Abstract
Alcohol consumption is common in Western countries and has been increasing in older adults. Latest figures from Great Britain suggest 75% of those over 65 years drink, an increase from 71% 10 years ago. Chronic heavy intake is a well-established cause of brain atrophy and dementia, with a recent long-term prospective study from the USA reporting a doubling of the odds of later severe memory impairment in those with a history of an alcohol use disorder. Drinking of moderate amounts has been reported to be protective for brain health in a number of epidemiological studies, including some claims of possibly reducing dementia risk. Rigorous recent research has questioned this belief, with new evidence of harmful associations in moderate drinkers compared with abstainers. This has raised suspicion that reported protective effects of moderate drinking were due to confounding by socioeconomic class and intelligence. Clinicians should look out for cognitive impairment in heavy drinkers, considering that abstinence may induce a degree of clinical improvement. Discussions with patients regarding moderate drinking should be informed by recent research. Health benefits of moderate drinking at least for cognitive function are questionable, and if they exist are probably limited to one unit of alcohol daily with respect to other body systems.
Keywords: substance misuse, dementia
Introduction
Alcohol use is widespread and increasing across the developed world. Latest figures suggest that 58% of the UK population is drinking in excess of existing safe limits,1 with 5% men and 4% of women consuming hazardous amounts (>50 units (400 g) for men and >35 units (280 g) for women weekly). Drinking in women and older adults of both sexes has been increasing,2 with the percentage of those >65 years drinking increasing from 71% to 75% between 2005 and 2016. Those over 65 years of age are now more likely than any other age group to have drunk on at least 5 days in the preceding week.1 Alcohol-related harm is estimated to cost England around £21 billion per year, with £3.5 billion to the NHS, £11 billion tackling alcohol-related crime and £7.3 billion from lost work days and productivity costs. Health concerns associated with alcohol use have focused on liver dysfunction and more recently cancer. The public are largely ignorant of the potential risks of drinking with regards to cognition, in contrast to widespread knowledge of effects on the liver.3 Recommended drinking guidelines have remained unchanged in the UK from 1987 until 2016. Safe limits for men were set at 21 units (168 g) and for women at 14 units (112 g) per week pre-2016. Recent evidence of associations with cancer risk4 at lower doses prompted revision of UK government alcohol guidance to 14 units (112 g) for both sexes, but current US guidelines still suggest that up to 24.5 units (196 g) weekly is safe for men.
This clinical review summarises established effects of chronic heavy drinking on brain and behaviour/cognitive function, as well as the poorly understood impact of moderate consumption. Implications for clinicians from these findings are also discussed.
Methods
MEDLINE, PubMed and Google Scholar were searched with the following keywordskeywords: ‘alcohol’ AND (‘dementia’ OR ‘cognition’ OR ‘brain’). Primary research and review articles published in English until 10 September 2017 were examined for information pertinent to the topic. Reference lists from included papers were hand searched for additional relevant articles.
Results
Our search retrieved a large consistent evidence base to suggest an increased risk of dementia and cognitive impairment, and brain atrophy in the context of chronic heavy alcohol consumption. In contrast, data pertaining to risks or benefits of lesser intakes were conflicting, perhaps in part because definitions of ‘moderate’ consumption varied considerably. In this review we focus on five epidemiological studies and two meta-analyses pertaining to the risk of dementia with moderate alcohol use, five studies and two meta-analyses investigating the risk of cognitive impairment, and nine studies examining neuroimaging outcome measures. Table 1 gives a summary of the main findings.
Table 1.
