Abstract
Background:
Cognitive control processes are implicated in the behavioral treatment of Tourette’s Disorder (TD). However, the influence of these processes on treatment outcomes has received minimal attention. This study examined whether cognitive control processes and/or tic suppression predicted reductions in tic severity and treatment response to behavior therapy.
Method:
Fifty-three youth with TD or a pervasive tic disorder participated in a randomized wait-list controlled trial of behavior therapy. Following a baseline assessment to evaluate psychiatric diagnoses, tic severity, and cognitive control processes (e.g., response selection, inhibition, and suppression), youth were randomly assigned to receive 8 sessions of behavior therapy (n=23) or a waitlist of equal duration (n=28). Youth receiving immediate treatment completed a post-treatment assessment to determine improvement in tic severity. Meanwhile, youth in the waitlist condition completed another assessment to re-evaluate tic severity and cognitive control processes, and subsequently received 8 sessions of behavior therapy followed by a post-treatment assessment to determine improvement.
Results:
A multiple linear regression model found that pre-treatment inhibition/switching on the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System Color Word Interference Test predicted reductions in tic severity after behavior therapy (β=−0.36, t=−2.35, p=0.025, ƞ2 =0.15). However, other cognitive control processes and tic suppression did not predict treatment response and/or reductions in tic severity. Small non-significant effects were observed in cognitive control processes after behavior therapy.
Conclusion:
Cognitive control processes may influence tic severity reductions in behavior therapy. Notably, even when other cognitive control processes are impaired and youth are initially unable to voluntarily suppress their tics, youth with TD can still benefit from behavior therapy. Findings offer implications for clinical practice and research for TD.
Keywords: Cognitive control, inhibitory control, inhibition, tic suppression, behavior therapy
Introduction
Tourette’s Disorder and other persistent tic disorders (collectively referred to as TD) are neurodevelopmental conditions characterized by involuntary motor movements and/or vocalizations. Evidence suggests that TD develops in childhood and affects almost 2% of youth (Knight et al., 2012; Scahill, Specht, & Page, 2014). Youth with TD often experience co-occurring psychiatric conditions such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), and anxiety disorders (Freeman et al., 2000; Specht et al., 2011). Tics and co-occurring conditions can cause significant impairment for youth with TD (Cloes et al., 2017; Stiede et al., 2018) and contribute to a reduced quality of life (Conelea et al., 2011; Storch et al., 2007). Therefore, effective treatments are critical for youth with TD.
Behavior therapy is recommended as the first line intervention for youth with TD by numerous professional organizations (Murphy, Lewin, Storch, Stock, & AACAP Committee on Quality Issues, 2013; Pringsheim et al., 2019). Behavioral therapies such as habit reversal training (HRT) and its successor the Comprehensive Behavioral Intervention for Tics (CBIT; (Woods et al., 2008) have demonstrated moderate-to-large reductions in tic severity that maintain over time with no significant adverse effects or risk for symptom substitution (McGuire et al., 2014; McGuire et al., 2019; Peterson et al., 2016; Piacentini et al., 2010; Woods et al., 2011). Despite its therapeutic benefit, only 50% of youth with TD exhibit a positive treatment response to behavior therapy and many treatment responders continue to experience bothersome tics (Piacentini et al., 2010). Therefore, it is critical to understand the factors that influence treatment response to behavior therapy. This knowledge can provide critical insights into underlying therapeutic mechanisms and inform the development of strategies to optimize treatment outcomes.
Behavior therapy is predicated on a neurobehavioral model of TD. This model acknowledges the neurological origin of tics and TD (see Augustine & Singer, 2018 for a review of the neurobiology of TD), and highlights that internal and external contextual factors influence the expression and maintenance of tics (Conelea & Woods, 2008; Woods et al., 2008). These internal (e.g., premonitory urges, mood states) and external factors (e.g., situations, activities) serve as key targets in behavior therapy to reduce tic expression. For instance, youth with TD often experience internal aversive sensations called premonitory urges, which precede tics and are alleviated by tic expression (Capriotti, Brandt, Turkel, Lee, & Woods, 2014; Himle, Woods, Conelea, Bauer, & Rice, 2007; Leckman, Walker, & Cohen, 1993; McGuire et al., 2016; Specht et al., 2013; Woods, Piacentini, Himle, & Chang, 2005). Consequently, tic expression can become unintentionally reinforced due to the reduction in premonitory urge, which leads to the increased likelihood of tic expression in response to subsequent premonitory urges. A similar relationship also holds true for external contextual factors. Youth with TD often have difficulty managing tics during certain activities and situations (e.g., completing assignments or household chores, attending social functions; Capriotti et al., 2015; Himle et al., 2014; Storch et al., 2017). When tics are expressed during these activities, it can result in the disruption, early discontinuation, and/or eventual avoidance of these activities (e.g., tic severity increases during homework, so homework time is shortened by parents). Over time, as these activities are prematurely discontinued or avoided altogether, the expression of tics in these situations becomes unintentionally reinforced and results in heightened tic occurrence during these situations in the future (e.g., when homework attempted in the future, tic expression increases). Indeed, this can be further complicated by the common co-occurrence of anxiety, which can exacerbate tics in previously avoided activities and/or situations. Behavior therapy emphasizes decreasing environmental triggers for the tics, eliminating reinforcing social reactions to the tics, teaching the patient to become more aware of his or her premonitory urges, and then implementing competing responses contingent upon detection of tics to prevent further tic expression.
Despite its established efficacy, the precise mechanisms underlying behavior therapy are not fully explicated. While some evidence exists for habituation as a possible mechanism (Houghton et al., 2017; Verdellen et al., 2008), cognitive control represents another hypothesized mechanism that has received minimal investigation. Broadly, cognitive control refers to a system that modulates the operation of other cognitive and emotional systems in the service of goal-directed behavior (Insel et al., 2010). Cognitive control processes are typically engaged during contexts in which appropriate responses need to be selected from competing alternative responses. There are several cognitive control processes implicated in the behavioral treatment of TD: goal selection, response selection, inhibition/suppression, and performance monitoring. In behavior therapy for TD, goal selection is involved in attaining awareness to internal and external contexts that elicit tic expression. Meanwhile, response selection is implicated in the acquisition and selection of an appropriate behavioral response—tic suppression or competing responses—contingent upon awareness of internal and/or external contexts. Relatedly, inhibition/suppression is involved in the successful implementation of a behavioral response to prevent tic expression. Finally, performance monitoring is evident in adjustments in behavioral implementation based on the success/failure of inhibition suppression. Therefore, youth with TD exhibiting greater baseline cognitive control with respect to these processes would be anticipated to experience increased therapeutic benefit from behavior therapy.
To date, there have only been a handful of studies that have examined the relationship between neurocognitive processes of cognitive control (e.g., response inhibition) and behavior therapy outcomes. Deckersbach and colleagues (2006) found that greater baseline response inhibition on a visuospatial priming task predicted a positive treatment response among 30 adults with TD (Deckersbach et al., 2014). However in a large clinical trial of behavior therapy for adults with TD, the change in tic severity and treatment response to behavior therapy were not associated with baseline performance on neurocognitive tasks of inhibitory control (i.e., Go-No Go, Stroop Color-Word Test; Abramovitch et al., 2017). Finally, in a clinical trial of behavior therapy for youth with TD, Chang and colleagues (2018) found that baseline performance on neurocognitive tasks of inhibitory control were not predictive of reduction in tic severity and/or treatment response to behavior therapy (e.g., Stop Signal Task, Stroop Color-Word Test). Given the few studies, inconsistent findings, and developmental differences between youth and adults with TD, further research is critical to clarify the role of cognitive control in the behavioral treatment of TD. This line of research could elucidate neural mechanisms of treatment response to behavior therapy in youth with TD, inform strategies to optimize current behavioral treatments, and personalize treatment recommendations based on baseline cognitive control processes.
This study examined cognitive control processes in youth with TD and their relationship to behavior therapy treatment outcomes in a randomized controlled trial. The principle goal of this secondary outcome analysis was to determine whether baseline performance on tasks of cognitive control predicted reductions in symptom severity and treatment response to behavior therapy. As cognitive control processes may be separate from tic suppression abilities, our secondary goal was to examine whether baseline tic suppression capabilities predicted tic severity reductions and treatment response to behavior therapy. Finally, we explored whether baseline cognitive control processes improved over the course of behavior therapy.
METHODS
Participants
To participate in this randomized waitlist-controlled trial, youth with TD met the following inclusion criteria: (1) 9 to 14 years of age, (2) diagnostic criteria for a TD, (3) a Yale Global Tic Severity Scale (YGTSS) total tic score > 14 or >10 if only motor tics present (Leckman, Riddle, Hardin, & Ort, 1989), and (4) be fluent in English. Exclusion criteria included the following: (1) lifetime diagnosis of an autism spectrum disorder, mania, or psychotic disorder, (2) a psychiatric or psychosocial condition that requires immediate treatment not provided in the study (e.g., substance use, conduct disorder), (3) full scale intelligence quotient < 80, and (4) four or more prior behavior therapy sessions. Youth were required to be either medication free or taking a stable dose of psychiatric medication for at least six weeks prior to enrollment, with no planned changes for the duration of study participation.
Participants were 53 youth (M=10.93 years, SD=1.62 years, Range: 9–14 years) who met diagnostic criteria for Tourette’s Disorder or a Chronic Motor Tic Disorder. Youth were predominantly non-Hispanic White (58.50%), male (71.70%), and had an average intelligence (M=107.37, SD=12.49). Common co-occurring psychiatric conditions included anxiety disorders (n=26, 49%), ADHD (n=19, 35.8%), obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD; n=16, 30%), and depressive disorders (n=1, 1.8%). At the baseline assessment, youth had a moderate level of tic severity on the YGTSS total tic score (M=25.83, SD=6.19, Range: 15–43) and 5 youth (9.4%) were taking a tic influencing psychotropic medication (e.g., alpha-2 agonist, antipsychotic).
Measures
Psychiatric Diagnostic Interview.
Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia– Present and Lifetime version (K-SADS-PL) was used to assess psychiatric diagnoses (Kaufman et al., 1997). The K-SADS is a structured psychiatric interview that is commonly used and has good psychometric properties.
Tic Severity and Impairment.
The Yale Global Tic Severity Scale (YGTSS) is a clinician-rated scale used to assess tic severity (Leckman et al., 1989). Motor and phonic tics are rated separately across five domains: number, frequency, intensity, complexity, and interference. Items are summed to produce a Total Tic Score (range: 0 – 50). Clinicians also rate impairment caused by tics using the Impairment Scale (range: 0 – 50). The YGTSS has good reliability and validity (Leckman et al., 1989; McGuire et al., 2018).
Treatment Response.
Signal detection analyses show that a 25%−35% reduction on the YGTSS Total Tic score corresponds with a positive treatment response (Jeon et al., 2013; Storch et al., 2011). Given the preliminary nature of these investigations, the higher benchmark of improvement (a 35% reduction on the YGTSS Total Tic score) was used to classify treatment responder status.
Premonitory Urge Severity.
The Premonitory Urge for Tics Scale (PUTS) is a 10-item scale that measures the severity of premonitory sensations that precede tics (Woods et al., 2005). Items are rated on a 4-point Likert scale (range: 1 – 4), and the first nine items are summed to produce a total urge severity score (Woods et al., 2005).
Cognitive Control Processes of Response Selection, Inhibition, and Suppression.
There were four assessments used to measure cognitive control processes: the Attention Network Task, the Stop Signal Task, the Go-No Go Task, and the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System Color-Word Interference Test. The Attention Network Task (ANT) required subjects to respond by pressing the left or right mouse button that corresponded to the direction of a target arrow in the center of a visual display. The target arrow was flanked on both sides by cues that could be congruent (arrows of the same direction), incongruent (opposite direction), or neutral (dashes) to the target arrow (Eriksen & Eriksen, 1974). The ANT produces several scores, with the incongruent accuracy score percentage correct representing a measure of inhibitory control. The Stop-Signal Task (SST) required youth to perform a choice response time task on each trial, with responses withheld if an auditory beep was heard (Logan, Cowan, & Davis, 1984). Stop-signal reaction time (SSRT) is the amount of time needed to inhibit a response after presentation of the stop signal, and represents a measure of inhibitory control (Logan, Schachar, & Tannock, 1997). In the Go-No Go (Go-No Go) task, letters A through Z appeared on the screen one at a time and youth were required to press the space bar when any letter (“Go” trial) except the target letter “X” appeared (“NoGo” trial) (Serrien, Orth, Evans, Lees, & Brown, 2005). Errors of commission, or the percentage of “NoGo” trials that were incorrectly classified as “Go” trials represents a measure of inhibitory control and sustained attention (Abramovitch et al., 2017). Finally, the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System Color-Word Interference Test (D-KEFS CWIT) is a Stroop-like task that consists of four parts: color naming, word reading, inhibition, and inhibition/switching (Delis, Kaplan, Kramer, Delis, & Kramer, 2001). The inhibition/switching scaled score represents a measure of inhibitory control.
Tic Suppression Task.
Prior to treatment assignment, youth completed a tic suppression task that included a 5-minute baseline condition (i.e., tic freely) and a 5-minute tic suppression condition (Himle & Woods, 2005; Woods & Himle, 2004). Participants were assigned to a counterbalanced order of baseline and tic suppression conditions generated by randomization. In the baseline condition, youth were prompted to “tic as much or as little as needed” and to try not to suppress tics. During the tic suppression condition, youth were verbally instructed to fixate on a dot for 5 minutes and try not to exhibit tics. Youth were informed that they could suppress tics in any way they wanted, as long as they remained seated in the chair and refrained from covering their face/head with their hands. In both conditions, youth were instructed to remain seated with their hands in their lap or on the armrests of the chair. After each condition, youth completed a questionnaire that asked if they actively tried to suppress tics during the condition. All youth reported that they did not try to suppress tics during the baseline condition, and actively tried to suppress tics during the suppression condition. Sessions were recorded and reviewed by research assistants masked to condition (i.e., tic freely or tic suppression). Research assistants were oriented to operational definitions of each participants’ tics, which were initially generated by the independent evaluator based on tics identified during administration of the YGTSS. Research assistants independently coded tic occurrence during each 5-minute condition using Behavioral Observation Research Initiative Software version 7.9.7 (BORIS; Friard & Gamba, 2016). Interrater reliability for tic frequency ratings was calculated using an intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) in SPSS 26.0, with good agreement observed (ICC = .84; Koo & Li, 2016).
Procedures
Study procedures were approved by the local institutional review board, and the trial was registered on ClinicalTrials.gov (NCT00675675). Recruitment took place at the University of California, Los Angeles between July 2007 and December 2011. After obtaining consent, participants completed a baseline assessment to characterize psychiatric diagnoses, tic severity (YGTSS), premonitory urges (PUTS), cognitive control processes (ANT, SST, Go-No Go, and D-KEFS CWIT), and tic suppression. Afterwards, youth were randomly assigned to receive 8 sessions of manualized behavior therapy over a period of 10 weeks (Piacentini et al., 2010; Woods et al., 2008) or a waitlist of equal duration to control for the waxing and waning nature of tic severity. Youth completed a post-treatment assessment after 10 weeks to determine change in tic severity and evaluate treatment response to study condition (YGTSS). All assessments were conducted by trained independent evaluators (IEs) masked to treatment condition. Youth who were randomized to the waitlist condition received 8 sessions of behavior therapy over 10 weeks and completed another assessment to determine change in tic severity and treatment response to behavior therapy (YGTSS).
Analytic Plan
An analysis of variance (ANOVA) and chi-square tests compared baseline group differences between the immediate treatment and waitlist conditions (see Table 1). For those youth in the waitlist condition, preliminary analyses compared ‘waitlist’ baseline and ‘treatment’ baseline characteristics of neurocognitive and behavioral outcomes (D-KEFS CWIT inhibition/switching, Go-No Go commission, SST reaction time, ANT incongruent, PUTS total, YGTSS total score). A repeated measures ANOVA where visit (‘waitlist’ baseline, ‘treatment’ baseline) was entered as a predictor only found a small significant difference for YGTSS impairment (p=.04, ƞ2=.06). Meanwhile, all other comparisons were not statistically significant (p=.20–.96). As there were minimal differences between ‘waitlist’ and ‘treatment’ baselines, ‘treatment’ baselines was used as baseline for participants in the waitlist group because it was more representative of the participants functioning immediately prior to treatment.
Table 1.
Comparison of Baseline Characteristics Between Treatment Groups (N = 53)
Entire Sample (N = 53) |
Behavior Therapy (n = 25) |
Waitlist Condition (n = 28) |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
N (%) | N (%) | N (%) | χ2 | p-value | |
Demographics | |||||
Age | 10.93 (1.62) | 10.80 (1.85) | 11.05 (1.41) | 0.31 | .58 |
Male | 38 (71.7%) | 18 (72%) | 20 (71.4%) | .002 | 0.96 |
Non-Hispanic White | 32 (60.38%) | 14 (56%) | 18 (64%) | 0.38 | 0.54 |
Psychiatric Diagnosis | |||||
Anxiety Disorders1 | 26 | 14 | 12 | 0.91 | 0.34 |
ADHD | 19 | 11 | 8 | 1.16 | 0.28 |
OCD | 16 | 8 | 8 | 0.03 | 0.85 |
Depressive Disorders2 | 1 | 1 | 0 | 1.14 | 0.29 |
Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | F | p-value | |
Clinical Characteristics | |||||
YGTSS Total Tic Score | 25.83 (6.19) | 26.36 (5.71) | 25.36 (6.67) | .34 | 0.56 |
YGTSS Impairment Score | 25.04 (8.55) | 27.70 (8.96) | 22.78 (7.64) | 4.39 | 0.04 |
PUTS Total Score | 23.17 (5.36) | 23.56 (5.36) | 22.82 (5.44) | 0.25 | 0.62 |
Cognitive Control Processes | |||||
ANT Incongruent Accuracy | 39.62 (8.64) | 39.48 (8.94) | 39.74 (8.52) | 0.01 | 0.91 |
SST Reaction Time | 303.83 (102.96) | 296.38 (102.47) | 310.63 (105.23) | 0.21 | 0.65 |
Go-No Go Commission Errors | 22.96 (7.62) | 24.42 (7.42) | 21.71 (7.70) | 1.65 | 0.21 |
D-KEFS Color-Word Interference | 10.71 (2.78) | 11 (2.87) | 10.46 (2.73) | 0.47 | 0.49 |
Tic Suppression | |||||
Tic Suppression Score | 42.94 (42.72) | 52.35 (42.94) | 37.15 (43.25) | 0.52 | 0.48 |
Note: ADHD = Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder, OCD = Obsessive Compulsive Disorder, YGTSS = Yale Global Tic Severity Scale, PUTS = Premonitory Urge to Tic Scale, SWAN = Strengths and Weaknesses of ADHD Symptoms and Normal Behavior, ANT = Attention Network Task, SST = Stop-signal task, D-KEFS = Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System
Anxiety Disorders included the following conditions: Social phobia, separation anxiety, generalized anxiety, specific phobia
Depressive Disorders included the following conditions: major depressive disorder, dysthymia, depressive disorder NOS
For our primary aim to examine whether baseline performance on tasks of cognitive control predicted reductions in tic severity and treatment response, the active treatment and waitlist control groups were pooled in to a single behavior therapy group (N=53). Logistic regression was used to evaluate the influence of neurocognitive predictors (ANT incongruent accuracy, SST reaction time, Go-No Go commission errors, D-KEFS CWIT inhibition/switching condition) on YGTSS responder status (responder/non-responder) at post-treatment. Multiple linear regression was used to evaluate the influence of neurocognitive predictors on post-treatment YGTSS total score, controlling for treatment baseline YGTSS total score.
For our secondary aim to determine if baseline tic suppressibility predicted treatment response, first a tic suppressibility score was calculated using the following formula [baseline tic frequency – suppression tic frequency)/BL tic frequency*100] used in prior research (Conelea et al., 2018). Then a logistic regression analysis was used and included baseline PUTS total score and tic suppressibility score as predictors of YGTSS responder status at post-treatment. Finally, a multiple linear regression was used to evaluate the influence of baseline PUTS total score and tic suppressibility on YGTSS total score post-treatment, controlling for pre-treatment YGTSS total score.
Finally for our exploratory aim to determine whether changes in cognitive control processes occurred over the course of treatment, multiple mixed effects regression models were used. Models included time and treatment response as predictors with a random intercept. The dependent outcome was the post-treatment performance on the cognitive control task (ANT incongruent accuracy, SST reaction time, Go-No Go commission errors, D-KEFS CWIT inhibition/switching condition).
Results
Treatment Response and Efficacy of Behavior Therapy
When examining the efficacy of behavior therapy during treatment, a repeated measures ANOVA found a significant effect of time, F(1,48)=37.55, p<.001, ƞ2=.44, where marginal means indicate a significant reduction in YGTSS tic severity (MTXbaseline=24.67, SE=.83; MTXendpoint=17.26, SE=.86, see Supplemental Table 1). Twenty-two youth (41.5%) exhibited a treatment response to behavior therapy. When examining changes in tic-related impairment, a repeated measures ANOVA found a significant effect of time, F(1,45)=45.79, p<.001, ƞ2=.50, where the marginal means indicate a significant reduction in YGTSS Impairment score (MTXbaseline=22.52, SE=.1.05; MTXendpoint=12.05, SE=1.09, Supplemental Table 1).
Cognitive Control Processes Predicting Behavior Therapy Outcomes
Baseline cognitive control processes did not significantly predict treatment response status at post-treatment (n=37; D-KEFS CWIT inhibition/switching z=0.64, p=0.52; Go-No Go commission z=−0.69, p=0.49; SST reaction time z=−0.22, p=0.82; ANT incongruent accuracy z=−0.60, p=.55, see Supplemental Table 2). When using a dimensional therapeutic improvement outcome (YGTSS total tic score; n=37), there was one notable distinction (see Supplemental Table 3). When controlling for baseline YGTSS total tic score (t=2.00, p=0.054), D-KEFS CWIT inhibition/switching score was the only cognitive control indicator to significantly predict tic severity at post-treatment. A measure of inhibition/switching, a higher D-KEFS CWIT inhibition/switching score at baseline was predictive of lower YGTSS severity post-treatment (β=−0.36, t=−2.35, p=.025, ƞ2 =0.15). Meanwhile, none of the other cognitive control predictors in the model predicted YGTSS total tic score at post-treatment (Go-No Go commission z=−0.29, p=0.78; SST reaction time z=−.52, p=0.60; ANT incongruent accuracy z=−0.72, p=0.48, see Supplemental Table 3).
Tic Suppression Abilities Predicting Behavior Therapy Outcomes
A logistic regression model (n=16) indicated that neither tic suppressibility (OR=1.01, z=−0.77, p=0.44) nor PUTS total score (OR=0.95, z=−0.55, p=0.58) predicted treatment response status at post-treatment (see Supplemental Table 4). When controlling for pre-treatment YGTSS total score (t=1.91, p=0.08), the PUTS total score (t=−0.56, p=0.58) and tic suppressibility (t=−1.07, p=0.30) did not predict YGTSS total tic score at post-treatment (see Supplemental Table 5).
Exploring the Effect of Behavior Therapy on Cognitive Control Processes
Mixed-effect regression models found no significant change in cognitive control processes on neurocognitive tasks (D-KEFS CWIT inhibition/switching, b=−.009, p>.05, treatment response, b=−.44, p>.05; Go-No Go commission, b=−.07, p>.05, treatment response, b=.20, p>.05; SST reaction time b=−1.90, p>.05, treatment response, b=−3.95, p>.05; and ANT incongruent accuracy b=−.097, p>.05, treatment response, b=−1.68, p>.05, see Supplemental Table 6). As shown in Table 2, the enhancing effects of behavior therapy on cognitive control processes were relatively small for both responders (d = 0.04 – 0.38) and non-responders (d = 0.04 – 0.22).
Table 2.
Baseline and Post-Treatment Neurocognitive Performance by Treatment Response Status
Non-Responder | Responder | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Baseline | Post-Treatment | Effect Sizea | Baseline | Post-Treatment | Effect Sizea | |
Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | |||
ANT Incongruent Accuracy | 41.5 (4.75) | 40.91 (7.06) | 0.07 | 40.33 (10.55) | 38.94 (7.14) | 0.16 |
SST Reaction Time | 277.19 (113.06) | 270.06 (103.04) | 0.07 | 275.26 (81.91) | 260.20 (72.34) | 0.15 |
Go-No Go Commission Errors | 23.80 (6.66) | 22.14 (6.57) | 0.22 | 23.00 (7.36) | 23.21 (7.71) | 0.03 |
D-KEFS Color-Word Interference | 11.27 (2.81) | 11.39 (3.12) | 0.04 | 11.18 (3.24) | 10.55 (2.48) | 0.38 |
Note: ANT = Attention Network Task, SST = Stop-signal task, D-KEFS = Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System
Effect size calculated by difference in means between baseline and post-treatment, divided by the standard deviation for the entire sample at baseline
Discussion
This study examined the relationship between cognitive control processes and behavior therapy outcomes in a randomized controlled trial of youth with TD. Findings revealed that cognitive control processes related to inhibition/switching performance (D-KEFS CWIT inhibition/switching score) predicted reductions in tic severity on the YGTSS. This is the second report to find a neurocognitive predictor of behavior therapy in TD (e.g., visuo-spatial priming task; Deckersbach, Rauch, Buhlmann, & Wilhelm, 2006). Recent meta-analytic work also suggests that inhibitory control is impaired among patients with TD (e.g., verbal inhibition, Stroop CW Interference score; Morand-Beaulieu et al., 2017). While Chang and colleagues (2018) found that baseline performance on neurocognitive tasks of inhibitory control was not predictive of tic severity reductions or treatment response to behavior therapy (e.g., Stop Signal Task, Stroop Color-Word Test), there are some noted distinctions between the D-KEFS CWIT and Stroop Color-Word test tasks. Specifically, during the Stroop task, participants are required to name the color of the ink instead of the word (i.e., a less automated task) and inhibit the interference arising from the more automated task (i.e., reading the word). In contrast, during the D-KEFS CWIT task, participants must switch back and forth between reading the word and naming the color of the ink. The D-KEFS CWIT task is more cognitively demanding and requires both suppression/inhibition and set shifting/cognitive flexibility to perform well. Increased cognitive demand may also explain why the visuo-spatial priming task that requires four categories and inhibition predicts behavior therapy outcomes in TD (Deckersbach et al., 2006), whereas more simplistic Go-No Go and Stop Signal tasks that have only two response categories do not. Given that both the visuo-spatial priming task and the D-KEFS CWIT task also capture aspects of cognitive flexibility, this particular facet of executive functioning paired with inhibition may be an important baseline factor in the treatment of TD. Unfortunately, executive functioning in TD has received minimal examination and may warrant further investigation.
No predictive relationship for other neurocognitive markers of cognitive control was found for behavior therapy outcomes. This is consistent with prior studies and suggests that inhibitory control performance on tasks such as the Go/No-Go or Stop-Signal Task is not related to treatment processes (Abramovitch et al., 2017; Chang et al., 2018; Morand-Beaulieu et al., 2017). Given that some youth with TD are reported to have difficulties with neurocognitive performance, these findings are promising and suggest these youth can still benefit from behavior therapy. Behavior therapy can reduce tic severity and does not diminish cognitive functioning among youth with TD (Chang et al., 2018). This further supports the recommendation of behavior therapy as a first line treatment for all youth with TD, regardless of neurocognitive performance. Although our single-site sample size is smaller than prior multisite clinical trials (e.g., Piacentini et al., 2010), behavior therapy reduced tic severity and impairment with therapeutic effects comparable to other child treatment studies. Beyond providing further support that behavior therapy reduces tic severity, this suggests that the findings observed here are generalizable to definitive large-scale, multisite, clinical trials.
Interestingly, baseline tic suppression did not predict treatment outcome. For this task, youth were verbally prompted to suppress tics. Reliance on a verbal request to suppress tics is in contrast to the added analysis of contingency management and/or contingent reinforcement (e.g., providing tokens for periods in which youth did not tic) used in prior tic suppression studies (Greene et al., 2015; Himle, Woods, & Bunaciu, 2008; Woods & Himle, 2004; Conelea et al., 2018). Although limited by a small sample size, there are two implications. First, youth with TD who have difficulty suppressing tics at baseline would still likely benefit from behavior therapy. Youth who had difficulty suppressing tics still exhibited reductions in tic severity after behavior therapy, and may be more motivated to consistently implement behavior therapy strategies to reduce tic severity (instead of relying in part on tic suppression abilities). Second, contingency management and/or reward learning may likely play a critical role in tic suppression in addition to the cognitive control process of inhibition/suppression (Conelea et al., 2018). Indeed, contingencies and rewards are implicated in the neurobehavioral model that underlies behavior therapy (e.g., social rewards for inhibiting tics during social interactions, relief from premonitory urges; Himle et al., 2014; Houghton et al., 2017). Thus, future research should compare the role of tic suppression both with and without contingency management and/or reinforcement learning in behavioral treatment outcome. Furthermore, when examining tic suppression in future research, it will be important to consider that some youth may be more (or less) experienced implementing tic suppression as a tic management strategy. Future studies should consider prior experience utilizing tic suppression, and explore the possible benefit of ‘tic suppression training sessions’ to determine whether performance relates to a lack of tic suppression capabilities or rather a lack of knowledge of how to engage in tic suppression. Finally when exploring the effects of behavior therapy on cognitive control processes, there were only small non-significant changes in cognitive control processes. This preliminary finding suggests that the effects of behavior therapy are largely tic specific (i.e., reductions in tic severity on the YGTSS) and do not generalize to the broad domain of cognitive control. However, the exploratory nature and small sample size warrant careful consideration before over interpretation of this finding.
Despite the numerous strengths of this study (e.g., clinical trial methodology, raters masked to treatment condition), a few limitations exist. First, this study selected a priori aspects of cognitive control hypothesized to be relevant to behavior therapy for TD. As such, there are other facets of cognitive control that were not fully examined and warrant further investigation. Second, no data imputation strategies were applied to address missing data. Although data were missing at random (Little’s MCAR, p > .05), imputation of missing neurocognitive performance and tic suppressibility were not applied due to the limited information about imputation on these constructs at baseline and post-treatment. However, we did examine different approaches when data on tic severity outcomes were inferred using post-baseline observations carried forward. Given that findings regarding neurocognitive predictors were not notably different, the more conservative findings are presented here.
These limitations notwithstanding, there are clear clinical and research implications for these findings. From a clinical perspective, the D-KEFS CWIT task is relatively quick to administer and could be completed during a clinical evaluation to assess the generalizability of these findings to clinical care. If it continued to predict treatment response to behavior therapy, this information could help effectively allocate scarce therapeutic resources of trained behavior therapy providers (Woods, Conelea, & Himle, 2010). Meanwhile from a research perspective, these findings offer the possibility of a new treatment target to improve treatment response rates to behavior therapy. For instance, cognitive strategies and pharmacological interventions that enhanced inhibition/suppression performance on the D-KEFS CWIT task may result in improved behavior therapy outcomes for TD.
In summary, this study found that baseline inhibition/suppression predicted treatment response to behavior therapy in youth with TD. While these findings are promising and highlight the contribution of cognitive control processes to behavior therapy outcomes, there are numerous other related constructs (e.g., executive functioning) and learning processes (e.g., associative learning, reinforcement/reward learning) that remain largely unexamined in the context of behavior therapy. Thus, considerable more research is needed to both replicate and extend these findings on cognitive control processes before drawing definitive conclusions. Specifically, given the multiple mechanisms that are involved over the course of behavioral treatment, it is important to further understand these hypothesized processes and their interactions to determine the optimal approach to enhance behavior therapy outcomes in TD.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments:
This work was supported in part by grants from the Tourette Association of American (Dr. McGuire), American Academy of Neurology (Dr. McGuire), American Psychological Foundation (Dr. McGuire), and NIMH grants K23MH113884 (Dr. Ricketts), P50MH077248 (Dr. McCracken), and T32MH073517 (Drs. McCracken and Piacentini). The views expressed within this article represent those of the authors, were not influenced by the funding sources, and are not intended to represent the position of the NIMH.
References
- Abramovitch A, Hallion LS, Reese HE, Woods DW, Peterson A, Walkup JT, … Wilhelm S (2017). Neurocognitive predictors of treatment response to randomized treatment in adults with tic disorders. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 74, 9–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Augustine F, & Singer HS (2018). Merging the pathophysiology and pharmacotherapy of tics. Tremor and Other Hyperkinetic Movements, 8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Capriotti MR, Brandt BC, Turkel JE, Lee H-J, & Woods DW (2014). Negative Reinforcement and Premonitory Urges in Youth With Tourette Syndrome An Experimental Evaluation. Behavior modification, 38, 276–296. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Capriotti MR, Piacentini JC, Himle MB, Ricketts EJ, Espil FM, Lee HJ, … Woods DW (2015). Assessing environmental consequences of ticcing in youth with chronic tic disorders: The Tic Accommodation and Reactions Scale. Children’s Health Care, 44, 205–220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chang SW, McGuire JF, Walkup JT, Woods DW, Scahill L, Wilhelm S, … Piacentini J (2018). Neurocognitive correlates of treatment response in children with Tourette’s Disorder. Psychiatry research, 261, 464–472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cloes KI, Barfell KSF, Horn PS, Wu SW, Jacobson SE, Hart KJ, & Gilbert DL (2017). Preliminary evaluation of child self‐rating using the Child Tourette Syndrome Impairment Scale. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 59, 284–290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conelea CA, Wellen B, Woods DW, Greene DJ, Black KJ, Specht M, … Capriotti M (2018). Patterns and predictors of tic suppressibility in youth with tic disorders. Frontiers in psychiatry, 9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conelea CA, & Woods DW (2008). The influence of contextual factors on tic expression in Tourette’s syndrome: a review. Journal of psychosomatic research, 65, 487–496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conelea CA, Woods DW, Zinner SH, Budman C, Murphy T, Scahill LD, … Walkup J (2011). Exploring the impact of chronic tic disorders on youth: results from the Tourette Syndrome Impact Survey. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 42, 219–242. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deckersbach T, Chou T, Britton JC, Carlson LE, Reese HE, Siev J, … Walkup JT (2014). Neural correlates of behavior therapy for Tourette׳ s disorder. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 224, 269–274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deckersbach T, Rauch S, Buhlmann U, & Wilhelm S (2006). Habit reversal versus supportive psychotherapy in Tourette’s disorder: a randomized controlled trial and predictors of treatment response. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1079–1090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delis DC, Kaplan E, Kramer JH, Delis D, & Kramer J (2001). Delis-Kaplan executive function system (D-KEFS). Examiner’s manual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Eriksen BA, & Eriksen CW (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & psychophysics, 16, 143–149. [Google Scholar]
- Freeman RD, Fast DK, Burd L, Kerbeshian J, Robertson MM, & Sandor P (2000). An international perspective on Tourette syndrome: selected findings from 3500 individuals in 22 countries. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 42, 436–447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friard O, & Gamba M (2016). BORIS: a free, versatile open‐source event‐logging software for video/audio coding and live observations. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 7, 1325–1330. [Google Scholar]
- Himle MB, Capriotti MR, Hayes LP, Ramanujam K, Scahill L, Sukhodolsky DG, … Specht MW (2014). Variables associated with tic exacerbation in children with chronic tic disorders. Behavior modification, 38, 163–183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Himle MB, & Woods DW (2005). An experimental evaluation of tic suppression and the tic rebound effect. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 1443–1451. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Himle MB, Woods DW, Conelea CA, Bauer CC, & Rice KA (2007). Investigating the effects of tic suppression on premonitory urge ratings in children and adolescents with Tourette’s syndrome. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 2964–2976. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Houghton DC, Capriotti M, Franklin MR, Scahill L, Wilhelm S, Peterson AL, … Woods DW (2017). Investigating Habituation to Premonitory Urges in Behavior Therapy for Tic Disorders: Evidence from a Randomized Controlled Trial. Behavior Therapy, 48, 834–846. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Insel T, Cuthbert B, Garvey M, Heinssen R, Pine DS, Quinn K, … Wang P (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. 167, 748–751. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jeon S, Walkup JT, Woods DW, Peterson A, Piacentini J, Wilhelm S, … Scahill L (2013). Detecting a clinically meaningful change in tic severity in Tourette syndrome: A comparison of three methods. Contemporary clinical trials, 36, 414–420. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Flynn C, Moreci P, … Ryan N (1997). Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 980–988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knight T, Steeves T, Day L, Lowerison M, Jette N, & Pringsheim T (2012). Prevalence of tic disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatric neurology, 47, 77–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koo TK, & Li MY (2016). A guideline of selecting and reporting intraclass correlation coefficients for reliability research. Journal of chiropractic medicine, 15, 155–163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leckman JF, Riddle MA, Hardin MT, & Ort SI (1989). The Yale Global Tic Severity Scale: Initial testing of a clinician-rated scale of tic severity. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 566–573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leckman JF, Walker DE, & Cohen DJ (1993). Premonitory urges in Tourette’s syndrome. The American Journal of Psychiatry. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Logan GD, Cowan WB, & Davis KA (1984). On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Logan GD, Schachar RJ, & Tannock R (1997). Impulsivity and inhibitory control. Psychological science, 8, 60–64. [Google Scholar]
- McGuire JF, McBride N, Piacentini J, Johnco C, Lewin AB, Murphy TK, & Storch EA (2016). The premonitory urge revisited: An individualized premonitory urge for tics scale. Journal of psychiatric research, 83, 176–183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGuire JF, Piacentini J, Brennan EA, Lewin AB, Murphy TK, Small BJ, & Storch EA (2014). A meta-analysis of behavior therapy for Tourette Syndrome. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 50, 106–112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGuire JF, Piacentini J, Storch EA, Murphy TK, Ricketts EJ, Woods DW, … Scahill L (2018). A multicenter examination and strategic revisions of the Yale Global Tic Severity Scale. Neurology, 90, e1711–e1719. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGuire JF, Ricketts EJ, Scahill L, Wilhelm S, Woods DW, Piacentini J, … Peterson AL (2019). Effect of Behavior Therapy for Tourette’s Disorder on Psychiatric Symptoms and Functioning. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morand-Beaulieu S, Grot S, Lavoie J, Leclerc JB, Luck D, & Lavoie ME (2017). The puzzling question of inhibitory control in Tourette syndrome: A meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 80, 240–262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy TK, Lewin AB, Storch EA, Stock S, & AACAP Committee on Quality Issues. (2013). Practice parameter for the assessment and treatment of children and adolescents with tic disorders. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 52, 1341–1359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peterson AL, McGuire JF, Wilhelm S, Piacentini J, Woods DW, Walkup JT, … Scahill L (2016). An empirical examination of symptom substitution associated with behavior therapy for Tourette’s Disorder. Behavior therapy, 47, 29–41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piacentini J, Woods DW, Scahill L, Wilhelm S, Peterson AL, Chang S, … Walkup JT (2010). Behavior therapy for children with Tourette disorder: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical Association, 303, 1929–1937. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pringsheim T, Okun MS, Müller-Vahl K, Martino D, Jankovic J, Cavanna AE, … Roessner V (2019). Practice guideline recommendations summary: Treatment of tics in people with Tourette syndrome and chronic tic disorders. Neurology, 92, 896–906. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scahill L, Specht M, & Page C (2014). The prevalence of tic disorders and clinical characteristics in children. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 3, 394–400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Serrien DJ, Orth M, Evans AH, Lees AJ, & Brown P (2005). Motor inhibition in patients with Gilles de la Tourette syndrome: functional activation patterns as revealed by EEG coherence. Brain, 128, 116–125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Specht MW, Woods DW, Nicotra CM, Kelly LM, Ricketts EJ, Conelea CA, … Walkup JT (2013). Effects of tic suppression: Ability to suppress, rebound, negative reinforcement, and habituation to the premonitory urge. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 51, 24–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Specht MW, Woods DW, Piacentini J, Scahill L, Wilhelm S, Peterson AL, … Flessner C (2011). Clinical characteristics of children and adolescents with a primary tic disorder. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 23, 15–31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stiede JT, Alexander JR, Wellen B, Bauer CC, Himle MB, Mouton-Odum S, & Woods DW (2018). Differentiating tic-related from non-tic-related impairment in children with persistent tic disorders. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 87, 38–45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Storch EA, De Nadai AS, Lewin AB, McGuire JF, Jones AM, Mutch PJ, … Murphy TK (2011). Defining treatment response in pediatric tic disorders: a signal detection analysis of the Yale Global Tic Severity Scale. Journal of child and adolescent psychopharmacology, 21, 621–627. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Storch EA, Johnco C, McGuire JF, Wu MS, McBride NM, Lewin AB, & Murphy TK (2017). An initial study of family accommodation in children and adolescents with chronic tic disorders. European child & adolescent psychiatry, 26, 99–109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Storch EA, Merlo LJ, Lack C, Milsom VA, Geffken GR, Goodman WK, & Murphy TK (2007). Quality of life in youth with Tourette’s syndrome and chronic tic disorder. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 36, 217–227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Verdellen CW, Hoogduin CA, Kato BS, Keijsers GP, Cath DC, & Hoijtink HB (2008). Habituation of premonitory sensations during exposure and response prevention treatment in Tourette’s syndrome. Behavior modification, 32, 215–227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woods D, Piacentini J, Scahill L, Peterson A, Wilhelm S, Chang S, … Conelea C (2011). Behavior therapy for tics in children: acute and long-term effects on psychiatric and psychosocial functioning. Journal of Child Neurology, 26, 858–865. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woods DW, Conelea CA, & Himle MB (2010). Behavior therapy for Tourette’s disorder: Utilization in a community sample and an emerging area of practice for psychologists. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 41, 518. [Google Scholar]
- Woods DW, & Himle MB (2004). Creating tic suppression: Comparing the effects of verbal instruction to differential reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37, 417–420. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Woods DW, Piacentini J, Chang S, Deckersbach T, Ginsburg G, Peterson A, … Wilhelm S (2008). Managing Tourette Syndrome: A Behavioral Intervention for Children and Adults Therapist Guide: A Behavioral Intervention for Children and Adults Therapist Guide: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Woods DW, Piacentini J, Himle MB, & Chang S (2005). Premonitory Urge for Tics Scale (PUTS): initial psychometric results and examination of the premonitory urge phenomenon in youths with tic disorders. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 26, 397–403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.