Skip to main content
Children logoLink to Children
. 2023 May 28;10(6):960. doi: 10.3390/children10060960

Digital Devices Use and Fine Motor Skills in Children between 3–6 Years

Francesca Felicia Operto 1,*, Andrea Viggiano 1, Antonio Perfetto 1, Gabriella Citro 1, Miriam Olivieri 1, Valeria de Simone 1, Alice Bonuccelli 2, Alessandro Orsini 2, Salvatore Aiello 1, Giangennaro Coppola 1, Grazia Maria Giovanna Pastorino 1
Editor: Carlos Luz
PMCID: PMC10296991  PMID: 37371192

Abstract

(1) Background: The principal aim of our research was to explore the relationship between digital devices use and fine motor skills in children aged three to six years and to explore the effect of some socio-demographic factors. (2) Methods: we enrolled 185 children aged between three to six years. The parents of all the participants fulfilled a questionnaire to explore the digital device use, and their children performed a standardized test to assess fine motor skills (APCM-2). We performed the Spearman correlation test to explore the relationship between different variables. (3) Results: the children spent an average of 3.08 ± 2.30 h/day on digital devices. We did not find a significant association between the time of use of digital devices and fine motor skills (p = 0.640; r = −0.036). The youngest children experienced digital tools earlier than older ones (p < 0.001; r = 0.424) and they were also the ones who used digital tools more time afterwards (p = 0.012; −0.202). The children who had working parents spent more time on digital devices (p = 0.028; r = 0.164/p = 0.037; r = 0.154) and used digital devices earlier (p = 0.023; r = 0.171). (4) Conclusions: This data suggest that it would be useful to monitor the use of digital tools, especially in the very first years of life. Future studies are needed to further explore this topic.

Keywords: digital devices, digital tools, digital screen, children, fine motor skills

1. Introduction

In recent years, digital technologies have progressively become an integral part of the life of children and adolescents as indispensable tools for communication, relationships, and learning [1,2,3,4].

In particular, the “intuitive” technology of the new digital tools (e.g., smartphones and tablets) is very attractive and greatly stimulates the curiosity of younger children, that, with a single gesture of the finger, can access screens, images, videos, and sounds [5].

A recent Italian survey showed that 97% of children under 36 months and 89% under 18 months have already used digital device at least once [6]. Frequently, the parents use digital media as “pacifiers” (to keep calm the children, during meals, and before bedtime) [7,8,9,10,11,12,13].

The COVID-19 pandemic and its social consequences have widened digital device use and further lowered the age of those that access digital technologies, reinforcing a trend already underway in recent years [14,15,16,17,18].

Digital tools offer children many growth opportunities, but, at the same time, they can lead to risks. Some authors suggested that the use of digital tools constitutes a “passive” behavior, which takes the child away from other more useful learning experiences [19,20,21]. According to this, some recent reviews suggest that excessive and too early use of digital tools in children may be associated with delays in some cognitive, linguistic, and social skills [22,23,24].

On the other hand, some research suggests that the use of digital tools may have benefits in terms of learning and development for children, depending on many factors. It seems that the factors correlated to better learning are the use of interactive tools (e.g., touch-screen, tablet, etc.), rather than passive ones (e.g., television), as well as the use of so-called “educational” applications, co-viewing with parents, and a higher age in children. [25,26,27,28,29].

From the point of view of the scientific community, there are indications on generic exposure to digital technologies in the pediatric age, but not everyone agrees on the effects on psycho-physical wellbeing, which are not unique, depend on many factors, and can be both positive and negative.

The World Health Organization’s 24 h movement guidelines do not recommended screen time in children ≤1 year and suggested no more than 1 h of screen use in children between two to four years (less is better) [30].

The official recommendations of the Italian Society of Pediatrics on the use of media devices (mobile phones, smartphones, tablets, PCs, etc.) in children aged zero to eight take into consideration both the positive and negative effects on physical health and mental health of boys and girls: technology should not be rejected, on the contrary it should be used to the fullest, for example, by avoiding the use of smartphones to calm or distract the children [31]. The recommendations strongly advise the use of quality apps (e.g., educational games), but also indicate the times for exposure to digital devices in relation to the age and development of the children. It is not recommended to expose to smartphones and tablets before the age of two, during meals, and before going to sleep, namely, when the tablet or mobile phone should perform the function of calming or silencing the expression of needs. It is preferable to limit the use of these devices to a maximum of 1 h/day in children between two to five years or 2 h a day for those between the ages of five and eight [31].

The development of motor skills in children evolves gradually, according to well defined phases that begin in the first months of the child’s life [32]. In particular, fine motor skills require the coordination of small muscles of the hands, fingers, and eyes, and they allow us the movements necessary for some daily actions, such as handwriting, drawing, building small objects, typing, buttoning clothes, etc. [32]. The achievement of motor development milestones at the appropriate age allows the clinician to monitor the correct development of the child. Some studies suggest an association between fine motor coordination skills in children and later academic performance, and a delay in acquiring fine motor developmental milestones may be indicative of cognitive or medical problems and should be further explored [33,34,35].

The relationship between fine motor skill development and digital device use in children is currently under debate. However, the evidence of recent the literature remains inconclusive, and there is a lack of studies specifically focused on the different content of digital devices.

The recent review by Arabiat and colleagues (2022) [36] reported that interactive digital technology was negatively associated or unrelated to motor skills. Moon et al. (2019) [37] reported that smart device use was positively related with fine motor skills in children aged three, but no significant relationship was found for children aged four to five years.