Outcome | Chronic heavy intake | Moderate intake |
Cognitive outcomes | ||
Dementia risk | Consistently increased11 | Conflicting results, with some studies reporting a protective effect26 28 31 which others have failed to replicate29 30 |
Cognitive decline | Consistently increased8 9 | Conflicting results, with some studies reporting a protective effect34 35 which others have failed to replicate32 36 37 46 |
Brain structure | ||
Grey matter | Widespread atrophy, especially of frontal lobes12 | Two studies reported atrophy40 41 but others have not39 42 |
White matter | Widespread atrophy, including of frontal lobes, cerebellum and corpus callosum13 20 | Two studies have found negative associations44–46 and others have not43 |
Subcortical volumes | Hippocampi,51 amygdalae,16 mammillary bodies, hypothalamic and thalamic nuclei all potentially affected52 | Two studies report conflicting associations with hippocampal size43 46 |
Cerebellum | Atrophy17 | Insufficient evidence |
Overall, acute intoxication with alcohol results in behavioural disinhibition, disrupted socioemotional processing and impaired psychomotor performance. The molecular basis for these actions is thought to include NMDA and GABA-A receptors. High doses of alcohol acutely reduce prefrontal and temporal lobe function, including planning, verbal fluency, memory and complex motor control including cerebellar function.5 The pattern of impairment has been compared with that seen in hippocampal damage. Excessive alcohol consumption can also lead to a number of conditions with psychiatric symptoms, including psychotic disorders and delirium. In this review we focus on structural brain and cognitive sequelae of chronic alcohol consumption.
Chronic heavy alcohol
Cognitive sequelae
Chronic heavy drinking is associated with a number of serious neurological conditions, one of which is Wernicke’s encephalopathy. This is an acute, often lethal, but potentially reversible neurological disorder caused by a severe deficit in thiamine (vitamin B1) aggravated by carbohydrate overload. It is characterised by the clinical triad of oculomotor disturbance, cerebellar dysfunction and altered mental state. Chronic alcohol users are at risk of this due to multiple potential factors including: poor diet, impaired absorption and altered metabolism of thiamine. The estimated mortality of Wernicke’s encephalopathy is 17%. Untreated, 80% of survivors of Wernicke’s encephalopathy will progress to the severe and usually permanent Korsakoff’s syndrome.6 This is characterised by anterograde (and retrograde) amnesia, and frequently occurs together with prefrontal deficits. Even in ‘uncomplicated’ alcoholism, without Wernicke’s encephalopathy or Korsakoff’s syndrome, over 80% of individuals have executive deficits,7 although of a lesser severity. A strong evidence base supports the association of heavy alcohol consumption over long periods with increased dementia risk and cognitive decline.8 A recent long-term prospective study from the USA reported that individuals with a history of an alcohol use disorder have more than double the odds of later severe memory impairment compared with controls (OR 2.21, 95% CI 1.27 to 3.85).9 There is some debate as to whether primary alcohol dementia exists, as a result of direct neurotoxicity, or whether cognitive deficits are entirely secondary to an additional pathology, for example, due to thiamine deficiency. It is difficult to disentangle these possibilities, particularly in view of multiple confounders in dependent drinkers, such as smoking, comorbid substance abuse and increased vascular risk. Similarly, establishing the prevalence of alcohol-related dementia is difficult, due to a lack of operationally defined diagnostic criteria. Estimates differ from 9% to 22% of all patients with dementia,10 or even higher in nursing homes. Dementia of alcoholic aetiology may be particularly prevalent in earlier-onset dementia. There is no clear answer, as to what level of consumption is sufficient to cause Korsakoff’s syndrome. Oslin et al 11 have suggested a 5-year intake greater than 35 units (280 g) weekly for men and 28 units (224 g) for women, but this needs validation. Thresholds are likely to be different according to sex, comorbid conditions and genetic susceptibility.
Effects on brain structure
Brain atrophy in chronic alcoholism is well described.12 The frontal lobes are thought to be particularly vulnerable. Kril et al found frontal cortex reductions of 23% in uncomplicated alcoholism, replicating earlier findings. MRI studies have also reported widespread cortical atrophy, which may particularly affect the frontal lobes.13 Interestingly, longitudinal MRI has shown a degree of tissue recovery, especially of white matter, on abstinence.14
The hippocampus is consistently affected in animal models of chronic alcoholism. Despite the preclinical data, evidence for hippocampal involvement in humans is less convincing. However, Bengochea and Gonzalo, among others, have described hippocampal loss in individuals with chronic alcoholism compared with controls.15 Reduced amygdala volume has also been described in those with lifelong high alcohol consumption (defined as >80 g daily for the majority of their adult lives), and including those with Wernicke-Korsakoff’s syndrome.16 The cerebellum, known for its importance in motor function, is now additionally thought to play a role in memory disturbance.17 A number of neuropathological studies have established cerebellar atrophy in chronic alcohol abuse.18 Mammillary body atrophy is almost universal in chronic alcoholism. However, animal studies suggest that other hypothalamic nuclei, particularly the supraoptic and paraventricular, may also be affected.19 Anterior thalamic nuclei may be affected in humans.
