Abstract
The study of factors, across species, that allow some individuals to age more successfully than others has important implications for individual wellbeing as well as health education, policy and intervention. Design of studies and communication across investigators in this area has been hampered by a diversity of terminology. The Collaboratory on Research Definitions for Reserve and Resilience in Cognitive Aging and Dementia was funded by the National Institute on Aging and established in 2019 as a 3-year process of developing consensus definitions and research guidelines. The proposed Framework is based on an iterative process including 3 annual Workshops, focused workgroups, and input from numerous international investigators. It suggests the overarching term: resilience, and presents operational definitions for 3 concepts: cognitive reserve, brain maintenance, and brain reserve. Twelve pilot studies that integrate these definitions are presented. The use of a common vocabulary and operational definitions will facilitate even greater progress in understanding the factors that are associated with successful aging.
Keywords: Resilience, Cognitive reserve, Brain maintenance, Brain reserve
1. Introduction
The study of factors that allow some individuals to age more successfully than others, including for example genetics and life exposures, has important implications for individual wellbeing as well as health education, policy and intervention. Moreover, identifying factors that are relevant across species (i.e., humans and nonhumans) is fundamentally necessary to facilitate studies of the neurobiological underpinnings of such factors.
In this context, overarching concepts like reserve and resilience are often invoked for capturing differential susceptibility to brain aging and disease. However, design of studies and communication across investigators in this area has been hampered by a diversity of terminology. Several groups have published proposed nomenclature and operational definitions for concepts including resilience, cognitive reserve (CR), brain reserve (BR), brain maintenance (BM), compensation, scaffolding, resistance, and resilience. Across these papers there are often disparate definitions for the same term. In addition, most of these papers focus on human studies, so the definitions and nomenclature are not optimally suitable for nonhuman studies.
The Collaboratory on Research Definitions for Reserve and Resilience in Cognitive Aging and Dementia was funded by the National Institute on Aging of the National Institutes of Health in the USA and established in 2019 as a 3-year process of developing consensus definitions and research guidelines for CR and related concepts. The present document is the result of an iterative process including 3 large annual Workshops, input from focused workgroups, and the extensive participation and consultation of over 40 selected, international expert investigators who utilize multiple research approaches and study both humans and nonhumans. Here we present a framework that includes definitions for 3 concepts, CR, BM, and BR, along with suggested operational definitions to help guide the design of research investigating these concepts. We also include resilience as an overarching term that subsumes all of the concepts presented.
Our aim is to present a well-defined set of operational definitions in order to encourage, advance, and develop research on these concepts. At the same time, we want to encourage investigators who have different views or use a given concept differently to note how their definitions relate or differ with one of those described here. Similarly, this framework provides a basis for describing how the operational definition of another concept differs from those suggested here.
Our intention is not to limit the creativity or ingenuity of investigators, or to claim that the framework presents the only way to investigate these important concepts. We hope to encourage research that provides either evidence-based support for these concepts or that presents data that cannot be accommodated by the proposed operational definitions of these concepts. We also hope that referring to this framework will facilitate collaboration and comparison of findings across studies and species.
The Collaboratory also sponsored 12 studies that were intended to implement the suggested research guidelines and thus provide experimental examples of their operational utility. This disparate set of studies incorporates humans and nonhumans, as well as multiple approaches including epidemiologic, neuroimaging, and interventions. We include in the supplementary material descriptions of the pilot projects as wells as a table that summarizes the projects and how they incorporate the framework presented here. These provide useful real-world examples that illustrate how study designs can incorporate the suggested framework.
Our hope for this framework is that the use of a common vocabulary and operational definitions will facilitate even greater progress in understanding the factors that are associated with successful aging.
2. Resilience
The term resilience has been used in many contexts. Here we consider it a general term that subsumes any concept that relates to the capacity of the brain to maintain cognition and function with aging and disease. There can be substantial variability in the mechanisms underlying resilience. Here, we present 3, CR, BM, and BR.
