Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2023 Jul 27;18(7):e0288332. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0288332

Benefit finding and well-being over the course of the COVID-19 pandemic

Jessie B Moore 1,*, Katharine C R Rubin 1, Catherine A Heaney 1,2
Editor: Qin Xiang Ng3
PMCID: PMC10374125  PMID: 37498840

Abstract

This study focuses on understanding benefit finding, the process of deriving growth from adversity, and its relationship to well-being amidst the COVID-19 pandemic. Participants (n = 701) completed online surveys at 1, 3, 6, and 12 months after a shelter-in-place mandate was announced in California, USA. Identifying as female or of Asian descent, having a supportive social network, and reporting more distress were associated with higher levels of general benefit finding at all data collection points, while other demographics were not. Benefit finding exhibited small but statistically significant associations with two measures of well-being. Understanding the extent to which various groups of people experience benefit finding during ongoing adversity and how such benefit finding is associated with well-being may help to promote mental health during a collective trauma like the COVID-19 pandemic.

Introduction

As the world has been experiencing the COVID-19 pandemic, the popular media has focused on both the negative and positive ways in which people’s lives have changed as a result. The pandemic can be considered a world-wide collective trauma [1], defined by Hirschberger as a “cataclysmic event that shatters the basic fabric of society” [2 p. 1]. Research has documented the toll that the pandemic has had on both physical and mental health, with further knowledge accruing on a daily basis [35]. Amidst all of the suffering, some people have coped more resiliently than others and some have been able to derive some benefit from their pandemic experiences [6]. In this paper, we examine the extent to which benefit finding has been experienced during different phases of the pandemic, the demographic characteristics of those most likely to experience benefit finding, and the role of distress and social relationships in this experience. Lastly, we explore the relationship between benefit finding and well-being.

Nature of benefit finding

While the concept of experiencing transformative positive change in response to trauma is far from new, the last two decades have seen a large uptick in research on this topic. The term post-traumatic growth (PTG) has been used to describe new ways of thinking, feeling and behaving that people may experience after surviving a trauma [7]. As its name clearly implies, PTG connotes changes that occur after a traumatic event rather than during the event, often after the person has had the opportunity to reflect on the experience. However, during traumatic events of long duration and multiple phases, it is possible that such reflection may occur during the experience of the trauma. Thus, the experience of benefit finding may begin before the traumatic event is over. In this study, we explore the experience of benefit-finding at different stages of the COVID-19 pandemic when the risk of infection and mandates for protective behavior were waxing and waning.

Various types of benefits or growth have been documented in the literature. These include new perceptions of one’s abilities, strengths and resilience in the face of challenges; new ways of thinking about and relating to others; a greater appreciation for life; explorations of new life possibilities or pathways; and changes in one’s spiritual views [810]. These changes are meant to be transformative, indicating meaningful and important transitions. The extent to which these changes endure is not well-demonstrated empirically, but the theoretical expectation is that they are long-lasting [7].

When and by whom benefit finding is likely to be experienced

The current literature offers inconsistent findings in terms of the relationships between demographic characteristics and benefit finding. There is some evidence that women and people of color are more likely to experience benefit finding than are men and white people, but the findings for other demographic characteristics (e.g., marital status, age, and educational attainment) are mixed [8, 9, 11].

There is little research evidence suggesting that different types of traumas are likely to elicit varying quantities or qualities of benefit finding. In a collective trauma such as the COVID pandemic, people may experience both personal suffering (e.g., through personal infection, loss of a loved one, loss of one’s job) as well as societal suffering (e.g., over-run medical care establishments, high unemployment rates, increased preventable mortality). A few studies have demonstrated that in response to collective traumas such as natural disasters, the likelihood of experiencing changes in how one thinks about or relates to others is greater than the likelihood of other potential perceived benefits [12]. This may be because the salience of others’ coping and helping behaviors is greater when everyone in the community has to respond to the trauma.

The availability of supportive relationships has been associated with benefit finding [13]. Having supportive interactions with others may facilitate disclosure of one’s personal suffering and discussion of societal suffering more generally. Such discussions may generate more of an ability to reflect on one’s experience of the trauma and to identify positive sequelae. They may also help to generate new or revised narratives about the nature of the trauma and the effectiveness of coping alternatives. Lastly, receiving or even simply observing the provision of instrumental support by others may catalyze new beliefs about personal and collective strengths. Thus, we hypothesize that individuals that report stronger support networks will be more likely to experience subsequent benefit finding.

The levels of distress reported by people during the pandemic are likely to vary due to both personal and societal suffering. Because benefit finding is thought to be catalyzed by the distress caused by the trauma [8], we hypothesize that people who report higher levels of distress (regardless of whether the source of the distress is personal or societal) will be more likely to experience subsequent benefit finding.

The time trajectory of benefit finding has two component parts. First, how long after the trauma is benefit finding most likely to occur? Second, once experienced, how long will the positive changes inherent in benefit finding be maintained? Neither of these questions have clear answers in the extant literature. Much of the literature has been cross-sectional, thus not allowing strong inferences to be made about time trajectories. The few longitudinal studies have found a variety of different trajectories. Studies of veterans [14], breast cancer survivors [15], and young adults with cancer [16] found a fair amount of stability over time in terms of benefit finding (i.e., participants with high or low levels of benefit finding maintained those levels over time). However, there were also participants for whom the experience of benefit finding waxed and waned over time. In this study, we will explore the trajectory of benefit finding during a year of the pandemic. We hypothesize that the amount of benefit finding will increase as the pandemic progresses and individuals have more opportunity to gain insight about their experiences.

Benefit finding and well-being

The relationship between benefit finding and well-being has been much explored, but has yielded quite mixed results [79]. This may be due to variations among the research studies in terms of the nature of the traumas experienced, the timing of the measurements, and the conceptualization (and thus operationalization) of well-being. In their meta-analysis, Helgeson and her colleagues [8] found the strongest association between benefit finding and well-being when measures of positive well-being (i.e., positive affect, self-esteem, life satisfaction) were used. They found no relationship between benefit finding and subjective reports of physical health nor with comprehensive, multi-faceted measures of well-being such as quality of life. In this present study, we use both a novel broad multi-faceted measure of well-being and a more limited, well-validated measure of positive well-being. We hypothesize a positive relationship between benefit finding and subsequent well-being, particularly for the latter measure of well-being. For our broad multi-faceted measure of well-being, our analysis is more exploratory.

Methods

Overall design

Participants were recruited from an online data registry, the Stanford WELL for Life initiative [17]‬‬‬‬‬. T‬‬‬‬‬. Those who provided data at all time points were included in this study. The study was approved by the Stanford University Institutional Review Board.‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬ Informed written consent was obtained from all participants prior to the start of the study. ‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬‬

Context

The majority of participants (81.0% of our sample) resided in the San Francisco Bay Area, where a regional SIP order was imposed on March 17th 2020, mandating closure of all non-essential businesses and directing individuals to shelter at their place of residence. Essential services included grocery stores, health care facilities, pharmacies, gas stations, convenience stores, banks and laundromats. Restaurants were only permitted to offer delivery and takeout services. Nonessential gatherings of any size were banned, and residents were instructed to only leave their houses for groceries and supplies, care for family members, and outdoor exercise.

Many restrictions remained in place through the end of May 2020, and some continued throughout 2021 [18]. Restrictions in California were constantly being put in place and then lifted as rates of coronavirus infection fluctuated. For example, when first announced, masks were required in high-risk indoor settings such as grocery stores, but soon were also required outdoors when distancing was not possible (less than 6 feet), and eventually mandatory for all indoor and outdoor activities [19].

