Abstract
Cognitive impairment after traumatic brain injury (TBI) is persistent and disabling. Assessing cognitive function in a reliable and valid manner, using measures that are sensitive to the integrity of underlying neural substrates, is crucial in clinical research. The Attention Network Test (ANT) is one such assessment measure that has demonstrated associations with neural regions involved in attention; however, clinical utility of the ANT is limited because its relationship with neuropsychological measures of cognitive function (i.e., its construct validity) has not yet been established in TBI. We evaluated the association between the ANT and 1) a neuropsychological battery assessing executive function and memory and 2) global function assessed by the Glasgow Outcome Scale-Extended (GOSE). Forty-eight adults with complicated mild-severe TBI were evaluated ∼5 months post-injury. Using principal component analysis and multi-variate linear regression adjusted for age, gender, education, and cause of injury, we found that ANT reaction time and executive network scores predicted a principal component assessing processing speed and executive function. Conversely, the ANT did not predict a principal component assessing memory. The ANT was weakly associated with the GOSE. Among persons with TBI during the post-acute phase of recovery, the ANT has good construct validity as evidenced by its associations with neuropsychological measures of processing speed and executive function, but not memory. Given that ANT networks are known to relate to specific neuroanatomical regions, the ANT may be a useful outcome measure for evaluating novel therapeutics targeting attention and executive functions after TBI.
Keywords: attention, brain injury, cognition, executive function, neuropsychology
Introduction
Cognitive dysfunction is a common and disabling sequela after traumatic brain injury (TBI).1,2 Among cognitive domains, impairments are frequently observed in attention, processing speed, executive functions, and memory.3–6 There are currently no disease-modifying therapeutic interventions. Assessing cognitive function in a reliable and valid manner, using tools that are sensitive to underlying brain network changes, is critical to evaluating the efficacy of interventions.
The Attention Network Test (ANT) is a behavioral measure that is potentially well suited to assess attentional impairment after TBI. The ANT was developed based on Posner's seminal model of attention7 to assess three relatively distinct networks of attention8–10: 1) the alerting network, which allows the person to maintain a state of alertness in preparation to respond to a stimulus, and is assessed in the ANT by change in reaction time (RT) when cued to stimulus versus not cued; (2) the orienting network, which enables the person to attend to the spatial location of an upcoming stimulus, and is assessed in the ANT based on change in RT when cued to location; and (3) the executive network, which allows the person to resolve conflict and inhibit distraction, and is assessed in the ANT by change in RT to trials with low and high distractors using the flanker paradigm.11
Converging evidence from structural and functional neuroimaging paradigms suggest that the three attention networks are dissociable. Anatomical imaging has shown cortical thickness in parietal regions to be associated with alerting attention and cortical thickness in the anterior cingulate and lateral pre-frontal cortex to be associated with executive attention.12 Structural connectivity assessed using diffusion magnetic resonance imaging and graph measures of local efficiency correlate differentially with executive attention (lateral pre-frontal cortex), alerting attention (thalamus, inferior parietal), and orienting attention (paracentral lobule, occipital region).13 Studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging are generally consistent, showing that alerting attention is associated with activation in frontoparietal and thalamic regions; orienting attention with activation in the superior parietal lobe, frontal eye fields, and midbrain; and executive attention with activation in the anterior cingulate cortex and lateral pre-frontal cortex.14–16 Graph measures of global and local functional connectivity also differ by attention network.17 Interestingly, there is electrophysiological and behavioral evidence that the executive network may be particularly distinct from the alerting and orienting networks.18–20
That the ANT shows strong associations with underlying neural substrates, and that it can be administered concurrently with neuroimaging paradigms, affords it an advantage in the armamentarium of cognitive tests available to use in clinical research in TBI. However, to be suitable for clinical research studies and potentially as an end-point in clinical trials, the ANT should ideally demonstrate sound psychometric properties. Investigations into the psychometric properties of the ANT have provided initial support for its reliability. This includes evidence for split-half reliability as well as test-retest reliability over intervals of 1 week and 6 months.21,22 In multiple sclerosis, the ANT demonstrates good reliability when administered monthly over 6 months.23
Construct validity, an important psychometric property of an assessment measure, has been demonstrated in healthy young and old adults through associations between the ANT and neuropsychological measures.22,24 Though the ANT has been used in TBI,19,25,26 to our knowledge, the relationship between the ANT and neuropsychological measures in TBI (i.e., its construct validity) has not yet been evaluated; that is, there is a gap in our understanding of whether and how disruption of attentional networks assayed by the ANT is, in fact, related to clinical neuropsychological deficits observed in persons with TBI. Because the construct validity of measures such as the ANT is specific to disease populations, it is important to test whether and how the ANT relates to neuropsychological measures in TBI, which would provide support for the ANT's utility in TBI clinical research.