3. CR
3.1. Definition
CR is a property of the brain that allows for cognitive performance that is better than expected given the degree of life-course related brain changes and brain injury or disease:
Property of the brain refers to multiple potential mechanisms including molecular, cellular and network levels. The working hypothesis is that these mechanisms help cope with or compensate for brain changes and the consequences of brain injury or disease.
These mechanisms can be characterized via biological or cognitive-experimental approaches.
Better than expected cognitive performance refers to differences ideally measured longitudinally.
CR can be influenced by multiple genetic and environmental factors, operating at various points or continuously across the lifespan.
3.2. Operational definition: general considerations
Research aimed at further elucidating CR requires the inclusion of 3 components:
Measures of life course-related brain changes, insults, disease, or risk factors that theoretically impact cognitive outcomes,
Measures of associated change in cognition, and
A variable that influences the relationship between 1 and 2.
Ideally, the aim is to demonstrate that any proposed CR measure (e.g., a sociocultural or functional brain measure) moderates the relationship between 1 and 2. For example, in an analysis where change in brain atrophy/pathology measures (component 1) predict change in cognition (component 2), and includes education as a hypothesized CR proxy (component 3), there is a statistical interaction between brain measures and education, such that level of education significantly moderates the association of brain measures with cognitive change.
Even without evidence for moderation, it can also be sufficient to demonstrate that a hypothesized CR proxy or measure is associated with cognitive performance over and above (e.g., after adjusting for) the effects of brain change, pathology, or insult. For example, in a multiple regression analysis of change in cognition that includes brain atrophy/pathology measures and a hypothesized CR proxy, the proxy should account for variance in cognitive performance. In this analysis, the CR proxy simply adds predictive information (a protective factor), a weaker form of CR evidence than moderation.
All 3 components are needed when investigating CR. For example:
Demonstrating that expression of higher connectivity within a specific resting BOLD network is associated with slower cognitive decline is not sufficient to conclude that expression of this network reflects CR. To make a claim about CR it must also include measures of age-related brain change, insult or disease that theoretically impact cognitive outcomes.
Similarly, a relationship between a particular genotype and rate of cognitive decline would not be sufficient to conclude that this genotype is associated with CR. It would be important to demonstrate that the genotype’s relationship to reduced rates of cognitive decline is expressed through moderation of age-related brain change or reduction of the expected impact on cognitive performance of a given brain insult or disease.
3.3. Specification of the 3 components needed to elucidate CR
3.3.1. Measures of life course-related brain developmental changes, injury, or disease that theoretically impact cognitive outcomes
This could consist, for example, of measures of anatomic changes such as loss of brain volume or white matter tract integrity, or onset and progression of disease pathology such as biomarkers of neurodegenerative disease.
These changes could be more extensively specified. Measures/mechanisms underlying aging that impact cognitive outcomes could include change in structure or function of synapses, oxidative damage/stress, impaired stress response signaling, Ca2 + dyshomeostasis and/or dysregulation, mitochondrial function, impaired waste disposal, inflammation, epigenetics, stem cell depletion, and altered neuronal activity/connectivity.
It is likely that unmeasured or unknown brain or pathologic changes contribute to inter-individual variance in the cognitive outcomes. Their eventual inclusion would increase the precision of elucidating CR.
3.3.2. Measures of cognition
This term encompasses measures of cognition and day-to-day function that change with aging and disease. When possible, it would be useful to adopt cognitive tests that show changes with age or brain disease, and that can be used across species. In this case, it is important to be mindful that formal operational similarity between human and nonhuman tasks is not sufficient, or even necessary; the tasks need to tap similar underlying neural systems.
3.3.3. CR proxy/mechanism: a hypothesized variable that influences the relationship between 1 and 2
As the definition of CR states, these mechanisms can be characterized via biological or cognitive experimental approaches.
Proxies for CR in human studies have included features associated with both endowment and experience, including early age IQ, cognitively stimulating exposures across the age span, education, occupational exposures, leisure activity, social networks, or other exposures, hypothesized or to be discovered, that might impart CR. Similar proxies such as behavioral training, physical exercise, environmental enrichment, social housing, or diet are applicable to nonhuman studies.