Other evidence of observational studies suggests a negative association between digital tools use in children and fine motor abilities [38,39,40,41]. On the contrary, other evidence showed an improvement in fine motor skills in children who use new digital tools (e.g., tablets) [42,43,44,45]. Probably, these discordant data are the consequence of the heterogeneity of the studies and of the difficulty to accurately measure the exposure time to digital devices. However, most studies agree that “active” digital technologies (tablets, smartphones, etc.) have a better impact on children’s developmental skills than “passive” ones (television, video viewing, etc.). Furthermore, high-quality learning apps would be preferable, and co-viewing by an adult is very important to mediate the use of the device in order to improve the child’s learning experience.

Based on this evidence, the principal aim of our research was to evaluate the relationship between fine motor skills and digital device use in children aged three to five years. The secondary aim was to explore the modality of use of digital tool in this age group and to evaluate how some socio-demographic variables can affect the relation between fine motor abilities and digital device use.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Our cross-sectional observational study explored digital devices use and fine motor abilities in children, recruited from four kindergartens of the city of Salerno (Italy).

We included all the children aged between 37–72 months; the exclusion criteria were: (i) medical or neuropsychiatric diagnoses that could affect neuropsychomotor development (autism spectrum disorder, intellectual disability, language disorder, psychomotor development delay), (ii) poor child compliance to perform neuropsychological evaluation, and (iii) poor parental compliance.

The parents of all the participants had a preliminary meeting with a child neuropsychiatrist, who informed them about the methods and the purpose of the study. Written informed consent was collected for all the participants.

All the parents that took part in the study completed the Digital Devices Questionnaire, a non-standardized tool that investigated the frequency and the modality of digital device use by their children.

Subsequently, the children underwent a standardized neuropsychological evaluation, administered by two child neuropsychologists, aimed to assess fine motor skills (APCM-2–Abilità Prassiche e della Coordinazione Motoria–2a Edizione) [46].

Our study was conducted in agreement with the Helsinki declaration and was approved by the Campania Sud Ethics Committee (number of the protocol = 0033986 of 5 March 2019).

2.2. Questionnaire on Digital Device Use

The questionnaire on digital devices is composed of two sections: a first part, in which general data, socio-demographic information, and medical history (age, sex, household members, siblings number, parents’ educational background and employment, pregnancy, childbirth, psycho-motor progression, and medical conditions) were collected, and a second part examining the use of digital devices in children, based on 12 items, as follows:

  • digital devices provided at home

  • children’s favorite tools

  • age from when digital device use started

  • medium daily use time

  • modality of use (with/without parents’ monitoring)

  • chosen content (with/without dialogues)

  • parent control (independent choice of children/choice guided by the parent)

  • behaviors implemented through the use of digital tool (level of frustration, reaction to names, social attention)

  • reasons for that the parents allow digital devices to their child (to entertain, to calm, during sleep time/meal time)

  • parents’ perception of the risks associated with digital device use for their children

  • parents’ seeking advice from pediatrician regarding use of digital device in children

  • time that the child spends in social activities with their peers.

2.3. APCM-2—Abilità Prassiche e Della Coordinazione Motoria—2a Edizione

The test “Abilità Prassiche e della Coordinazione Motoria (Praxic and Motor Coordination Skills)—2nd Edition” (APCM-2) is an Italian test developed and validated by Sabbadini [46]. This test aims to assess motor and praxis skills in children aged between 2–8 years. This tool includes 6 forms, divided into different by age subgroups: 24–36 months (short version and complete versions), 37–48 months, 49–60 months, 61–72 months, and 6.1–8 years. This test includes the evaluation of two scales: (a) motor strategies (eye motions, movements of hand and fingers, balance and coordination, sequencing), and (b) adaptive cognitive processes (symbolic gestures, constructive praxis abilities, manual skills, graphomotor skills, dynamic coordination). In our research, we considered only the last scale mentioned. Raw scores were converted into weighted scores and percentiles scores (a score <5° percentile was considered under the norm).

The standardization sample comprised 261 children aged from three–eight (54% = boys, 46% = girls). Internal consistency for both scales (motor strategies and adaptive cognitive processes) was higher than 0.75.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

We expressed the continuous variables as mean ± standard deviation and categorical variables as proportions/percentage.

We performed the Shapiro-Wilk normality test to verify the data distribution. Due to the presence of data with non-normal distribution, we employed non-parametric tests for our analysis. We performed a Spearman correlation test and multivariate regression analysis in order to explore the relationship between the different variables; p < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Statistical Package for Social Science, version 23.0 (IBM Corp, 2015) was employed to analyze all data.

3. Results

3.1. Sample Characteristics

We recruited 185 children (105 males = 57%; 80 females = 43%) aged between 37–72 months (mean = 55.02 ± 9.50). All kindergarten children were recruited (except two children with certified disabilities). All the subjects recruited agreed to participate in the study. All parents filled in the questionnaires, and all children had good compliance with the APCM-2 test, so there was no missing data.

The socio-demographic features of the sample are reported in Table 1.

Table 1.

Sample characteristics. m = mean; SD = standard deviation; MS = middle school; HS = High School; UN = University; U/H = unemployed/housewife; SW = skilled worker; OW/T = office worker, teacher; SE = self-employed.

