Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2023 Apr 15;17(4):395–402. doi: 10.1007/s11682-023-00767-5

Structural brain network connectivity in trichotillomania (hair-pulling disorder)

Annerine Roos 1,2,, Jean-Paul Fouche 3, Dan J Stein 1,2, Christine Lochner 2
PMCID: PMC10435646  PMID: 37059898

Abstract

Neuroimaging studies suggest involvement of frontal, striatal, limbic and cerebellar regions in trichotillomania, an obsessive-compulsive related disorder. However, findings regarding the underlying neural circuitry remains limited and inconsistent. Graph theoretical analysis offers a way to identify structural brain networks in trichotillomania. T1-weighted MRI scans were acquired in adult females with trichotillomania (n = 23) and healthy controls (n = 16). Graph theoretical analysis was used to investigate structural networks as derived from cortical thickness and volumetric FreeSurfer output. Hubs, brain regions with highest connectivity in the global network, were identified, and group differences were determined. Regions with highest connectivity on a regional level were also determined. There were no differences in small-worldness or other network measures between groups. Hubs in the global network of trichotillomania patients included temporal, parietal, and occipital regions (at 2SD above mean network connectivity), as well as frontal and striatal regions (at 1SD above mean network connectivity). In contrast, in healthy controls hubs at 2SD represented different frontal, parietal and temporal regions, while at 1SD hubs were widespread. The inferior temporal gyrus, involved in object recognition as part of the ventral visual pathway, had significantly higher connectivity on a global and regional level in trichotillomania. The study included women only and sample size was limited. This study adds to the trichotillomania literature on structural brain network connectivity. Our study findings are consistent with previous studies that have implicated somatosensory, sensorimotor and frontal-striatal circuitry in trichotillomania, and partially overlap with structural connectivity findings in obsessive-compulsive disorder.

Keywords: Structural network connectivity, Graph theoretical analysis, Trichotillomania, Obsessive-compulsive related disorders

Introduction

Trichotillomania (TTM, or hair-pulling disorder), one of the obsessive-compulsive and related disorders (OCRDs) in DSM-5, is associated with repetitive hair-pulling resulting in hair loss and repeated attempts to decrease or stop the behaviour. The condition is associated with significant distress and impairment in several life domains (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). In a large recent study, point prevalence of trichotillomania in the US was reported to be 1.7% with a lifetime prevalence estimate of 2.5%. Common comorbidities in TTM include obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) (Grant et al., 2020). Neuropsychological deficits in TTM include those in response inhibition, working memory, visual memory, and divided attention (Slikboer et al., 2018).

Neuroimaging studies suggest involvement of frontal, striatal, limbic, and cerebellar regions in TTM (Chamberlain et al., 2009; Stein et al., 1997; van den Heuvel et al., 2010). Yet, evidence about underlying neural networks remains limited (Grant, 2019). To our knowledge, there is only one published brain network connectivity study in TTM to date. This study assessed functional connectivity of reward circuitry using resting state MRI, suggesting lower connectivity in frontal, striatal, and limbic regions involved in reward processing (White et al., 2013). The microstructural integrity of white matter tracts has been investigated in TTM in two studies that used diffusion tensor imaging. The one study suggested altered white matter integrity in the anterior cingulate, temporal, and pre-supplementary motor regions (Chamberlain et al., 2010). The other study reported no differences in white matter integrity between TTM patients and controls, but found that white matter integrity in the frontal-striatal-thalamic pathway was associated with illness duration and severity (Roos et al., 2013). Findings from these few studies have thus not always been consistent, emphasizing the need for further study to investigate underlying neural circuitry in TTM.

The global arrangement of a brain network represents a “small-world” organization that optimizes local and global information processing. The changes in structural (and functional) connectivity that have been reported in several neuropsychiatric disorders may be associated with altered topology including in small-world organization, and in the balance between the integration and segregation of information (Bassett & Bullmore, 2009; Fornito et al., 2015; Lord et al., 2017; Menon, 2011). The aim of this study was to derive structural brain networks and investigate its topology in TTM using graph theoretical analysis.