In addition, the nature of the CR proxy or mechanism that influences the relationship between component 1 and 2 can be explored. For example, investigators might explore whether differential expression of a specific functional network is associated the degree of sustained cognitive function in the face of age-related brain changes that impact cognition. More generally, mechanisms underlying CR could be specified at the molecular, cellular or network levels.
3.4. Example of studies of CR
In studies of CR, longitudinal designs optimally address the 3 features underlying the concept of CR. However, rich information can be gained from cross-sectional studies including discovering variables that appear to be critical for CR, establishing preliminary observations, providing insight into neurobiological mechanisms and developing research or conceptual approaches.
3.4.1. Longitudinal study incorporating measures of brain and cognitive change
In a longitudinal study, one could explore whether some life exposure conceptually linked to CR moderates the relationship between change in brain status (e.g., volume, white matter tract integrity, white matter hyperintensity burden) and change in cognition. For example, one could establish a relationship between age-related changes in cortical thickness/surface area, brain volume, and white matter tract integrity with changes in cognition. The potential moderation by education of this relationship could then be explored. Such moderation would provide support for the idea that higher education is associated with CR.
Some longitudinal studies may have no direct measures of brain change. Analyses that assume parity across all followed individuals or incorporate risk factors for brain changes could suggest hypotheses and guide subsequent studies.
3.4.2. Neural implementation of CR
Although variables such as IQ, education, occupational attainment etc. can be associated with CR as described in 4.1, that is, moderating between measures that theoretically impact cognitive outcomes (component 1) and measures of associated change in cognition (component 2), more insight into the mechanisms underlying CR might be obtained from studies that directly examine neural mechanisms. In both human and nonhuman studies, imaging techniques including functional MRI (fMRI), spectroscopy, and EEG are uniquely suited for longitudinal measurements, providing in-depth assessments of brain structure, neural activity, and the chemistry in the aging brain. CSF, plasma/serum, and extracellular vesicle biology in blood are advancing rapidly and may provide a translatable fluid biopsy for relevant brain changes in this context.
Thus, one goal might be to identify functional networks or circuits, whose differential expression moderates the relationship between age-related brain changes that impact cognitive outcomes and the associated change in cognition. For example, longitudinal studies of aging or neurodegenerative disease can investigate how the relationship between changes in structure/function and cognition/clinical status can be moderated by proposed reserve-related networks. It would be of interest to determine whether differential expression of this network is related to life exposures such as education or occupational experience. This would create a relationship between a proxy for CR and a potential brain mechanism underlying that proxy.
3.4.3. Intervention studies and natural experiments
Intervention studies can most directly test whether some exposure or mechanism underlies CR by examining whether the intervention moderates the effect of age-related brain changes on cognitive outcomes. These studies can help explore mechanisms underlying CR.
Similarly, controlled perturbations such as transcranial magnetic or direct current stimulation could model brain insult, stressor or disease. Alternately, they could be used to modulate activity in networks/circuits associated with CR, and by suppressing it or facilitating it, gain causal and mechanistic insights, and even potentially explore therapeutic interventions.
Sometimes, environmental changes can be used as natural experiments. A natural experiment is a situation when some change occurs in the environment that is not under experimental control and approximates random assignment. An example of such a natural experiment is changes to compulsory schooling laws. Conversely, animal models that feature increased individual differences in cognitive aging, under conditions of tightly controlled life-course exposures, can test for inherent genetic and biological moderators or mediators of CR. Quasi-experimental twin design is closest to this experimental design in humans. This design can utilize co-twins with different levels of exposures such as educational or occupational attainment.
4. BM
4.1. Definition
BM refers to the relative absence of changes in neural resources or neuropathologic change over time as a determinant of preserved cognition in older age.
BM can be influenced by multiple genetic and environmental factors, operating at various points across the lifespan.
4.2. Operational definition
BM is influenced by factors (genes, sex, early life influence or differential experiences) that slow or prevent brain changes associated with aging and disease. The emphasis centers on change over time. Thus, BM may be operationalized as minimal changes in brain markers of aging or disease associated with preservation of cognitive function.
Research aimed at further elucidating BM requires the inclusion of 3 components.
Measures of age-related brain changes, injury or disease that theoretically impact cognitive outcomes,
Measures of change in cognition.
Demonstrating a link between less change in 1 and less change in 2 would be evidence of BM.
To investigate potential mechanisms of individual differences in BM one could examine:
A hypothetical variable that influences 1.
This variable can encompass many of the same exposures potentially associated with CR. However, their impact on BM in this context would be specific to maintaining the structural and functional integrity of the brain.
4.3. Example studies of BM
BM is optimally ascertained in longitudinal designs. A single time point measurement cannot definitively differentiate people who have maintained their brain from those who did not but started at a higher baseline level. In both human and nonhuman studies this issue can be addressed to some degree by determining what level of brain status is expected for a particular age, or considering a given brain measure relative to the distribution seen in younger subjects. However, longitudinal designs are preferable to examine factors underlying interindividual differences in the change in neural resources that are in turn associated with differences in cognitive outcomes.
4.3.1. Longitudinal study of BM
A general approach to studying BM would be to examine longitudinally whether individual differences in the rate of age- or disease-related brain anomalies accumulated over time are related to individual differences in the rate of cognitive change.
4.3.2. Exposures related to BM
An extension of study 3.1 would be to assess potential proxies or mechanisms (e.g., genetic, lifestyle, neural) that are associated with these different trajectories of BM/change.
In summary, BM and CR are complementary concepts. BM accounts for individual differences in cognitive trajectories that are associated with differences in rate of brain change. In contrast, CR addresses individual differences in cognitive trajectories controlling for changes in neural resources or neuropathology.
5. BR
5.1. Definition
BR has been used to reflect the neurobiological status of the brain (numbers of neurons, synapses, etc.) at any point in time. BR does not involve active adaptation of functional cognitive processes in the presence of injury or disease as does CR.
5.2. Operational definition and example studies
Research aimed at further elucidating BR requires the inclusion of 2 components:
Measures of brain features that theoretically are associated with cognition.
Associated measures of cognition.
5.3. Example studies of BR
Longitudinally, differences in BR at a point in time could account for the observation that individuals starting at a different level of cognition may show the same rate of age- or disease-related cognitive decline. This could reflect different initial levels (intercepts) due to variation in BR, but similar rates of change (slopes) due to similar depletion of BR. This is distinguished from BM, where slopes would differ as a function of the degree of BM.
BR has also been associated with individual differences in level of cognition given a specific amount of brain change, injury or disease, such as amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tangles. This association could rely on a threshold model, where a specific amount of depletion of neurobiological capital results disease-related changes. Those who initially have a higher BR can tolerate more depletion before they show symptoms.
6. Conclusion
Here we present a framework that includes well defined operational definitions for 3 concepts: CR, BM and BR. We also propose the term resilience to subsume all of the concepts presented. The operational definitions were carefully designed to be applicable to both human and nonhuman studies.
We believe that the use of a common vocabulary and operational definitions will facilitate research design and communication. The framework also provides a basis for describing how the operational definition of another concept differs from those suggested here.
Our hope for this framework is that a common vocabulary and operational definitions will facilitate even greater progress in understanding the factors that are associated with successful aging and lifelong brain health.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the National Institute on Aging, National Institutes of Health, R24 AG06142, and in part by the Intramural Research Program of the National Institute on Aging.
The authors gratefully acknowledge the support and guidance of Drs Molly Wagster and Jonathan King, who conceived of and guided this project. In addition we acknowledge the contribution of numerous investigators who contributed to the development of the Framework. A listing is found in the supplementary materials.
Abbreviations:
- CR
Cognitive reserve
- BM
Brain maintenance
- BR
Brain reserve
Footnotes
Supplementary materials
Supplementary material associated with this article can be found, in the online version, at doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2022.10.015. This includes: 1. Aknowledgment list 2. descriptions of funded pilot projects and 3,a table that summarizes the projects and how they incorporate the framework presented here.
Disclosure statement
None of the authors have actual or potential conflicts of interest to disclose.
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.