While the COVID-19 pandemic is a truly unprecedented global event that has impacted the lives of our participants, late spring and early summer 2020 also saw a wave of attention to the issues of racial injustice following the murder of George Floyd. As civil unrest marched on, the United States also endured an economic recession with record-high unemployment. A timeline of the study duration is presented in Fig 1, showing major events taking place throughout the study and the number of daily new cases of coronavirus in the San Francisco Bay Area.

Fig 1. Timeline of data collections, COVID-19 daily cases, and concurrent events.

Fig 1

Participant sample

The Stanford WELL for Life data registry is composed of people at least 18 years of age and living in the US. Data registry participants were recruited through listservs, social media, and community partnerships [20]. We sent an email to all registry participants inviting them to join our longitudinal COVID-19 study. All individuals who agreed to participate, provided informed consent, and responded to the surveys at all 4 waves of data collection are included in this study (n = 701). Token incentives (e.g., reusable water bottles) were provided as a show of gratitude for participants’ time and attention.

Measures

Benefit finding

As a measure of benefit finding, we chose to use the Post-Traumatic Growth Inventory because it had been used effectively in previous studies of collective traumas [21, 22]. The Post-Traumatic Growth Inventory (PTGI) short-form is a 10-item questionnaire used to assess positive outcomes experienced by individuals who have been through a traumatic event [23]. Five types of possible benefits are measured by two items each: Relating to Others, New Possibilities, Personal Strength, Spiritual Change, and Appreciation of Life. Participants were asked to rate the extent to which they had experienced each as a result of the COVID-19 pandemic on a scale ranging from 1 = “I did not experience this” to 6 = “I experienced this to a very great extent.”

To explore the prevalence of different types of benefit finding, we followed the suggestion of Tedeschi and colleagues [7]. We identified participants who answered either “I experienced this to a great extent” or “I experienced this to a very great extent” as having experienced significant benefit finding of the type described by each individual item. Additionally, we measured the overall prevalence of significant benefit finding as those who selected “…to a great extent” or “…to a very great extent” on at least one benefit finding questionnaire item.

Other studies have averaged all 10 inventory items to create a single overall continuous score [24, 25]. However, our factor analysis suggested using a 2-item scale for spiritual change and an 8-item scale for all other benefits (labeled general benefits). The Spiritual Benefits score and General Benefits score were calculated by taking the mean of the component items. Each score ranges from 1 to 6. The Cronbach alphas for the spiritual benefits scale and the general benefits scale at 1 month were 0.84 and 0.88, respectively. Similar Cronbach alphas were seen at all four timepoints. The distribution of the General Benefits variable approximated normal. However, the distribution of the Spiritual Benefits variable was highly skewed, with 60% or more of the study participants having responded “I did not experience this” at each of the four timepoints. Thus, for analysis purposes, we dichotomized the Spiritual Benefits variable (0 = those who responded “I did not experience this”, 1 = all others).

Well-being

The WELL score is a multi-faceted comprehensive measure of well-being. Previous qualitative work identified domains of well-being and then survey questions were constructed and tested to measure these domains. Nineteen survey items were used in the WELL for Life registry to measure these domains of well-being: social connectedness, experience of positive emotions, experience of negative emotions, physical health, stress, resilience, purpose and meaning, sense of self, financial security, spirituality and religiosity, and exploration and creativity. A confirmatory factor analysis showed adequate measurement model fit. Fit was examined using the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA = 0.05) and the comparative fit index (CFI = 0.96). The responses to each item were recoded to 0–100 points A mean score for all the items in a particular domain was calculated, and then a mean of the 11 domain scores was calculated to obtain a participant’s WELL score. Therefore, scores could potentially range from 0 to 100 with higher scores indicating higher well-being.

The World Health Organization Well-Being Questionnaire (WHO-5) is a well-validated measure of positive well-being [26]. The measure consists of 5 items (e.g. “I have felt calm and relaxed”) that ask participants to rate how often they have been feeling certain ways over the last two weeks on a scale from 0 = “At no time” to 5 = “All of the time.” Scores were calculated through the summation of all five responses and multiplied by a factor of 4. Therefore, scores ranged from 0 to 100, where a higher score indicates more positive well-being.

Other measures

Social connectedness was measured with 2 questions adapted from the UCLA Loneliness Scale [27]. The questions asked participants, “During the last two weeks, how often did you feel that there were people you could talk to?” and “During the last two weeks, how often did you feel that there were people you could rely on?” Thus, this is a measure of the perception that one is part of a supportive network. To make the scoring consistent with the WELL Score, the responses were recoded to 0–100 points and, then a mean of the 2 questions was calculated. However, the distribution of the social connectedness variable did not approximate normal. Thus, we categorized participants into high, medium, and low social connectedness based on natural breaks in the distribution.

A measure of distress was adapted from the National Comprehensive Cancer Center Network Distress Thermometer, a well-known tool for identifying distress levels [28]. The item asks respondents to select “the number (0–10) that best describes how much distress [they] have been experiencing in the past week including today.” A score of 10 indicates extreme distress and a score of 0 indicates no distress [29].

Analysis plan

Linear and logistic regression modeling were used to examine the personal characteristics associated with benefit finding. Linear regression was conducted with the general benefits scale as the dependent variable and social connectedness, distress, and demographic variables as the predictors. Similarly, a logistic regression was conducted using the dichotomous variable for the experience of spiritual benefits (1 = any spiritual change, 0 = no spiritual change) as the dependent variable and using the same set of predictors. Demographic predictors included age, gender, educational attainment, and race.

To assess individuals’ changes in benefit finding over time, the Reliable Change Index was utilized to calculate significant change [3032]. Cut points derived from this procedure allowed for the categorization of participants into those who have decreased, increased, or remained unchanged in their general benefit finding between any 2 data collection timepoints. Reliable Change was calculated to be any change in benefit finding greater than or equal to 0.75.

To examine the relationship between well-being and general benefit finding over time, linear regression was conducted with subsequent well-being as the outcome. Previous well-being, previous general benefits score, and a set of dummy variables indicating whether the experience of general benefit finding had decreased, increased, or stayed the same since the previous time point were used as the predictors.

Results

Description of the sample

Table 1 shows the sociodemographic characteristics of the participants in terms of their age, gender, race, marital status, education level, employment status, and yearly income. The sample is predominantly female (78%), white (72%), and highly educated (54% with a graduate degree). Participants vary in age but tend toward the older age categories. Thus, 22% of the participants report being retired and only 26% have children living at home. It is also notable that less than 1% of the participants reported that anyone in their households had been diagnosed with COVID-19 during the year of the study.

Table 1. Participants’ sociodemographics.