The primary goal of the present study was to evaluate the relationship between the ANT and neuropsychological measures of cognitive function in persons with TBI with heterogenous pathology across a range of clinical severity. The neuropsychological measures that we included in the study were assessments of processing speed, executive function, and memory. To provide evidence of construct validity, we expected that the ANT would have stronger associations with neuropsychological measures of processing speed and executive functions and relatively weak associations with measures of memory. Among specific networks, we hypothesized that the ANT executive network would show the strongest divergence (i.e., correlation with processing speed/executive function measures and lack of correlation with memory measures) and that this pattern would be less evident in the ANT alerting and orienting networks.
A secondary goal of this study was to determine the association between attention networks assessed by the ANT and global function using the Glasgow Outcome Scale-Extended (GOSE). We conducted this secondary analysis to explore the potential association between the ANT and “real-world” function, to better understand the ecological validity of the ANT. Past research has shown attentional performance on the ANT to correlate with everyday cognitive failures and with driving in healthy adults,27,28 but how the ANT relates to function in TBI remains unknown. We hypothesized that worse performance on the ANT, particularly the ANT executive network, would be associated with worse function as assessed by the GOSE.
Methods
Participants
We recruited 48 community-dwelling adults with TBI who met the following criteria: ≥18 years of age; English speaking; capable of providing informed consent independently or through a proxy/legally authorized representative; not currently taking any psychoactive medication; no history of schizophrenia or substance- or alcohol-use disorder; no history of epilepsy, stroke, dementia, or other neurological illness by self-report; and not pregnant. Participants in the TBI group were required to have sustained a complicated mild (Glasgow Coma Scale [GCS] score of 13–15 with evidence of intracranial lesion as verified on acute neuroimaging) or moderate-severe TBI (GCS score ≤12) within the last 6 months. All participants had previously been treated at large, urban medical centers in the New York City metropolitan area. All study activities took place at Weill Cornell Medicine and were approved by the Weill Cornell Medicine Institutional Review Board. Informed consent was obtained in writing from all participants before study activities.
Assessments
The Attention Network Test
During the ANT,8 participants view brief presentations of a set of five arrows (Fig. 1). The task is to indicate the direction of the target (center) arrow by pushing the left or right button on a mouse. There are two target conditions: 1) congruent, all five arrows point in the same direction, and 2) incongruent, center arrow is in the opposite direction to flanker arrows. Participants are asked to fixate on a cross on the center of the screen before each trial begins. Three cue conditions precede the target—no cue, center cue, and spatial cue (96 trials each). Center cues alerted participants to the timing of the upcoming target, whereas spatial cues alerted participants to the location (above or below fixation) of the upcoming target. There was a training block of 24 trials with feedback, followed by three blocks of 96 trials each with no feedback. Participants performed the task in a quiet, private room and took brief breaks between the blocks if needed.
We calculated network measures, for correct trials only, by subtracting median RTs between contrasting trials, as follows: 1) alerting = RTno cue −RTcentral cue; 2) orienting = RTcenter cue −RTspatial cue; and 3) executive = RTincongruent flanker −RTcongruent flanker. Whereas larger alerting and orienting scores indicate faster cue-related performance, larger executive scores indicate worse performance (longer time to resolve conflict). We additionally calculated the mean RT across the 288 trials.
Neuropsychological assessment
The neuropsychological assessment battery encompassed measures of attention, processing speed, executive function, and memory and included: the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV) Digit Span (total correct), Letter-Number Sequencing (total correct), Coding (total correct), and Symbol Search subtests (accuracy); the Trail Making Test-A (TMT-A) and -B (TMT-B; time to completion); the Stroop Color Word Test (number correct on word reading, color naming, and color-word interference); and the California Verbal Learning Test–Second Edition (CVLT-II; number of words recalled on Trials 1–5, short delay free recall, and long delay free recall). We chose these measures because they allowed us to evaluate the specificity of the association between ANT network scores and the cognitive functions most commonly affected in TBI,3–6 as well as test the hypothesis that the ANT would be more strongly associated with measures of attention, processing speed, and executive function compared to measures of memory.