Several preclinical and postmortem studies have provided evidence of prominent white matter loss following chronic heavy alcohol use, which may exceed grey matter loss.20 The frontal lobes and cerebellum are particularly vulnerable. Furthermore, white matter loss in the temporal lobes has been linked to alcohol withdrawal seizures.21 The corpus callosum, a major commissural tract, may be particularly vulnerable. In their postmortem study, Harper and Kril reported that corpus callosum thickness in a group of alcoholics (3.19 mm) was significantly reduced compared with controls (4.02 mm).
A number of different mechanisms have been proposed to account for the deleterious effects of chronically high alcohol consumption on the brain. Damage to the brain from chronically high levels of alcohol is thought to result, at least in part, from thiamine deficiency. Deficiency of thiamine has been linked to oxidative stress, excitotoxicity, inflammatory responses, dysfunction of the blood-brain barrier and lactic acidosis. Ethanol could also be directly neurotoxic. Alcohol inhibits glutamate receptors. Chronic exposure induces upregulation of NMDA receptors, which leaves neurons vulnerable to excitotoxicity from massive calcium influx. This may be driven especially by repeated binges and withdrawal. Cell death could also be via neuroinflammation, the induction of inflammatory mediators and microglia.22 Additionally, alcohol-related liver dysfunction could mean that neurotoxic substances such as ammonia and manganese are not removed from the blood, resulting in cerebral effects.
Moderate chronic alcohol use
There is no universal agreement about the definition of ‘light’ or ‘moderate’ drinking. Moderate drinking is defined variably in the literature from 9 units to 18 units (72–144 g) weekly.23 24 In contrast to heavy consumption, the long-term effects of moderate alcohol consumption on cognition are poorly understood. A J-shaped relationship of cognitive function with alcohol use has been suggested, similar to that with cardiovascular disease, that is, small amounts of alcohol are associated with a reduced risk than abstinence, but large amounts with the greatest risk. Ethanol’s inhibition of platelet aggregation, reduction in inflammatory markers and alteration of plasma lipid profile have been proposed as underlying mechanisms for protection from cardiovascular disease, in addition to the antioxidant action of polyphenols present in some alcoholic drinks.25
Risk of dementia
Several large epidemiological studies have reported a reduced risk of dementia (of vascular and non-vascular aetiology) in light drinkers to moderate drinkers compared with abstainers. For example, Ruitenberg et al found those drinking one to three drinks (14–46 g) per day had a reduced risk of dementia (HR 0.58, 95% CI 0.38 to 0.9) compared with abstainers in the Rotterdam Study.26 Subjects were over 55 years old at baseline, and followed up for an average of 6 years. Dementia was excluded at baseline using a variety of screening tests, such as the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), Geriatric Mental State (GMS) and Cambridge Cognitive Examination (CAMCOG). In the French prospective PAQUID Study, individuals who were at least 65 years old at baseline were followed up for 3 years. Both ‘light’ wine drinkers (<1–2 drinks (14–32 g) daily) and ‘moderate’ wine drinkers (3–4 drinks (46–64 g) daily) had reduced odds of incident Alzheimer’s disease.27 Similarly, Mukamal et al 28 reported a lower dementia risk in those drinking one to six drinks (14–84 g) per day in the Cardiovascular Health Study, using a nested case-control design. Those thought to have dementia were excluded at baseline. A large Norwegian longitudinal study reported increased dementia-related deaths in moderate drinkers compared with abstainers.29 However, others have not replicated these findings, including a recent study in an Australian cohort, which found no association with incident dementia or cognitive decline, and no interaction between alcohol and the ApoE4 genotype.30
Meta-analyses of such studies have cited a protective effect of light to moderate alcohol consumption on the risk of dementia including Alzheimer’s disease.31–33 Anstey et al analysed 15 prospective studies and reported a pooled risk reduction of 25%–8% with late-life drinking. They cautioned however that it was unclear whether this represented selection bias, that is, those still drinking in late life were likely to be those without dementia, or a genuine protective effect.