N (%)
Age (n = 701)
 • 18–29 76 (10.9%)
 • 30–39 125 (17.8%)
 • 40–49 106 (15.1%)
 • 50–59 129 (18.4%)
 • 60–69 140 (20.0%)
 • 70+ 125 (17.8%)
Gender (n = 698)
 • Female 546 (78.2%)
 • Male 148 (21.2%)
 • Non-binary 4 (0.6%)
Race (n = 695)
 • White/Caucasian 502 (72.2%)
 • Asian/Pacific Islander 161 (23.2%)
 • Other 32 (4.6%)
Marital Status (n = 701)
 • Married / Living with partner 500 (71.3%)
 • Previously married 76 (10.9%)
 • Single 125 (17.8%)
Education Level (n = 696)
 • No college degree 73 (10.5%)
 • Bachelor’s degree 244 (35.1%)
 • Post-Graduate 379 (54.4%)
Employment Status (n = 674)
 • Working full time 413 (61.3%)
 • Working part time 51 (7.6%)
 • Temporarily laid off / unemployed 13 (1.9%)
 • Retired 150 (22.2%)
 • Homemaker / Student / Disabled / Other 47 (7.0%)
Yearly Income (n = 670)
 • $0 - $49,999 59 (8.8%)
 • $50,000 - $99,999 162 (24.2%)
 • $100,000 - $149,999 155 (23.2%)
 • $150,000 - $249,999 179 (26.7%)
 • $250,000 - $499,999 88 (13.1%)
 • $500,000 or more 27 (4.0%)
Having Children at Home During COVID-19 (n = 701)
 • Those with children at home 184 (26.2%)
 • Those without children at home 517 (73.8%)

Type and level of benefit finding at different time points during the pandemic

Fig 2 presents the percentage of participants who responded that they experienced the type of benefit finding asked by an item to “a great extent” or “a very great extent” at each of the four time points. Having a greater appreciation for the value of one’s life was the benefit most often experienced at all time points (17% to 20% of participants experienced this to a great or very great extent across all the time points). For nearly all the items, the highest percentages were at the final time point, 12 months following the shelter-in-place mandate. Benefit finding in the areas of experiencing new possibilities and perceiving personal strengths was experienced by a significantly higher percentage of participants at the 12-month mark than at the previous time points.

Fig 2. Proportion of participants who answered “I experienced this to a great extent” or “I experienced this to a very great extent” on the benefit finding questions.

Fig 2

Note: The red asterisks represent a time point that is significantly different (p<0.05) from all other time points, while the other asterisks show pairwise differences.

However, there are some notable exceptions to this pattern. For example, the highest percentage of participants reported that they “learned a great deal about how wonderful people are” at the beginning of the pandemic. This item exhibited a different pattern over time than did the other items. Spiritual change was experienced by only 5%–10% of participants at each of the data points, with no significant change over time.

The overall prevalence of significant benefit finding, measured as those who answered “I experienced this to a great extent” or “I experienced this to a very great extent” on at least one question, ranged from 34% to 42% for our participants throughout the pandemic. The lowest percentage occurred at the 6-month time point and the highest at the 12-month time point.

Table 2 presents the descriptive statistics for the major study variables at each of the study time points. The levels of spiritual benefits remained quite low across all time points. While the means for both of the benefit finding scales are quite similar across the first 3 data points, the means increase enough at the 12-month data point to elicit a significant F-test p-value generated by repeated measures ANOVAs. The well-being measures follow a similar pattern. Distress levels were notably higher at 6 months and lower at 12 months. Levels of social connectedness did not differ over time.

Table 2. Descriptive statistics for major study variables at all four time points.

*Calculated by repeated measures ANOVA F-test.

1 Month 3 Months 6 Months 12 Months
Study Variable Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD P-value*
Distress Thermometer 3.81 2.49 3.81 2.44 4.26 2.64 3.43 2.62 <0.01
Well-being
 WELL Score 70.40 14.51 69.97 13.99 68.98 14.70 71.35 14.52 <0.01
 WHO-5 Score 56.68 22.32 56.59 21.41 54.10 22.50 58.21 22.04 <0.01
Benefit Finding
 General Benefits 2.49 1.05 2.54 1.04 2.51 1.06 2.68 1.09 <0.01
 Spiritual Benefits N (%) N (%) N (%) N (%)
  Experienced to any extent 267 (38%) 272 (39%) 257 (37%) 278 (40%) n.s.
Social Connectedness N (%) N (%) N (%) N (%)
 Low 105 (15%) 121 (17%) 118 (17%) 106 (15%) n.s.
 Medium 296 (43%) 298 (43%) 285 (41%) 293 (42%) n.s.
 High 294 (42%) 278 (40%) 292 (42%) 294 (43%) n.s.

Despite the small amount of aggregate change over time, individuals may still have been experiencing changes in benefit finding between time points. Fig 3 presents the percentage of participants who experienced increased, decreased or unchanged scores on the general benefits scale between the study time points. The majority of participants’ experiences of benefit finding remained constant over time, but there was a sizable proportion of participants that experienced either an increase or decrease in general benefit finding between time points. Notably, fewer participants experienced a decrease in benefit finding between 6 and 12 months in comparison to the previous time period, and more participants experienced an increase in benefit finding between 6 and 12 months than during previous time periods.

Fig 3. Proportion of individuals’ general benefits scores that increased, decreased, or remained unchanged between time points.

Fig 3

Note: ** p<0.01; *** p<0.001.

Cross-sectional associations between personal characteristics and benefit finding

Table 3 presents the results of a linear regression model predicting the experience of general benefits, using demographic variables, self-reported distress, and social connectedness as independent variables at the 1-month time point. Results from a binomial logistic regression predicting spiritual benefits (1 = experience of any, 0 = none) using the same independent variables are also presented. Participants who identified as Asian or Pacific Islander were more likely to experience benefit finding across both measures. Identifying as female was strongly associated with greater experience of general benefits, but the same relationship was not observed with spiritual benefits. Participants in the younger two age groups (18–29 and 30–39) were less likely to experience spiritual benefits, while participants in just the youngest age group (18–29) were less likely to experience general benefits. Educational attainment was negatively associated with both measures of benefit finding. Having children living at home was positively associated with general benefit finding but not with spiritual benefits. Having a supportive social network was more strongly positively associated with experiences of general benefits than with spiritual benefits. Lastly, greater experiences of distress during the last week were associated with greater benefit finding, with a stronger effect seen for general benefits. The 3 month, 6 month, and 12 month regression results presented similar findings with only minor differences, and can be found in the S1S3 Tables.

Table 3. Regression models predicting spiritual benefits and general benefits at 1 month.

Spiritual Benefits Logistic Regression General Benefits Linear Regression
Odds Ratio (95% CI) P-value Coefficient SE P-value
Intercept 0.26 (0.13–0.52) <0.01 1.54 0.17 <0.01
Gender
 Female 1.14 (0.76–1.73) 0.53 0.39 0.10 <0.01
 Male (ref.)
Race
 Asian/Pacific Islander 1.86 (1.25–2.77) <0.01 0.40 0.10 <0.01
 Other 1.26 (0.58–2.66) 0.54 0.25 0.18 0.17
 White/Caucasian (ref.)
Age (years)
 18–29 0.31 (0.15–0.62) <0.01 -0.34 0.16 0.03
 30–39 0.45 (0.25–0.81) <0.01 -0.17 0.14 0.22
 40–49 0.85 (0.45–1.59) 0.61 -0.17 0.16 0.28
 50–59 1.00 (0.57–1.74) 0.99 -0.06 0.14 0.67
 60–69 1.05 (0.62–1.75) 0.87 -0.12 0.13 0.36
 70+ (ref.)
Education
 No college degree 1.47 (0.85–2.53) 0.16 0.34 0.13 0.01
 Bachelor’s degree 1.49 (1.04–2.12) 0.03 0.19 0.09 0.03
 Graduate degree (ref.)
Having children at home during COVID-19
 With children 1.33 (0.88–2.03) 0.18 0.21 0.10 0.04
 Without children (ref.)
Social connectedness
 High 1.53 (0.93–2.57) 0.10 0.44 0.12 <0.01
 Medium 1.33 (0.82–2.20) 0.26 0.25 0.12 0.04
 Low (ref.)
Distress thermometer 1.08 (1.01–1.16) 0.03 0.06 0.02 <0.01

Note: Spiritual Benefits is a dichotomous variable in which 0 represents those who responded “I did not experience this” and 1 represents all others.