Glasgow Outcome Scale-Extended
The GOSE is a structured interview designed to assess global function and recovery after a TBI. It is administered as a standardized interview with guided questions designed to assess each domain of functioning (consciousness, function in home, function outside the home, work/study, social and leisure activities, family and friendships, and symptoms) while allowing the examiner to apply his or her clinical judgment. Based on the standardized interview, each participant is assigned a value on an 8-point scale, where higher scores reflect better global function.
Statistical analysis
Statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS (SPSS, Inc., Chicago, IL) and Jamovi. Demographic and clinical characteristics, including impairment on neuropsychological measures, were evaluated descriptively. We calculated norm-referenced z-scores for each participant's performance on each neuropsychological measure using published demographically corrected normative data. For descriptive analyses of the ANT, we analogously calculated norm-referenced z-scores for each participant's performance using an unpublished, laboratory-based data set of 66 healthy adults ANT scores (mean age = 43.9 years, standard deviation [SD] = 15.8, range = 22–86). With this data set, we created a regression model with ANT network scores as the dependent variable and mean RT from age as a predictor variable (i.e., ANT predicted score = y + B * age). We calculated z-scores for subjects with TBI by subtracting their predicted ANT score from the age-predicted score (using the equation estimated by our regression) and dividing by the SD of the healthy adult database. Signs were flipped for ANT executive and mean RT so that negative values always denote worse performance.
To test our hypotheses, we first conducted a principal components analysis (PCA) on the neuropsychological measures (raw scores) to identify the core constructs assessed in our neuropsychological battery, and mitigate the risk of false-positive results arising from multiple comparisons from analyzing each neuropsychological measure separately. We used the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) test and Bartlett's test of sphericity to determine whether the obtained results met assumptions of PCA. We examined the scree plot and extracted components with eigenvalues >1. We used the varimax rotation to obtain the rotated component matrix. Individual scores for each component for each subject were generated in SPSS using the regression method of computing factor scores.
Separate ordinary least squares linear regression models were used to test the association between ANT variables (alerting network score, orienting network score, executive network score, and mean response time) and PCA component 1 (processing speed and executive function) and PCA component 2 (working and episodic memory). Age, education, cause of injury, and sex were included as covariates. All models were evaluated to ensure that assumptions of linear regression were met, including lack of multi-collinearity, normal distribution of residuals, acceptable number of outliers, and homoscedasticity. A similar model was computed with GOSE as the outcome using ordinal logistic regression.
Results
Demographics and clinical characteristics
Of the 48 recruited participants, 1 was excluded from data analysis because they were not able to complete the full neuropsychological battery, 1 because of stimulant use which is an exclusion criterion but was not reported at the time of initial enrollment, and 1 because they performed at chance on the ANT, possibly suggesting poor effort. This resulted in a final analyzed sample of 45. Table 1 provides demographic and clinical characteristics of the final sample used in subsequent analyses. In brief, subjects ranged in age from 19 to 85 years, had an average of 14.8 years of education, and ranged in injury severity from complicated mild to severe.
Table 1.
Demographics/characteristics | Mean (SD) or N (%) |
---|---|
Age (years) | 47.5 (16.4) |
Education (years) | 14.8 (3.3) |
Sex Male Female |
32 (71%) 13 (29%) |
Race/ethnicity White Black Asian/Pacific Islander Hispanic origin Other Declined to report |
21 (47%) 8 (18%) 5 (11%) 4 (9%) 4 (9%) 3 (6%) |
Initial GCS (median; range) | 14 (3–15) |
Injury severity Complicated mild (GCS 13–15) Moderate (GCS 9–12) Severe (GCS 3–8) |
28 (67%) 5 (12%) 9 (21%) |
Cause of injury Vehicular accident Fall Assault Other (e.g., gunshot wound) |
17 (39%) 15 (34%) 9 (21%) 3 (7%) |
Time post-injury (months) | 5.0 (1.3) |
GOSE 1: Death 2: Vegetative state 3: Lower severe disability 4: Upper severe disability 5: Lower moderate disability 6: Upper moderate disability 7: Lower good recovery 8: Upper good recovery |
0 (0%) 0 (0%) 5 (11%) 7 (15%) 7 (15%) 6 (13%) 11 (24%) 10 (22%) |
TBI, traumatic brain injury; GCS, Glasgow Coma Scale; GOSE, Glasgow Outcome Scale-Extended; SD, standard deviation.
Performance on the Attention Network Test and the neuropsychological battery
Participants showed a range of performance on the ANT measures (Fig. 2), with the greatest proportion of scores below age expectations (negative z-scores) for mean RT. On neuropsychological measures, we followed consensus guidelines29 and defined an average or above score (collapsed into one group for simplicity) as a norm-referenced z-score of >−0.67 (≥25th percentile), a low average score as a norm-referenced z-score ranging from −0.67 to −1.33 (9–24th percentile), a below-average/borderline score as a norm-referenced z-score ranging from −1.22 to −2.25 (2–8th percentile), and an exceptionally low score as a norm-referenced z-score <−2.25 (<1st percentile).
As shown in Figure 3, performance on individual neuropsychological measures varied, with generally worse performance on measures of processing speed and executive function than on measures of memory. A large percentage of the sample scored below the average range on TMT-B and Stroop Color Naming. TMT-B had the highest percentage of scores in the extremely low range (<1st percentile).
Principal component analysis on neuropsychological battery
Variables entered into the PCA were raw scores on Digit Span Total, Letter Number Sequencing, Coding, Symbol Search, TMT-A, TMT-B, Stroop Word Reading, Stroop Color Naming, Stroop Color-Word, CVLT-II Immediate Recall, CVLT-II Short Delay Free Recall, and CVLT-II Long Delay Free Recall. After a varimax rotation of the PCA results, our KMO test of sampling adequacy demonstrated a strong relationship among the variables (KMO = 0.81). Bartlett's test of sphericity identified significance among the correlations within the correlation matrix (χ2(66) = 374.483, p < 0.001).
The PCA yielded a two-component solution (i.e., two components with initial eigenvalues >1; Table 2). Component 1 mapped onto neuropsychological measures of processing speed and executive function and had an eigenvalue of 6.38 and accounted for 53.2% of the total variance. Component 2 mapped onto measures of working and episodic memory and had an eigenvalue of 1.68 and explained a further 14% of the variance. Varimax rotation led to a new eigenvalue of 4.43 for component 1 (processing speed and executive function) and 3.64 for component 2 (working and episodic memory; 36.9% and 30.3% of the variance, respectively). Rotated loadings of each neuropsychological measure on both components are shown in Table 2.
Table 2.
Shading depicts assignment (loading) of measures onto components based on the strength of correlation between individual neuropsychological measures and the components.
WAIS-IV, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Fourth Edition; TMT-A, Trail Making Test-A; TMT-B, Trail Making Test-B; CVLT-II, California Verbal Learning Test–Second Edition.
Associations between the Attention Network Test, cognitive domain scores, and functional outcomes
For the regression model with the outcome of component 1 (processing speed and executive function), overall regression with ANT performance was statistically significant (R2 = 0.67, F(10,30) = 6.03, p < 0.001; Table 3A). The ANT executive network score was significant in the model (β = 0.26, p = 0.03), as was ANT mean RT (β = −0.49, p < 0.001). Figure 4 demonstrates the unadjusted correlations between ANT scores and component 1 as scatter plots.
Table 3.
Model fit | R2 | Adjusted R2 | F | df | p value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
0.67 | 0.56 | 6.03 | 10, 30 | <0.001 |
Parameters | Standardized coefficient (β) |
95% CI lower | 95% CI upper | t | p value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age |
–0.09 |
–0.39 |
0.21 |
–0.63 |
0.53 |
Education |
0.30 |
0.02 |
0.57 |
2.20 |
0.04 |
Sex (female vs. male) |
0.08 |
–0.46 |
0.61 |
0.29 |
0.77 |
Cause of injury (fall vs. vehicular accident) |
−0.62 |
–1.15 |
–0.09 |
–2.38 |
0.02 |
Cause of injury (assault vs. vehicular accident) |
–0.21 |
–0.83 |
0.41 |
–0.70 |
0.49 |
Cause of injury (other vs. vehicular accident) |
–2.18 |
–3.73 |
–0.63 |
–2.87 |
0.007 |
ANT alerting |
–0.07 |
–0.31 |
0.17 |
–0.58 |
0.57 |
ANT orienting |
0.05 |
–0.30 |
0.40 |
0.30 |
0.77 |
ANT executive |
0.26 |
0.02 |
0.51 |
2.22 |
0.03 |
ANT mean RT | –0.49 | –0.76 | –0.22 | –3.72 | <0.001 |
ANT, Attention Network Test; RT, reaction time; CI, confidence interval.
Overall regression for model 2 with the outcome of component 2 (working and episodic memory) and performance on the ANT was not statistically significant (R2 = 0.34, F(10,30) = 1.51, p = 0.18; Table 3B). None of the ANT variables predicted scores on component 2.