Risk of cognitive impairment or decline
In a large study of women in the USA, the Nurses Health Study, Stampfer et al 34 followed up women aged 30–55 years at baseline, using a cognitive test modelled on MMSE and later with a verbal recall task. Those consuming one drink daily had better cognitive scores than non-drinkers at baseline, and less cognitive decline at 2-year follow-up. The analysis was controlled for age, education level, social integration and cardiovascular risk factors. In another US study, Ganguli et al 35 followed up individuals aged at least 65 years at baseline for 2 years on a range of cognitive tests, including MMSE, Trail Making Test word recall and category fluency. Both minimal (defined as <1 drink (14 g) monthly) and moderate (<1 drink (14 g) daily) drinkers displayed less decline on MMSE and traits tests compared with abstainers. Interestingly, the protective effects were more pronounced when the comparison group of abstainers also included former drinkers rather than only lifelong abstainers.
However, not all studies have replicated a protective effect of light drinking on cognition. Lobo et al 36 did not find less MMSE decline in light drinkers than in abstainers. Bos et al 37 recently reported increased risk of cognitive decline (non Alzheimer types) with increased alcohol. Others have found a protective effect of moderate alcohol use only in those not carrying the ApoE4 allele (a risk gene for late-onset Alzheimer’s disease).38
Meta-analyses have been limited by the paucity of studies. Peters et al 32 found no protective effect of moderate alcohol consumption on cognitive decline. Similarly, Anstey et al 31 found no significant effect of alcohol consumption on cognitive decline, but their analysis was limited to two studies and consequently had a high degree of heterogeneity.
Brain correlates
No convincing neural correlate for a protective effect of small amounts of alcohol on human brain structure has been found, although the field is poorly studied. Reported results are inconsistent.39 Moderate alcohol consumption in older subjects has been associated with reduced total brain volume, increased ventricle size,40 grey matter atrophy41 and reduced frontal and parietal grey matter density, but others have not found such relationships,39 or only at higher consumption levels.42 Associations between moderate alcohol consumption and white matter findings are also inconsistent. De Bruin reported increased white matter volume in moderate drinkers compared with abstainers,43 whereas Anstey found the inverse relationship.44 Similarly, increased white matter hyperintensities have been described in moderate drinkers compared with abstainers45 but others found no association.
In a cohort of 550 adults, we examined structural neuroimaging outcomes and cognitive decline in relation to alcohol consumption over the preceding 30 years. We found a novel dose-dependent association between alcohol intake and hippocampal atrophy. Just 14–21 units (112–168 g) of alcohol weekly was associated with almost three times odds of hippocampal atrophy compared with abstinence.46 Additionally, alcohol consumption in non-dependent drinkers was associated with lower white matter integrity, particularly of the corpus callosum, and faster cognitive decline on lexical fluency, a complex executive task necessitating generation of words beginning with a specific letter within a time limit. Interestingly, we did not find moderate drinkers declining faster on semantic fluency (generation of words within a specific category) or memory recall, especially surprising given the hippocampal associations. Two possible explanations we can think of are that there are greater practice effects for semantic memory that were inadequately controlled for (despite best efforts), or that hippocampal atrophy represents an intermediate phenotype, similarly to in Alzheimer’s disease, and cognitive symptoms were not yet evident at the last testing point in the study.
Discussion
How can the seemingly contradictory epidemiological protective claims be explained given the recent harmful brain associations of moderate drinking? One explanation is confounding. Moderate alcohol consumption is highly allied to socioeconomic status and education.1 Therefore characteristics that predict higher performance on cognitive testing, or a later diagnosis of dementia, are also associated with a likelihood of alcohol consumption. One method to try to obviate the problems of residual confounding is the use of Mendelian randomisation.47 This technique is akin to a randomised controlled drug trial, except instead of a medication being randomly allocated to individuals, genetic variants are the instrumental variable, allocated at meiosis. Alcohol metabolism genes, for example, ADH or ALDH, explain some of the variance of consumption and have been used to investigate associations with cognition or dementia. Two studies thus far have attempted to apply this technique to the question of moderate alcohol and cognition. Almeida et al found no protective effect of moderate drinking on cognitive decline (defined as MMSE <23/30) over a 6-year period in older men.48 Nor did Yeung et al 47 find a protective association with cross-sectional performance on MMSE and word recall using Mendelian randomisation of the ALDH2 genotype in the Guangzhou Biobank. However, both these studies may have been underpowered.
Clinical implications
Identification of patients drinking large amounts of alcohol over a long period is important given the high risk of cognitive impairment and dementia, particularly as abstinence may improve symptoms to some degree. Screening instruments, such as CAGE or the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT), may be helpful in achieving this. They are quick to administer, so applicable in primary care, and have been found to be superior to laboratory tests, including the best performing gamma- glutamyl transpeptidase (GGT) test (sensitivity 33%) at detecting excessive drinkers (>16 drinks (224 g) daily), with a sensitivity of 93%.49 Clinicians should be vigilant for cognitive symptoms in such individuals. Neuroimaging in suspected alcohol-related cognitive impairment is an informative adjunctive source of information in the assessment. Cerebral atrophy (particularly of frontal white matter), white matter lesions and hippocampal atrophy are consistent with alcohol-related brain damage, although not discriminative from vascular or Alzheimer’s dementias.
Recent associations between moderate alcohol intake and adverse brain outcomes should be highlighted in discussions with patients about their drinking. Any health benefits are likely to be limited to one unit (8 g) daily, and even this has increased risks of breast cancer. Justification of moderate drinking on the grounds of brain health has become a little harder.50
Footnotes
Contributors: AT developed the idea for the study, conducted the literature search, interpreted the literature and wrote the manuscript. KPE developed the idea for the study, interpreted the literature and contributed to the manuscript.
Funding: This work was supported by the HDH Wills 1965 Charitable Trust (Nr: 1117747).
Competing interests: None declared.
Provenance and peer review: Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.
References
- 1. Office for National Statatistics. Opinions and lifestyles survey: adult drinking habits in great britain, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- 2. Blazer DG, Wu LT. The epidemiology of alcohol use disorders and subthreshold dependence in a middle-aged and elderly community sample. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2011;19:685–94. 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3182006a96 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Office for National Statatistics. Omnibus survey report drinking: adults' behaviour and knowledge in 2009. [Google Scholar]
- 4. Cao Y, Willett WC, Rimm EB, et al. Light to moderate intake of alcohol, drinking patterns, and risk of cancer: results from two prospective US cohort studies, 2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Peterson JB, Rothfleisch J, Zelazo PD, et al. Acute alcohol intoxication and cognitive functioning. J Stud Alcohol 1990;51:114–22. 10.15288/jsa.1990.51.114 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Kopelman MD, Thomson AD, Guerrini I, et al. The Korsakoff syndrome: clinical aspects, psychology and treatment. Alcohol Alcohol 2009;44:148–54. 10.1093/alcalc/agn118 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Giancola PR, Moss HB. Executive cognitive functioning in alcohol use disorders. Recent Dev Alcohol 1998;14:227-51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Sabia S, Elbaz A, Britton A, et al. Alcohol consumption and cognitive decline in early old age. Neurology 2014;82:332–9. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000063 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Kuźma E, Llewellyn DJ, Langa KM, et al. History of alcohol use disorders and risk of severe cognitive impairment: a 19-year prospective cohort study. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2014;22:1047–54. 10.1016/j.jagp.2014.06.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Ritchie K, Villebrun D. Epidemiology of alcohol-related dementia. Handb Clin Neurol 2008;89:845–50. 10.1016/S0072-9752(07)01273-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Oslin D, Atkinson RM, Smith DM, et al. Alcohol related dementia: proposed clinical criteria. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 1998;13:203–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Kril JJ, Halliday GM. Brain shrinkage in alcoholics: a decade on and what have we learned? Prog Neurobiol 1999;58:381–7. 10.1016/S0301-0082(98)00091-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Chanraud S, Martelli C, Delain F, et al. Brain morphometry and cognitive performance in detoxified alcohol-dependents with preserved psychosocial functioning. Neuropsychopharmacology 2007;32:429–38. 10.1038/sj.npp.1301219 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Pfefferbaum A, Sullivan EV, Mathalon DH, et al. Longitudinal changes in magnetic resonance imaging brain volumes in abstinent and relapsed alcoholics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1995;19:1177–91. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1995.tb01598.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Bengochea O, Gonzalo LM. Effects of alcoholization on the rat hippocampus. Neurosci Lett 1991;123:112–4. 10.1016/0304-3940(91)90170-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Alvarez I, Gonzalo LM, Llor J. Effects of chronic alcoholism on the amygdaloid complex. A study in human and rats. Histol Histopathol 1989;4:183–92. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Zahr NM, Pitel AL, Chanraud S, et al. Contributions of studies on alcohol use disorders to understanding cerebellar function. Neuropsychol Rev 2010;20:280–9. 10.1007/s11065-010-9141-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Ferrer I, Fabregues I, Pineda M, et al. A Golgi study of cerebellar atrophy in human chronic alcoholism. Neuropathol Appl Neurobiol 1984;10:245–53. 10.1111/j.1365-2990.1984.tb00357.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Harding AJ, Halliday GM, Ng JL, et al. Loss of vasopressin-immunoreactive neurons in alcoholics is dose-related and time-dependent. Neuroscience 1996;72:699–708. 10.1016/0306-4522(95)00577-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Harper CG, Kril JJ, Holloway RL. Brain shrinkage in chronic alcoholics: a pathological study. BMJ 1985;290:501–4. 10.1136/bmj.290.6467.501 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Sullivan EV, Marsh L, Mathalon DH, et al. Relationship between alcohol withdrawal seizures and temporal lobe white matter volume deficits. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1996;20:348–54. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1996.tb01651.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. He J, Crews FT. Increased MCP-1 and microglia in various regions of the human alcoholic brain. Exp Neurol 2008;210:349–58. 10.1016/j.expneurol.2007.11.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Rimm EB, Williams P, Fosher K, et al. Moderate alcohol intake and lower risk of coronary heart disease: meta-analysis of effects on lipids and haemostatic factors. BMJ 1999;319:1523–8. 10.1136/bmj.319.7224.1523 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Stampfer MJ, Colditz GA, Willett WC, et al. A prospective study of moderate alcohol consumption and the risk of coronary disease and stroke in women. N Engl J Med 1988;319:267–73. 10.1056/NEJM198808043190503 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Brust JC. Ethanol and cognition: indirect effects, neurotoxicity and neuroprotection: a review. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2010;7:1540–57. 10.3390/ijerph7041540 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Ruitenberg A, van Swieten JC, Witteman JC, et al. Alcohol consumption and risk of dementia: the Rotterdam Study. Lancet 2002;359:281–6. 10.1016/S0140-6736(02)07493-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Orgogozo JM, Dartigues JF, Lafont S, et al. Wine consumption and dementia in the elderly: a prospective community study in the Bordeaux area. Rev Neurol 1997;153:185–92. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Mukamal KJ, Kuller LH, Fitzpatrick AL, et al. Prospective study of alcohol consumption and risk of dementia in older adults. JAMA 2003;289:1405–13. 10.1001/jama.289.11.1405 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Ormstad H, Rosness TA, Bergem AL, et al. Alcohol consumption in the elderly and risk of dementia related death--a Norwegian prospective study with a 17-year follow-up. Int J Neurosci 2016;126:135–44. 10.3109/00207454.2014.997876 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Heffernan M, Mather KA, Xu J, et al. Alcohol consumption and incident dementia: evidence from the sydney memory and ageing study. J Alzheimers Dis 2016;52:529–38. 10.3233/JAD-150537 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Anstey KJ, Mack HA, Cherbuin N. Alcohol consumption as a risk factor for dementia and cognitive decline: meta-analysis of prospective studies. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2009;17:542–55. 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181a2fd07 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Peters R, Peters J, Warner J, et al. Alcohol, dementia and cognitive decline in the elderly: a systematic review. Age Ageing 2008;37:505–12. 10.1093/ageing/afn095 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Xu W, Wang H, Wan Y, et al. Alcohol consumption and dementia risk: a dose-response meta-analysis of prospective studies. Eur J Epidemiol 2017;32:31–42. 10.1007/s10654-017-0225-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Stampfer MJ, Kang JH, Chen J, et al. Effects of moderate alcohol consumption on cognitive function in women. N Engl J Med 2005;352:245–53. 10.1056/NEJMoa041152 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Ganguli M, Vander Bilt J, Saxton JA, et al. Alcohol consumption and cognitive function in late life: a longitudinal community study. Neurology 2005;65:1210–7. 10.1212/01.wnl.0000180520.35181.24 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Lobo E, Dufouil C, Marcos G, et al. Is there an association between low-to-moderate alcohol consumption and risk of cognitive decline? Am J Epidemiol 2010;172:708–16. 10.1093/aje/kwq187 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Bos I, Vos SJ, Frölich L, et al. The frequency and influence of dementia risk factors in prodromal Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 2017;56:33–40. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.03.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Luchsinger JA, Tang MX, Siddiqui M, et al. Alcohol intake and risk of dementia. J Am Geriatr Soc 2004;52:540–6. 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52159.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Gu Y, Scarmeas N, Short EE, et al. Alcohol intake and brain structure in a multiethnic elderly cohort. Clin Nutr 2014;33:662–7. 10.1016/j.clnu.2013.08.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Ding J, Eigenbrodt ML, Mosley TH, et al. Alcohol intake and cerebral abnormalities on magnetic resonance imaging in a community-based population of middle-aged adults: the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) study. Stroke 2004;35:16–21. 10.1161/01.STR.0000105929.88691.8E [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Mukamal KJ, Longstreth WT, Mittleman MA, et al. Alcohol consumption and subclinical findings on magnetic resonance imaging of the brain in older adults: the cardiovascular health study. Stroke 2001;32:1939–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Kubota M, Nakazaki S, Hirai S, et al. Alcohol consumption and frontal lobe shrinkage: study of 1432 non-alcoholic subjects. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2001;71:104–6. 10.1136/jnnp.71.1.104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. de Bruin EA, Hulshoff Pol HE, Schnack HG, et al. Focal brain matter differences associated with lifetime alcohol intake and visual attention in male but not in female non-alcohol-dependent drinkers. Neuroimage 2005;26:536–45. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.01.036 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Anstey KJ, Jorm AF, Réglade-Meslin C, et al. Weekly alcohol consumption, brain atrophy, and white matter hyperintensities in a community-based sample aged 60 to 64 years. Psychosom Med 2006;68:778–85. 10.1097/01.psy.0000237779.56500.af [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Fukuda K, Yuzuriha T, Kinukawa N, et al. Alcohol intake and quantitative MRI findings among community dwelling Japanese subjects. J Neurol Sci 2009;278:30–4. 10.1016/j.jns.2008.11.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Topiwala A, Allan CL, Valkanova V, et al. Moderate alcohol consumption as risk factor for adverse brain outcomes and cognitive decline: longitudinal cohort study. BMJ 2017;357:j2353. 10.1136/bmj.j2353 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Au Yeung SL, Jiang C, Cheng KK, et al. Moderate alcohol use and cardiovascular disease from Mendelian randomization. PLoS One 2013;8:e68054. 10.1371/journal.pone.0068054 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. Almeida OP, Hankey GJ, Yeap BB, et al. Alcohol consumption and cognitive impairment in older men: a mendelian randomization study. Neurology 2014;82:1038–44. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000255 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Bernadt MW, Mumford J, Taylor C, et al. Comparison of questionnaire and laboratory tests in the detection of excessive drinking and alcoholism. Lancet 1982;1:325–8. 10.1016/S0140-6736(82)91579-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Welch KA. Alcohol consumption and brain health. BMJ 2017;357:j2645. 10.1136/bmj.j2645 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Bengochea O, Gonzalo L. Effect of chronic alcoholism on the human hippocampus, 1990. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Harding A, Halliday G, Caine D, et al. Degeneration of anterior thalamic nuclei differentiates alcoholics with amnesia. Brain 2000;123:141–54. 10.1093/brain/123.1.141 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]