The extent to which the demographic sub-groups associated with higher levels of benefit finding were also experiencing higher levels of distress was also examined. At the 1-month time point, women reported significantly (p<0.01) higher levels of distress (M = 4.16, SD = 2.30) compared to men (M = 2.97, SD = 2.26). Additionally, adults who had children at home during COVID-19 reported significantly (p<0.01) higher levels of distress (M = 4.31, SD = 2.37) compared to those who did not have children at home (M = 3.75, SD = 2.33). For both educational attainment and race, distress did not significantly differ.

Longitudinal associations between well-being and benefit finding

Cross-sectionally, general benefit finding and well-being are modestly but significantly positively correlated at each of the four time points (see S4 Table). Table 4 shows the results of longitudinal regression analyses using two different measures of well-being–the Stanford WELL score and the WHO-5 well-being index. As described earlier, to predict well-being at a given subsequent time point, we used the well-being score from the previous time point, general benefits score from the previous time point, and whether there was an increase or decrease in benefit-finding from the previous time point as predictors. Results show that prior well-being scores were highly associated with subsequent well-being scores for both measures, indicating strong stability over time. The magnitude of the contribution of benefit finding variables to each of the regression models is small but statistically significant. Lagged effects of general benefits on subsequent well-being are not much in evidence. Those who decreased their benefit finding between 1 month and 3 months saw a significant decrease in well-being for both well-being measures. However, during the other study time periods, we see different results for the two well-being measures. For the WELL score, both the associations of decreases in benefit finding and increases in benefit finding with well-being are evident. For the WHO-5, increases in benefit finding are more strongly associated with well-being than are decreases in benefit finding.

Table 4. Longitudinal regression models using general benefit finding to predict two measures of well-being.

1 Month to 3 Months 3 Months to 6 Months 6 Months to 12 Months
Coefficient SE P-value Coefficient SE P-value Coefficient SE P-value
WELL Score Intercept 11.16 1.76 <0.01 10.40 2.02 <0.01 15.71 2.06 <0.01
WELL score 0.83 0.02 <0.01 0.87 0.02 <0.01 0.78 0.02 <0.01
General Benefits 0.18 0.29 0.53 -0.06 0.33 0.85 0.82 0.35 0.02
Increase in benefit finding* 1.19 0.80 0.14 2.42 0.97 0.01 1.70 0.86 0.05
Decrease in benefit finding* -2.48 0.94 <0.01 -1.30 1.03 0.21 -2.69 1.16 0.02
R2 0.74 0.70 0.66
Delta R2 ** 0.004 0.02 0.004 0.02 0.007 <0.01
Well-Being Index (WHO-5) Intercept 20.50 2.66 <0.01 13.58 2.64 <0.01 22.86 2.70 <0.01
WHO-5 score 0.66 0.03 <0.01 0.75 0.03 <0.01 0.68 0.03 <0.01
General Benefits 0.03 0.60 0.96 1.16 0.60 0.05 0.32 0.63 0.61
Increase in benefit finding* 1.98 1.71 0.25 5.00 1.77 <0.01 5.82 1.57 <0.01
Decrease in benefit finding* -4.05 1.94 0.04 -2.66 1.81 0.14 -2.11 2.11 0.32
R2 0.49 0.55 0.49
Delta R2 ** 0.005 0.07 0.008 <0.01 0.012 <0.01

Note: All models were controlled for gender, age, race, education, and whether they had children at home during the COVID-19 pandemic.

*Both the increase and decrease in benefit finding variables coded (0,1) were compared to a reference group of individuals whose benefit finding scores stayed the same between the respective time points.

**The comparison model excluded the following independent variables: General Benefits, Increase in benefit finding, Decrease in benefit finding.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to understand the experience of benefit finding during the course of the COVID-19 pandemic. Prior research on benefit finding has largely been conducted after the traumatic event has ended. As we emerge from the turbulence of the COVID-19 pandemic, understanding benefit finding in the midst of a collective trauma is both essential and novel.

At no point during our study did more than 20% of the participants strongly experience any one specific type of benefit finding, with some types of benefit finding experienced by fewer than 10%. However, between 34% and 42% experienced some type of benefit finding at various points during the pandemic. It is difficult to compare these percentages with other studies because various methods have been used to designate whether a person has experienced benefit finding [7]. In the extant literature, the percentage of participants in any given study that are experiencing benefit finding has ranged from 3% to 98% [9]. Additionally, mean scores across studies are difficult to compare since the measures used often included different survey items using different response scales. Therefore, it is quite ambitious to draw any conclusion as to how the levels of benefit finding in the current study compare with those in previous studies.

However, quite consistently across studies, the standard deviations of benefit finding scores are quite large. In studies examining benefit finding during the COVID-19 pandemic in Greece, China, and the US, standard deviations were approximately 54%, 39%, and 78% of the relevant means [3335]. In studies exploring traumas such as earthquakes, the experiences of Syrian refugees, or the loss of a child, standard deviations also were consistently high at 34%, 59%, and 41% of the mean scores, respectively [21, 22, 36]. Another way to conceptualize the extent of variation is to compare the standard deviation to the potential range of the score. The standard deviations were typically between 20% to 30% of the possible range. Thus, there is much variation in the extent to which individuals experience benefit finding.

Time elapsed since the trauma has been shown to be a positive predictor for benefit finding [37]. As individuals have more time to reflect and recover, they may be more able to extract wisdom, strength, and overall positive changes from the process of experiencing the trauma. However, in our study, the different data collection time points do not reflect time since a time-bounded trauma has occurred, but rather the time since the beginning of an ongoing collective trauma, as well as the changing nature of the pandemic.

The 12 months of this study’s duration were marked by cascading collective traumas, associated with a rise in mental health problems across the United States, leading to pleas for policymakers to support community mental health in an effort to “strengthen the social fabric and ease the mental and physical health burden of these trying times” [38]. In previous studies, the number of traumas experienced or the amount of trauma-related exposure has been associated with higher levels of benefit finding [14, 39]. In this study, there are so many changes and potential traumas occurring that it is difficult to make clear interpretations about the cascading traumas and associations with other study variables. For example, at the six month data collection during which participants were experiencing the beginning of a spike in COVID-19 cases, the effects of wildfires, and continuing political unrest due to racial inequities, we see the highest levels of distress (see Table 2) and the highest proportions of those who decreased and the lowest proportions of those who increased in terms of general benefit finding (refer to Fig 3). However, these differences between timepoints are small in magnitude. Once vaccine availability entered the picture, distress significantly decreased and benefit finding and well-being exhibited small but significant increases. Very little is known about the long-term effects of cascading collective traumas like those experienced during this study, and our work is only the beginning of what we must understand in order to inform action.

Additionally, our findings show positive cross-sectional associations between experiences of distress and benefit finding, which contradicts some previous findings showing that distress had little or no association with benefit finding [8, 40]. In a study of post-traumatic growth of breast cancer patients in treatment and early survivorship, while cancer-specific stress and general anxiety were related to higher post-traumatic growth, overall general distress had minimal association with benefit finding. A complex rendering of the relationship between distress and benefit finding emerged from the 2006 meta-analysis [8]. Benefit finding was unrelated to global distress. Time since trauma was a significant moderator of the association between benefit finding and several mental and physical health outcomes. Most notably, benefit finding was related to more distress only when less than 2 years had passed since the traumatic event took place. Since our data collection not only occurred within two years of the traumatic event, but actually during the turbulence of the COVID-19 pandemic, it is reasonable to conclude that distress acts as a catalyst for benefit finding when the trauma is fresh in the minds of individuals.

Identifying as female, having Asian origins, living with children, lower educational attainment, and having supportive social networks were positively significantly associated with benefit finding at all four timepoints in our data. Some of these associations may be due to a concentration of exposure to trauma-related events in specific sub-groups of the population. For example, in the United States, having Asian origins has been associated with elevated levels of racial discrimination and violence during the pandemic. A survey conducted in April 2021 of a representative US sample reported that 81% of Asian American adults reported that violence against Asian Americans has been rising [41]. Obviously, this could be experienced as an additional trauma. The gender effect might be similarly explained. Many previous studies of collective traumas such as earthquakes, forced displacement from war, and COVID-19 in other countries, have shown gender differences in benefit finding with women exhibiting significantly higher scores [12, 22, 33]. Women, commonly the primary caregiver in US families, were more likely to have gone without medical care since SIP, to have lost their jobs due to the pandemic, or to have taken unpaid sick leaves to care for their children [42, 43]. Thus, the severity of the trauma of the pandemic may be greater for women than for men.

The same logic might apply to those with children at home and to those with lower educational attainment. Parents with children at home were more likely to have increased familial responsibilities due to widespread closures of schools and other childcare facilities. Lastly, individuals with lower educational attainment have fared substantially worse throughout the pandemic. In May 2020, the unemployment rate for individuals in the US with a high school diploma or less rose 12.0 percentage points compared to the 5.5 percentage points for individuals with a bachelor’s degree [44]. It is possible that these sub-groups in our study who experienced higher levels of benefit finding were also the individuals who were facing heightened exposure to personal aspects of the collective trauma and/or additional traumas. However, further research is needed to test this hypothesis.

In addition to more severe exposure, higher levels of distress may play a role in increasing benefit finding for these sub-groups. As reported earlier, women and participants with children living at home reported high levels of distress. However, as shown in Table 3, the relationship between these sub-groups and benefit finding is still present when the contribution of distress is controlled for.

Our findings suggest a positive association between a supportive social network and the experience of benefit finding. Research has consistently found a positive relationship between social support and physical and mental health [45]. Particularly, strong social connections help lessen stress reactions in various situations. In the context of trauma and adverse life events, supportive social relationships have been shown to help individuals suffering from childhood abuse [46], mothers of children with chronic physical conditions [47], and cancer patients [13]. In the collective trauma literature, social support is consistently significantly associated with increased benefit finding. In a study of Gulf War Veterans, post-deployment social support from family, friends, coworkers, employers, and the community was the only significant predictor of benefit finding in their final model [48]. Jia and colleagues found that social support was predictive of subsequent post-traumatic growth in a population of Chinese adults affected by the Wenchuan Earthquake [21], supporting a causal effect of social support on benefit finding. While the specific types of social support needed to catalyze the experience of benefit finding are not yet known, there is sufficient evidence to suggest that focusing on enhancing one’s supportive network may increase benefit finding.

Lastly, research on the association between benefit finding and well-being has had inconsistent results [8, 49]. In this study, the cross-sectional associations were consistently positive but small in magnitude across the four data points. Our longitudinal models also showed associations of small magnitude—as benefit finding increased or decreased over time, so did well-being as measured by both the WELL score and the WHO-5. The causal direction of the associations cannot be assessed from our analysis. Increases in benefit finding were more strongly associated with changes in the WHO-5 than with the WELL score. This may be due to the WHO-5 emphasis on positive emotions as opposed to the broad content of the WELL score. Further research is needed to more fully understand the role of benefit-finding in the process of responding to traumas. For example, post-traumatic growth, or benefit finding, has shown to have a moderating effect on the relationship between post-traumatic stress and both depression and quality of life among breast cancer survivors [49]. Similar studies of responses to collective traumas would be informative.

Limitations

The most significant limitation of our study is that it is a non-representative sample that includes high proportions of individuals who are female, highly educated, wealthy, racially identify as white or Asian/Pacific Islander, and reside in the San Francisco Bay Area. Thus, further research is necessary to assess these same research questions among individuals of other sociodemographic backgrounds. Additionally, the PTGI only measures a specific set of benefits that may exclude additional benefits experienced during the pandemic. Specifically, our measure of benefit finding failed to assess benefits that have arisen due to the specific nature of the COVID-19 pandemic such as additional time with family, the elimination of lengthy commutes for many, and an enhanced appreciation for nature and outdoor activities [50, 51]. Lastly, indicators of severity of exposure to the collective trauma were not included in the study and would have offered an enhanced understanding of the determinants of benefit finding and well-being.

Implications for promoting well-being

Even though the association between benefit finding and well-being does not seem to be strong, increasing people’s opportunities and abilities for experiencing benefits is likely to play a role in reducing stress and enhancing well-being. Some previous research has demonstrated that the experience of benefit finding can be increased, for example in response to work-related traumas [52] and in response to cancer [53]. However, further research is needed to assess the effectiveness of strategies to enhance benefit finding in response to collective traumas. This study suggests that a supportive social network and time to reflect may increase the likelihood of benefit finding and contribute to its positive effects.

Conclusion

Benefit finding has been widely studied weeks, months, and years after a personal traumatic event, but little research has been conducted on benefit finding while a collective trauma is still occurring. As COVID-19 persists and additional variants continue to spread around the world, there is a heightened need to understand concurrent benefit finding and its potential to promote well-being.

Supporting information

S1 Table. Regression models for predicting spiritual benefits and general benefits at 3 months.

(TIF)

S2 Table. Regression models for predicting spiritual benefits and general benefits at 6 months.

(TIF)

S3 Table. Regression models for predicting spiritual benefits and general benefits at 12 months.

(TIF)

S4 Table. Cross-sectional correlations between the general benefits score and two measures of well-being.

*The General Benefits Score at 1 month, 3 months, 6 months, and 12 months was used, respectively.

(TIF)

Acknowledgments

We thank Katy Peng for assistance in compiling the dataset. We also thank Ann Hsing for her tremendous support and guidance.

Data Availability

Data relevant to this study are available from the Harvard Dataverse at doi:10.7910/DVN/GNIOTF.

Funding Statement

Initial foundational funding for the Stanford Wellness Living laboratory (WELL) was provided by Amway via an unrestricted gift through the Nutrilite Health Institute Wellness Fund to Stanford University. Preparation of this manuscript was supported in part by the Stanford Thailand Research Consortium.