Total Sample
Sample Size n = 185
Sex
male 105 (57%)
female 80 (43%)
Age—m ± SD 55.02 ± 9.50 months
Pregnancy problems 29 (16%)
Birth problems 33 (18%)
Perinatal problems 15 (8%)
Birth week—m ± SD 38.67 ± 2.12
Birth weight—m ± SD 3.16 ± 0.55 kg
Crawling 44 (24%)
First step age m ± SD 13.22 ± 2.5 months
First word age m ± SD 14.85 ± 6.68 months
Medical pathologies 29 (16%)
Family members 4.08 ± 0.98
Siblings —m ± SD 0.93 ± 0.85
Mother age—m ± SD 37.64 ± 4.84 years
Father age—m ± SD 41.23 ± 5.74 years
Educational level MS HS UN
Mother 21 (11%) 70 (38%) 94 (51%)
Father 32 (17%) 79 (43%) 71 (38%)
Job U/H SW OW/T SE
Mother 45 (24%) 20 (11%) 71 (38%) 47 (25%)
Father 1 (0.5%) 45 (24%) 69 (37%) 65 (35%)

3.2. Use of Digital Devices by Children

In our sample, all the children (185/185, 100%) used of at least one digital device, as reported by their parents, and the digital devices that they preferred were television (134/185, 72%), smartphone (132/185, 71%), and tablet (75/185, 41%).

The first digital tool that children used was television (mean age = 17.15 ± 8.99 months), followed by smartphone (29.56 ± 13.30 months) and tablet (31.66 ± 12.41 months).

In our sample, the children spent a total mean time on digital device of 3.08 ± 2.30 h/day, preferring television (1.57 ± 1.22 h/day) and smartphone (0.88 ± 1.02 h/day).

In our sample, the parents reported that most of the children used the digital devices with their parents (119/185, 64%) or brothers/sisters (21/185, 11%) or with parental control (31/185, 18%), and an amount of 11% of children used digital devices alone (21/185).

All the children preferred contents with dialogues (185/185, 100%), and almost half of them played videogames (91/185, 49%).

The parents allowed their children to use digital devices for entertainment (129/185, 70%), during mealtimes (56/185, 30%), to calm the child (46/185, 25%), and before the child went to sleep (24/185, 13%).

Some parents reported absorbent behaviors in their child when they used digital devices. In particular, some children did not respond when called (10/185, 5%), did not interact with others (3/185, 2%), or appeared frustrated when the parents tried to lure them away from the device (14/185, 7%).

Sleep problems are reported in about 19% (36/185) of the children.

Finally, 43% of parents (80/185) were concerned about the consequences of the digital device use on their children health, but only 18% of them (34/185) turned to the pediatrician for advice. These results were summarized in Table 2.

Table 2.

Questionnaire on digital device use; m = mean; SD = standard deviation; DD = digital devices; * (only for children ≥12 months, total sample size = 236).

Total Sample
Sample Size 185
Use of DD by children
(at least one) 185 (100%)
DD available at home
Smartphone 131 (71%)
Tablet 100 (54%)
Personal Computer 45 (24%)
Television 175 (95%)
Videogames/console 18 (10%)
Children’s favorite DD
Smartphone 132 (71%)
Tablet 75 (41%)
Personal Computer 17 (9%)
Television 134 (72%)
Videogames/console 17 (9%)
Age of start using DD months (m ± SD)
Smartphone 29.56 ± 13.30
Tablet 31.66 ± 12.41
Personal Computer 34.90 ± 16.74
Television 17.15 ± 8.99
Videogames/console 43.71 ± 16.88
Time of use DD h/day (m ± SD)
Smartphone 0.88 ± 1.02
Tablet 0.43 ± 0.71
Personal Computer 0.14 ± 0.50
Videogames 0.06 ± 0.22
Television 1.57 ± 1.22
Total Time 3.08 ± 2.30
Modality of use
With parent/caregiver 119 (64%)
With brother/sister 21 (11%)
With parent-control 34 (18%)
alone 21 (11%)
Favorite contents
With dialogue 185 (100%)
Without dialogue 17 (6%)
Videogames 91 (49%)
Contents selection
Independent choice 150 (81%)
Choice guided by parents 35 (19%)
Reasons for granting DD
To entertain 129 (70%)
To calm down 46 (25%)
To let the child sleep 24 (13%)
During mealtime 56 (30%)
Concerns about DD use
Yes 80 (43%)
No 105 (57%)
Request to the pediatrician
Yes 34 (18%)
No 151 (82%)
Behavior during DD use
absent partial immediate
response to name 10 (5%) 110 (59%) 67 (36%)
absent partial adequate
Social attention 3 (2%) 31 (17%) 150 (81%)
high medium low
Frustration 14 (7%) 83 (45%) 90 (49%)
Social activities *
Less than once a week 9 (5%)
About once a week 48 (26%)
Several times a week 91 (49%)
Often or every day 41 (22%)
Sleep problems 36 (19%)

3.3. Relation between Digital Devices Use and Fine Motor Skills in Children

We did not find statistically significant relationship between the APCM-2 scores and digital device daily time use (smartphone, tablet, personal computer, videogames) (Table 3).

Table 3.

Spearman correlation test between fine motor skills and use of digital device. p < 0.05 are in bold.

Daily Time of Digital Device Use
Smartphone Tablet Computer Television Videogames Total
APCM-2 (fine motor skills)
Weighted score r = −0.095
p = 0.213
r = 0.014
p = 0.851
r = 0.007
p = 0.931
r = 0.007
p = 0.922
r = −0.029
p = 0.703
r = −0.036
p = 0.640
Percentile score r = −0.075
p = 0.326
r = 0.005
p = 0.952
r = 0.074
p = 0.330
r = 0.026
p = 0.731
r = 0.004
p = 0.958
r = −0.002
p = 0.983
Start Age of Digital Device Use
Smartphone Tablet Computer Television Videogames Total
APCM-2 (fine motor skills)
Weighted score r = 0.219
p = 0.008
r = 0.277
p = 0.008
r = 0.313
p = 0.098
r = 0.157
p = 0.064
r = 0.354
p = 0.235
r = 0.200
p = 0.009
Percentile score r = 0.046
p = 0.584
r = 0.129
p = 0.226
r = 0.221
p = 0.249
r = 0.091
p = 0.284
r = −0.120
p = 0.695
r = 0.044
p = 0.564

We found a positive significant relationship between start age of use smartphone/tablet and the APCM-2 weighted score (Table 3; Figure 1).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Relationship between age of start using smartphone and APCM-2 weighted score (r = 0.200; p = 0.009.). x axis = age of start using smartphone (in months); y axis = APCM-2 weighted score.

To exclude the effect of age on this relationship, we performed a multivariate regression analysis. Adding age as a covariate the relationship between start age of use and the APCM-2 weighted score is no longer statistically significant (smartphone p = 0.109, ß = 0.239; tablet p = 0.056, ß = 0.393; total p = 0.297, ß =0.178).

We found that total daily time of digital device use, daily time of smartphone use, and daily time of television use were negatively related to age to start using digital device (Table 4; Figure 2).

Table 4.

Spearman correlation test between daily time of digital device use and other socio-demographic characteristics. p value < 0.05 are in bold.

Daily Time of Use DD
Smartphone Tablet Computer Television Videogames Total
Start Age digital Device Use r = −0.321
p < 0.001
r = 0.074
p = 0.480
r = −0.060
p = 0.755
r = −0.183
p = 0.023
r = −0.270
p = 0.350
r = −0.202
p = 0.012
sex r = −0.144
p = 0.051
r = 0.102
p = 0.168
r = 0.068
p = 0.361
r = −0.095
p = 0.198
r = 0.155
p = 0.036
r = 0.056
p = 0.445
age r = 0.040
p = 0.587
r = 0.068
p = 0.357
r = −0.069
p = 0.354
r = −0.096
p = 0.194
r = 0.111
p = 0.132
r = 0.034
p = 0.641
Father age r = −0.038
p = 0.609
r = 0.012
p = 0.868
r = −0.005
p = 0.944
r = 0.668
p = 0.362
r = 0.046
p = 0.537
r = 0.021
p = 0.774
Father Degree r = −0.157
p = 0.034
r = 0.028
p = 0.710
r = −0.132
p = 0.076
r = −0.011
p = 0.884
r = −0.036
p = 0.630
r = 0.068
p = 0.365
Father job r = 0.205
p = 0.006
r = 0.100
p = 0.183
r = 0.058
p = 0.442
r = 0.030
p = 0.685
r = −0.015
p = 0.838
r = 0.164
p = 0.028
Mother age r = −0.086
p = 0.251
r = −0.054
p = 0.468
r = 0.079
p = 0.292
r = −0.032
p = 0.666
r = 0.057
p = 0.446
r = −0.071
p = 0.342
Mother degree r = 0.104
p = 0.157
r = 0.004
p = 0.955
r = −0.170
p = 0.021
r = −0.050
p = 0.500
r = 0.069
p = 0.353
r = 0.032
p = 0.670
Mother job r = 0.218
p = 0.003
r = 0.073
p = 0.324
r = −0.053
p = 0.475
r = 0.003
p = 0.972
r = 0.048
p = 0.523
r = 0.154
p = 0.037

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Relationship between age of start using smartphone and daily time of smartphone use r = 0.202; p = 0.012). x axis = Daily time of smartphone use (in hours); y axis = age of start using smartphone (in months).

Daily time of smartphone use was positively related to the mothers’ and fathers’ jobs and negatively related to the fathers’ degree (Table 4).

Daily time of personal computer use was negatively related to the mothers’ degree (Table 4).

Daily total time of all digital device use was positively related to the mothers’ and fathers’ jobs (Table 4).

The age of start to use all digital device was positively related to the age of child, except for television (Table 5; Figure 3).

Table 5.

Spearman correlation test between age of start to use digital device and other socio-demographic characteristics. p value < 0.05 are in bold.

Start Age of Use DD
Smartphone Tablet Computer Television Videogames Total
Daily Time of Digital Device Use r = −0.321
p < 0.001
r = 0.074
p = 0.480
r = −0.060
p = 0.755
r = −0.183
p = 0.023
r = −0.270
p = 0.350
r = −0.202
p = 0.012
sex r = −0.012
p = 0.886
r = 0.023
p = 0.824
r = 0.079
p = 0.678
r = 0.030
p = 0.721
r = 0.226
p = 0.437
r = 0.037
p = 0.621
age r = 0.363
p < 0.001
r = 0.350
p = 0.001
r = 0.559
p = 0.001
r = 0.139
p = 0.092
r = 0.702
p = 0.005
r = 0.424
p < 0.001
Father age r = 0.071
p = 0.380
r = −0.037
p = 0.724
r = 0.181
p = 0.348
r = −0.013
p = 0.876
r = 0.373
p = 0.189
r = −0.013
p = 0.859
Father Degree r = 0.031
p = 0.705
r = 0.309
p = 0.003
r = 0.155
p = 0.423
r = −0.052
p = 0.536
r = 0.185
p = 0.526
r = −0.013
p = 0.859
Father job r = 0.074
p = 0.367
r = 0.173
p = 0.097
r = −0.037
p = 0.856
r = −0.006
p = 0.947
r = 0.086
p = 0.779
r = 0.171
p = 0.023
Mother age r = 0.123
p = 0.130
r = −0.070
p = 0.505
r = 0.183
p = 0.334
r = 0.145
p = 0.079
r = 0.010
p = 0.972
r = −0.007
p = 0.928
Mother degree r = −0.046
p = 0.567
r = 0.271
p = 0.008
r = −0.239
p = 0.204
r = 0.126
p = 0.127
r = 0.167
p = 0.568
r = 0.082
p = 0.268
Mother job r = 0.084
p = 0.303
r = 0.121
p = 0.244
r = −0.238
p = 0.204
r = −0.094
p = 0.256
r = 0.065
p = 0.832
r = 0.137
p = 0.066

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Relationship between children age and age of start using smartphone (r = 0.424; p < 0.001). x axis = children age (in months); y axis = age of start using smartphone (in months).

