LPA results and are described in the Supplemental Material.
Results
Model 1: GM
Class Determination.
The structural GM measures and neuropsychological indicator variables were fit into latent profile models with k=2 to 5 classes. Models with 2 to 4 classes replicated the log-likelihood values and were well-identified. The 5-class model had untrustworthy standard error parameter estimates, indicating poor model identification. The 4-class model minimized the log-likelihood values, AIC, BIC, and SABIC relative to the 2- and 3-class models. The BLRT demonstrated that the 4-class model performed significantly better than a 3-class model. While the LMR for the 4-class model was not significant, the entropy values were well above the .8 threshold and were relatively stable for all k-class models up to k=4 (Table 2). One-way ANOVAs comparing class membership for the 4-class model demonstrated good separation between the classes for the neuropsychological indicators (p’s < 0.007) and for ventricular volume (p=0.04), but not for the subcortical and cortical GM indicators.
Table 2.
Model Fit Statistics for Gray Matter and White Matter Latent Profile Analyses.
Classes | Class Proportions | Loglikelihood | AIC | BIC | SABIC | BLRT | p BLRT | LMR | p LMR | Entropy |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
GM Model (n = 156) | ||||||||||
2 | 0.56 / 0.44 | −8539.163 | 17188.33 | 17356.07 | 17181.98 | 619.91 | p<.001 | 613.52 | .002 | .933 |
3 | 0.33 / 0.49 / 0.19 | −8435.054 | 17018.11 | 17243.80 | 17009.56 | 208.22 | p<.001 | 206.07 | .337 | .933 |
a4 | 0.21 / 0.38 / 0.22 / 0.19 | −8340.402 | 16866.80 | 17150.44 | 16856.07 | 189.30 | p<.001 | 187.35 | .194 | .947 |
5 | Model not well-identified | |||||||||
WM Model (n = 144) | ||||||||||
2 | 0.54 / 0.46 | −8419.603 | 16961.21 | 17142.37 | 16949.35 | 548.95 | p<.001 | 543.74 | .003 | .922 |
3 | 0.51 / 0.33 / 0.15 | −8316.315 | 16796.63 | 17040.16 | 16780.69 | 206.58 | p<.001 | 204.62 | .178 | .933 |
a4 | 0.19 / 0.19 / 0.24 / 0.38 | −8231.739 | 16669.48 | 16975.37 | 16649.45 | 169.15 | p<.001 | 167.55 | .417 | .939 |
5 | 0.16 / 0.23 / 0.33 / 0.15 / 0.13 | −8168.981 | 16585.96 | 16954.22 | 16561.85 | 125.51 | p<.001 | 124.32 | .348 | .956 |
6 | Model not replicated |
Note: GM = Gray matter; WM = White matter; AIC = Akaike Information Criterion; BIC = Bayesian Information Criterion; SABIC = sample-size adjusted BIC; BLRT = Bootstrapped Likelihood Ratio Test; LMR = Lo-Mendell-Rubin Likelihood.
Lower values of AIC, BIC, SABIC demonstrate better model fit. pBLRT < .05 indicates that the k-class model is a better fit to the data than the k-1 class model. pLMR small p-values indicate that the k-class model fits better to data than the k-1 class model. Entropy value close to 1 indicates excellent classification of subjects into latent classes.
Denotes the selected models. Although the 5-class WM model (under WM model) is the best fit model regarding the fit indices, it does not demonstrate the appropriate separation of the classes compared to 4-class model.
Model Interpretation.
Figure 1 panel A demonstrates how the GM and neuropsychological indicators load for the 4-class model (labels adopted from Esopenko et al.34). The neuropsychological indicators loaded according to the classes: 1) High Functioning, 2) Normal Functioning, 3) Cognitive Symptoms, 4) Psychological Symptoms. Military personnel demonstrating cognitive symptoms showed elevated ventricular volumes (p=.031) relative to the normal functioning group. There were no other group differences. Age and education did not differ between the classes.
Figure 1. Gray matter (GM) model.
Panel A. Standardized means plotted for the latent profile analysis with GM and neuropsychological indicators. The 4-class GM model identified high functioning, normal functioning, psychological symptoms, and cognitive deficit classes. Right inset: A magnified view of the class loadings of the gray matter indicators for the 4-class GM model. Panel B. Distribution of participants from each diagnostic classification to each class for the GM model. Left: The allocation of participants from each diagnostic class to each of the classes of the 4-class GM model. The thickness of each line between diagnostic group and class represents number of participants (thicker line = more participants). Right: The percentage of class assignments for each diagnostic classification in the 4-class GM model. GM Indicators: Subcortical GM = total subcortical gray matter volume; Cortical GM = total cortical gray matter volume; Ventricles = total ventricular volume. Psychological Indicators: AUDIT = Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test; NSI = Neurobehavioral Symptom Inventory; PCL-M = Posttraumatic stress disorder Checklist – Military Version; Depression = Symptom Checklist 90 (SCL-90) Depression subscale; Anxiety = SCL-90 Anxiety Subscale; Cognitive Indicators: Total Recall = California Verbal Learning Test-II (CVLT-II) total recall; Short Delay = CVLT-II short delay free recall; Long Delay = CVLT-II long delay free recall; Recog Hits = CVLT-II recognition hits; Cat Fluency = Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS) category fluency; Lett Fluency = D-KEFS Letter Fluency; PASAT = Paced auditory serial addition test; TMT B-A = Trail Making Test Part B minus Part A; Proc speed = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV (WAIS-IV) – Processing Speed Index; Work Mem = WAIS-IV Working Memory Index. OI Control = orthopedic injury control; PTSD = post-traumatic stress disorder only; mTBI = mild traumatic brain injury only; mTBI+PTSD = comorbid mTBI and PTSD.
Diagnostic Classification.
Figure 1 panel B shows how participants from each diagnostic category were distributed to each class for the 4-class model, which was similar to the distribution in the neuropsychological model34.
Exploratory post-hoc analyses did not significantly differentiate subcortical ROI indicators (see Supplemental Material).
Model 2: WM
Class Determination.
The structural WM measures and neuropsychological indicator variables were fit into latent profile models with k=2 to 6 classes. Models with 2 to 5 classes replicated the log-likelihood values and were well-identified; the 6-class model was not. The 5-class model minimized the log-likelihood values, AIC, BIC, and SABIC relative to the other models. The BLRT demonstrated that the 5-class model performed significantly better than a 4-class model. While the LMR for the 5-class model was not significant, the entropy values were well above the .8 threshold and were relatively stable for all k-class models up to k=5 (Table 2). One-way ANOVAs comparing class membership for the 5-class model demonstrated good separation between the classes for the neuropsychological indicators (p’s < 0.004), but not for the WM indicators (p’s > 0.089). Only the 4-class model demonstrated separation between the classes for the neuropsychological indicators (p’s < 0.015) as well as FA (p=0.024) and RD (p=0.025) and thus was selected as the best fitting model. Age and education did not differ between the classes.
Model Interpretation.
Figure 2 panel A demonstrates how the WM and neuropsychological indicators load for the 4-class model (labels adopted from Esopenko et al.34). The neuropsychological indicators loaded according to the classes: 1) High Functioning, 2) Cognitive Symptoms, 3) Psychological Symptoms, 4) Combined Cognitive and Psychological Symptoms. Relative to the high functioning group, the psychological symptoms subgroup demonstrated similar WM volumes, but altered WM microstructure with reduced FA (p=0.018) and elevated RD (p=0.034). There were no other group differences.
Figure 2. White matter (WM) model.
Panel A: Standardized means plotted for the latent profile analysis with WM and neuropsychological indicators. The 4-class WM model identified high functioning, cognitive symptoms, psychological symptoms, and combined cognitive / psychological symptom classes. Right inset: A magnified view of the class loadings of the WM indicators for the 4-class WM model. Panel B: Distribution of participants from each diagnostic classification to each class for the WM model. Left: The allocation of participants from each diagnostic class to each of the classes of the 4-class WM model. The thickness of each line between diagnostic group and class represents number of participants (thicker line = more participants). Right: The percentage of class assignments for each diagnostic classification in the 4-class WM model. WM Indicators: WM Vol = Total white matter volume; FA = Fractional anisotropy; MD = Mean diffusivity; AD = Axial diffusivity; RD = Radial diffusivity. Psychological Indicators: AUDIT = Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test; NSI = Neurobehavioral Symptom Inventory; PCL-M = Posttraumatic stress disorder Checklist – Military Version; Depression = Symptom Checklist 90 (SCL-90) Depression subscale; Anxiety = SCL-90 Anxiety Subscale; Cognitive Indicators: Total Recall = California Verbal Learning Test-II (CVLT-II) total recall; Short Delay = CVLT-II short delay free recall; Long Delay = CVLT-II long delay free recall; Recog Hits = CVLT-II recognition hits; Cat Fluency = Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS) category fluency; Lett Fluency = D-KEFS Letter Fluency; PASAT = Paced auditory serial addition test; TMT B-A = Trail Making Test Part B minus Part A; Proc speed = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV (WAIS-IV) – Processing Speed Index; Work Mem = WAIS-IV Working Memory Index. OI Control = orthopedic injury control; PTSD = post-traumatic stress disorder only; mTBI = mild traumatic brain injury only; mTBI+PTSD = comorbid mTBI and PTSD.
Diagnostic Classification.
Figure 2 panel B shows how participants from each diagnostic category were distributed to each class for the 4-class model. The distribution was similar to the GM model as well as the neuropsychological model34.
Exploratory post-hoc analyses showed that the psychological symptoms and combined cognitive and psychological symptoms groups demonstrated reduced FA in the splenium of the CC, CR, and SLF compared to the cognitive symptoms and high functioning groups. Further, the psychological symptoms group had reduced FA in the IC and PTR compared to the cognitive symptoms and high functioning groups (see Supplemental Material).
Discussion
MTBI and PTSD commonly occur in OEF/OIF Veterans3,22 and have diverse, but overlapping effects on neuropsychological outcomes25,26 as well as on brain structure30. These effects are often exacerbated in those with a history of mTBI and comorbid PTSD27-29,31-33. We previously showed that neuropsychological performance can differentiate military personnel with mTBI and/or PTSD into distinct symptom profiles 34. Here, we extended that work to examine whether the addition of whole brain GM and WM neuroimaging metrics with neuropsychological performance better cluster military personnel with mTBI and PTSD into distinct profiles.
The findings mostly reproduced the neuropsychological results reported by Esopenko et al.34 in a smaller subset of the sample, demonstrating the robustness of neuropsychological measures to classify participants. However, our results indicated that the neuroimaging metrics were less able to differentiate classes, as evidenced by the stark distinction between the performance across neuropsychological indicators but less distinct differences in GM and WM metrics between the classes. Moreover, compared to the neuropsychological only model described in Esopenko et al.34, the GM model omitted the combined cognitive and psychological symptoms class, whereas the WM model omitted the normal functioning class. This may be partially due to the smaller sample size resulting in 48% of Active-Duty Service Members in the combined cognitive and psychological symptoms and 36% in the normal functioning classes in the neuropsychological model not being included in the structural models due to missing neuroimaging data, likely reducing the sensitivity of the models to detect these classes. However, the neuropsychological measures were better able to differentiate classes relative to global structural variables given the limited sample size, likely due to the inclusion of multiple measures that captured subtle distinctions in cognitive and psychological function. Nonetheless, the GM model identified elevated ventricular volumes in Active-Duty Service Members with cognitive symptoms, while the WM model identified reduced FA and elevated RD in those with psychological symptoms.
Across models, the cognitive symptoms group primarily contained military personnel with mTBI, either alone or with PTSD. In the GM model, only the ventricles showed differences between the classes, with the cognitive symptoms group (~40% with mTBI; ~32% mTBI+PTSD) showing larger ventricles relative to the other classes, yet the WM pattern was less distinct for the cognitive symptoms group. This is consistent with past work indicating that ventricular size is an indicator of gross, overall brain health44, with larger size typically associated with increased age and/or reduced health. While a longitudinal study of military Service Members with mTBI showed no differences in ventricle-to-brain ratios compared to no mTBI history48, the results may support that ventricular expansion is predictive of cognitive complaints49. Contrary to these results, we had expected a more definitive structural injury related to mTBI encompassing cortical and subcortical GM and WM. Exploratory post-hoc analyses did not reveal subcortical specific effects of mTBI, possibly reflecting the gross nature of GM subcortical volume measurements. The exploratory analyses also did not distinguish the region-specific FA measures in the cognitive symptoms group (see Supplemental Material). The lack of effect for WM may be due to the heterogenous effects of mTBI on these structures across individuals that becomes diluted when averaged at the group level9. Furthermore, we expected a worse structural injury pattern in those with mTBI and comorbid PTSD; however, the combination of diagnoses may have obscured the global neural signature. These findings underscore the difficulty of diagnosing those with both mTBI and PTSD, and further, demonstrates the heterogeneity of symptom presentation and lack of consistent neural signature.
Similar to our past work, the psychological symptoms group included mainly PTSD and mTBI with current PTSD and showed reduced FA and elevated RD; no GM effects were shown. Given past studies demonstrating reduced GM volumes in the hippocampus and amygdala in PTSD50,51, the absence of a global GM signature may suggest a more subtle, region-specific pattern of GM abnormalities that cannot be detected using whole brain measures. As such, exploratory post-hoc analyses that examined subcortical ROIs were performed but did not show evidence of altered regional volumes in the psychological symptoms group (see Supplemental Material). The WM signature is consistent with past studies showing WM abnormalities in PTSD52,53 and given the sensitivity of these MRI metrics may represent a better way of assessing differences between these patient groups. Similarly, the exploratory post-hoc analyses showed reduced FA in several WM regions including the splenium of the CC CR, IC, PTR, and SLF in the psychological symptoms group (see Supplemental Material). Larger samples will be needed to replicate these findings and explore WM microstructure in other ROIs. Together, these results suggest that reduced WM microstructure with evidence of psychological symptoms may be a biomarker of PTSD and/or mTBI with comorbid PTSD.
For those with mTBI and comorbid PTSD, whole brain structural neuroimaging in the current statistical models did not add any additional information beyond the information found in the neuropsychological performance when classifying patients. Thus, use of state-of-the-art neuropsychological measures are currently the most beneficial for clinicians to use to direct and track treatment strategies. Future prospective research studies that examine rehabilitation methods may want to use the latent neuropsychological features described in these manuscripts as a way of determining the efficacy of various treatment strategies as one might expect that different neuropsychological profiles warrant different or more coordinated treatment approaches. Nonetheless, the findings provide preliminary evidence supporting the potential for neuroimaging markers to distinguish mTBI and PTSD in clinical settings.
Limitations
Study limitations are discussed in detail in Esopenko et al.34, but for the current work main limitations are that the overall sample size was reduced due to missing neuroimaging data, had few female Service Members, and the sample sizes were not equal across the diagnostic classifications. Moreover, mTBI with comorbid PTSD was not diagnosed prior to study enrollment. Instead, cut-off scores on the PCL-M were used to define this group. As this is a self-report measure, the outcomes on the PCL-M may not match clinician-confirmed diagnoses. However, the PCL has good sensitivity and specificity in high-risk populations and is a reasonable alternative to the gold standard diagnostic interview54. Future studies should examine individuals diagnosed by clinicians using current diagnostic criteria. TBI assessments used standardized language and injuries were corroborated with in theatre medical records. However, assessments focused on deployment related injuries only and a validated instrument assessing lifetime TBI was not used. Further, while efforts were made to age match groups during recruitment, the mTBI group was significantly younger than the other groups (see Table 1). The effect of age was not examined in this study, but aging has been shown to significantly affect total brain, GM, and WM volumes and microstructure55, and should be examined in future studies when sample sizes allow. Finally, whole brain measures were used, and while exploratory post-hoc analyses were performed, the study was not powered to robustly examine all ROIs that may be sensitive to the effects of mTBI and PTSD. In addition, while atrophy is a common consequence of pathology, it is possible to have dysfunctional tissue that is not necessarily atrophy. As such, future studies should assess whether the properties of specific regions provide better classification of individuals with mTBI and/or PTSD.
Conclusions
The findings demonstrate a lack of consistent global neural signature in military mTBI and PTSD which may be one mechanism underlying the heterogeneity of symptom presentation and difficulty in treating mTBI and PTSD. Clinically, these results further suggest that, unless otherwise indicated, neuropsychological measures alone may be the best tool for identifying distinct symptom profiles that clinicians can use to guide treatment planning. However, the findings shed light on the potential for neuroimaging markers to distinguish military personnel with mTBI and PTSD.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
The view(s) expressed herein are those of the author and do not reflect the official policy or position of the Traumatic Brain Injury Center of Excellence (TBICoE), Brooke Army Medical Center, the U.S. Army Medical Department, the U.S. Army Office of the Surgeon General, the Department of the Army, Department of Defense, or the U.S. Government.
Conflicts of Interest and Source of Funding
Carrie Esopenko has received presentation honoraria from New York University, the International Neuropsychological Society, and National Athletic Trainers Association. Douglas Cooper is employed as a researcher by the Defense and Veterans Brain Injury Center and the Foundation for Advancing Veterans Health Research. Amy O. Bowles is currently receiving grants (W81XWH-18-2-0070, W81XWH-11-2-0222, W81XWH-15-PORP-ARA) through the Department of Defense Congressionally Directed Medical Research Programs (CDMRP). For the remaining authors none were declared.
This work is supported in part by the Traumatic Brain Injury Center of Excellence (TBICoE), the U.S. Army Medical Research and Materiel Command (USAMRMC; W81XWH-13-2-0025), and also supported by the Assistant Secretary of Defense for Health Affairs endorsed by the Department of Defense, through the Psychological Health/Traumatic Brain Injury Research Program LongTerm Impact of Military Relevant Brain Injury Consortium (LIMBIC) Award W81XWH18PH/TBIRPLIMBIC under Awards No. W81XWH1920067 and W81XWH1320095, and by the U.S. Department of Veterans Affairs Awards No. I01 CX002097, I01 CX002096, I01 HX003155, I01 RX003444, I01 RX003443, I01 RX003442, I01 CX001135, I01 CX001246, I01 RX001774, I01 RX 001135, I01 RX 002076, I01 RX 001880, I01 RX 002172, I01 RX 002173, I01 RX 002171, I01 RX 002174, and I01 RX 002170. The U.S. Army Medical Research Acquisition Activity, 839 Chandler Street, Fort Detrick MD 217025014 is the awarding and administering acquisition office. This work is also supported in part by R61NS120249 to FGH, ELD, DFT, and EAW. Financial support was provided to CE through the School of Health Professions at Rutgers Biomedical and Health Sciences.
References
- 1.Burke HS, Degeneffe CE, Olney MF. A New Disability for Rehabilitation Counselors: Iraq War Veterans with Traumatic Brain Injury and Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder. J Rehabil. 2009;75(3):5–14. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Dolan S, Martindale S, Robinson J, et al. Neuropsychological sequelae of PTSD and TBI following war deployment among OEF/OIF veterans. Neuropsychol Rev. 2012;22(1):21–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Hoge CW, McGurk D, Thomas JL, Cox AL, Engel CC, Castro CA. Mild Traumatic Brain Injury in U.S. Soldiers Returning from Iraq. N Engl J Med. 2008;358(5):453–463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Schneiderman AI, Braver ER, Kang HK. Understanding sequelae of injury mechanisms and mild traumatic brain injury incurred during the conflicts in Iraq and Afghanistan: persistent postconcussive symptoms and posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Epidemiol. 2008;167(12):1446–1452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Health.mil. DoD TBI Worldwide Numbers. https://health.mil/About-MHS/OASDHA/Defense-Health-Agency/Research-and-Development/Traumatic-Brain-Injury-Center-of-Excellence/DOD-TBI-Worldwide-Numbers. Published 2021. Accessed June 6, 2021.
- 6.Department of Veterans Affairs, Defense Do . VA/DoD Clinical Practice Guideline for the Management of Concussion-Mild Traumatic Brain Injury. In:2016.
- 7.Rice VJ, Schroeder PJ, Cassenti DN, Boykin GL. The Effect of Traumatic Brain Injury (TBI) on Cognitive Performance in a Sample of Active Duty U.S. Military Service Members. Mil Med. 2020;185(Suppl 1):184–189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Golding H, Bass E, Percy A, Goldberg M. Understanding recent estimates of PTSD and TBI from Operations Iraqi Freedom and Enduring Freedom. J Rehabil Res Dev. 2009;46(5):vii–xiv. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Bigler ED. Volumetric MRI Findings in Mild Traumatic Brain Injury (mTBI) and Neuropsychological Outcome. Neuropsychol Rev. 2021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Bolzenius JD, Wade BSC, Velez CS, et al. Relationships Between Subcortical Shape Measures and Subjective Symptom Reporting in US Service Members With Mild Traumatic Brain Injury. J Head Trauma Rehabil. 2018;33(2):113–122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Tate DF, Wade BS, Velez CS, et al. Volumetric and shape analyses of subcortical structures in United States service members with mild traumatic brain injury. J Neurol. 2016;263(10):2065–2079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Tate DF, Wade BSC, Velez CS, et al. Subcortical shape and neuropsychological function among U.S. service members with mild traumatic brain injury. Brain Imaging Behav. 2019;13(2):377–388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Bigler ED, Maxwell WL. Neuropathology of mild traumatic brain injury: relationship to neuroimaging findings. Brain Imaging Behav. 2012;6(2):108–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Shenton ME, Hamoda HM, Schneiderman JS, et al. A review of magnetic resonance imaging and diffusion tensor imaging findings in mild traumatic brain injury. Brain Imaging Behav. 2012;6(2):137–192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Mac Donald CL, Dikranian K, Bayly P, Holtzman D, Brody D. Diffusion tensor imaging reliably detects experimental traumatic axonal injury and indicates approximate time of injury. J Neurosci. 2007;27(44):11869–11876. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Hayes JP, Bigler ED, Verfaellie M. Traumatic Brain Injury as a Disorder of Brain Connectivity. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2016;22(2):120–137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Jorge RE, Acion L, White T, et al. White matter abnormalities in veterans with mild traumatic brain injury. Am J Psychiatry. 2012;169(12):1284–1291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Morey RA, Haswell CC, Selgrade ES, et al. Effects of chronic mild traumatic brain injury on white matter integrity in Iraq and Afghanistan war veterans. Hum Brain Mapp. 2013;34(11):2986–2999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Mac Donald CL, Johnson AM, Cooper D, et al. Detection of blast-related traumatic brain injury in U.S. military personnel. N Engl J Med. 2011;364(22):2091–2100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Hayes JP, Miller DR, Lafleche G, Salat DH, Verfaellie M. The nature of white matter abnormalities in blast-related mild traumatic brain injury. Neuroimage Clin. 2015;8:148–156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Lindsey HM, Hodges CB, Greer KM, Wilde EA, Merkley TL. Diffusion-Weighted Imaging in Mild Traumatic Brain Injury: A Systematic Review of the Literature. Neuropsychol Rev. 2021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Yurgil KA, Barkauskas DA, Vasterling JJ, et al. Association between traumatic brain injury and risk of posttraumatic stress disorder in active-duty Marines. JAMA Psychiatry. 2014;71(2):149–157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Vasterling JJ, Duke LM, Brailey K, Constans JI, Allain AN Jr., Sutker PB. Attention, learning, and memory performances and intellectual resources in Vietnam veterans: PTSD and no disorder comparisons. Neuropsychology. 2002;16(1):5–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Fang SC, Schnurr PP, Kulish AL, et al. Psychosocial Functioning and Health-Related Quality of Life Associated with Posttraumatic Stress Disorder in Male and Female Iraq and Afghanistan War Veterans: The VALOR Registry. J Womens Health. 2015;24(12):1038–1046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Dieter JN, Engel SD. Traumatic Brain Injury and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: Comorbid Consequences of War. Neurosci Insights. 2019;14:1179069519892933. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Vasterling JJ, Jacob SN, Rasmusson A. Traumatic Brain Injury and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: Conceptual, Diagnostic, and Therapeutic Considerations in the Context of Co-Occurrence. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2018;30(2):91–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Combs HL, Berry DT, Pape T, et al. The Effects of Mild Traumatic Brain Injury, Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder, and Combined Mild Traumatic Brain Injury/Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder on Returning Veterans. J Neurotrauma. 2015;32(13):956–966. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Lopez KC, Leary JB, Pham DL, Chou YY, Dsurney J, Chan L. Brain Volume, Connectivity, and Neuropsychological Performance in Mild Traumatic Brain Injury: The Impact of Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder Symptoms. J Neurotrauma. 2017;34(1):16–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Shandera-Ochsner AL, Berry DT, Harp JP, et al. Neuropsychological effects of self-reported deployment-related mild TBI and current PTSD in OIF/OEF veterans. Clin Neuropsychol. 2013;27(6):881–907. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.McAllister TW, Stein MB. Effects of psychological and biomechanical trauma on brain and behavior. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2010;1208:46–57. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Sydnor VJ, Bouix S, Pasternak O, et al. Mild traumatic brain injury impacts associations between limbic system microstructure and post-traumatic stress disorder symptomatology. Neuroimage Clin. 2020;26:102190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Lepage C, de Pierrefeu A, Koerte IK, et al. White matter abnormalities in mild traumatic brain injury with and without post-traumatic stress disorder: a subject-specific diffusion tensor imaging study. Brain Imaging Behav. 2018;12(3):870–881. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Santhanam P, Teslovich T, Wilson SH, Yeh PH, Oakes TR, Weaver LK. Decreases in White Matter Integrity of Ventro-Limbic Pathway Linked to Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder in Mild Traumatic Brain Injury. J Neurotrauma. 2019;36(7):1093–1098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Esopenko C, de Souza NL, Jia Y, et al. Latent Neuropsychological Profiles to Discriminate Mild Traumatic Brain Injury and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder in Active-Duty Service Members. J Head Trauma Rehabil. 2022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Cooper DB, Bowles AO, Kennedy JE, et al. Cognitive Rehabilitation for Military Service Members With Mild Traumatic Brain Injury: A Randomized Clinical Trial. J Head Trauma Rehabil. 2017;32(3):E1–e15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Karstoft KI, Andersen SB, Bertelsen M, Madsen T. Diagnostic accuracy of the posttraumatic stress disorder checklist-civilian version in a representative military sample. Psychol Assess. 2014;26(1):321–325. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Bolzenius JD, Velez CS, Lewis JD, et al. Diffusion Imaging Findings in US Service Members With Mild Traumatic Brain Injury and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. J Head Trauma Rehabil. 2018;33(6):393–402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Blanchard EB, Jones-Alexander J, Buckley TC, Forneris CA. Psychometric properties of the PTSD Checklist (PCL). Behav Res Ther. 1996;34(8):669–673. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, et al. Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron. 2002;33(3):341–355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Jahanshad N, Kochunov PV, Sprooten E, et al. Multi-site genetic analysis of diffusion images and voxelwise heritability analysis: A pilot project of the ENIGMA-DTI working group. Neuroimage. 2013;81:455–469. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Johansen-Berg H, et al. Tract-based spatial statistics: Voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data. Neuroimage. 2006;31(4):1487–1505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Smith SM, Johansen-Berg H, Jenkinson M, et al. Acquisition and voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data with tract-based spatial statistics. Nat Protoc. 2007;2(3):499–503. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Mori S, Oishi K, Jiang H, et al. Stereotaxic white matter atlas based on diffusion tensor imaging in an ICBM template. Neuroimage. 2008;40(2):570–582. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Bigler ED, Neeley ES, Miller MJ, et al. Cerebral volume loss, cognitive deficit and neuropsychological performance: comparative measures of brain atrophy: I. Dementia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10(3):442–452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Masyn KE. Latent Class Analysis and Finite Mixture Modeling. In: Little T, ed. The Oxford Handbook of Quantitative Methods. OUP; USA; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Lo Y, Mendell NR, Rubin DB. Testing the number of components in a normal mixture. Biometrika. 2001;88(3):767–778. [Google Scholar]
- 47.McLachlan GJ. On Bootstrapping the Likelihood Ratio Test Statistic for the Number of Components in a Normal Mixture. J R Stat Soc Ser C Appl Stat. 1987;36(3):318–324. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Davenport ND, Gullickson JT, Grey SF, Hirsch S, Sponheim SR, Chronic Effects of Neurotrauma C. Longitudinal evaluation of ventricular volume changes associated with mild traumatic brain injury in military service members. Brain Inj. 2018;32(10):1245–1255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Himanen L, Portin R, Isoniemi H, Helenius H, Kurki T, Tenovuo O. Cognitive functions in relation to MRI findings 30 years after traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj. 2005;19(2):93–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Logue MW, van Rooij SJH, Dennis EL, et al. Smaller Hippocampal Volume in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: A Multisite ENIGMA-PGC Study: Subcortical Volumetry Results From Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Consortia. Biol Psychiatry. 2018;83(3):244–253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.O'Doherty DC, Chitty KM, Saddiqui S, Bennett MR, Lagopoulos J. A systematic review and meta-analysis of magnetic resonance imaging measurement of structural volumes in posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2015;232(1):1–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Dennis EL, Disner SG, Fani N, et al. Altered white matter microstructural organization in posttraumatic stress disorder across 3047 adults: results from the PGC-ENIGMA PTSD consortium. Mol Psychiatry. 2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.O'Doherty DCM, Ryder W, Paquola C, et al. White matter integrity alterations in post-traumatic stress disorder. Hum Brain Mapp. 2018;39(3):1327–1338. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Spoont MR, Williams JW Jr., Kehle-Forbes S, Nieuwsma JA, Mann-Wrobel MC, Gross R. Does This Patient Have Posttraumatic Stress Disorder?: Rational Clinical Examination Systematic Review. JAMA. 2015;314(5):501–510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Irimia A Cross-Sectional Volumes and Trajectories of the Human Brain, Gray Matter, White Matter and Cerebrospinal Fluid in 9473 Typically Aging Adults. Neuroinformatics. 2021;19(2):347–366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.