References

  • 1.Saul J. Collective Trauma, Collective Healing: Promoting Community Resilience in the Aftermath of Disaster. Routledge; 2022. 188 p.
  • 2.Hirschberger G. Collective Trauma and the Social Construction of Meaning. Front Psychol. 2018. Aug 10;9:1441. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01441 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Cullen W, Gulati G, Kelly BD. Mental health in the COVID-19 pandemic. QJM: An International Journal of Medicine. 2020. May 1;113(5):311–2. doi: 10.1093/qjmed/hcaa110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Galea S, Merchant RM, Lurie N. The Mental Health Consequences of COVID-19 and Physical Distancing: The Need for Prevention and Early Intervention. JAMA Internal Medicine. 2020. Jun 1;180(6):817–8. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2020.1562 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Tison GH, Avram R, Kuhar P, Abreau S, Marcus GM, Pletcher MJ, et al. Worldwide Effect of COVID-19 on Physical Activity: A Descriptive Study. Ann Intern Med. 2020. Nov 3;173(9):767–70. doi: 10.7326/M20-2665 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Miao J, Zeng D, Shi Z. Can neighborhoods protect residents from mental distress during the COVID-19 pandemic? Evidence from Wuhan. 2020. Sep 24; [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Tedeschi RG, Shakespeare-Finch J, Taku K, Calhoun LG. Posttraumatic Growth: Theory, Research, and Applications. New York: Routledge; 2018. 264 p. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Helgeson VS, Reynolds KA, Tomich PL. A meta-analytic review of benefit finding and growth. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2006;74(5):797–816. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.74.5.797 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Linley PA, Joseph S. Positive change following trauma and adversity: a review. J Trauma Stress. 2004. Feb;17(1):11–21. doi: 10.1023/B:JOTS.0000014671.27856.7e [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Sawyer A, Ayers S, Field AP. Posttraumatic growth and adjustment among individuals with cancer or HIV/AIDS: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review. 2010. Jun 1;30(4):436–47. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.02.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Tang ST, Lin KC, Chen JS, Chang WC, Hsieh CH, Chou WC. Threatened with death but growing: changes in and determinants of posttraumatic growth over the dying process for Taiwanese terminally ill cancer patients. Psychooncology. 2015. Feb;24(2):147–54. doi: 10.1002/pon.3616 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Marshall EM, Frazier P, Frankfurt S, Kuijer RG. Trajectories of posttraumatic growth and depreciation after two major earthquakes. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy. 2015;7(2):112–21. doi: 10.1037/tra0000005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Li MY, Yang YL, Liu L, Wang L. Effects of social support, hope and resilience on quality of life among Chinese bladder cancer patients: a cross-sectional study. Health Qual Life Outcomes. 2016. May 6;14:73. doi: 10.1186/s12955-016-0481-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Tsai J, Sippel LM, Mota N, Southwick SM, Pietrzak RH. Longitudinal course of posttraumatic growth among U.S. military veterans: Results from the National Health and Resilience in Veterans Study. Depression and Anxiety. 2016;33(1):9–18. doi: 10.1002/da.22371 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Danhauer SC, Russell G, Case LD, Sohl SJ, Tedeschi RG, Addington EL, et al. Trajectories of Posttraumatic Growth and Associated Characteristics in Women with Breast Cancer. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2015. Oct 1;49(5):650–9. doi: 10.1007/s12160-015-9696-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Husson O, Zebrack B, Block R, Embry L, Aguilar C, Hayes-Lattin B, et al. Posttraumatic growth and well-being among adolescents and young adults (AYAs) with cancer: a longitudinal study. Support Care Cancer. 2017. Sep 1;25(9):2881–90. doi: 10.1007/s00520-017-3707-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Stanford WELL for Life: learning what it means to be well‬ [Internet]. 2017 [cited 2021 Aug 19]. https://scholar.google.com/citations?view_op=view_citation&hl=en&user=jbJ02vQAAAAJ&citation_for_view=jbJ02vQAAAAJ:qjMakFHDy7sC
  • 18.Orders—COVID-19 Health Orders—San Francisco Department of Public Health [Internet]. [cited 2021 Apr 7]. https://www.sfdph.org/dph/alerts/coronavirus-healthorders.asp
  • 19.guidance for face coverings [Internet]. [cited 2021 Aug 19]. https://www.cdph.ca.gov/Programs/CID/DCDC/Pages/COVID-19/guidance-for-face-coverings.aspx
  • 20.Chrisinger BW, Gustafson JA, King AC, Winter SJ. Understanding Where We Are Well: Neighborhood-Level Social and Environmental Correlates of Well-Being in the Stanford Well for Life Study. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2019. Jan;16(10):1786. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16101786 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Jia X, Liu X, Ying L, Lin C. Longitudinal Relationships between Social Support and Posttraumatic Growth among Adolescent Survivors of the Wenchuan Earthquake. Frontiers in Psychology. 2017;8:1275. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01275 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Ersahin Z. Post-traumatic growth among Syrian refugees in Turkey: the role of coping strategies and religiosity. Curr Psychol [Internet]. 2020. Apr 29 [cited 2021 Apr 7]; doi: 10.1007/s12144-020-00763-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Tedeschi RG, Calhoun LG. The Posttraumatic Growth Inventory: measuring the positive legacy of trauma. J Trauma Stress. 1996. Jul;9(3):455–71. doi: 10.1007/BF02103658 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Giusti EM, Veronesi G, Callegari C, Castelnuovo G, Iacoviello L, Ferrario MM. The North Italian Longitudinal Study Assessing the Mental Health Effects of SARS-CoV-2 Pandemic Health Care Workers-Part II: Structural Validity of Scales Assessing Mental Health. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022. Aug 3;19(15):9541. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19159541 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Shiratani KN. Psychological changes and associated factors among patients with tuberculosis who received directly observed treatment short-course in metropolitan areas of Japan: quantitative and qualitative perspectives. BMC Public Health. 2019. Dec 5;19(1):1642. doi: 10.1186/s12889-019-8001-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.World Health Organization. Wellbeing measures in primary health care/the DEPCARE project: report on a WHO meeting, Stockholm, Sweden 12–13 February 1998. In: Wellbeing measures in primary health care/the DEPCARE project: report on a WHO meeting, Stockholm, Sweden 12–13 February 1998. 1998.
  • 27.Russell D, Peplau LA, Cutrona CE. The revised UCLA Loneliness Scale: Concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1980;39(3):472–80. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.39.3.472 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Riba MB, Donovan KA, Andersen B, Braun, Breitbart WS, Brewer BW, et al. Distress Management, Version 3.2019, NCCN Clinical Practice Guidelines in Oncology. Journal of the National Comprehensive Cancer Network. 2019. Oct 1;17(10):1229–49. doi: 10.6004/jnccn.2019.0048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Jacobsen PB, Donovan KA, Trask PC, Fleishman SB, Zabora J, Baker F, et al. Screening for psychologic distress in ambulatory cancer patients. Cancer. 2005;103(7):1494–502. doi: 10.1002/cncr.20940 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Zahra D, Hedge C. The reliable change index: Why isn’t it more popular in academic psychology. Psychology Postgraduate Affairs Group Quarterly. 2010;76(76):14–9. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Statistical and Clinical Significance | Effect Size Calculators [Internet]. [cited 2023 Apr 14]. https://lbecker.uccs.edu/clinsig
  • 32.Jacobson NS, Truax P. Clinical significance: a statistical approach to defining meaningful change in psychotherapy research. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1991. Feb;59(1):12–9. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.59.1.12 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Kalaitzaki A, Rovithis M. Secondary traumatic stress and vicarious posttraumatic growth in healthcare workers during the first COVID-19 lockdown in Greece: The role of resilience and coping strategies. Psychiatriki. 2021. Apr 19;32(1):19–25. doi: 10.22365/jpsych.2021.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Park CL, Wilt JA, Russell BS, Fendrich M. Does perceived post-traumatic growth predict better psychological adjustment during the COVID-19 pandemic? Results from a national longitudinal survey in the USA. J Psychiatr Res. 2022. Feb;146:179–85. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.12.040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Yan S, Yang J, Ye M, Chen S, Xie C, Huang J, et al. Post-traumatic Growth and Related Influencing Factors in Discharged COVID-19 Patients: A Cross-Sectional Study. Frontiers in Psychology [Internet]. 2021. [cited 2022 Jun 8];12. Available from: https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.658307 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Huh HJ, Kim KH, Lee HK, Chae JH. Attachment Style, Complicated Grief and Post-Traumatic Growth in Traumatic Loss: The Role of Intrusive and Deliberate Rumination. Psychiatry Investig. 2020. Jul 15;17(7):636–44. doi: 10.30773/pi.2019.0291 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Xia R. Associated Factors of Posttraumatic Growth: A Meta-Analysis. Masters Theses [Internet]. 2017 Jan 1; https://thekeep.eiu.edu/theses/3162
  • 38.Silver RC, Holman EA, Garfin DR. Coping with cascading collective traumas in the United States. Nat Hum Behav. 2021. Jan;5(1):4–6. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-00981-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Xu J, Liao Q. Prevalence and predictors of posttraumatic growth among adult survivors one year following 2008 Sichuan earthquake. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2011. Sep 1;133(1):274–80. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.03.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Dekel S, Mandl C, Solomon Z. Shared and unique predictors of post-traumatic growth and distress. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2011;67(3):241–52. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20747 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Ruiz NG, Edwards K, Lopez MH. One-third of Asian Americans fear threats, physical attacks and most say violence against them is rising [Internet]. Pew Research Center. 2021 [cited 2021 Jul 27]. https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2021/04/21/one-third-of-asian-americans-fear-threats-physical-attacks-and-most-say-violence-against-them-is-rising/
  • 42.Women’s Experiences with Health Care During the COVID-19 Pandemic: Findings from the KFF Women’s Health Survey [Internet]. KFF. 2021 [cited 2021 Jun 16]. https://www.kff.org/womens-health-policy/issue-brief/womens-experiences-with-health-care-during-the-covid-19-pandemic-findings-from-the-kff-womens-health-survey/
  • 43.Women, Caregiving, and COVID-19 | CDC Women’s Health [Internet]. 2021 [cited 2021 Jun 16]. https://www.cdc.gov/women/caregivers-covid-19/index.html
  • 44.Parkinson C. COVID-19, educational attainment, and the impact on American workers: Monthly Labor Review: U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics [Internet]. 2020 [cited 2021 Jul 27]. https://www.bls.gov/opub/mlr/2020/beyond-bls/covid-19-educational-attainment-and-the-impact-on-american-workers.htm
  • 45.Ozbay F, Johnson DC, Dimoulas E, Morgan CA, Charney D, Southwick S. Social Support and Resilience to Stress. Psychiatry (Edgmont). 2007. May;4(5):35–40. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Afifi TO, MacMillan HL, Taillieu T, Turner S, Cheung K, Sareen J, et al. Individual- and Relationship-Level Factors Related to Better Mental Health Outcomes following Child Abuse: Results from a Nationally Representative Canadian Sample. Can J Psychiatry. 2016. Dec;61(12):776–88. doi: 10.1177/0706743716651832 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Horton TV, Wallander JL. Hope and social support as resilience factors against psychological distress of mothers who care for children with chronic physical conditions. Rehabilitation Psychology. 2001;46(4):382–99. [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Maguen S, Vogt DS, King LA, King DW, Litz BT. Posttraumatic Growth Among Gulf War I Veterans: The Predictive Role of Deployment-Related Experiences and Background Characteristics. Journal of Loss and Trauma. 2006. Dec 1;11(5):373–88. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Morrill EF, Brewer NT, O’Neill SC, Lillie SE, Dees EC, Carey LA, et al. The interaction of post-traumatic growth and post-traumatic stress symptoms in predicting depressive symptoms and quality of life. Psycho-Oncology. 2008. Sep;17(9):948–53. doi: 10.1002/pon.1313 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Lossio-Ventura JA, Lee AY, Hancock JT, Linos N, Linos E. Identifying Silver Linings During the Pandemic Through Natural Language Processing. Frontiers in Psychology [Internet]. 2021 [cited 2022 Sep 12];12. Available from: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.712111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Morse JW, Gladkikh TM, Hackenburg DM, Gould RK. COVID-19 and human-nature relationships: Vermonters’ activities in nature and associated nonmaterial values during the pandemic. PLOS ONE. 2020. Dec 11;15(12):e0243697. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0243697 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Maitlis S. Posttraumatic Growth at Work. Annu Rev Organ Psychol Organ Behav. 2020. Jan 21;7(1):395–419. [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Ochoa Arnedo C, Sánchez N, Sumalla EC, Casellas-Grau A. Stress and Growth in Cancer: Mechanisms and Psychotherapeutic Interventions to Facilitate a Constructive Balance. Front Psychol [Internet]. 2019. [cited 2021 Aug 3];0. Available from: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00177/full [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Qin Xiang Ng

15 Feb 2023

PONE-D-22-33849Benefit finding and well-being over the course of the COVID-19 pandemicPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Moore,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. There are issues with the conceptualization and statistical handling of the data. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Apr 01 2023 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Qin Xiang Ng, MBBS, MPH

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. In the Methods section of your manuscript, please provide additional details regarding participant consent. In the ethics statement in the Methods and online submission information, please ensure that you have specified what type you obtained (for instance, written or verbal, and if verbal, how it was documented and witnessed). If your study included minors, state whether you obtained consent from parents or guardians. If the need for consent was waived by the ethics committee, please include this information.

3. Please ensure that you include a title page within your main document. We do appreciate that you have a title page document uploaded as a separate file, however, as per our author guidelines (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-title-page) we do require this to be part of the manuscript file itself and not uploaded separately.

Could you therefore please include the title page into the beginning of your manuscript file itself, listing all authors and affiliation.

4. Thank you for stating the following financial disclosure:

"Initial foundational funding for the Stanford Wellness Living laboratory (WELL) was provided by Amway via an unrestricted gift through the Nutrilite Health Institute Wellness Fund to Stanford University. Preparation of this manuscript was supported in part by the Stanford Thailand Research Consortium."

Please state what role the funders took in the study.  If the funders had no role, please state: "The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript."

If this statement is not correct you must amend it as needed.

Please include this amended Role of Funder statement in your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

4. Please include your full ethics statement in the ‘Methods’ section of your manuscript file. In your statement, please include the full name of the IRB or ethics committee who approved or waived your study, as well as whether or not you obtained informed written or verbal consent. If consent was waived for your study, please include this information in your statement as well.

5. We note that you have stated that you will provide repository information for your data at acceptance. Should your manuscript be accepted for publication, we will hold it until you provide the relevant accession numbers or DOIs necessary to access your data. If you wish to make changes to your Data Availability statement, please describe these changes in your cover letter and we will update your Data Availability statement to reflect the information you provide.

6. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Partly

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This manuscript examines if benefit finding is associated with mental well-being over the COVID-19 pandemic in a large sample of American participants in California. Overall, the manuscript is well written and its findings should be of interest to the general readership of PLoS ONE. I believe that it should be appropriate for publication after addressing several concerns that I have.

1. I think the manner in which the participants are classified as having increments, decrements, or no change between described on p. 10 needs more justification. Why is half a point chosen as a cut-off? Any prior evidence to suggest that this cut-off value is appropriate? Have the authors considered the Reliable Change Index (Jacobson and Truax, 1991)? This index allows for the estimation of reliable change (after accounting for measure unreliability) over time for an individual. The authors would be able to derive the percentage of people who have decreased, increased, or remained unchanged in benefit finding.

2. Table 3, for the results concerning the logistic regression (spiritual benefits), the authors should also present the odds ratios for the individual predictors. Odds ratios are more intuitive to interpret in this context than are regression coefficients.

3. While the results presented in Table 4 are interesting, the use of the categorical variable of benefit finding is a limitation. Why not simply use the original measure (as a continuous variable) so that statistical information is not lost when one transform a continuous variable to a categorical one? In addition, the analyses presented in Table 4 resemble a cross-lagged panel model (CLPM). I would encourage the authors to conduct the CLPM as an integrative approach rather than conducting multiple regression analyses. For example, the longitudinal associations between benefit finding and WELL scores can be modelled over the 4 time points. A separate model can be conducted with benefit finding and WHO-5 scores. The cross-lagged associations will address the question of whether prior benefit finding would predict subsequent well-being, or vice versa.

4. In the Discussion, the authors suggest that being women, Asian, having lower educational attainment status, and less social support are being more exposed to the negative and traumatic effects of COVID-19, and hence the observed heightened benefit finding. The authors offer this possibility (i.e., being more exposed to trauma) and thinks that future is needed. Hence, I find it bizarre that in this study, the authors have not tracked people’s exposure to trauma, or some proxy of trauma. This would be a major study limitation. The only measure that is close to trauma exposure would be the one-item measure of “distress”. Perhaps this distress can be correlated with all these predictor variables, and see if the proposed mechanism is plausible (e.g., having lower educational attainment status is associated with more distress)? Rather than speculations in the Discussion, there is some avenue for the authors to test some of these assertions. Granted, a distress measure is not the same as trauma exposure, but at least it could provide the authors with some support of their assertions if support is found.

In conclusion, I think there is promise in this manuscript. The findings are interesting, and have potential practical implications. Hence, I encourage the authors to consider the abovementioned issues in their revision.

Reviewer #2: 1. Overview

This study aimed to estimate the prevalence of benefit finding behavior in the course of COVID-19 pandemic, describe its pattern of variation, and examine its effect on individual well-being. It tested four hypotheses below using data from a questionnaire survey of a sample of Californians:

H1: individuals that report stronger support networks will be more likely to experience subsequent benefit finding.

H2. people who report higher levels of distress (regardless of whether the source of the distress is personal or societal) will be more likely to experience subsequent benefit finding

H3. amount of benefit finding will increase as the pandemic progresses and individuals have more opportunity to gain insight about their experiences.

H4: a positive relationship between benefit finding and subsequent well-being, particularly for the latter measure of well-being.

2. Specific comments

This study can potentially contribute to the body of knowledge on the factors associated with benefits finding and the effect of benefit finding on well-being. However, there are major conceptual and methodological issues (see below) that the authors need to address to improve the credibility of the study findings.

2.1. Introduction

The author claimed that the COVID-19 pandemic to be a collective trauma (Line 21) citing Tedeschi et al.’s definition of trauma “a highly stressful and challenging life altering event” (Lines 22) as reference for the concept of collective trauma. However, the cited definition is unable to distinguish collective trauma from personal trauma such as a fatal disease. The authors are advised to check out Kai Erikson’s definition of collective trauma which draws an intentional and clear distinction from personal drama: “a blow to the basic tissues of social life that damages the bonds attaching people together and impairs the prevailing sense of communality” (Erikson, 1976, p153). It is not apparent that COVID-19 meets the definition of a collective trauma by this definition, although it is apparently a collective stressor. The authors need to reconsider the validity of conceptualising COVID-19 as a collective trauma and provide strong and convincing justification for their conceptualisation.

2.2. Methods:

• Sampling method: The authors indicated that the study participants were recruited from an online registry—Stanford WELL for Life initiative (Lines 111-112). This is the sampling framework. It is important to describe the sampling procedure too i.e., how participants were selected from this registry, for example, by random sampling method?

• Selection of measurement scale for benefit finding: The authors ought to provide theoretical or methodological justification for their choice of PTGI as the measurement of the key concept of benefit finding over competing measurement tools such as the Benefit Finding Scale (Tomich & Helgeson, 2004) and the Perceived Benefit Scale (McMillen & Fisher, 1996).

• Dichotomisation of PTGI scale: The authors dichotomized participants into those who answered either “I experienced this to a great extent” or “I experienced this to a very great extent” as having experienced significant benefit finding and all others as not having experienced benefit finding for each individual item and citied Tedeschi et al.’s work as precedence. They ought to discuss critically the pros and cons of this practice and provide strong justification for its adoption.

• Inconsistency of measurement level for the same construct: In Lines 206-208, the authors stated that a logistic regression was conducted using a dichotomous variable for the experience of spiritual benefits (1=any spiritual change, 0=no spiritual change) as the dependent variable. However, in the earlier part of the manuscript (Lines 166-168), the authors had stated that the Spiritual Benefits score was calculated by taking the mean of the component items. Each score ranges from 1 to 6. It is unclear how a mean score of two 6-point Likert scales became a dichotomous measure. The authors ought to reconcile this inconsistency.

• Categorisation of continuous variables: The authors stated that the changes of PTGI scores over time were categorized into three groups signifying whether the experience of benefit finding had decreased, increased, or stayed the same from the previous time point. Scores were considered to have decreased if they had fallen by more than half a point, and to have increased if they had risen by more than half a point. If scores changed by less than half a point, they were considered to have stayed the same (Lines 212-216). Categorising continuous measures is a problematic practice. The authors are advised to refer to the following articles on the cost of dichotomisation and reconsider their analytical decision or at least discuss the cost of this decision.

o Altman DG, Royston P. The cost of dichotomising continuous variables. BMJ. 2006 May 6;332(7549):1080. doi: 10.1136/bmj.332.7549.1080. PMID: 16675816; PMCID: PMC1458573.

o Royston P, Altman DG, Sauerbrei W. Dichotomizing continuous predictors in multiple regression: a bad idea. Stat Med. 2006 Jan 15;25(1):127-41. doi: 10.1002/sim.2331. PMID: 16217841.

2.3. Findings

The authors found that for the WELL score, decreases in benefit finding are associated with

decreases in well-being; for the WHO-5, whereas increases in benefit finding are associated with

increases in well-being (Lines 311-313). As WELL and WHO-5 are measures of the same construct well-being, these seemingly paradoxical findings need to be explained in the discussion section.

2.4. Minor Issues:

• Lines 173-174, the meaning of “a strong confirmatory factor analysis (CFA)” is unclear.

• Line 174: “the score is composed of 19 items”, did the authors mean “scale” by the word “score”?

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Decision Letter 1

Qin Xiang Ng

26 Jun 2023

Benefit finding and well-being over the course of the COVID-19 pandemic

PONE-D-22-33849R1

Dear Dr. Moore,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Qin Xiang Ng, MBBS, MPH

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The authors have taken into account most of my previous comments and addressed them adequately. I think that the revised manuscript has improved in the statistical treatment of the data, and the interpretation of the results. This is a paper worthy of publication. Congrats to the authors!

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

**********

Acceptance letter

Qin Xiang Ng

18 Jul 2023

PONE-D-22-33849R1

Benefit finding and well-being over the course of the COVID-19 pandemic

Dear Dr. Moore:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Qin Xiang Ng

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Table. Regression models for predicting spiritual benefits and general benefits at 3 months.

    (TIF)

    S2 Table. Regression models for predicting spiritual benefits and general benefits at 6 months.

    (TIF)

    S3 Table. Regression models for predicting spiritual benefits and general benefits at 12 months.

    (TIF)

    S4 Table. Cross-sectional correlations between the general benefits score and two measures of well-being.

    *The General Benefits Score at 1 month, 3 months, 6 months, and 12 months was used, respectively.

    (TIF)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Fig 2.eps

    Data Availability Statement

    Data relevant to this study are available from the Harvard Dataverse at doi:10.7910/DVN/GNIOTF.


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES