Table 2.
author(s) | country | article type | type of research | total sample size | compared with young adults | compared with learning alone | relationship with co-learners | materials | outcomes |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Cheng & Strough [84] | United States | research article | randomized experimental studya | 97 | Y | Y | familiar | everyday problem-solving task | pairs were more accurate than individuals. Younger adults were faster and more accurate than older adults |
Crompton [82] | United Kingdom | PhD thesis | non-randomized experimental study | 48 | Y | N | familiar, unfamiliar | adapted version of Map Task | no effect of age group or familiarity. Younger adults did have improved delayed recall |
United Kingdom | PhD thesis | non-randomized experimental study | 24 | N | N | perceived human versus computer partner | adapted version of Map Task | older adults modified their interaction when learning with a human or a computer. Older adults recalled fewer details of routes learned with the computer partner and found the learning experience more difficult | |
Crompton & MacPherson [85] | United Kingdom | research article | non-randomized experimental study | 24 | N | N | perceived human versus computer partner | referential task/Barrier Task | participants in the computer condition were slower, took fewer turns, and recalled less |
Crompton et al. [86] | United Kingdom | research article | non-randomized experimental study | 48 | Y | N | familiar, unfamiliar | referential task/Barrier Task | older adults’ performance became indistinguishable from younger adults over time, no effect of familiarity |
Derksen et al. [7] | United States | research article | non-randomized experimental study | 68 | Y | N | familiar | referential task/Barrier Task | older adults' performance became indistinguishable from younger adults over time |
Filer & Scukanec [87] | United States | research article | non-randomized experimental study | 8 | Y | N | unfamiliar | referential task/Barrier Task | younger women used fewer words, turns, and had fewer errors than older adult women |
Gould et al. [88] | United States | research article | non-randomized experimental study | 60 | Y | N | familiar, unfamiliar | referential task/Barrier Task | younger adults performed better overall (including number of words, turns, and optimal descriptions) compared to older adults, with no effect of familiarity |
Hupet et al. [89] | Belgium | research article | non-randomized experimental study | 40 | Y | N | unfamiliar | referential task/Barrier Task | older adults’ performance improved but not to the extent that younger adults' performance did |
Kannampallil et al. [81] | United States | research article | randomized experimental study | 144 | N | N | unfamiliar | medication schedulers | older adults created more accurate medication schedules when using a structured aid, with no difference between structured and unstructured aids when considering medication complexity |
Margrett et al. [90] | United States | research article | non-randomized experimental study | 22 | Y | N | familiar | adapted version of VW task | couples actively worked together, most often tutoring each other. Middle-aged adults outperformed older adults |
Margrett & Willis [91] | United States | research article | randomized experimental study | 98 | N | Y | familiar | multi-week training on inductive reasoning strategies | receiving training is generally beneficial, there was no difference in accuracy between working alone and working together |
Morrow et al. [83] | United States | research article | randomized experimental study | 96 | N | N | unfamiliar | medication scheduler | aids are associated with higher accuracy, especially when the medical scenario is complex, compared to an unstructured aid (pen and paper) or no aid |
United States | research article | randomized experimental study | 64 | N | N | unfamiliar | medication scheduler | participants were more accurate when using the medication scheduler, but there was no difference in speed | |
Peter-Wight & Martin [53] | Switzerland | research article | non-randomized experimental study | 141 | N | Y | familiar | Black Box task | participants' performance was better when working collaboratively versus working alone |
Rodrigues et al. [92] | United States | research article | non-randomized experimental study | 32 | Y | N | familiar, unfamiliar | referential task/Barrier Task | participants became more efficient over time, regardless of age and familiarity. Younger adults were more efficient in communicating with a familiar partner, older adults were more efficient in communicating with an unfamiliar partner |
Seah et al. [79] | Canada | research article | non-randomized mixed method study (quantitative and qualitative) | 50 | N | N | largely unfamiliar | bingo nutrition and health game | participants significantly improved their knowledge of health and nutrition, had a more positive attitude towards digital games, and their social connectedness increased. Common themes in the interviews consisted of social connectedness and knowledge of technology, health and nutrition |
Saczynski et al. [93] | United States | research article | randomized experimental study | 96 | N | Y | familiar | multi-week training on inductive reasoning strategies | individual and collaborative training groups did not differ in performance, except when both completing a collaborative recall test (in which individuals working together did worse) |
Sayago et al. [78] | United Kingdom, Spain | research article | ethnographical study with semi-structured interviews | 420 | N | N | largely unfamiliarb | multi-week training on basic computing skills | during initial learning, participants mostly worked with the same partner. Participants found interacting with unfamiliar peers difficult due to different strategies or strategies had to be created and agreed |
Vazquez et al. [94] | United States | research article | randomized experimental study | 466 | N | Y | largely unfamiliarb | tutorial on searching for health information online (NIH) | no effect of collaboration in terms of satisfaction or performance. Previous IT experience may mediate the relationship between learning outcomes and learning methods |
Vrkljan [95] | Canada | research article | non-randomized experimental study | 44 | N | N | familiar | using a GPS system | couples less experienced with technology had higher levels of assistance, mostly supportive. Couples more experienced with technology provided less assistance but the assistance that they did provide was more directive |
Vrkljan [96] | Canada | research article | non-randomized experimental study | 40 | N | N | familiar | using a GPS system | couples more experienced with technology made fewer errors overall, performed each task at a faster rate, and provided less assistance |
Wei et al. [97] | Canada | research article | qualitative research | 22 | N | N | familiar | semi-structured interview and open-ended questions | older adults often search online for daily tasks, interests, and curiosity. The latter is often to settle a disagreement or to discuss a topic together |
Wood et al. [80] | Canada | research article | non-randomized experimental study | 64 | N | N | largely familiarb | training sessions to develop computing skills | collaborations were considered helpful and effective, and participants were more involved |
Xie [103] | United States | research article | randomized experimental study | 124 | N | Y | largely unfamiliarb | tutorial on evaluating Internet health information (NIH) | performance improved regardless of condition or familiarity |
Xie [104] | United States | research article | non-randomized experimental study | 172 | N | N | familiar, unfamiliarb | tutorial on searching for health information online (NIH) | performance improved regardless of familiarity |
Xie [105] | United States | research article | randomized experimental study | 146 | N | Y | largely unfamiliarb | tutorial on helping older adults search for health information online (NIH) | performance improved regardless of collaboration or learning design |
Yoon & Stine-Morrow [98] | United States | research article | non-randomized experimental study | 48 | Y | N | unfamiliar | referential task | older adults found it more difficult to identify the source of a description and adjusted their descriptions less when working with a naïve partner compared to younger adults |
Zhang et al. [99] | Canada | research article | observational study | 22 | N | N | unfamiliarc | Wii Sports Resort games | older adults learn through questions and feedback of their younger partners, such as pointing out errors or explaining concepts |
aIndicates studies that are largely randomized but have made minor changes or specifications that were not random.
bIndicates study designs that were largely unfamiliar or unfamiliar, but also had some participants included that did not fit this design.
cBoth younger and older adults were involved in this study. However, the younger adults were involved as a guide and their learning performance was not assessed. As such, this study is not included in the younger versus older category.