Skip to main content
Journal of Epidemiology logoLink to Journal of Epidemiology
. 2007 Nov 30;12(2):93–104. doi: 10.2188/jea.12.93

Rural-urban Differences in Sociodemographic, Social Network and Lifestyle Factors Related to Mortality of Middle-aged Japanese Men from the Komo-Ise Cohort Study

Motoki Iwasaki 1, Tetsuya Otani 1, Akiko Ohta 1, Sasazawa Yosiaki 1, Masaya Kuroiwa 1, Shosuke Suzuki 1
PMCID: PMC10468338  PMID: 12033534

Abstract

To examine rural-urban differences in the relationships of sociodemographic, social network, and lifestyle factors to mortality in middle-aged men, we used the data from a community based prospective cohort study, the Komo-Ise study. The subjects were all men aged 40-69 years living in Komochi Village, the rural group (n=2,295), or the downtown district of Isesaki City, the urban group (n=3,334), as of 1993. They completed a self-administered questionnaire in 1993 and were followed for all-cause deaths until 2000. The Cox proportional hazards model was used to compute relative risks (RRs) with 95% confidence intervals (CIs). Low educated men and men without a spouse in the rural group had an increased risk of mortality (RR=4.4; 95%CI: 1.1-18.2, RR=2.4; 95%CI: 1.2-4.5). Men who did not enjoy good fellowship with their neighbors in the rural group had a decreased risk of mortality (RR=0.58; 95%CI: 0.35-0.97). Mortality risks were significantly higher in urban men not participating in hobbies, club activities or community groups (RR=1.6; 95%CI: 1.1-2.4). These variables remained significant risk factors, even after controlling for all sociodemographic, social network, lifestyle, and health status variables. Educational level, marital status and relation to neighborhoods showed significant rural-urban differences.

Keywords: rural-urban differences, sociodemographic factors, lifestyle, social networks, mortality

INTRODUCTION

The dramatic shift in the major causes of disability and death from infectious to chronic diseases has made theories of disease etiology shift from a single factor to multiple factors including behavioral and environmental as well as biologic and genetic factors. Among these factors, many epidemiologists have focused on health behavior or lifestyle factors, such as smoking habit, overweight, alcohol consumption, physical inactivity and so on, for the past several decades1-4). A number of prospective studies based on randomly selected cohort groups have indicated these to be the major determinants of premature and preventable disease and/or death1-4). In addition to health behavior and lifestyle factors, poor social networks and social support have been recognized as independent risk factors for mortality5-7), since Cassel8) hypothesized that psychosocial effects in the environment increased a person’s resistance to risk factors.

Among the earlier large investigations of the relationship between health and human ties, was the study performed in Alameda County, California. Berkman et al.5) examined four social network sources: 1) marriage; 2) contacts with close friends and relatives; 3) church membership, and 4) informal and formal group associations. Their findings revealed social and community ties to be associated with mortality risk and each of the four sources to be a predicted risk factor for mortality independently from the other three. However, the Tecumseh Community Health Study9) and the Evans County Cardiovascular Epidemiologic Study10) results were not consistent with those of the Alameda County Study. The Tecumseh Community Health Study9) replicated and extended Berkman’s work in the Alameda County Study. House et al.9) reported only men with higher levels of social relationships and activities to be significantly less likely to die during the follow-up period. Notable differences were somewhat weaker associations of mortality with friend and relative contact and religious involvement, and the much stronger association of mortality with other organizational involvements in the Tecumseh community as compared to those in the Alameda County Study. The Evans County Cardiovascular Epidemiologic Study10) also replicated Berkman’s study of social networks and mortality in Alameda County. Schoenbach et al.10) found evidence of a relationship between social networks and mortality only among white males and that the social network effects among white females, black males, and black females were weaker and clearly non-significant. These findings suggested social networks to have different meanings and effects according to locality7). Seeman et al.11) hypothesized that previous inconsistencies in the findings from different studies might indeed reflect sociocultural differences among the various samples based on a comparative analysis of three communities for elderly people.

In a number of subsequent studies, not only social networks but also social supports constituting social relationships have increasingly been recognized as risk factors for mortality and morbidity12,13). In addition to social support, the effect of stress on mortality has been analyzed simultaneously14). Social relationship studies have been designed and conducted. Large amounts of interesting knowledge have thus been accumulated for many years. Nonetheless, the impact of intercommunity variations on the association between social networks and mortality in middle-aged people remains unknown.

We have accumulated little knowledge on the association between social networks and mortality among middle-aged Japanese men, especially rural-urban differences in mortality risk. We conducted a community based prospective study, the Komo-Ise Study, that began with a baseline sample in 1993 for the purpose of clarifying the association between sociodemographic, social network, and lifestyle factors and mortality in middle-aged residents of Gunma prefecture. Some notable results have already been reported from baseline data15-17). Herein, the purpose of this study is to clarify rural-urban differences in the relationships of sociodemographic, social network and lifestyle factors to mortality in middle-aged men. We report the results, focusing especially on social network factors.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Study population

The Komo-Ise Study includes two research areas. One is Komochi Village which is located in the center of the Japan Archipelago, in the northwest comer of the Kanto Plain. The other is Isesaki City which is located in the southeast of Komochi Village. The village had a population of 12,141 with 3,284 households and the city had a population of 120,236 with 40,335 households, according to the 1995 census18). All male and female residents aged 40 to 69 years, 4,875 in Komochi Village and 7,755 in the downtown district of Isesaki City, were selected from registration records. In this study, “downtown district” denoted former Isesaki Town historically that has been the center of Isesaki City for more than 50 years. Self-administered questionnaires were distributed through the respective municipal government offices to all of Komochi Village in January 1993 and to those of the downtown district of Isesaki City in October 1993, and completed questionnaires were collected in sealed envelopes. A total of 11,565 subjects in both areas responded to the questionnaire (response rate: 91.6%) in the baseline survey. Ohta et al.17) have already reported that non-response bias and selection bias were inconsiderable in this baseline study. The present study analyzed data from all men aged 40-69 years living in Komochi Village (n=2,295, a rural group) or the downtown district of Isesaki City (n=3,334, an urban group).

Study variables

The questionnaire consisted of items on sociodemographic characteristics, social network, lifestyle, health status, and a symptom checklist known as the Todai Health Index (THI)19,20).

Sociodemographic items included sex, age, occupation, and educational background. Respondents were asked about the longest-held occupation, not employment status in this study. They were grouped into the following four categories: salaried workers; self-employed workers; agriculture and forestry; and no occupation. Educational background was coded into dichotomous categories: compulsory education, high school, vocational school or special school, versus junior college and college or higher.

Social network items encompassed seven factors in the present study, that is, marital status (Marriage), household size (Household), number of meeting close relatives (Relatives), having reliable friends (Friends), participation in activities (Participation), going to any religious services (Religion), and enjoying good fellowship with neighbors (Neighborhood). Respondents were asked the following questions to assess each social network item: 1) “What is your current marital status: married, single, divorced, or widowed?”; 2) “How many people do you live with?”; 3) “How often do you meet close relatives not living with you: almost daily, more than ten times a month, 5 to 9 times a month, one to 4 times a month, a few times a year or rarely or never?”; 4) “Do you have any close friends in need: yes or no?”; 5) “How often do you take part in hobbies, club activities or community groups: very often, often, sometimes, or never?”; 6) “Do you go to any religious services: yes or no?”; 7) “Do you enjoy good fellowship with your neighbors: yes or no?” These seven items were coded into dichotomous categories, except for household size (Household). Marital status (Marriage) was categorized into married and others: single, divorced, or widowed. Number of meeting close relatives (Relatives) combined these responses: almost daily, more than ten times a month, 5 to 9 times a month and one to 4 times a month into one category. Participation in activities (Participation) combined these responses: very often, often, and sometimes in one category.

Lifestyle and health status items included smoking habit, alcohol consumption, physical activity, BMI (body mass index: weight (kg)/height (m)2), chronic diseases, and the depression scale of the Todai Health Index (THI). Smoking habit was coded as never smoked, former smokers, and current smokers. Alcohol consumption was assessed by asking, “Do you drink a lot of alcoholic beverages: yes, drink a little, or never?” Responses were coded into dichotomous categories: yes versus drink a little and never. Physical activity was assessed by asking “Do you exercise regularly: often, sometimes, or never?” Responses were coded into dichotomous categories: often and sometimes, versus never. BMI, calculated from self-reported data, was coded into three categories: less than 18.5, 18.5 to less than 25.0, and 25.0 or more. Respondents were also asked whether they had any chronic diseases. The THI consists of 130 questions about subjective physical and psychological symptoms19). Each question has three possible responses (affirmative, negative, neutral). The THI has 12-scaled scores and three discriminant functions that are calculated on the basis of the scaled score. The subjective physical symptoms and psychological complaints (130 items) addressed by the questionnaire are classified into those 12 scales, derived by means of factor analysis. A given scaled score is correlated with the frequency of physical symptoms and psychological complaints. The depression scale of the THI was established for use in epidemiologic surveys and monitoring the severity of the depressive state, by testing its content and cross validity in relation to the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale and the Zung Self-rating Depression Scale20,21).

Previous investigations have found these sociodemographic factors, lifestyle factors, health status and depression, to be related to social networks and mortality22). Thus, these are included in the analysis primarily as a means of controlling for potential confounders.

Follow up procedures

Information on deaths and migrations in the cohort study was obtained from the municipal resident registration file, Jumin Kihon Daicho, from 1993 to 2000 in each area. The cause of death for deceased cases was obtained from death certificates, Shibo-Kohyo, in the local public health center with permission from the Management and Coordination Agency, the Government of Japan. All-cause mortality was used as the main outcome variable, though underlying causes of death obtained from death certificates were categorized according to the International Statistical Classification of Diseases, 10th Revision (ICD-10). We added a mail inquiry as a means of reaching subjects who had migrated out of the study area. Subjects who did not respond to the mail inquiry or who had not been reached were regarded as censored cases17). The observation period for one subject in each area had not been confirmed in spite of careful follow-up survey (n=2,294, the rural group, and n=3,333, the urban group).

Statistical methods

Prior health status was thought to have the largest effect during the early follow-up period, as those already ill at the time of the baseline study may be more isolated and have a higher probability of death. Thus, in order to reduce the possible effect on mortality due to unreported or unrecognized health conditions at baseline, cases dying within the first year were excluded from this analysis.

The Cox proportional hazards model was used to estimate relative risks (RRs) with 95% confidence intervals (CIs) for assessing the associations of sociodemographic, social network, and lifestyle factors with all-cause mortality. Age-adjusted relative risks were computed for each area, and multivariate relative risks adjusted for age, occupation, educational background, marital status (Marriage), household size (Household), number of meeting close relatives (Relatives), having reliable friends (Friends), participation in activities (Participation), going to any religious services (Religion), and enjoying good fellowship with neighbors (Neighborhood), smoking habit, alcohol consumption, physical activity, BMI, chronic diseases, and depressive status were calculated for each area.

Analyses were conducted using SPSS version 10.0J for Windows.

RESULTS

Table 1 shows the baseline data from the rural and urban groups which were already excluded the first year deaths. The mean ages of the rural and urban subjects were 52.6 and 54.8 years, respectively. The proportion of younger subjects was greater in the rural group and approximately 20% worked in agriculture and forestry. On the other hand, the age distributions were nearly identical and about 40% were self-employed in the urban group. In addition, a large proportion of urban subjects was high educated. The two groups were quite similar in marital status (Marriage) and participation in activities (Participation). Household size (Household) tended to be smaller in the urban than in the rural group. The proportion of subjects who often or sometimes met close relatives (Relatives) was 68.6% in the rural and 71.7% in the urban group. The proportion of subjects with reliable friends (Friends) was 63.5% in the rural and 56.2% in the urban group. The proportion of subjects going to any religious services (Religion) was only 3.7% in the rural and 6.1% in the urban group. The proportion of subjects enjoying good fellowship with neighbors (Neighborhood) was 39.5% in the rural and 32.9% in the urban group. The proportions of current and former smokers were 59.4% and 15.8%, respectively, in the rural group, and 56.2% and 17.5%, respectively, in the urban group. The proportion of heavy drinkers was 27.6% in the rural and 25.9% in the urban group. The proportion of subjects who reported that they often or sometimes exercised was 40.0% in the rural and 49.6% in the urban group. The proportions of rural and urban subjects in the BMI category of 18.5 to less than 25 were 75.1% and 73.1%, and the proportions with a BMI of less than 18.5 were 3.1% and 4.8%, respectively.

Table 1. Baseline sociodemographic, social network, and lifestyle factors, and health status in Komochi Village (rural) and the downtown district of Isesaki City (urban).

Variable or characteristics Rural Urban


No. of
subjects
% No. of
deaths
% No. of
subjects
% No. of
deaths
%
Age
 40-49 years 968 42.4 18 13.7 1018 30.7 21 10.1
 50-59 years 700 30.7 35 26.7 1124 33.9 57 27.5
 60-69 years 614 26.9 78 59.5 1173 35.4 129 62.3
Occupation
 Salaried worker 1129 52.1 44 35.8 1827 56.5 115 58.1
 Self-employed 522 24.1 37 30.1 1232 38.1 65 32.8
 Agriculture and forestry 467 21.5 36 29.3 25 0.8 2 1.0
 No occupation 50 2.3 6 4.9 147 4.5 16 8.1
Education
 Junior college and college or higher 261 11.9 3 2.5 795 25.1 36 18.3
 Except above 1929 88.1 119 97.5 2377 74.9 161 81.7
Marriage
 Married 1819 87.4 94 82.5 2757 86.2 170 84.2
 Except above 263 12.6 20 17.5 441 13.8 32 15.8
Household
 Alone 48 2.2 3 2.4 158 4.9 13 6.4
 2 363 16.3 38 29.9 831 25.8 78 38.4
 3 459 20.6 22 17.3 781 24.3 56 27.6
 4 481 21.6 21 16.5 676 21.0 32 15.8
 5 357 16.1 11 8.7 404 12.6 10 4.9
 6 300 13.5 17 13.4 260 8.1 8 3.9
 More than 7 216 9.7 15 11.8 109 3.4 6 3.0
Relatives
 Often, sometimes 1099 68.6 78 72.9 1770 71.7 127 74.3
 A few, never 502 31.4 29 27.1 698 28.3 44 25.7
Friends
 Yes 1393 63.5 77 64.2 1797 56.2 111 56.1
 No 800 36.5 43 35.8 1398 43.8 87 43.9
Participation
 Yes 1582 72.0 73 61.9 2338 72.9 124 62.9
 No 616 28.0 45 38.1 869 27.1 73 37.1
Religion
 Yes 82 3.7 7 5.6 199 6.1 14 6.9
 No 2112 96.3 117 94.4 3067 93.9 189 93.1
Neighborhood
 Yes 870 39.5 69 55.6 1059 32.9 70 35.5
 No 1334 60.5 55 44.4 2164 67.1 127 64.5
Smoking habit
 Never 549 24.8 19 15.1 851 26.3 47 23.6
 Former 349 15.8 34 27.0 567 17.5 26 13.1
 Current 1315 59.4 73 57.9 1820 56.2 126 63.3
Alcohol consumption
 Never, light 1602 72.4 85 70.2 2387 74.1 148 76.3
 Heavy 610 27.6 36 29.8 836 25.9 46 23.7
Physical activity
 Often, sometimes 875 40.0 39 32.0 1608 49.6 88 44.2
 Never 1312 60.0 83 68.0 1632 50.4 111 55.8
BMI, Body mass index
 18.5≦BMI<25.0 1685 75.1 90 70.3 2386 73.1 144 70.9
 BMI<18.5 70 3.1 11 8.6 158 4.8 25 12.3
 25.0≦BMI 490 21.8 27 21.1 718 22.0 34 16.7
Chronic diseases
 No 1568 71.4 55 44.7 1975 61.0 85 42.7
 Yes 628 28.6 68 55.3 1264 39.0 114 57.3

Table 2 shows survival status for this cohort. After excluding the first year deaths, there were 131 deaths (5.7%) among the 2,282 rural subjects and 207 deaths (6.2%) among the 3,315 urban subjects during the 1993 to 2000 follow-up period. There were 42 (1.8%) and 102 (3.1%) censored cases in the rural and urban groups, respectively (Table. 2). These small numbers were considered to have negligible effects on the results of this study17).

Table 2. Number of subjects, censored (%) and deaths (%) in the two cohorts: rural and urban.

Rural Urban


Total Without early
one-year-death cases
Total Without early
one-year-death cases
No. of subjects 2294 2282 3333 3315
No. of censored (%) 42(1.8) 42(1.8) 102(3.1) 102(3.1)
No. of deaths (%) 143(6.2) 131(5.7) 225(6.8) 207(6.2)
Person-year of follow-up 17157.94 17152.22 22059.95 22050.39

Table 3 presents age-adjusted relative risks for mortality in the rural and urban groups. With respect to sociodemogaphic variables, self-employed and low educated subjects had a significantly increased risk of mortality in the rural group. Educational background was not significantly associated with mortality in the urban group, though self-employed subjects had a significantly decreased mortality risk. In terms of social network variables, marital status (Marriage), participation in activities (Participation) and enjoying good fellowship with neighbors (Neighborhood) were significantly associated with mortality in the rural groups. Marital status (Marriage), household size (Household) and participation in activities (Participation) were significantly associated with mortality in the urban groups. Number of meeting close relatives (Relatives), having reliable friends (Friends) and going to any religious services (Religion) were not significantly associated with age-adjusted relative risks. Subjects without a spouse (Marriage) and those who did not participate in activities (Participation) had a significantly increased risk of mortality for both groups as compared with married subjects and those who participated in activities. Subjects who did not enjoy good fellowship with their neighbors (Neighborhood) had a significantly lower relative risk than those who did in the rural group, though there were no significant associations in the urban group. Subjects with a larger household size (Household) had a significantly lower relative risk than those with a smaller household size in the urban group, despite the lack of any significant associations in the rural group. In regard to lifestyle variables, smoking habit and BMI were significantly associated with mortality in the rural group. On the other hand, physical activity and BMI were significantly associated with mortality in the urban groups. Alcohol consumption showed no significant association with age-adjusted relative risks. Low BMI (less than 18.5) was associated with a significantly increased risk of mortality for both groups as compared to medium BMI (18.5 to less than 25.0). Current and former smokers had significantly higher relative risks of mortality than non-smokers in the rural group but there were no significant associations in the urban group. Subjects who did not exercise had a significantly higher relative risk than those who often or sometimes exercised in the urban group, despite there being no significant associations in the rural group. Chronic diseases and depressive status were both associated with significantly increased risks of mortality for both groups.