The age of starting use of tablets was positive related to the fathers’ and mothers’ degrees (Table 5).

The age of starting use any digital device was positive related to the fathers’ job (Table 5).

4. Discussion

The principal aim of our research was to explore the digital devices use in children aged between three to five years and their relationship with fine motor abilities. We considered a sample of 185 typically developing children (57% male; mean age = 55.02 ± 9.50 months; Table 1) that performed a standardized test that assessed fine motor abilities (APCM-2), while the modality of use of digital device was explored through a questionnaire addressed to their parents.

Regarding the first question of our study, we did not find a significant relation between time of digital device use and fine motor abilities in our sample (Table 3). In particular, the time spent on digital devices was not significantly associated with neither an improvement, nor a deterioration, of performance in grapho-motor skills and manual coordination. We must underline that we have not performed qualitative analyses on the modality of digital device use (use of “educational” applications, “active” or “passive” use of digital tools, and possible parent/caregiver involvement during the use).

We found a relationship between the first age of use of digital device and fine motor skills (Table 4) (the children who used digital tools early had worse fine motor performance). However, this result cannot be considered significant because it is affected by the effect of the age of children on this relationship.

We also found that some socio-demographic characteristics were associated with the daily time of use of digital tool, such as the age of first use (Table 4). Interestingly, the correlation analysis showed that children who used digital devices earlier were those who spent more hour/day with them. It would be interesting, in future studies, to explore the relationship between the first age of use of digital tools by children and the time spent with them in the school age and in adolescence, in consideration of the growing phenomenon of “digital addiction”.

In addition, we found that the start age of use of digital device was also related to the age of the child (Table 5), confirming the phenomenon of “digital anticipation”, because younger children use digital technologies more and more precociously [1,2,3,4].

Another significant correlation was found between the daily time on digital devices/age of start to use digital device and the parent’s job. The children who had working parents spent more time on digital devices (Table 4) and used digital devices earlier (Table 5). In this regard, we must not forget the fundamental role played by parents in digital device exposure [12,13].

Finally, the correlation analysis did not show significant differences between males and females in the modality and in times of use of digital devices, except for video games, which were preferred by males (Table 4 and Table 5).

Our research agreed with the cross-sectional study of Moon and colleagues (2019) [37]. The authors enrolled 117 children between three to five years, divided into three age groups, and administered to their parents a questionnaire to evaluated the relationship between smart device use and language, as well as motor and social skills. In children aged three years, the authors found that smart device use was positively related with fine motor skills and negatively associated with language, but no significant relationship was found for children aged four to five years.

Contrary to our study, there is some evidence of both negative and positive correlation between digital device use and fine motor abilities.

The review of Arabiat and colleagues [37], which considered 53 studies on children under seven years, concluded that children’s use of interactive digital tools was positively associated with language abilities and negatively associated or unrelated to motor skills. Furthermore, the authors highlighted that the measurements of intensity and duration of digital device exposure were often imprecise.

The study of Zheng and Sun (2021) [41], which involved 877 children aged three-six years, concluded that the time spent on digital tools negatively predict fine and gross motor skills of children (particularly the passive use was negatively associated to school performance and fine motor abilities).

Kiefer et al. (2015) [38] considered 23 preschool children divided into two groups, the first group of 11 children using the keyboard and the second of 12 children using handwriting, with the aim of determining whether there was a difference between the two groups in writing skills. The authors found that the handwriting group had better writing skills.

Lin et al., (2017) [39] monitored 40 preschool children who used tablets for more than 60 min per week for 24 weeks, comparing them to 40 age- and gender-matched children participating in a hand skills program. The authors found that motor performance improved only in the control group.

In another study by Lin (2019) [40], the authors compared 36 tablet-using children and 36 non-tablet-using preschoolers, showing that the children in the non-tablet group performed significantly better in fine motor tasks.

Some studies also found some benefits in fine motor abilities of children using an iPad [42] or notebook application [44]. On the contrary, Axford et al. (2018) [42] suggested no benefits for children that use an iPad over 30 min/day.

The cross-section study of Souto et al. (2020) [45] considered 78 children aged between 24–48 months, divided into two groups: frequent use of tablet (n = 26) and no experience of tablet use (n = 52). The Bayley Scale of Infant Development III was performed to explore the relationship between fine motor abilities and interactive tablet use. Most of the children performed both active and passive activities, usually accompanied by their parents, not exceeding the recommended times. The major finding of the study was that the children using tablet performed slightly better than the other group.

This apparent discrepancy between our results and those found in the literature may be due to the fact that, in our study, we only considered the total time of digital device use, but we did not consider other factors, such as the employing of “educational” applications and the role of parents while children used the digital tools.

Analyzing the digital device questionnaire, we found that all the children used at least one of the digital tools (smartphones, tablets, television, personal computers, or videogames) (Table 2). These data agree with the recent literature studies, according to which the use of digital tools in pre-school age has greatly increased, reflecting both the increasing use of digital tools by families and contemporary society [1,4].

The children preferred television (72%), smartphone (71%), and tablet (41%) (Table 2). Comparing these data with our previous research, we can observe an increase in the preference for the Tablet in children aged three to five years vs. children under three years [6].

The digital tool used first by children was television (mean age of first use = 17.15 ± 8.99 months), followed by smartphone (29.56 ± 13.30 months), tablet (31.66 ± 12.41 months), PC (34.90 ± 16.74 months), and videogames (43.71 ± 16.88 months) (Table 2). These results, compared to those of children under the age of three, show that, in younger children, there is an increasingly precocious exposure to digital devices (for example, in children between the ages of three to five, the first use of the smartphone was at 29.56 ± 13.30 months, while, in children under three, it was 15.6 ± 5.8 months). This “anticipation” phenomenon had already been underlined in several previous studies [6,47,48].

