Skip to main content
. 2023 Mar 10;26(7):1358–1367. doi: 10.1017/S1368980023000460

Table 1.

Cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between food insecurity and intuitive eating

Analysis Food insecurity measure Unadjusted Adjusted
b se t P f 2 B se t P f 2
Cross-sectional
 Adolescence Household −0·08 0·03 2·41 0·02 0·004 −0·06 0·03 1·70 0·09 0·003
Adolescent experience −0·14 0·05 3·08 0·002 0·01 −0·10 0·05 2·27 0·02 0·01
Emerging adulthood Emerging adult experience −0·12 0·04 3·19 0·001 0·01 −0·13 0·04 3·43 < 0·001 0·01
Longitudinal
 Adolescence to emerging adulthood Household −0·11 0·03 3·33 < 0·001 0·01 −0·08 0·03 2·45 0·01 0·01
Adolescent experience −0·09 0·05 1·94 0·045 0·002 −0·04 0·05 0·87 0·38 0·001

IE, intuitive eating.

Adjusted models included parent education, race/ethnicity, sex and age as covariates; longitudinal models were additionally adjusted for baseline IE. Food-secure participants were the reference group across analyses. The unstandardised β coefficient represents the average IE difference between food-secure and food-insecure participants (after accounting for covariates in adjusted models). For example, average IE scores, which range from 1 to 4, were 0·10 lower in adolescents experiencing food insecurity than adolescents who were food-secure. Effect sizes are presented for the individual FI variable (v. the entire model); Cohen’s conventions for interpreting f 2 effect sizes are 0·02 = small, 0·15 = medium and 0·35 = large(43).