Table 3. Age-adjusted relative risk (RR) with 95% confidence interval (CI) for deaths from all causes by Cox proportional hazards model in the two cohorts: rural and urban.

Variable Rural Urban


RR (95% CI) RR (95% CI)
Age
1.11 (1.08, 1.13)* 1.09 (1.07, 1.11)*
Occupation
 Salaried worker 1.00 1.00
 Self-employed 1.93 (1.24, 2.98)* 0.72 (0.53, 0.98)*
 Agriculture and forestry 1.07 (0.68, 1.68) 0.83 (0.20, 3.34)
 No occupation 1.47 (0.62, 3.48) 0.98 (0.58, 1.67)
Education
 Junior college and college or higher 1.00 1.00
 Except above 4.08 (1.30, 12.85)* 1.28 (0.89, 1.84)
Marriage
 Married 1.00 1.00
 Except above 1.73 (1.06, 2.80)* 1.65 (1.13, 2.42)*
Household
 Per each additional person 0.96 (0.87, 1.05) 0.86 (0.77, 0.95)*
Relatives
 Often, sometimes 1.00 1.00
 A few, never 0.88 (0.57, 1.35) 1.06 (0.75, 1.50)
Friends
 Yes 1.00 1.00
 No 0.95 (0.66, 1.39) 0.93 (0.70, 1.23)
Participation
 Yes 1.00 1.00
 No 1.52 (1.05, 2.21)* 1.79 (1.34, 2.40)*
Religion
 Yes 1.00 1.00
 No 0.80 (0.37, 1.72) 0.90 (0.52, 1.55)
Neighborhood
 Yes 1.00 1.00
 No 0.66 (0.46, 0.94)* 1.13 (0.84, 1.52)
Smoking habit
 Never 1.00 1.00
 Former 2.46 (1.40, 4.32)* 0.72 (0.45, 1.17)
 Current 1.80 (1.08, 2.98)* 1.35 (0.97, 1.89)
Alcohol consumption
 Never, light 1.00 1.00
 Heavy 1.42 (0.95, 2.10) 1.02 (0.73, 1.43)
Physical activity
 Often, sometimes 1.00 1.00
 Never 1.26 (0.86, 1.85) 1.39 (1.05, 1.84)*
BMI, Body mass index
 18.5≦BMI<25.0 1.00 1.00
 BMI<18.5 2.68 (1.43, 5.02)* 2.06 (1.34, 3.16)*
 25.0≦BMI 1.21 (0.79, 1.87) 0.84 (0.58, 1.22)
Chronic diseases
 No 1.00 1.00
 Yes 2.36 (1.64, 3.38)* 1.61 (1.21, 2.14)*
Depression
 Per each additional scaled score 1.08 (1.04, 1.13)* 1.05 (1.02, 1.09)*

* p<0.05

Table 4 presents multivariate relative risks identified using the Cox proportional hazards model adjusted simultaneously for all variables; age, occupation, educational background, marital status (Marriage), household size (Household), number of meeting close relatives (Relatives), having reliable friends (Friends), participation in activities (Participation), going to any religious services (Religion), enjoying good fellowship with neighbors (Neighborhood), smoking habit, alcohol consumption, physical activity, BMI, chronic diseases, and depressive status. Tendencies were similar to age-adjusted relative risks. Educational background, marital status (Marriage), enjoying good fellowship with neighbors (Neighborhood), and chronic diseases remained as significant as the associations in the rural group. Participation in activities (Participation), BMI, and chronic diseases also remained as significant as the associations in the urban group. The multivariate relative risk for low educated subjects was 4.4 (95%CI: 1.1-18.2) as compared to high educated subjects in the rural group. Subjects without a spouse (Marriage) had an increased risk of mortality (RR=2.4; 95%CI: 1.2-4.5) in the rural group. Subjects not enjoying good fellowship with their neighbors (Neighborhood) had a decreased risk of mortality (RR=0.58; 95%CI: 0.35-0.97) in the rural group. It was surprising that enjoying good fellowship with neighbors (Neighborhood) was adversely associated with mortality. The multivariate relative risk of subjects who did not participate in activities (Participation) was 1.6 (95%CI: 1.1-2.4) as compared to subjects who participated in activities in the urban group. Four other social network variables: household size (Household), number of meeting close relatives (Relatives), having reliable friends (Friends), and going to any religious services (Religion), were not associated with mortality. These results provided estimates of the independent associations of marital status (Marriage), enjoying good fellowship with neighbors (Neighborhood) and participation in activities (Participation), as social network factors, with mortality risk. With respect to lifestyle variables, only BMI (less than 18.5) was associated with a significantly increased risk of mortality (RR=2.1; 95%CI: 1.2-3.5) as compared to medium BMI (18.5 to less than 25.0) in the urban group but there were no significant associations in the rural group. Other lifestyle variables, such as smoking habit, alcohol consumption and physical activity, were not significantly associated with mortality for both groups.

Table 4. Multivariate relative risk (RR) with 95% confidence interval (CI) for deaths from all causes by Cox proportional hazards model in the two cohorts: rural and urban.

Variable Rural Urban


RR (95% CI) RR (95% CI)
Age
1.09 (1.06, 1.13)* 1.09 (1.06, 1.12)*
Occupation
 Salaried worker 1.00 1.00
 Self-employed 1.28 (0.72, 2.27) 0.80 (0.55, 1.15)
 Agriculture and forestry 0.72 (0.37, 1.39) 1.93 (0.46, 8.05)
 No occupation 1.47 (0.54, 3.98) 0.94 (0.49, 1.80)
Education
 Junior college and college or higher 1.00 1.00
 Except above 4.39 (1.06, 18.21)* 1.07 (0.69, 1.64)
Marriage
 Married 1.00 1.00
 Except above 2.35 (1.22, 4.53)* 1.12 (0.64, 1.99)
Household
 Per each additional person 0.95 (0.82, 1.09) 0.87 (0.76, 1.00)
Relatives
 Often, sometimes 1.00 1.00
 A few, never 0.62 (0.35, 1.11) 0.97 (0.64, 1.46)
Friends
 Yes 1.00 1.00
 No 0.80 (0.47, 1.35) 0.73 (0.50, 1.05)
Participation
 Yes 1.00 1.00
 No 1.60 (0.95, 2.71) 1.61 (1.10, 2.37)*
Religion
 Yes 1.00 1.00
 No 1.10 (0.34, 3.60) 0.84 (0.44, 1.60)
Neighborhood
 Yes 1.00 1.00
 No 0.58 (0.35, 0.97)* 1.00 (0.69, 1.45)
Smoking habit
 Never 1.00 1.00
 Former 2.02 (0.97, 4.23) 0.77 (0.45, 1.32)
 Current 1.23 (0.63, 2.40) 1.15 (0.77, 1.72)
Alcohol consumption
 Never, light 1.00 1.00
 Heavy 1.51 (0.88, 2.60) 1.22 (0.82, 1.82)
Physical activity
 Often, sometimes 1.00 1.00
 Never 1.16 (0.67, 1.98) 1.14 (0.80, 1.64)
BMI, Body mass index
 18.5≦BMI<25.0 1.00 1.00
 BMI<18.5 1.87 (0.78, 4.48) 2.05 (1.21, 3.47)*
 25.0≦BMI 0.86 (0.45, 1.63) 0.76 (0.47, 1.23)
Chronic diseases
 No 1.00 1.00
 Yes 1.71 (1.05, 2.80)* 1.47 (1.03, 2.10)*
Depression
 Per each additional scaled score 1.07 (1.00, 1.15) 1.01 (0.96, 1.06)

* p<0.05

DISCUSSION

Effects of sociodemographic factors on mortality

Occupation

Ohta et al.15) reported that the self-employed men in a rural area felt that their job was hard, had higher job satisfaction, and had slightly more subjective physical and mental complaints, while blue-collar workers had the poorest perceived health with many complaints in the rural area. In contrast, Cavelaars et al.23) showed self-employed men to generally be healthier than the average population with respect to morbidity indicators including perceived health, long-term disabilities, chronic conditions and any long-standing health problem. The present findings indicate self-employed rural men to have a significantly increased age-adjusted mortality risk as compared to salaried workers, while the other two occupations did not have significantly higher risk. On the other hand, self-employed men in the urban group had a significantly decreased age-adjusted mortality risk as compared to salaried workers, while other occupations showed no significant associations. The multivariate model showed occupation to not be significantly associated with mortality in either rural or urban groups. Although these findings might indicate that the impact on mortality in self-employed men differs between rural and urban groups, the reason remains unclear. However, Hart24) reported that the major explanation for occupational mortality differences should be sought in socioeconomic factors such as wealth and income, housing, educational and employment opportunities, and behavioral characteristics. Judging from the lack of an association with multivariate mortality risk in terms of occupation, potential confounding factors in the rural group may differ from those in the urban group. In regard to this point, explanations for these rural-urban differences merit further examination.

Education

Those with low education in the rural group had a significantly increased age-adjusted and multivariate mortality risk. Educational levels were not significantly related to mortality risk in the urban group. Smith et al.25) reported that rural men were exposed to greater risk in association with lower educational levels, low income, and loss of a spouse, especially through divorce or separation. Some studies also showed low education to be associated with high mortality26-28), though regional differences were not taken into account. Schrijvers et al.28) concluded that the association between educational level and all-cause mortality was largely explained by a differential distribution of behavioral (alcohol, smoking, BMI, physical activity) and material (financial problems, employment status, income proxy) factors across educational groups. In this study, the relative risk for low educated subjects in the rural group was obviously higher than that for urban subjects. One possible interpretation is that differences between rural and urban may reflect inequality of income distribution which is depended on educational background. According to second wave survey in Komo-Ise 2000, average household income by area groups and educational levels showed that high educated men in the rural group tended to have higher average household income than those in the urban group. In contrast, low educated men in the rural group tended to have lower average household income than those in the urban group. Bigger income difference among rural men may account for the difference in mortality risk between rural and urban29). However, no adjustment was made for economic status in the multivariate model. Education was considered to be one of the most important factors explaining rural-urban differences.

Effects of social network factors on mortality

Marriage

In this study, though subjects without a spouse (Marriage) had a significantly increased age-adjusted mortality risk in both groups, this relationship remained in the rural group even after adjusting for potential confounding factors. This suggests that marital status is an independent risk factor in rural subjects. A few reports have referred to area differences in terms of marital status and mortality, though a number of studies have repeatedly shown increased mortality in non-married persons, i.e., widows and widowers, divorced, and the never married, as compared with married persons26,30). According to Smith et al.25), the adverse effect of divorce and separation among rural residents was explained as divorce or separation possibly being more psychologically and socially damaging because a rural community regards durable marriages as generally meeting desired standards of social behavior as compared to an urban community where marriage failure is more common. Our findings are partially consistent with theirs, though the reason for the difference has not yet been systematically determined. The impact of marital status to mortality may reflect sociocultural difference between rural and urban. One speculation is that men among the rural group might tend to be more dependent on their spouse than those among the urban group, judging from smaller and denser personal networks among the rural group which consisted of mainly family and kinship31). The death rates of divorced, widowed, and never-married men might differ in our present study, but there were too few deaths to examine them separately according to each marital status category.

Participation

Lack of participation in activities (Participation) was associated with a significantly increased age-adjusted mortality risk in both groups. In a multivariate model, this relationship was preserved only in the urban, not in the rural group. However, participation in activities (Participation) was regarded as an important predictor of mortality in middle-age men irrespective of group, as the relative risks were very similar (1.60 rural vs. 1.61 urban). Welin et al.32) reported high levels of social, home, and outside home activities to protect middle-age men from premature deaths. Social activities had a particularly strong association with mortality. Bygren et al.33) also pointed out the importance of attending cultural events in terms of protecting against premature deaths, though there were areas of overlap among several variables. Sugisawa et al.13) revealed social participation of Japanese people who were elderly rather than middle-aged to produce direct and indirect effects on morality linked with functional status and self-rated health, when physical health and health behavior were hypothesized to mediate the effects of social networks and social support on mortality. According to Sasazawa et al.16), physical activity was associated with better perceived health and social network in a cross-sectional survey. These reports indicated that not only the direct effect but also the indirect effect on mortality should be clarified to determine the impact of social network on mortality in middle-aged Japanese men.

Friends, Relatives and Religion

Number of meeting close relatives (Relatives), having reliable friends (Friends), and going to any religious services (Religion) were not found to have statistically significant effects on the risk of death in either group. These findings are considered to reflect a lack of rural-urban differences. Previous studies indicated inadequate social relations as indicated by relatively few social ties and a low level of social support to be associated with an excess mortality risk in Western developed countries5,34,35). Berkman et al.5) reported the more intimate ties of marriage and contact with friends and relatives to be stronger predictors than the ties of church and group membership. As these findings might reflect the Western sociocultural context, there is no guarantee that the same results would be obtained in a Japanese population. Indeed, findings from previous studies of elderly Japanese were much less consistent with findings in Western countries. Fujita et al.36) reported that there was no statistically significant association between social network and mortality and no differences among three socioeconomically diverse communities. According to Sugisawa et al.13) social participation had a significant effect on the risk of death in elderly Japanese while marital status and social contacts were not statistically associated with mortality. The impact of marital status on mortality was greatest in the younger age group, diminishing in the older groups27). As to this point, these findings were considered to be similar to our present results except for enjoying good fellowship with neighbors (Neighborhood) among rural men.

Household

Household size (Household) was not associated with statistically significant effects on mortality in age-adjusted and multivariate models in the rural group. On the other hand, larger household size (Household) related significantly to lower age-adjusted mortality risk in the urban group, though this relationship was come near to preserving after adjusting for potential confounding factors (the observed significance level of the test was 0.0501). Sorlie et al.26) reported quite similar findings, i.e., household size was not significantly associated with multivariate adjusted mortality risk for middle-age men, while large household size decreased age-adjusted mortality risks. Judging from the multivariate model, our findings suggest the difficulty to conclude rural-urban differences with respect to household size (Household).

Neighborhood

One of our notable findings is the adverse association between enjoying good fellowship with neighbors (Neighborhood) and mortality, though the interpretation and explanation of this association is unclear. Beggs et al.31) reported the characteristics of personal networks for rural residents, that is, ties were of greater intensity, were based more on kinship and neighborhood and were smaller and denser than personal networks for urban residents. These findings may indicate that rural residents are rooted and have relations of longer duration to community, family, and neighborhood. Shimada et al.37) pointed out the characteristics of rural societies in Japan that had formal neighborhood relationships. In a non-urbanized rural society, well-organized small groups, which are called Kumi or Han, contribute to provide support for each group member in daily life, for example taking turns sweeping, and cooperating and participating in marriage, funeral and ancestral ceremonies, and so on. Each member was required to help others, to take part in their duties, and to play their roles regardless of their personal desires. The formal neighborhood relationships did not always involve absolute intimacy, though rural people tended to have stronger neighborhood relationships than those of relatives. In other words, this neighborhood relationship in the rural society sometimes may force residents to be under high demands and low control that produce stress to them. As a result, they who are involved in this situation may be more likely to have health problems. Although this hypothesis has been confirmed that the situation under high demands and low control related to incidence and mortality of chronic disease among office workers38), we have no evidence among residents as far as we know. According to Katsura et al.39), Japanese middle-aged rural residents recognized relatively trivial affairs as stressful in routine daily human relationships, such as the relation to one’s neighborhood, as well as bereavement of their spouse or a close family member. Miller et al.40) showed an interesting relationship between the number of acquaintances and a number of psychological and physical symptom scores. Persons with a lot of acquaintances did not exactly have the lower the scores, i.e., persons with “some acquaintances” had the lowest scores. These reports indicated two important limitations to our study, though we suspect that enjoying good fellowship with neighbors (Neighborhood) increased the risk of mortality among rural residents. One possibility is investigating not only the quantity but also the quality of social relationships and the other is estimating the effect of stress quantitatively to adjust for confounding factors in multivariate models. Social relationships are considered to consist of both social networks and social support. Social networks are often defined as the quantity or structural aspects of relationships. On the other hand, social support is commonly explained as the quality or functional aspects of relationships. Cohen et al.41) showed social support to affect the physical and mental health of the individual via two main pathways. One is a direct positive effect and the other is a buffer or modifier of the effects of psychosocial and physical stress. The stress-buffering model proposes that there is an interaction of stress and social relations on health. Stress has an adverse effect on health and wellbeing only for those with inadequate social relations. With respect to relations with neighbors (Neighborhood) among rural men, we must pay attention not only to its interpretation but also give greater consideration to this issue.

Effects of lifestyle factors on mortality

BMI

The relation between body weight and mortality remains controversial. Some prospective studies have shown the relationship between BMI and mortality to be represented by a U-shaped or L-shaped curve in which the risk was increased among the very heavy and the very lean or among only the very lean2,17,42). On the other hand, Lee et al.43) reported a J-shaped relation between BMI and mortality and concluded that there was no evidence of excess risk among lean men. In this study, the lean group (BMI<18.5) had a significantly increased age-adjusted mortality risk in both groups. In the urban group especially, this relationship remained after adjusting for potential confounding factors. In the rural group, the excess risk (RR=1.87) did not reach statistical significance. With respect to BMI, our findings were considered to reflect the lack of a difference between rural and urban groups. Manson et al.44) emphasized the importance of assessing subclincal conditions related to weight loss at baseline. Although the subjects were asked about their health conditions including chronic diseases at baseline and we excluded deaths within the first year from this analysis, these steps might not have been enough to eliminate the influence of subclincal conditions.

Smoking and Physical activities

Though current and former smokers in the rural group had a significantly increased age-adjusted mortality risk, this relationship did not remain after adjusting for sociodemographic, social network, and other lifestyle factors. On the other hand, current and former smokers in the urban group did not show significant excess risk in either model. In contrast to smoking habit, urban subjects who did not exercise had a significantly increased age-adjusted mortality risk, though this was not demonstrated by the multivariate model. Rural subjects who did not exercise did not show a significant association with age-adjusted and multivariate mortality risks. Although studies have repeatedly shown smoking habit and physical activity to be independent risk factors for mortality1,4), our findings were not consistent with those of previous studies. However, several studies have reported smoking habit and physical activity to be associated with educational level16,45,46). This might suggest that regional differences to be explained by educational level rather than lifestyle factors, such as smoking habit and physical activity. Low educated people with adverse lifestyle factors should be targeted for prevention of premature deaths by means of public health measures.

Limitations of this study

Some limitations of this study should be taken into account. First, this study focused only on men because data from the Tecumseh Community Health Study indicated that social networks might be less important for women than for men9). Furthermore, fewer women than men died, such that there were too few to conduct multivariate analyses. Given these two issues, we did not analyze the data for women in this study. Second, our baseline data did not include objective data such as physical and physiological examination results, and laboratory data, as our baseline survey was conducted using a self-administered questionnaire. Third, it must be kept in mind that other confounding factors may remain, though we took several potential confounding factors into consideration. As mentioned earlier, it must be emphasized again that the effects of stress are not included in this study, despite the importance of social relationships as well as mortality14,41). Fourth, this study examined all explanatory and control variables obtained at the time the baseline data were obtained. During the approximately seven year follow-up periods, these variables may have changed with the passage of time. The impacts of such changes on the risk of mortality should be taken into account47). Fifth, we cannot completely rule out the possibility of the indirect effects of social networks on mortality, as we focused on direct effects in this study22). Therefore, the possibility exists that men without a spouse (Marriage) and/or participation in activities (Participation) develop a less healthy lifestyle than others.

Despite these limitations, this research has provided significant new knowledge for Japanese middle-aged men, regarding rural-urban differences in associations of sociodemographic, social network, and lifestyle factors to mortality. Participation in activities (Participation) could be important for preventing premature death in middle-age Japanese men. Rural-urban differences were identified for education, martial status (Marriage) and enjoying good fellowship with neighbors (Neighborhood) which had strong impacts on mortality in the rural group, though differences between the rural and urban groups in lifestyle factors and social network factors including household size (Household), number of meeting close relatives (Relatives), having reliable friends (Friends), and going to any religious services (Religion) were not significant.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

This research was supported by Grant-in-Aid (11694243) for Scientific Research from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology, Japan and Gerontology and Health Grant from Gunma Prefecture. The authors wish to express our gratitude to the mayor and the staff of Komochi Village and Isesaki City for their support, and Dr. Tomoyuki Kawada for his generous support and advice.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Berkman LF, Breslow L: Health and Ways of Living: The Alameda County Study. Oxford University Press, New York, 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Calle EE, Thun MJ, Petrelli JM, Rodriguez C, Heath CW Jr. Body-mass index and mortality in a prospective cohort of U.S. adults. N Engl J Med, 1999; 341: 1097-1105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Tsugane S, Fahey MT, Sasaki S, Baba S. Alcohol consumption and all-cause and cancer mortality among middle-aged Japanese men: seven-year follow-up of the JPHC study Cohort I. Japan Public Health Center. Am J Epidemiol, 1999; 150: 1201-1207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Kaplan GA, Strawbridge WJ, Cohen RD, Hungerford LR. Natural history of leisure-time physical activity and its correlates: associations with mortality from all causes and cardiovascular disease over 28 years. Am J Epidemiol, 1996; 144: 793-797. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Berkman LF, Syme SL. Social networks, host resistance, and mortality: A nine-year follow-up study of Alameda County residents. Am J Epidemiol, 1979; 109: 186-204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Broadhead WE, Kaplan BH, James SA, et al. The epidemiologic evidence for a relationship between social support and health. Am J Epidemiol, 1983; 117: 521-537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.House JS, Landis KR, Umberson D. Social relationships and health. Science, 1988; 241: 540-545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Cassel J. The contribution of the social environment to host resistance: the Fourth Wade Hampton Frost Lecture. Am J Epidemiol, 1976; 104: 107-123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.House JS, Robbins C, Metzner HL. The association of social relationships and activities with mortality: prospective evidence from the Tecumseh Community Health Study. Am J Epidemiol, 1982; 116: 123-140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Schoenbach VJ, Kaplan BH, Fredman L, Kleinbaum DG. Social ties and mortality in Evans County, Georgia. Am J Epidemiol, 1986; 123: 577-591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Seeman TE, Berkman LF, Kohout F, et al. Intercommunity variations in the association between social ties and mortality in the elderly. A comparative analysis of three communities. Ann Epidemiol, 1993; 3: 325-335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Hanson BS, Isacsson SO, Janzon L, Lindell SE. Social network and social support influence mortality in elderly men. The prospective population study of “Men born in 1914,” Malmo, Sweden. Am J Epidemiol, 1989; 130: 100-111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Sugisawa H, Liang J, Liu X. Social networks, social support, and mortality among older people in Japan. J Gerontol, 1994; 49: S3-13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Rosengren A, Orth-Gomer K, Wedel H, Wilhelmsen L. Stressful life events, social support, and mortality in men born in 1933. BMJ, 1993; 307: 1102-1105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Ohta A, Takeuchi K, Sasazawa Y, Suzuki S. Differences in lifestyle and perceived health in different occupations in a community. J Occup Health, 1998; 40: 325-333. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Sasazawa Y, Takeuchi K, Ohta A, Tajima K, Suzuki S. Relationship between regular exercise and life style, social network, education and subjective symptoms in Japanese middle aged and elderly residents. Nippon Koshu Eisei Zasshi, 1999; 46: 624-637 (in Japanese). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Ohta A, Aoki S, Takeuchi K, Sasazawa Y, Suzuki S. Lifestyle and sociodemographic risk factors for death among middle-aged and elderly residents in Japan from a five-year follow-up cohort study. J Epidemiol, 2001; 11: 51-60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Statistics Bureau Management and Coordination Agency Government of Japan. 1995 Population census of Japan vol. 2-2-10 prefecture and municipalities Gunma-Ken. Statistics Bureau Management and Coordination Agency Government of Japan, Tokyo, 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Suzuki S, Aoki S, Yanai H. The THI handbook/methods and applications of the Todai Health Index. Shinohara Press, Tokyo, 1989. (in Japanese) [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Suzuki S, Roberts RE, eds. Methods and applications in mental health surveys: the Todai Health Index. University of Tokyo Press, Tokyo, 1991. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Kawada T, Suzuki S, Kubota F, Ohnishi N, Satoh K. Content and cross validity of the Todai Health Index depression scale in relation to the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale and the Zung Self-rating Depression Scale. J Occup Health, 1999; 41: 154-159. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Liang J, Bennett JM, Krause NM, et al. Stress, social relations, and old age mortality in Taiwan. J Clin Epidemiol, 1999; 52: 983-995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Cavelaars AE, Kunst AE, Geurts JJ, et al. Morbidity differences by occupational class among men in seven European countries: an application of the Erikson-Goldthorpe social class scheme. Int J Epidemiol, 1998; 27: 222-230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Hart N. Inequalities in heath: The individual versus the environment. J R Stat Soc Ser A, 1986; 149: 228-235. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Smith MH, Anderson RT, Bradham DD, Longino CF Jr. Rural and urban differences in mortality among Americans 55 years and older: analysis of the National Longitudinal Mortality Study. J Rural Health, 1995; 11: 274-285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Sorlie PD, Backlund E, Keller JB. US mortality by economic, demographic, and social characteristics: the National Longitudinal Mortality Study. Am J Public Health, 1995; 85: 949-956. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Lantz PM, House JS, Lepkowski JM, et al. Socioeconomic factors, health behaviors, and mortality: results from a nationally representative prospective study of US adults. JAMA, 1998; 279: 1703-1708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Schrijvers CT, Stronks K, van de Mheen HD, Mackenbach JP. Explaining educational differences in mortality: the role of behavioral and material factors. Am J Public Health, 1999; 89: 535-540. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Lynch JW, Kaplan GA, Shema SJ. Cumulative impact of sustained economic hardship on physical, cognitive, psychological, and social functioning. N Engl J Med, 1997; 337: 1889-1895. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Ebrahim S, Wannamethee G, McCallum A, Walker M, Shaper AG. Marital status, change in marital status, and mortality in middle-aged British men. Am J Epidemiol, 1995; 142: 834-842. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Beggs JJ, Haines VA, Hurlbert JS. Revisiting the rural-urban contrast: personal networks in nonmetropolitan and metropolitan settings. Rural Sociol, 1996; 61: 306-325. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Welin L, Tibblin G, Svardsudd K, et al. Prospective study of social influences on mortality. The study of men born in 1913 and 1923. Lancet, 1985; 1(8434): 915-918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Bygren LO, Konlaan BB, Johansson SE. Attendance at cultural events, reading books or periodicals, and making music or singing in a choir as determinants for survival; Swedish interview survey of living conditions. BMJ, 1996; 313: 1577-1580. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Orth-Gomer K, Unden AL. The measurement of social support in population surveys. Soc Sci Med, 1987; 24: 83-94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Grand A, Grosclaude P, Bocquet H, Pous J, Albarede JL. Disability, psychosocial factors and mortality among the elderly in a rural French population. J Clin Epidemiol, 1990; 43: 773-782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Fujita T, Hatano S. Factors predicting mortality in two years among the elderly at home in three different socioeconomic areas. Nippon Koshu Eisei Zasshi, 1990; 37: 1-8. (in Japanese) [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Shimada K, Takimoto T, Suzuki O. Kinrin shakai no ningen kankei (Human relationship in neighborhood). Brain Shuppan, Tokyo, 1988. (in Japanese)
  • 38.Karasek R, Theorell T. Healthy work. Basic Books, New York, 1990.
  • 39.Katsura T, Nojiri M, Nakano M. A basic study on aging and stress: stress appraisal in rural inhabitants of middle and advanced age. Nippon Noson Igakkai Zasshi (Jpn Assoc Rural Med), 1996; 45: 483-492. (in Japanese) [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Miller PM, Ingham JG. Friends, confidants, and symptoms. Soc Psychiatry, 1976; 11: 51-58. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Cohen S, Wills TA. Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychol Bull, 1985; 98: 310-357. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Nakayama K, Koyohara Y, Kato I, et al. Effect of body mass index on morbidity and mortality in a general Japanese population: the Hisayama study. Nippon Ronen Igakkai Zasshi, 1997; 34: 935-941. (in Japanese) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Lee IM, Manson JE, Hennekens CH, Paffenbarger RS Jr. Body weight and mortality. A 27-year follow-up of middle-aged men. JAMA, 1993; 270: 2823-2828. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Manson JE, Stampfer MJ, Hennekens CH, Willett WC. Body weight and longevity. A reassessment. JAMA, 1987; 257: 353-358. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Wagenknecht LE, Perkins LL, Cutter GR, et al. Cigarette smoking behavior is strongly related to educational status: the CARDIA study. Prev Med, 1990; 19: 158-169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Kaplan GA, Lazarus NB, Cohen RD, Leu DJ. Psychosocial factors in the natural history of physical activity. Am J Prev Med, 1991; 7: 12-17. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Cerhan JR, Wallace RB. Change in social ties and subsequent mortality in rural elders. Epidemiology, 1997; 8: 475-481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Epidemiology are provided here courtesy of Japan Epidemiological Association

RESOURCES