The parents reported that the mean daily use of digital device was about 3 h/day, according to many recent survey [1,2,3,4] (Table 2). The most commonly used digital tools are television (1.57 ± 1.22 h/day), the smartphone (0.88 ± 1.02 h/day), and the tablet (0.43 ± 0.71) (Table 2). It is important to underline that, compared to children under the age of three, children in our sample spent an average of one hour more a day using digital tools [6], exceeding the limits recommended by scientific societies [30,31].

An amount of 64% of children used digital devices with parent/caregiver or siblings (11%), but 11% and 18% of them used digital tools alone or with automatic parental control, respectively (Table 2).

The parents allowed their children to use digital devices to entertain (70%), during mealtime (30%), to calm down (25%), and to let the children sleep (13%), suggesting that the parents mainly using these tools as “peacekeeper” while they are engaged in other activities [12,13,49] (Table 2).

Some parents reported atypical behaviors in their children during the digital device use: the children did not respond when called (5%), they did not interact (2%), and they appeared frustrated when the digital tool was take away (7%) (Table 2). This result confirms that digital devices use in some children can compromise social interaction and lead to an emotional deregulation [36,37].

In our sample, sleep problems were reported in 19% of children (Table 2). A relationship between sleep problems and excessive digital media use by children had already been highlighted [50,51].

An amount of 43% of parents say they are concerned about the digital devices use on their child’s health. However, only 18% of them asked pediatricians for advice on this topic (Table 2).

This study has several limitations. First of all, the use of parental questionnaires to monitor the modality of use of digital tools is not free from possible bias. Furthermore, the study only included children aged three to five years, which limits the generalizability of its findings to other age groups. Additionally, the study did not investigate all potential confounding factors that could influence the relationship between digital device use and fine motor skills. Finally, we did not consider other environmental factors that could impact childrens’ digital habits, such as time spent on digital devices by their parents, as well as the use of “educational apps”, and we did not differentiate between “active” or “passive” co-viewing. We, therefore, propose to explore these aspects in future research.

The strength of our work is the sample size and the use of standardized and direct neuropsychological tests to assess fine motor skills in children.

The results of our study have several practical implications, as they help to better define the possible impact of digital devices on the psychomotor development of children. Based on our results, it would be useful to monitor the time spent by children on digital tools, the age of first use, but also the modality of digital device use in order to ensure the most correct use possible. Our results highlighted, also, that most parents are concerned about the effect of digital tools on their children’s health, but only few of them ask for the support of pediatricians. Therefore, it would be important to implement the communication on this topic through information campaigns. In the future, it would be interesting to perform prospective studies that allow us to monitor the relationship between the digital devices use and other development skills (academic skills, as well as emotional and social-relational skills) during school age and adolescence, also with the aim of prevent the phenomena of digital addiction.

5. Conclusions

In our study, the children spent an average of about three hours a day on digital devices. We did not find a significant relationship between the time of use of digital devices and fine motor skills.

The youngest children experienced digital tools at an earlier age, and they were also the ones who used digital tools more time afterwards. This trend should be monitored over time in order to prevent the phenomenon of “digital addiction”, especially in adolescents.

Future studies are needed to confirm these data and further explore the relationship between digital device use and fine motor skills, taking into account other aspects, such as content of digital device.

In the meantime, parents and pediatricians should be aware that the use of digital devices in children can bring both risks and benefits. Therefore, it is important not to exceed the time of use, to prefer active co-viewing with parents, and to encourage the use of quality educational applications.

Acknowledgments

We thank all the participants for the time spent to take part in our research.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: F.F.O., G.M.G.P., V.d.S. and G.C. (Giangennaro Coppola); Data curation, V.d.S., A.P., G.C. (Gabriella Citro), M.O. and S.A.; Formal analysis: G.M.G.P. and A.V.; Methodology: F.F.O., G.M.G.P., A.B. and A.O.; Supervision: A.V., F.F.O. and G.C. (Giangennaro Coppola); Writing—original draft: G.M.G.P. and M.O; Writing—review and editing: G.M.G.P. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Campania Sud Ethics Committee (protocol number = 0033986; det. N. 32–05 March 2019).

Informed Consent Statement

Written informed consent has been obtained from the parents of the participants to publish this paper.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available upon request from the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Funding Statement

This research received no external funding.

Footnotes

Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

References

  • 1.Kılıç A.O., Sari E., Yucel H., Oğuz M.M., Polat E., Acoglu E.A., Senel S. Exposure to and use of mobile devices in children aged 1–60 months. Eur. J. Nucl. Med. Mol. Imaging. 2019;178:221–227. doi: 10.1007/s00431-018-3284-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Paudel S., Jancey J., Subedi N., Leavy J. Correlates of mobile screen media use among children aged 0–8: A systematic review. BMJ Open. 2017;7:e014585. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-014585. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Akbayin M., Mulliez A., Fortin F., Olagne M.V., Laporte C., Vorilhon P. Screen exposure time of children under 6 years old: A French cross-sectional survey in general practices in the Auvergne-Rhône-Alpes region. BMC Prim. Care. 2023;24:58. doi: 10.1186/s12875-023-02009-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Rideout V., Saphir M., Pai S., Rudd A. Zero to Eight: Children’s Media Use in America 2013. Common Sense Media; San Francisco, CA, USA: 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kabali H.K., Irigoyen M.M., Nunez-Davis R., Budacki J.G., Mohanty S.H., Leister K.P., Bonner R.L., Jr. Exposure and Use of Mobile Media Devices by Young Children. Pediatrics. 2015;136:1044–1050. doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-2151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Operto F.F., Pastorino G.M.G., Marciano J., de Simone V., Volini A.P., Olivieri M., Buonaiuto R., Vetri L., Viggiano A., Coppola G. Digital Devices Use and Language Skills in Children between 8 and 36 Month. Brain Sci. 2020;10:656. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10090656. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Radesky J.S., Christakis D.A. Increased Screen Time: Implications for Early Childhood Development and Behavior. Pediatr. Clin. N. Am. 2016;63:827–839. doi: 10.1016/j.pcl.2016.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Radesky J.S., Eisenberg S., Kistin C.J., Gross J., Block G., Zuckerman B., Silverstein M. Overstimulated Consumers or Next-Generation Learners? Parent Tensions About Child Mobile Technology Use. Ann. Fam. Med. 2016;14:503–508. doi: 10.1370/afm.1976. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Coenen P., Howiea E., Campbella A., Strakera L. Mobile touch screen device use among young Australian children–first results from a national survey; Proceedings of the 19th Triennial Congress of the IEA; Melbourne, Australia. 9–14 August 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Dinleyici M., Carman K.B., Ozturk E., Sahin-Dagli F. Media use by children and parent’s view on children’s media usage. Interact. J. Med. Res. 2016;5:e5668. doi: 10.2196/ijmr.5668. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Rideout V.J., Hamel E. The Media Family: Electronic Media in the Lives of Infants, Toddlers, Preschoolers, and Their Parents. Kaiser Family Foundation; Menlo Park, CA, USA: 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Radesky J.S., Weeks H.M., Ball R., Schaller A., Yeo S., Durnez J., Tamayo-Rios M., Epstein M., Kirkorian H., Coyne S., et al. Young Children’s Use of Smartphones and Tablets. Pediatrics. 2020;146:e20193518. doi: 10.1542/peds.2019-3518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Radesky J.S., Kaciroti N., Weeks H.M., Schaller A., Miller A.L. Longitudinal Associations Between Use of Mobile Devices for Calming and Emotional Reactivity and Executive Functioning in Children Aged 3 to 5 Years. JAMA Pediatr. 2023;177:62. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2022.4793. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Yildiz D., Yalcin S.S. Change in screen time of preschool children according to parental attitudes during the COVID-19 pandemic with an online survey. Int. J. Environ. Health Res. 2023:1–10. doi: 10.1080/09603123.2023.2179027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Fitzpatrick C., Binet M.-A., Harvey E., Barr R., Couture M., Garon-Carrier G. Preschooler screen time and temperamental anger/frustration during the COVID-19 pandemic. Pediatr. Res. 2023:1–6. doi: 10.1038/s41390-023-02485-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Montag C., Elhai J.D. Discussing digital technology overuse in children and adolescents during the COVID-19 pandemic and beyond: On the importance of considering Affective Neuroscience Theory. Addict. Behav. Rep. 2020;12:100313. doi: 10.1016/j.abrep.2020.100313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Gueron-Sela N., Shalev I., Gordon-Hacker A., Egotubov A., Barr R. Screen media exposure and behavioral adjustment in early childhood during and after COVID-19 home lockdown periods. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2023;140:107572. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2022.107572. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Smirni D., Garufo E., Di Falco L., Lavanco G. The Playing Brain. The Impact of Video Games on Cognition and Behavior in Pediatric Age at the Time of Lockdown: A Systematic Review. Pediatr. Rep. 2021;13:401–415. doi: 10.3390/pediatric13030047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Reid Chassiakos Y.L., Radesky J., Christakis D., Moreno M.A., Cross C., Council on Communications and Media. Hill D., Ameenuddin N., Hutchinson J., Levine A., et al. Children and Adolescents and Digital Media. Am. Acad. Pediatrics. 2016;138:e20162593. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-2593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Lin H.-P., Chen K.-L., Chou W., Yuan K.-S., Yen S.-Y., Chen Y.-S., Chow J.C. Prolonged touch screen device usage is associated with emotional and behavioral problems, but not language delay, in toddlers. Infant Behav. Dev. 2020;58:101424. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2020.101424. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Stiglic N., Viner R.M. Effects of screentime on the health and well-being of children and adolescents: A systematic review of reviews. BMJ Open. 2019;9:e023191. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-023191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Lawrence A., Choe D.E. Mobile Media and Young Children’s Cognitive Skills: A Review. Acad. Pediatr. 2021;21:996–1000. doi: 10.1016/j.acap.2021.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Madigan S., McArthur B.A., Anhorn C., Eirich R., Christakis D.A. Associations Between Screen Use and Child Language Skills: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174:665–675. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.0327. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Reus E.J., Mosley I.T. The health and development correlates of screen media exposure in children 0–5 yrs: An integrative liter-ature review. Aust. J. Child Fam. Health Nurs. 2018;15:12. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Guellai B., Somogyi E., Esseily R., Chopin A. Effects of screen exposure on young children’s cognitive development: A review. Front. Psychol. 2022;13:4779. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.923370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Linebarger D.L., Vaala S.E. Screen media and language development in infants and toddlers: An ecological perspective. Dev. Rev. 2010;30:176–202. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2010.03.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Xie H., Peng J., Qin M., Huang X., Tian F., Zhou Z. Can Touchscreen Devices be Used to Facilitate Young Children’s Learning? A Meta-Analysis of Touchscreen Learning Effect. Front. Psychol. 2018;9:2580. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02580. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Lauricella A.R., Wartella E., Rideout V.J. Young children’s screen time: The complex role of parent and child factors. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2015;36:11–17. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2014.12.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Kulakci-Altintas H. Technological Device Use Among 0–3 Year Old Children and Attitudes and Behaviors of Their Parents Towards Technological Devices. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2019;29:55–61. doi: 10.1007/s10826-019-01457-x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Guidelines on Physical Activity, Sedentary Behaviour and Sleep for Children under 5 Years of Age. World Health Organization; Geneva, Switzerland: 2019. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Bozzola E., Spina G., Ruggiero M., Memo L., Agostiniani R., Bozzola M., Corsello G., Villani A. Media devices in pre-school children: The recommendations of the Italian pediatric society. Ital. J. Pediatr. 2018;44:69. doi: 10.1186/s13052-018-0508-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Gerber R.J., Wilks T., Erdie-Lalena C. Developmental Milestones: Motor Development. Pediatr. Rev. 2010;31:267–277. doi: 10.1542/pir.31-7-267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Strooband K.F.B., De Rosnay M., Okely A.D. Prevalence and risk factors of pre-schoolers’ fine motor delay within vulnerable Australian communities. J. Paediatr. Child Health. 2020;57:114–120. doi: 10.1111/jpc.15152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Strooband K.F.B.M., de Rosnay M., Okely A.D., Veldman S.L.C. Systematic Review and Meta-Analyses: Motor Skill Interventions to Improve Fine Motor Development in Children Aged Birth to 6 Years. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2020;41:319–331. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000779. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Flores P., Coelho E., Mourão-Carvalhal M.I., Forte P. Association between motor and math skills in preschool children with typical development: Systematic review. Front. Psychol. 2023;14:1105391. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1105391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Arabiat D., Al Jabery M., Robinson S., Whitehead L., Mörelius E. Interactive technology use and child development: A systematic review. Child Care Health Dev. 2022:1–37. doi: 10.1111/cch.13082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Moon J., Cho S.Y., Lim S.M., Roh J.H., Koh M.S., Kim Y.J., Nam E. Smart device usage in early childhood is differentially associated with fine motor and language development. Acta Paediatr. 2019;108:903–910. doi: 10.1111/apa.14623. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Kiefer M., Schuler S., Mayer C., Trumpp N.M., Hille K., Sachse S. Handwriting or typewriting? The influence of pen or key-board-based writing training on reading and writing performance in preschool children. Adv. Cogn. Psychol. 2015;11:136–146. doi: 10.5709/acp-0178-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Lin L.-Y., Cherng R.-J., Chen Y.-J. Effect of Touch Screen Tablet Use on Fine Motor Development of Young Children. Phys. Occup. Ther. Pediatr. 2017;37:457–467. doi: 10.1080/01942638.2016.1255290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Lin L.-Y. Differences between preschool children using tablets and non-tablets in visual perception and fine motor skills. Hong Kong J. Occup. Ther. 2019;32:118–126. doi: 10.1177/1569186119888698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Zheng P., Sun J. Preschool Children’s Use of Digital Devices and Early Development in Hong Kong: The Role of Family Socioeconomic Status. Early Educ. Dev. 2021;33:893–911. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2021.1920767. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Axford C., Joosten A.V., Harris C. iPad applications that required a range of motor skills promoted motor coordination in children commencing primary school. Aust. Occup. Ther. J. 2018;65:146–155. doi: 10.1111/1440-1630.12450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Bedford R., de Urabain I.R.S., Cheung C.H.M., Karmiloff-Smith A., Smith T.J. Toddlers’ Fine Motor Milestone Achievement Is Associated with Early Touchscreen Scrolling. Front. Psychol. 2016;7:1108. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Bonneton-Botté N., Fleury S., Girard N., Le Magadou M., Cherbonnier A., Renault M., Anquetil E., Jamet E. Can tablet apps support the learning of handwriting? An investigation of learning outcomes in kindergarten classroom. Comput. Educ. 2020;151:103831. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2020.103831. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Souto P.H.S., Santos J.N., Leite H.R., Hadders-Algra M., Guedes S.C., Nobre J.N.P., Santos L.R., Morais R.L.D.S. Tablet Use in Young Children is Associated with Advanced Fine Motor Skills. J. Mot. Behav. 2020;52:196–203. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2019.1602505. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Sabbadini A. APCM-2-Abilità Prassiche e della Coordinazione Motoria–2a Edizione. Hogrefe; Firenze, Italy: 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Chang H.Y., Park E.-J., Yoo H.-J., Lee J.W., Shin Y. Electronic Media Exposure and Use among Toddlers. Psychiatry Investig. 2018;15:568–573. doi: 10.30773/pi.2017.11.30.2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Vandewater E.A., Rideout V.J., Wartella E.A., Huang X., Lee J.H., Shim M.-S. Digital Childhood: Electronic Media and Technology Use Among Infants, Toddlers, and Preschoolers. Pediatrics. 2007;119:e1006–e1015. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-1804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Radesky J.S., Peacock-Chambers E., Zuckerman B., Silverstein M. Use of mobile technology to calm upset children: Associa-tions with social-emotional development. JAMA Pediatr. 2016;170:397–399. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.4260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Radesky J.S., Silverstein M., Zuckerman B., Christakis D.A. Infant Self-Regulation and Early Childhood Media Exposure. Pediatrics. 2014;133:e1172–e1178. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-2367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.LeBourgeois M.K., Hale L., Chang A.M., Akacem L.D., Montgomery-Downs H.E., Buxton O.M. Digital Media and Sleep in Childhood and Adolescence. Pediatrics. 2017;140((Suppl. 2)):S92–S96. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-1758J. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available upon request from the corresponding author.


Articles from Children are provided here courtesy of Multidisciplinary Digital Publishing Institute (MDPI)

RESOURCES