Model fit | R2 | Adjusted R2 | F | df | p value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
0.34 | 0.11 | 1.51 | 10, 30 | 0.18 |
Parameters | Standardized coefficient (β) |
95% CI lower | 95% CI upper | t | p value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age |
0.34 |
–0.08 |
0.77 |
1.64 |
0.11 |
Education |
0.08 |
–0.31 |
0.47 |
0.40 |
0.69 |
Sex (female vs. male) |
0.66 |
–0.10 |
1.42 |
1.78 |
0.09 |
Cause of injury (fall vs. vehicular accident) |
0.06 |
–0.69 |
0.81 |
0.17 |
0.86 |
Cause of injury (assault vs. vehicular accident) |
–0.43 |
–1.31 |
0.44 |
–1.01 |
0.32 |
Cause of injury (other vs. vehicular accident) |
0.73 |
–1.46 |
2.93 |
0.68 |
0.50 |
ANT alerting |
0.05 |
–0.29 |
0.39 |
0.30 |
0.77 |
ANT orienting |
0.03 |
–0.47 |
0.53 |
0.12 |
0.91 |
ANT executive |
–0.01 |
–0.34 |
0.34 |
–0.01 |
0.99 |
ANT mean RT | –0.17 | –0.55 | 0.21 | –0.91 | 0.37 |
ANT, Attention Network Test; RT, reaction time; CI, confidence interval.
A similar model using ordinal logistic regression was computed with GOSE scores as the outcome. Overall regression was statistically significant (Nagelkerke R2 = 0.18, χ2(10) = 21.6, p = 0.02; Table 3C). Among specific predictors, the ANT executive network score weakly, but significantly, predicted GOSE score (odds ratio [OR] = 1.02, p = 0.01).
Model fit | AIC | Nagelkerke R2 | χ2 | df | p value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
160 | 0.18 | 21.6 | 10 | 0.02 |
Parameters | Standardized coefficient (OR) |
95% CI lower | 95% CI upper | Z | p value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age |
1.01 |
0.96 |
1.06 |
0.47 |
0.64 |
Education |
1.22 |
0.98 |
1.53 |
1.93 |
0.05 |
Sex (female vs. male) |
0.51 |
0.12 |
2.17 |
–0.97 |
0.33 |
Cause of injury (fall vs. vehicular accident) |
0.36 |
0.09 |
1.42 |
–1.46 |
0.15 |
Cause of injury (assault vs. vehicular accident) |
0.37 |
0.06 |
2.05 |
–1.13 |
0.26 |
Cause of injury (other vs. vehicular accident) |
0.02 |
0.01 |
0.62 |
–2.42 |
0.02 |
ANT alerting |
1.01 |
0.98 |
1.03 |
0.72 |
0.47 |
ANT orienting |
0.98 |
0.98 |
1.03 |
–1.50 |
0.13 |
ANT executive |
1.02 |
1.003 |
1.03 |
2.51 |
0.01 |
ANT mean RT | 0.99 | 0.99 | 1 | –1.63 | 0.10 |
GOSE, Glasgow Outcome Scale-Extended; ANT, Attention Network Test; RT, reaction time; AIC, Akaike information criterion; OR, odds ratio; CI, confidence interval.
Associations between the Attention Network Test and cognitive domain scores by injury severity
Given the relative preponderance of complicated mild TBI in our sample, we explored the association between the ANT and cognitive function by injury severity by recomputing regression models predicting the principal components, with separate models in complicated mild TBI and in moderate-severe TBI. In participants with complicated mild TBI, the regression model predicting component 1 (processing speed and executive function) remained significant (R2 = 0.74, F(10,16) = 4.60, p = 0.003), with the executive network (p = 0.05) and mean response time (p = 0.04) significant in the model. The regression model predicting component 2 (memory) remained non-significant (R2 = 0.42, F(10,16) = 1.13, p = 0.40). In participants with moderate-severe TBI, neither regression model was significant (component 1: R2 = 0.97, F(10,1) = 3.00, p = 0.42; component 2: R2 = 0.90, F(10,1) = 0.94, p = 0.67).
Discussion
Our primary objective was to evaluate the relationship between the ANT and cognitive function in individuals with TBI. Our secondary objective was to assess the relationship between the ANT and global disability. ANT scores ranged from above to below age expectations, with a significant proportion of the sample performing below age expectation on mean RT. Neuropsychological scores varied, but were generally worse, on measures of processing speed and executive function than measures of memory. Global function on the GOSE also varied, but tended to cluster toward good recovery. PCA on the neuropsychological battery yielded two principal components, one comprising measures of processing speed/executive function and another comprising measures of memory. The ANT demonstrated a strong association with the processing speed/executive function component, but not the memory component. The ANT was a weak overall predictor of disability, although within ANT networks, the executive network showed an association with disability.
Consistent with our hypotheses, and with earlier findings in healthy adults, the present results demonstrated a moderately strong relationship between the ANT and a principal component assessing processing speed/executive function. Of the specific variables of the ANT, the executive network and mean RT demonstrated robust associations with this principal component. The association between the ANT executive network and processing speed/executive function may reflect the known role of the lateral pre-frontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex in executive functions.15,16,30–32 This association would also be expected because the flanker paradigm used to compute the ANT executive score is closely tied to inhibition, a core executive function.33 Our results are consistent with past work that has suggested that the executive network may be more distinct from the alerting and orienting networks.18,19
That ANT mean RT was associated with the processing speed/executive function component—and more strongly than the ANT executive network—is not surprising given that the neuropsychological tests that comprised this component were response-time dependent and relied heavily on rapid information processing. This difference in the relative strength of association between ANT RT and the executive network may be reflective of the predominance of speeded tasks among the measures comprising this component, which included five measures of visual attention/processing speed and only two measures of higher-order executive functions (TMT-B and Stroop Color-Word). Executive function is an umbrella term for various higher-order cognitive processes, and it is possible that if our battery included additional executive measures (e.g., of organization, decision making) with a more balanced weighting with processing speed measures, we may have observed an even stronger association with the ANT executive network. It is notable that we did find an association, given the relatively small sample size of the study.
As hypothesized, the present findings did not support a strong association between the ANT and the principal component comprising memory. Inherently, the ANT is a behavioral measure developed to target the three aspects of attention and neural networks involved in attention and executive processes, rather than memory. Thus, the lack of association with memory provides additional evidence in support of the construct validity of the ANT and its specificity of association with clinical assessment of executive function in TBI.
We explored the association between the ANT and global function to further understand the clinical utility and ecological validity of the ANT. The current study highlights two findings related to this analysis: 1) The overall model shows weak association with global function, and 2) within specific networks, the ANT executive network is significantly associated with function, though in the opposite direction than was hypothesized. That the overall model had a weak association with global function is unsurprising given that neuropsychological measures themselves only have modest associations with daily function.34 Even perfect scores on the GOSE are associated with a wide variability in neuropsychological performance, which may explain the weak association we found between the GOSE and the ANT. The finding of a significant association between the ANT executive network and disability is consistent with research demonstrating that executive functions are strong predictors of activities of daily living and everyday function in healthy adults.35,36
Interestingly, we found that a stronger executive network (lower score on the ANT) was associated with greater disability (lower score on the GOSE). It is unclear why this pattern emerged in our data, though this association may be moderated by other variables and factors that we did not assess in our study, such as neuropsychiatric symptoms including depression or anxiety. Further research is necessary to understand this association and evaluate the ecological validity of the ANT.
Limitations
Our sample was predominantly persons with complicated mild TBI and exploratory analyses evaluating the impact of injury severity suggested that the results were driven by persons with complicated mild TBI, but not persons with moderate-severe TBI. However, the number of persons with moderate-severe TBI was small, which risks a false negative (type 2 error), and thus caution is urged in generalizing findings to those with moderate-severe TBI until further evaluation is conducted. Our neuropsychological battery was limited with respect to the executive functions measured; however, all measures were chosen because of their sensitivity to deficits in TBI and to minimize participant burden. The neuropsychological battery did not include a measure of general intellectual function (IQ), which may have affected the results. The study was cross-sectional in nature, and future work will benefit from longitudinal follow-up to determine associations between the ANT, neuropsychological measures, and disability in the chronic stage of TBI. Finally, our sample size was relatively small, though it is notable that we found significant associations between the ANT and neuropsychological measures given this small sample.
Conclusion
Findings support the construct validity of the ANT in subacute TBI and suggest that it may be a useful assessment measure in TBI clinical research. With future research, the ANT may eventually inform targeted interventions for persons with TBI by assisting in the identification of injury pathology, tracking engagement of the appropriate neuroanatomical targets (e.g., of non-invasive brain stimulation), and serving as a physiologically relevant cognitive end-point for clinical trials.37
Acknowledgments
We thank Karen Wen, Meaghan Race, Nidha Samdhani, and Christopher Tapia for their assistance with data collection and management.
Abbreviations Used
- ANT
Attention Network Test
- CVLT-II
California Verbal Learning Test–Second Edition
- GCS
Glasgow Coma Scale
- GOSE
Glasgow Outcome Scale-Extended
- KMO
Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin
- OR
odds ratio
- PCA
principal components analysis
- RT
reaction time
- SD
standard deviation
- TBI
traumatic brain injury
- TMT-A
Trail Making Test-A
- TMT-B
Trail Making Test-B
- WAIS-IV
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Fourth Edition
Authors' Contributions
Abhishek Jaywant: conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, supervision, visualization, writing–original draft, writing–review and editing. Emily Blunt: conceptualization, visualization, writing–original draft, writing–review and editing. Keith Jamison: data curation, formal analysis. Nayoung Kim: project administration, data curation, formal analysis. Arindam RoyChoudhury: methodology, formal analysis. Nicholas D. Schiff: conceptualization, writing–review and editing. Amy Kuceyeski: funding acquisition, writing–review and editing. Kristen Dams-O'Connor: conceptualization, writing–review and editing. Sudhin Shah: funding acquisition, conceptualization, supervision, writing–review and editing.
Transparency, Rigor, and Reproducibility Summary
The present study was not formally registered on ClinicalTrials.gov because it is not a clinical trial. The analytical plan was not pre-specified because this article constitutes a secondary analysis of a molecular imaging longitudinal study. Approximately 350 subjects were screened, and of those 48 enrolled in the study at the time of analysis, 3 subjects were excluded, resulting in our final sample of 45. Examiners were trained and supervised by a clinical neuropsychologist. All neuropsychological measures, as well as the global disability measure, are well validated, have strong psychometric properties, and are frequently used in TBI research. Examiners were not aware of the aims and hypotheses of this article. All data were securely stored in REDCap. Data can be made available upon reasonable request to the corresponding author and a subsequent data-use agreement.
Funding Information
This project was supported by NIH grants R01NS102646 from the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS) and UL1 TR 002384 from the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences (NCATS).
Author Disclosure Statement
Dr. Kuceyeski is on the scientific advisory board for Neuronasal Inc.
Cite this article as: Jaywant A, Blunt E, Jamison K, et al. Association between the Attention Network Test, neuropsychological measures, and disability in post-acute traumatic brain injury. Neurotrauma Reports 2023:4(1):318–329. doi: 10.1089/neur.2022.0068.
References
- 1. Schretlen DJ, Shapiro AM. A quantitative review of the effects of traumatic brain injury on cognitive functioning. Int Rev Psychiatry 2003;15(4):341–349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Walz JA, Mani R, Alnawmasi MM, et al. . Visuospatial attention allocation as an indicator of cognitive deficit in traumatic brain injury: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front Hum Neurosci 2021;15:675376. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Fraser EE, Downing MG, Biernacki K, et al. . Cognitive reserve and age predict cognitive recovery after mild to severe traumatic brain injury. J Neurotrauma 2019;36(19):2753–2761; doi: 10.1089/neu.2019.6430 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Tsai YC, Liu CJ, Huang HC, et al. . A meta-analysis of dynamic prevalence of cognitive deficits in the acute, subacute, and chronic phases after traumatic brain injury. J Neurosci Nurs 2021;53(2):63–68; doi: 10.1097/JNN.0000000000000570 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Dymowski AR, Owens JA, Ponsford JL, et al. . Speed of processing and strategic control of attention after traumatic brain injury. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2015;37(10):1024–1035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Spitz G, Ponsford JL, Rudzki D, et al. . Association between cognitive performance and functional outcome following traumatic brain injury: a longitudinal multilevel examination. Neuropsychology 2012;26(5):604–612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Petersen SE, Posner MI. The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annu Rev Neurosci 2012;35(1):73–89. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Fan J, McCandliss BD, Sommer T, et al. . Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J Cogn Neurosci 2002;14(3):340–347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Veríssimo J, Verhaeghen P, Goldman N, et al. . Evidence that ageing yields improvements as well as declines across attention and executive functions. Nat Hum Behav 2022;6(1):97–110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. de Souza Almeida R, Faria-Jr A, et al. . On the origins and evolution of the Attention Network Tests. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2021;126:560–572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Eriksen BA, Eriksen CW. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Percept Psychophys 1974;16(1):143–149. [Google Scholar]
- 12. Westlye LT, Grydeland H, Walhovd KB, et al. . Associations between regional cortical thickness and attentional networks as measured by the Attention Network Test. Cereb Cortex 2011;21(2):345–356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Xiao M, Ge H, Khundrakpam BS, et al. . Attention performance measured by Attention Network Test is correlated with global and regional efficiency of structural brain networks. Front Behav Neurosci 2016;10:194; doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00194 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Backes V, Kellermann T, Voss B, et al. . Neural correlates of the attention network test in schizophrenia. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2011;261 Suppl 2:S155–S160; doi: 10.1007/s00406-011-0264-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Xuan B, Mackie MA, Spagna A, et al. . The activation of interactive attentional networks. NeuroImage 2016;129:308–319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Fan J, Mccandliss B, Fossella J, et al. . The activation of attentional networks. NeuroImage 2005;26(2):471–479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Markett S, Reuter M, Montag C, et al. . Assessing the function of the fronto-parietal attention network: insights from resting-state fMRI and the attentional network test: assessing the function of the fronto-parietal attention network. Hum Brain Mapp 2014;35(4):1700–1709. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Neuhaus AH, Urbanek C, Opgen-Rhein C, et al. . Event-related potentials associated with Attention Network Test. Int J Psychophysiol 2010;76(2):72–79. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Shah SA, Goldin Y, Conte MM, et al. . Executive attention deficits after traumatic brain injury reflect impaired recruitment of resources. NeuroImage Clin 2017;14:233–241. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Mannarelli D, Pauletti C, Currà A, et al. . The cerebellum modulates attention network functioning: evidence from a cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation and Attention Network Test study. Cerebellum 2019;18(3):457–468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Hahn E, Ta TMT, Hahn C, et al. . Test–retest reliability of Attention Network Test measures in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2011;133(1–3):218–222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Ishigami Y, Eskes GA, Tyndall AV, et al. . The Attention Network Test-Interaction (ANT-I): reliability and validity in healthy older adults. Exp Brain Res 2016;234(3):815–827. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Ishigami Y, Fisk JD, Wojtowicz M, et al. . Repeated measurement of the attention components of patients with multiple sclerosis using the Attention Network Test-Interaction (ANT-I): stability, isolability, robustness, and reliability. J Neurosci Methods 2013;216(1):1–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Wang B, Zhang J, Pan W, et al. . Differential influence of location-specific white-matter hyperintensities on attention subdomains measured using the Attention Network Test. Med Sci Monit 2020;26:e921874-1–e921874-24; doi: 10.12659/MSM.921874 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Chen A, Zhang Z, Cao C, et al. . Altered attention network in paratroopers exposed to repetitive subconcussion: evidence based on behavioral and event-related potential results. J Neurotrauma 2021;38(23):3306–3314; doi: 10.1089/neu.2021.0253 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Halterman CI, Langan J, Drew A, et al. . Tracking the recovery of visuospatial attention deficits in mild traumatic brain injury. Brain 2006;129(3):747–753. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Weaver B, Bédard M, McAuliffe J, et al. . Using the Attention Network Test to predict driving test scores. Accid Anal Prev 2009;41(1):76–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Roca J, Lupiáñez J, López-Ramón MF, et al. . Are drivers' attentional lapses associated with the functioning of the neurocognitive attentional networks and with cognitive failure in everyday life? Transp Res Part F Traffic Psychol Behav 2013;17:98–113; doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2012.10.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Guilmette TJ, Sweet JJ, Hebben N, et al. . American Academy of Clinical Neuropsychology consensus conference statement on uniform labeling of performance test scores. Clin Neuropsychol 2020;34(3):437–453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Fong AHC, Yoo K, Rosenberg MD, et al. . Dynamic functional connectivity during task performance and rest predicts individual differences in attention across studies. NeuroImage 2019;188:14–25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Saylik R, Williams AL, Murphy RA, et al. . Characterising the unity and diversity of executive functions in a within-subject fMRI study. Sci Rep 2022;12(1):8182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Venkatesan UM, Hillary FG. Functional connectivity within lateral posterior parietal cortex in moderate to severe traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology 2019;33(6):893–910. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, et al. . The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognit Psychol 2000;41(1):49–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Ruet A, Bayen E, Jourdan C, et al. . A detailed overview of long-term outcomes in severe traumatic brain injury eight years post-injury. Front Neurol 2019;10:120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Cornelis E, Gorus E, Van Schelvergem N, et al. . The relationship between basic, instrumental, and advanced activities of daily living and executive functioning in geriatric patients with neurocognitive disorders. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2019;34(6):889–899. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Nguyen CM, Copeland CT, Lowe DA, et al. . Contribution of executive functioning to instrumental activities of daily living in older adults. Appl Neuropsychol Adult 2020;27(4):326–333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Silverberg ND, Crane PK, Dams-O'Connor K, et al. . Developing a cognition endpoint for traumatic brain injury clinical trials. J Neurotrauma 2017;34(2):363–371; doi: 10.1089/neu.2016.4443 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]