Methods

Study design

Participants were recruited by means of general media, e.g., newspaper and online advertisements, and referrals by psychologists and psychiatrists. Individuals who showed interest in the study were screened telephonically and invited to attend comprehensive clinical assessment and brain imaging sessions as part of the study. Patients with a history of neurological illness, psychosis, substance or alcohol use disorder, head trauma, clinically significant depression, or a contraindication to MRI, were excluded from participation. Current daily psychotropic medication use within the last 12 months was also an exclusion criterium. Healthy controls had no current or lifetime history of any DSM-IV disorder, and were without significant neurological conditions and current or lifetime daily use of psychotropic medication. The study was approved by the Health Research Ethics Committees of Stellenbosch University (HREC Ref. M07/05/019) and the University of Cape Town (HREC Ref. 261/2007). Participants provided written informed consent. The study was conducted according to the ethical guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Clinical assessments

The Structured Clinical Interview for Obsessive-Compulsive Spectrum Disorders (SCID-OCSD) (du Toit et al., 2001) was updated to include DSM-5 criteria, and used to confirm a diagnosis of TTM. The Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview Plus (MINI Plus v5) (Sheehan et al., 1998) was used to assess comorbidity. The severity of TTM symptoms was assessed using the Massachusetts General Hospital Hair-pulling Scale (MGH-HPS) (Keuthen et al., 1995). For inclusion in the study, TTM participants had to present with significant hair-pulling, i.e., both in terms of the frequency of hair-pulling, the inability to control the behavior, and the resulting distress. The Clinical Global Impressions Scale - Severity (CGI-S) (Busner & Targum, 2007) was used to assess current global functioning in cases.

MRI procedures and analyses

Participants underwent structural MRI using a 3-T Siemens scanner to acquire high-resolution 3D-MPRAGE images. Scan parameters were: slab orientation, sagittal; TR/TE, 2300/3.93 ms; flip angle, 12°; FOV, 256 × 240 × 160 mm3; and voxel size, 1.3 × 1.0 × 1.0 mm3. Cortical thickness and subcortical volume estimates of 86 bilateral brain regions as determined by FreeSurfer v6 (Desikan et al., 2006; Fischl et al., 2004; Fischl & Dale, 2000), were used to construct structural brain networks using graph theoretical analysis (Hosseini et al., 2012; Rubinov & Sporns, 2010). Volumetric data were corrected for individual intracranial volume. The Graph Analyses Toolbox (Hosseini et al., 2012) was used to construct brain networks and determine group differences in brain network organization, while the Brain Connectivity Toolbox (Rubinov & Sporns, 2010) was used to quantify network measures.

Structural networks were created following the steps as described by Hosseini and colleagues (Hosseini et al., 2012, 2013). FreeSurfer morphometric data were corrected for age using linear regression analysis given the range of 18 to 61 years. Residuals from this analysis were utilized to create structural correlation networks. An 84 × 84 association matrix R was made for each group, and every node for a participant denoted a Pearson correlation between residuals of regions i and j (Bernhardt et al., 2011). A binary adjacency matrix A was then created from each R matrix with values set at 1 or 0. Negative correlation values were replaced by zero (Bernhardt et al., 2011; Fan et al., 2011). This provided a binary undirected graph with 86 nodes and a network density that is the fraction of connections to every potential connection.

Metrics defining small-world properties including the clustering coefficient and characteristic path length were determined (Hosseini et al., 2013; Rubinov & Sporns, 2010). The clustering coefficient denotes the mean number of connections of a region with nearby regions, while the mean clustering coefficient signifies network segregation. The characteristic path length denotes the mean shortest path length among pairs of regions that signifies network integration. These metrics are compared to random networks with similar connectivity and distribution of regions (nodes) and connections (edges), to quantify the brain network arrangement (Maslov & Sneppen, 2002; Milo et al., 2002). Such a network has a small-world index >1, including a clustering coefficient that has a ratio > 1 compared to random networks, and a characteristic path length with a ratio close to 1 that is like that of random networks. Other metrics tested for group differences were modularity and transitivity assessing network segregation into communities of regions or clusters respectively, and global efficiency as an indicator of network integration (Fornito et al., 2016; Vertes & Bullmore, 2015).

Regional network connectivity was derived by nodal betweenness centrality, i.e. a measure of regions with highest local connectivity, that is determined as the fraction of all shortest path lengths crossing a specific region (Hosseini et al., 2013). This metric also detects global network connectivity of regions, i.e., hubs that are most connected in the network. A region represents a hub when its nodal betweenness centrality is 1 to 2 standard deviations (SDs) above that of the mean network connectivity (Bernhardt et al., 2011).

Nonparametric permutation tests with 1000 permutations were used to determine group differences in network measures (He et al., 2008). As described by Hosseini and colleagues (Hosseini et al., 2012) in this analysis, residuals of individual participants were randomly re-allocated to either the TTM or control group while retaining the initial sample size per group. An association matrix was derived for every newly randomized group, followed by creation of binary adjacency matrices and determination of network measures at minimum network density. Thereafter, differences between randomized groups in each network measure were determined, creating a permutation distribution of difference below the null hypothesis. The real difference in network measures between TTM and controls were mapped in the relevant permutation distribution and a two-tailed p value determined according to its percentile position. The permutation tests inherently apply correction for multiple comparisons in assessing global metrics using maximal statistics (Nichols & Holmes, 2002; NISOx: SnPM, n.d.). Regional group results were corrected using false discovery rate (FDR) (Hosseini et al., 2012). Group differences in brain network parameters were identified at minimum network density (Bernhardt et al., 2011; He et al., 2008) and presented across a range of densities. A brain network has a specific density interval where connections are least random, and every node is connected to at least one other node (Kaiser & Hilgetag, 2006). The minimum density of networks for TTM and controls was 0.24, thus representing the lower bound where networks were not fragmented. The maximum density was 0.44 (small-world index <1.5) above which networks became increasingly random, thus connections would probably not refer to biologically relevant networks above this density (Kaiser & Hilgetag, 2006).

Results

Demographic and clinical information of participants is shown in Table 1. The sample included adult participants with TTM (n = 23) and controls (n = 16). Our cohort included women only. Age, level of education, and type of employment were similar between groups. Illness severity was generally mild to moderate in the TTM group (mean CGI-S severity score [SD] = 3.76 [1.37]).

Table 1.

Demographic and clinical information of participants

TTM Control p
mean (SD) / n, %
Age (years) 36.09 (14.52) 30.69 (8.19) 0.188
Educational level (n) 0.612
 School 9, 40% 5, 31%
 Tertiary 14, 60% 11, 69%
Employment 0.194
 Employed (paid) 13, 56% 12, 75%
 Homemaker (unpaid) 5, 22% 0, 0%
 Student/Scholar 5, 22% 4, 25%
Ethnicity 0.561
 Caucasian 21, 91% 14, 88%
 Mixed Race/Black/Other 2, 9% 2, 12%
Age of illness onset (years) 13.83 (8.04)
Illness duration (years) 22.26 (16.05)
MGH-HPS total 14.52 (6.26)
CGI-S total 3.76 (1.37) 1.00 (0.0)

Overall, the brain networks of groups were similar in network arrangement, thus adhering to specified small-world parameters (normalized small-world index p = 0.69) There were no group differences in network measures of segregation (normalized characteristic path length p = 0.37, modularity p = 0.60) or integration (normalized clustering coefficient p = 0.97, global efficiency p = 0.53).

Regarding global connectivity, in the TTM group hubs at 2SD were the left inferior temporal gyrus, parietal, and occipital regions, while hubs at 1SD additionally were in frontal and striatal regions (Table 2, Fig. 1). In contrast, in healthy controls hubs at 2SD were in different frontal, parietal and temporal regions compared to TTM. In healthy controls at 1SD hubs were widespread and generally in different regions, for instance, in other segments of the inferior frontal gyrus and the striatum.

Table 2.

Hubs of the TTM and control groups. Hubs are presented as regions with connectivity of 2 and 1 standard deviation (SD), respectively, above that of mean network connectivity. Hubs identified at both SD levels per group are indicated in italics, primarily representing parietal, temporal and occipital regions in TTM. Frontal and striatal hubs were evident in the TTM group at 1 SD above that of mean network connectivity

Lobe TTM Control TTM Control
2 SD 2 SD 1 SD 1 SD
Frontal R Pars opercularis L Pars orbitalis R Pars opercularis
R Pars orbitalis L Pars triangularis
L Lateral orbitofrontal L Frontal pole
R Lateral orbitofrontal
R Caudal anterior cingulate
Parietal L Postcentral R Posterior cingulate L Postcentral R Posterior cingulate
R Supramarginal R Supramarginal L Supramarginal
L Postcentral
R Postcentral
R Paracentral
Temporal/limbic/striatal L Inferior temporal* R Superior temporal L Inferior temporal* R Superior temporal
R Inferior temporal R Entorhinal R Inferior temporal R Entorhinal
L Transverse temporal L Transverse temporal R Banks superior temporal sulcus
R Isthmus of cingulate gyrus
R Parahippocampal
L Caudate R Ventral diencephalon
Occipital R Cuneus R Cuneus R Precuneus

*Highly connected both at a global and regional level

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Hub regions at 2 SD within the healthy control [top row] and the TTM group [bottom row]. The hubs in the TTM group were the right (R) supramarginal gyrus (SMG), bilateral inferior temporal gyrus (ITG), left (L) transverse temporal gyrus (TTG) and R cuneus (CUN). The hubs in the control group were the R pars opercularis part of the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), R posterior cingulate gyrus (PoCG), R superior temporal gyrus (STG) and R entorhinal gyrus (EG). The size of the red circle indicates the number of connections that a hub has in the network. The lobe is indicated at the bottom right by shape- and color-coded legends

Regarding regional network connectivity, there were significant differences between study groups in two regions. Local connectivity was significantly higher in TTM compared to controls in the left inferior temporal gyrus (p < 0.001). Local connectivity was significantly higher in controls compared to TTM in the right pericalcarine gyrus (p = 0.04), located below the cuneus in the occipital lobe.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this study is the first to use graph theoretical analysis to investigate structural brain networks in TTM. The main findings suggest 1) similar small-world properties and network measures including segregation and integration between patients and controls, 2) involvement of different hubs in the global network of TTM patients compared to controls, including inferior temporal, parietal and occipital hubs (at 2SD above mean network connectivity) and frontal and striatal hubs (at 1SD above mean network connectivity), and 3) a role for the inferior temporal gyrus on a global and regional level in TTM, involved in object recognition as part of the ventral visual pathway.

Corresponding small-worldness and network measures in both TTM and controls suggest brain networks that generally have similar network arrangement. This is partly consistent with some studies in OCD, the key example of the obsessive-compulsive and related disorders (OCRDs), that found similar small-world properties compared to controls (Kim et al., 2013; Reess et al., 2016). Earlier studies suggest involvement of cortical-striatal networks in both TTM and OCD (Chamberlain et al., 2009; de Wit et al., 2014; van den Heuvel et al., 2010). Potentially relevant is recent evidence in OCD that shows distributed involvement with altered connectivity in cortical-striatal-thalamic-cortical networks, the default mode network, in specific frontal, parietal, temporal and limbic regions within networks, and the cerebellum (Hou et al., 2013; Kim et al., 2013; Reess et al., 2016; Zhong et al., 2014).

Our findings indeed suggest a distributed set of regions operating as hubs in the global network in TTM. These include temporal, parietal, and occipital regions, as well as frontal and striatal regions. Hubs found across lobes in the TTM group broadly coincide with regions implicated in TTM using other imaging modalities, e.g. diffusion tensor imaging and resting state functional imaging (Slikboer et al., 2018). Notably, the inferior temporal gyrus had significantly high connectivity on a global and regional level. This gyrus has a role in object recognition as part of the ventral visual pathway (Conway, 2018; Kanwisher, 2010). Aberrant connectivity of the temporal and parietal hubs located in somatosensory and sensorimotor networks also likely underlie core symptomatology in TTM, including over-responsivity to external sensations in tactile and auditory domains (Falkenstein et al., 2018). This provides support for earlier findings suggesting involvement of the inferior and superior parietal cortex and left somatosensory cortices (Chamberlain et al., 2009, 2010; Swedo et al., 1991), and the left temporal lobe (Chamberlain et al., 2010; Odlaug et al., 2014; Warrier et al., 2009) in TTM. Regarding the cuneus of the occipital lobe, this hub functionally forms part of the primary visual and somatosensory networks (Tomasi & Volkow, 2011). Cortical thickness of the right cuneus has been associated with impulsivity (Kubera et al., 2018) and sensation seeking (Miglin et al., 2019) which, along with novelty-seeking, have been associated with TTM (Flessner et al., 2012). Higher volume of the right cuneus and superior occipital lobe where this region is located, has also previously been reported in TTM (Chamberlain et al., 2009; Grachev, 1997), and may suggest increased neural connections that affects its operation within networks. Altered structural connectivity of occipital lobe regions in visual and sensory networks may contribute to altered attention to and perception of spatial detail, and aberrant behavioral control in TTM.

The current study findings also suggest involvement of frontal-striatal circuitry in TTM. Consistent with aspects of earlier studies (Grachev, 1997; Odlaug et al., 2014), we found that anterior and inferior frontal regions were highly connected in the global network of the TTM group. Our hubs represented the lower segment of the inferior frontal gyrus, i.e. bilateral pars orbitalis, which is located adjacent to the lateral orbitofrontal cortex that was also a hub in our TTM group, and also structurally altered in OCD in previous studies (Fouche et al., 2017; Venkatasubramanian et al., 2012). Finally, our finding that the left caudate is a hub in TTM supports earlier work suggesting an association between symptom severity of TTM and left caudate activity (Stein et al., 2002). Of note is that structural abnormalities have also been reported in other parts of the dorsal striatum in TTM, including the right caudate (Isobe et al., 2018) and left putamen (Chamberlain et al., 2009; O’Sullivan et al., 1997). The dorsal striatum is involved in decision-making particularly about actions, reward, and habit formation resulting in automated behaviors based on sensorimotor, cognitive, affective and motivational determinants (Balleine et al., 2007; Lipton et al., 2019). Thus, although the evidence is limited, and somewhat mixed, it appears that the dorsal striatum may have altered structural connectivity and function in TTM.

Hubs in our controls represented principal regions typically found in structural networks of healthy adults, which include those of the default mode network (Hagmann et al. 2008; van den Heuvel & Sporns, 2013). On a regional level, the pericalcarine gyrus situated in the occipital lobe that operates within the greater sensorimotor network, had lower connectivity in TTM patients than controls. Interestingly, lower pericalcarine cortical density has been associated with sensation seeking behavior (Miglin et al., 2019), a tendency that may be relevant to TTM (Lejoyeux et al., 1998).

This study had limitations. First, the study included women only, in line with epidemiological data suggesting that the majority of individuals with TTM are female (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Lochner et al., 2010; Woods et al., 2006). Second, sample size was limited. Nevertheless, with our whole brain explorative approach, this study of structural brain network connectivity in TTM using graph theoretical analysis, is novel. Third, structural networks were created using inter-regional correlations at group level and therefore do not reflect networks at individual level. Therefore, other factors that may determine underlying structural covariance such as clinical measures could not be associated with network measures. Results may also be interpreted differently depending on parcellation strategies, differences in network density, the strength of connections and whether a network is weighted or not (Farahani et al., 2019). Although every network measure provides information on the topology of the brain, cortical thickness represents one of several types of parcellation schemes used to define structural connectivity or covariance.

Conclusions

Our findings largely coincide with previous studies implicating somatosensory, sensorimotor and frontal-striatal circuitry in TTM. Our findings also partially overlap with structural connectivity findings in OCD, a key example of the OCRDs. Further study in larger samples is needed to differentiate structural and functional networks of this condition.

Acknowledgements

We thank the Centre for High Performance Computing (Cape Town, SA) who provided a platform to analyze the structural data. We also thank the women who participated in this study.

Code availability

The software used is publicly available.

Funding sources

The study was funded by the National Research Foundation (NRF) of South Africa (reference number: 99099). Opinions expressed and conclusions arrived at, are those of the author and are not necessarily attributed to the NRF.

Author contributions

Author contributions included conception, study design and overseeing of data collection (CL), data analysis (JPF), interpretation of results (AR), drafting the manuscript (AR), and providing critical input and approval of the manuscript (All authors).

Funding

Open access funding provided by Stellenbosch University.

Data availability

Data of this study are available from the authors upon reasonable request as per cohort guidelines.

Declarations

Conflict of interest

There are no conflicts of interest to report.

Ethics approval and compliance with ethical standards

The study was approved by the Health Research Ethics Committees of Stellenbosch University (HREC Ref. M07/05/019) and the University of Cape Town (HREC Ref. 261/2007). The study was conducted according to the ethical guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Consent to participate

Participants provided written informed consent.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5. American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  2. Balleine BW, Delgado MR, Hikosaka O. The role of the dorsal striatum in reward and decision-making. Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27(31):8161–8165. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1554-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bassett, D. S., & Bullmore, E. T. (2009). Human brain networks in health and disease. Current Opinion in Neurology, 22(4), 340–347. 10.1097/WCO.0B013E32832D93DD [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  4. Bernhardt BC, Chen Z, He Y, Evans AC, Bernasconi N. Graph-theoretical analysis reveals disrupted small-world organization of cortical thickness correlation networks in temporal lobe epilepsy. Cerebral Cortex. 2011;21(9):2147–2157. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq291. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Busner J, Targum SD. The clinical global impressions scale: Applying a research tool in clinical practice. Psychiatry. 2007;4(7):28–37. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Chamberlain SR, Hampshire A, Menzies LA, Garyfallidis E, Grant JE, Odlaug BL, Craig K, Fineberg N, Sahakian BJ. Reduced brain white matter integrity in trichotillomania: A diffusion tensor imaging study. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2010;67(9):965–971. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Chamberlain SR, Odlaug BL, Boulougouris V, Fineberg NA, Grant JE. Trichotillomania: Neurobiology and treatment. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2009;33(6):831–842. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.02.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Conway BR. The organization and operation of inferior temporal cortex. Annual Review of Vision Science. 2018;4(1):381–402. doi: 10.1146/annurev-vision-091517-034202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. de Wit SJ, Alonso P, Schweren L, Mataix-Cols D, Lochner C, Menchon JM, Stein DJ, Fouche JP, Soriano-Mas C, Sato JR, Hoexter MQ, Denys D, Nakamae T, Nishida S, Kwon JS, Jang JH, Busatto GF, Cardoner N, Cath DC, van den Heuvel OA. Multicenter voxel-based morphometry mega-analysis of structural brain scans in obsessive-compulsive disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 2014;171(3):340–349. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13040574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Desikan RS, Segonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, Blacker D, Buckner RL, Dale AM, Maguire RP, Hyman BT, Albert MS, Killiany RJ. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. NeuroImage. 2006;31(3):968–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. du Toit PL, van Kradenburg J, Niehaus D, Stein DJ. Comparison of obsessive-compulsive disorder patients with and without comorbid putative obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorders using a structured clinical interview. Comprehensive Psychiatry. 2001;42(4):291–300. doi: 10.1053/comp.2001.24586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Falkenstein MJ, Conelea CA, Garner LE, Haaga DAF. Sensory over-responsivity in trichotillomania (hair-pulling disorder) Psychiatry Research. 2018;260:207–218. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.11.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Fan Y, Shi F, Smith JK, Lin W, Gilmore JH, Shen D. Brain anatomical networks in early human brain development. NeuroImage. 2011;54(3):1862–1871. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2010.07.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Farahani FV, Karwowski W, Lighthall NR. Application of graph theory for identifying connectivity patterns in human brain networks: A systematic review. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2019;13:585. doi: 10.3389/FNINS.2019.00585/BIBTEX. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Fischl B, Dale AM. Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2000;97(20):11050–11055. doi: 10.1073/pnas.200033797. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Fischl B, Salat DH, van der Kouwe AJ, Makris N, Segonne F, Quinn BT, Dale AM. Sequence-independent segmentation of magnetic resonance images. NeuroImage. 2004;23 Suppl 1:S69–S84. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Flessner CA, Knopik VS, McGeary J. Hair pulling disorder (trichotillomania): Genes, neurobiology, and a model for understanding impulsivity and compulsivity. Psychiatry Research. 2012;199(3):151–158. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.03.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Fornito, A., Zalesky, A., & Breakspear, M. (2015). The connectomics of brain disorders. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 16(3), 159–172. 10.1038/NRN3901 [DOI] [PubMed]
  19. Fornito A, Zalesky A, Bullmore ET. Fundamentals of brain network analysis. Elsevier; 2016. Fundamentals of brain network analysis. [Google Scholar]
  20. Fouche J-P, Du Plessis S, Hattingh C, Roos A, Lochner C, Soriano-Mas C, Sato JR, Nakamae T, Nishida S, Kwon JS, Jung WH, Mataix-Cols D, Hoexter MQ, Alonso P, De Wit SJ, Veltman DJ, Stein DJ, Van Den Heuvel OA. Cortical thickness in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Multisite mega-analysis of 780 brain scans from six centres. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2017;210(1):67–74. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.115.164020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Grachev ID. MRI-based morphometric topographic parcellation of human neocortex in trichotillomania. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 1997;51(5):315–321. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.1997.tb03205.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Grant J. Trichotillomania (hair pulling disorder) Indian Journal of Psychiatry. 2019;61(7):S136–S139. doi: 10.4103/psychiatry.IndianJPsychiatry_529_18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Grant JE, Dougherty DD, Chamberlain SR. Prevalence, gender correlates, and co-morbidity of trichotillomania. Psychiatry Research. 2020;288:112948. doi: 10.1016/J.PSYCHRES.2020.112948. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Hagmann P, Cammoun L, Gigandet X, Meuli R, Honey CJ, Van Wedeen J, Sporns O. Mapping the structural core of human cerebral cortex. PLoS Biology. 2008;6(7):1479–1493. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. He Y, Chen Z, Evans A. Structural insights into aberrant topological patterns of large-scale cortical networks in Alzheimer’s disease. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2008;28(18):4756–4766. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0141-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hosseini SM, Black JM, Soriano T, Bugescu N, Martinez R, Raman MM, Kesler SR, Hoeft F. Topological properties of large-scale structural brain networks in children with familial risk for reading difficulties. NeuroImage. 2013;71:260–274. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Hosseini SM, Hoeft F, Kesler SR. GAT: A graph-theoretical analysis toolbox for analyzing between-group differences in large-scale structural and functional brain networks. PLoS One. 2012;7(7):e40709. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0040709. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hou J, Song L, Zhang W, Wu W, Wang J, Zhou D, Qu W, Guo J, Gu S, He M, Xie B, Li H. Morphologic and functional connectivity alterations of corticostriatal and default mode metwork in treatment-naïve patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. PLoS One. 2013;8(12):e83931. doi: 10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0083931. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Isobe M, Redden SA, Keuthen NJ, Stein DJ, Lochner C, Grant JE, Chamberlain SR. Striatal abnormalities in trichotillomania: A multi-site MRI analysis. NeuroImage: Clinical. 2018;17:893–898. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.12.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kaiser M, Hilgetag CC. Nonoptimal component placement, but short processing paths, due to long-distance projections in neural systems. PLoS Computational Biology. 2006;2(7):e95. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.0020095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kanwisher N. Functional specificity in the human brain: A window into the functional architecture of the mind. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2010;107(25):11163–11170. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1005062107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Keuthen NJ, O’Sullivan RL, Ricciardi JN, Shera D, Savage CR, Borgmann AS, Jenike MA, Baer L. The Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH) hairpulling scale: 1. Development and factor analyses. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics. 1995;64(3–4):141–145. doi: 10.1159/000289003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kim SG, Jung WH, Kim SN, Jang JH, Kwon JS. Disparity between dorsal and ventral networks in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder: Evidence revealed by graph theoretical analysis based on cortical thickness from MRI. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2013;7:302. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kubera KM, Schmitgen MM, Maier-Hein KH, Thomann PA, Hirjak D, Wolf RC. Differential contributions of cortical thickness and surface area to trait impulsivity in healthy young adults. Behavioural Brain Research. 2018;350:65–71. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Lejoyeux M, Feuché N, Loi S, Solomon J, Adès J. Impulse-control disorders in alcoholics are related to sensation seeking and not to impulsivity. Psychiatry Research. 1998;81(2):149–155. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(98)00103-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Lipton DM, Gonzales BJ, Citri A. Dorsal striatal circuits for habits, compulsions and addictions. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 2019;13:28. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2019.00028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Lochner C, Seedat S, Stein DJ. Chronic hair-pulling: Phenomenology-based subtypes. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2010;24(2):196–202. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.10.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Lord, L. D., Stevner, A. B., Deco, G., & Kringelbach, M. L. (2017). Understanding principles of integration and segregation using whole-brain computational connectomics: Implications for neuropsychiatric disorders. Philosophical Transactions. Series A, Mathematical, Physical, and Engineering Sciences, 375(2096). 10.1098/RSTA.2016.0283 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  39. Maslov S, Sneppen K. Specificity and stability in topology of protein networks. Science. 2002;296(5569):910–913. doi: 10.1126/science.1065103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Menon, V. (2011). Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: A unifying triple network model. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(10), 483–506. 10.1016/J.TICS.2011.08.003 [DOI] [PubMed]
  41. Miglin R, Bounoua N, Goodling S, Sheehan A, Spielberg JM, Sadeh N. Cortical thickness links impulsive personality traits and risky behavior. Brain Sciences. 2019;9(12):373. doi: 10.3390/brainsci9120373. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Milo R, Shen-Orr S, Itzkovitz S, Kashtan N, Chklovskii D, Alon U. Network motifs: Simple building blocks of complex networks. Science. 2002;298(5594):824–827. doi: 10.1126/science.298.5594.824. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Nichols TE, Holmes AP. Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: A primer with examples. Human Brain Mapping. 2002;15(1):1. doi: 10.1002/HBM.1058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. NISOx: SnPM. (n.d.). Retrieved January 25, 2023, from http://www.nisox.org/Software/SnPM13/.
  45. O’Sullivan RL, Rauch SL, Breiter HC, Grachev ID, Baer L, Kennedy DN, Keuthen NJ, Savage CR, Manzo PA, Caviness VS, Jenike MA. Reduced basal ganglia volumes in trichotillomania measured via morphometric magnetic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry. 1997;42(1):39–45. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00297-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Odlaug BL, Chamberlain SR, Derbyshire KL, Leppink EW, Grant JE. Impaired response inhibition and excess cortical thickness ascandidate endophenotypes for trichotillomania. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2014;59:167–173. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Reess TJ, Rus OG, Schmidt R, De Reus MA, Zaudig M, Wagner G, Zimmer C, Van Den Heuvel MP, Koch K. Connectomics-based structural network alterations in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Translational Psychiatry. 2016;6(9):e882. doi: 10.1038/tp.2016.163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Roos A, Fouche J-P, Stein DJ, Lochner C. White matter integrity in hair-pulling disorder (trichotillomania) Psychiatry Research - Neuroimaging. 2013;211(3):246–250. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Rubinov M, Sporns O. Complex network measures of brain connectivity: Uses and interpretations. NeuroImage. 2010;52(3):1059–1069. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, Hergueta T, Baker R, Dunbar GC. The Mini-international neuropsychiatric interview (M.I.N.I.): The development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 1998;59 Suppl 2:22–57. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Slikboer R, Reser MP, Nedeljkovic M, Castle DJ, Rossell SL. Systematic review of published primary studies of neuropsychology and neuroimaging in trichotillomania. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2018;24(2):188–205. doi: 10.1017/S1355617717000819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Stein DJ, Coetzer R, Lee M, Davids B, Bouwer C. Magnetic resonance brain imaging in women with obsessive-compulsive disorder and trichotillomania. Psychiatry Research - Neuroimaging. 1997;74(3):177–182. doi: 10.1016/S0925-4927(97)00010-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Stein DJ, Van Heerden B, Hugo C, Van Kradenburg J, Warwick J, Zungu-Dirwayi N, Seedat S. Functional brain imaging and pharmacotherapy in trichotillomania: Single photon emission computed tomography before and after treatment with the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor citalopram. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 2002;26(5):885–890. doi: 10.1016/S0278-5846(01)00334-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Swedo SE, Rapoport JL, Leonard HL, Schapiro MB, Rapoport SI, Grady CL. Regional cerebral glucose metabolism of women with trichotillomania. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1991;48(9):828–833. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1991.01810330052008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Tomasi D, Volkow ND. Association between functional connectivity hubs and brain networks. Cerebral Cortex. 2011;21(9):2003–2013. doi: 10.1093/CERCOR/BHQ268. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. van den Heuvel MP, Sporns O. Network hubs in the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2013;17(12):683–696. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. van den Heuvel OA, van der Werf YD, Verhoef KMW, de Wit S, Berendse HW, Wolters EC, Veltman DJ, Groenewegen HJ. Frontal-striatal abnormalities underlying behaviours in the compulsive-impulsive spectrum. Journal of the Neurological Sciences. 2010;289(1–2):55–59. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2009.08.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Venkatasubramanian G, Zutshi A, Jindal S, Srikanth SG, Kovoor JME, Kumar JK, Janardhan Reddy YC. Comprehensive evaluation of cortical structure abnormalities in drug-naïve, adult patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder: A surface-based morphometry study. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2012;46(9):1161–1168. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Vertes, P. E., & Bullmore, E. T. (2015). Annual research review: Growth connectomics – the organization and reorganization of brain networks during normal and abnormal development. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 3, 299–320. 10.1111/jcpp.12365 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  60. Warrier C, Wong P, Penhune V, Zatorre R, Parrish T, Abrams D, Kraus N. Relating structure to function: Heschl’s gyrus and acoustic processing. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29(1):61–69. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3489-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. White MP, Shirer WR, Molfino MJ, Tenison C, Damoiseaux JS, Greicius MD. Disordered reward processing and functional connectivity in trichotillomania: A pilot study. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2013;47(9):1264–1272. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.05.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Woods DW, Flessner CA, Franklin ME, Keuthen NJ, Goodwin RD, Stein DJ, Walther MR. The trichotillomania impact project (TIP): Exploring phenomenology, functional impairment, and treatment utilization. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 2006;67(12):1877–1888. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v67n1207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Zhong Z, Zhao T, Luo J, Guo Z, Guo M, Li P, Sun J, He Y, Li Z. Abnormal topological organization in white matter structural networks revealed by diffusion tensor tractography in unmedicated patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 2014;51:39–50. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2014.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

Data of this study are available from the authors upon reasonable request as per cohort guidelines.


Articles from Brain Imaging and Behavior are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES