Summary
Reasoning about someone’s thoughts and intentions – i.e., forming a theory of mind – is an important aspect of social cognition that relies on association areas of the brain that have expanded disproportionately in the human lineage. We recently showed that these association zones comprise parallel distributed networks that, despite occupying adjacent and interdigitated regions, serve dissociable functions. One network is selectively recruited by theory of mind processes. What circuit properties differentiate these parallel networks? Here, we show that social cognitive association areas are intrinsically and selectively connected to regions of the anterior medial temporal lobe that are implicated in emotional learning and social behaviors, including the amygdala at or near the basolateral complex and medial nucleus. The results suggest that social cognitive functions emerge through coordinated activity between amygdala circuits and a distributed association network, and indicate the medial nucleus may play an important role in social cognition in humans.
Keywords: Amygdala, human, social cognition, theory of mind, default network, basolateral complex, medial nucleus, accessory basal nuclei, functional connectivity, high-resolution 7T functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)
Introduction
The ability to reason about another person’s intentions and beliefs – i.e., to form a ‘theory of mind’ (ToM) – is an important aspect of social cognition that assists the navigation of social groups.1 In humans, tasks targeting ToM activate a set of association regions of the brain that are late to mature and are disproportionately expanded in the hominin lineage,2–5 supporting that the primate brain may have expanded following evolutionary pressures associated with living in complex social groups.6 Additionally, evidence supports that evolutionarily ancient structures, such as the amygdala and related anterior medial temporal lobe (MTL) circuitry, are central to social behaviors. It is unclear how these lines of evidence intersect. Here, we show that human association regions that are active during ToM are intrinsically and selectively connected to the amygdala, including nuclei that are extensively implicated in social behaviors in rodents.7–10
Brain regions involved in ToM can be examined in humans by studying task-related changes in the blood-oxygenation-level-dependent (BOLD) signal11,12 during ‘false belief’ and ‘emotional pain’ tasks. In the false belief task,13,14 participants answer questions from the perspective of someone who holds a mistaken belief (e.g., Sally believes a cherry cake tastes of strawberries because it was mislabeled). In the ‘emotional pain’ task, participants rate the pain a protagonist may feel following an emotionally painful event (e.g., the loss of a family pet).15–17 Both tasks require thinking about someone else’s thoughts and activate a network of cortical regions that includes the temporoparietal junction, the ventromedial and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, the lateral temporal cortex, and posteromedial cortex.16,18–20 However, early observations noted that these same regions were recruited during other tasks,21–24 including those targeting autobiographical memory,25 self-oriented thinking,23,24,26–28 and ‘episodic projection’ (EP; i.e., thinking about the past or future).29–31 The same set of association regions was also conceptualized as the canonical ‘default network’ (DN),21,32,33 which is definable from resting-state correlations of the BOLD signal (i.e., functional connectivity or FC),34,35 exhibits connectivity to the MTL,21,35–37 and shows increased activity during resting periods between active tasks.38 This overlap between ToM, the DN, and other memory-related processes led to the idea that the DN plays a domain-general role in introspection and mind-wandering, which tends to include thoughts about others29,39–47 (see twelfth figure in Buckner et al.21 and first figure in Mars et al.22).
An alternative view is that the canonical DN appears to be domain-generalized because it is a coarse (i.e., blurry) estimate of finer-grained, domain-specialized networks. For instance, it was noted that even in group-averaged estimates, which tend to result in blurring over individual differences in anatomy,48–51 there are hints of substructure within the canonical DN;41,52–59 ToM tasks tend to activate a more anterior region of the inferior parietal lobe at or near the temporoparietal junction, while EP tasks recruit more posterior regions.55,57,58 Similarly, ToM tasks typically recruit dorsal posteromedial regions at or near the posterior cingulate cortex, while EP tasks typically recruit more ventral regions at or near the retrosplenial cortex. These distinctions presaged findings from finer-grained, individual-focused analyses. Within individuals, separable posteromedial regions were found to be active when participants are asked to think about relationships between people or places.20,60–63 This suggests a separation between social and episodic processes within the DN, and that EP-related activity is likely related to spatial mnemonic processes or mental ‘scene construction.’47,64,65 In these examples, social cognition was again located to more dorsal posteromedial regions, whereas EP-related activity was located in more ventral regions, matching the distinctions in group-averaged data. These findings indicate that the convergence of functions on the canonical DN may have been due to blurring across distinct, adjacent regions which separately support social and episodic functions.24
Recently, individual-level estimates of brain networks have been achieved by performing FC on extensively sampled individuals.66–69 We showed that at least two distinct networks can be identified within the canonical DN, and that the networks display adjacent, interdigitated regions in multiple association zones, leading to the appearance of parallel distributed networks.66,70 Pertinently, the two networks, termed “DN-A” and “DN-B”, recapitulated the aforementioned dorsal-ventral distinction in the posteromedial cortex, as well as the anterior-posterior distinction in the inferior parietal lobe, between ToM- and EP-related activity. Confirming this dissociation, the FC estimates of DN-B closely overlapped with activity evoked by ToM tasks within the same individuals, while the adjacent network, DN-A, instead showed activity during an EP task that involved mental scene construction.20 The correspondence between the FC and task-evoked responses was evident throughout the brain, rather than just within the posteromedial and inferior parietal components, suggesting network-level specialization of the parallel networks within the canonical DN.71
How do these two adjacent, interdigitated, networks support dissociable functions? And how do the circuit properties of DN-B enable social cognitive functions (i.e., ToM)? An answer may lie in how each network relates to more evolutionary ancient non-neocortical structures.72 We previously noted that DN-A contains a region in the posterior parahippocampal cortex66 and the subiculum,70 which suggests a link between the cortical regions of DN-A and the mnemonic and spatial functions of the posterior MTL.36,47,73–76 This link provides an explanation for why association regions that constitute DN-A are active during EP; they work with the hippocampal formation to enable mental scene construction.
Although DN-A and DN-B contain adjacent regions in multiple cortical zones, we did not observe an adjacent region of DN-B in the MTL in our prior estimates. However, there are reasons to think such a region may have been missed. First, anterior potions of the temporal lobe suffer from signal dropout in conventional BOLD imaging,77 which may have obscured characterization of this region. Smaller voxel sizes, such as those available at higher field strength (e.g., 7T), can reduce signal dropout.78 Second, network regions in the anterior MTL, particularly in the amygdala, may be small and fall beneath the resolution limits of conventional 3T fMRI. We previously showed that high-resolution 7T fMRI can reveal small DN-A regions at or near the subiculum that were missed at 3T70 (See also Sladky et al.79; Maass et al.80; Gorka et al.81). Third, there are reports of a link between the amygdala and the canonical DN in group-averaged FC data,81–85 though these prior efforts did not differentiate between regions supporting social and episodic processes (i.e., DN-A and DN-B). Finally, a recent investigation at 7T provided evidence for a link between the entorhinal cortex and a network resembling DN-B,86 supporting a closer link between DN-B and the anterior MTL.
There is also ample evidence to support a connection between the anterior MTL, specifically the amygdala, and socially relevant information processing.10,87,88 Lesions of the amygdala lead to loss of social status in primates and rodents,89 as well as decreased social affiliation, inappropriate responses to social cues, and both increases and decreases in avoidance.7,8,90,91 Amygdala neurons show increased responses when facial expressions and social interactions are viewed,92–97 as well as to eye contact from conspecifics.98 In humans, lesions of the amygdala lead to difficulties in recognizing emotions in facial expressions,91,99–105 and amygdala responses track the perceived emotion rather than visual properties of viewed expressions.96 Furthermore, the amygdala has been implicated in psychiatric disorders that are characterized by disrupted social functioning, including autism, as well as schizophrenia, anxiety, and depression.106–109 Thus, the evidence suggests that the amygdala is an evolutionarily conserved structure that is crucial for social behaviors and may work in tandem with cortical regions to enable complex social functions such as ToM.92,105,110 These findings motivate a closer investigation of the anterior MTL, including the amygdala, and the distributed association networks using high-resolution 7T fMRI.
Results
Discovery and replication strategy for high-resolution 7T fMRI data
This study analyzed high-resolution (1.8-mm isotropic) 7T fMRI resting-state data from 8 extensively sampled participants from the Natural Scenes Dataset (NSD).111 Two participants (S5 and S8) were excluded during quality control for excessive head motion, leading to a sample of 6 participants (ages 23–27 years) who provided 6–35 resting-state runs each (S1: 35 runs; S2: 6; S3: 16; S4: 12; S6: 19; and S7: 18). For participants providing 12 or more runs passing quality control, data were divided into 2 or 3 datasets for discovery (i.e., free exploration of FC patterns), replication and triplication within individuals. A temporal signal to noise ratio (tSNR) map confirmed that each participant provided high tSNR despite the small voxel size, including in anterior MTL regions critical to our hypothesis (see Supp. Fig. S1).
Replication of parallel distributed networks within the canonical default network
Using the discovery dataset, we followed Braga and Buckner66 to define DN-A and DN-B on surface-projected data112 using FC from seeds manually selected from the left dorsal prefrontal cortex. Once candidate seeds were selected, network definition was repeated using a data-driven multi-session hierarchical Bayesian model113 (MS-HBM; Fig. 1) to produce converging estimates of the networks. This provided another replication of the separation between the networks.20,70,86,115–118 Surface-based analysis allows easy observation of the cortical mantle, making it convenient for identifying the networks based on anatomical characteristics (see detailed descriptions in Braga and Buckner66 and Braga et al.70), but does not well-capture the anatomy of the MTL. Therefore, we re-defined DN-A and DN-B in the volume using manually selected seed voxels from left dorsal prefrontal cortex, and projected the volume-defined maps to the surface for comparison with surface-defined estimates to confirm that both approaches were targeting the same networks (Fig. 1). Following Braga and Buckner,70 we binarized the correlation maps above a threshold of r > 0.3 to compute an overlap map. In each participant, the primary distinctions between networks DN-A and DN-B, such as in the inferior parietal lobe and posteromedial cortex, were present in both surface and volume estimates (Fig. 1).
Figure 1: High-resolution functional connectivity (FC) separates default network A (DN-A) and B (DN-B) using both surface- and volume-based approaches.
The left column shows the networks defined on surface-projected data for each subject (S1–4, S6, S7) using data-driven clustering163 (k = 14). The surface visualization allows easier observation of the cortical sheet, but excludes medial temporal structures (see darker gray midline areas). The networks were therefore identified independently in the volume (right column) and projected to the surface for comparison between methods. The volume-defined maps were thresholded at 0.3 and binarized, to display where the estimates of DN-A (blue) and DN-B (yellow) are distinct and overlap (orange). Even in the surface, at high-resolution, DN-B showed evidence of anterior medial temporal regions (see comparison to 3T in Supp. Fig. S2).
Replication of functional dissociation between DN-A and DN-B
We previously showed that FC estimates of DN-B overlap closely with activity evoked by ToM tasks within individuals.20 To build confidence in this link, and support that our estimate of DN-B was delineating association regions related to social cognition, we replicated this result in a separate ‘detailed brain network organization’ (DBNO) dataset collected at Northwestern University. DN-A and DN-B were defined at 3T, using the same procedures as those used in the NSD data, in 8 individuals who also provided ToM (i.e., false-belief and emotional pain) and EP (i.e., mental ‘scene construction’) task data. Supp. Fig. S2 allows side-by-side comparison of maps of DN-A and DN-B defined in the 7T and 3T subjects, to show that the same networks are being studied in each dataset. In all 8 DBNO subjects, we observed selective overlap between DN-B and ToM-active regions (Fig. 2). Conversely, DN-A overlapped with regions active during mental scene construction. This confirms that the FC estimates of DN-B encapsulates regions that are specialized for social cognitive functions and are functionally dissociated from DN-A.
Figure 2: Functional imaging at 3T in eight additional individuals (DBNO_01–DBNO_08) confirms functional dissociation between DN-A and DN-B.
Black borders show the FC estimates of DN-A (left) and DN-B (right; from Supp. Fig. S2). The same participants provided data during tasks targeting theory of mind (ToM) and mental scene construction. The ToM task contrasts revealed increased activity in multiple regions that overlapped selectively with the boundaries of DN-B. In contrast, the scene construction contrast, which targeted episodic projection (EP), revealed that while participants imagined scenes, increased activity was evident in regions that overlapped with DN-A. The results confirm20 that network DN-B is selectively recruited during social cognitive processes, and is functionally dissociated from DN-A.
Discovery that DN-B contains bilateral regions in the amygdala
The surface-based estimates of DN-B from the 7T fMRI data displayed hints of regions in the anterior MTL not clearly evident at 3T (Supp. Fig. S2; and see other 3T estimates in second, ninth and seventeenth figures in DiNicola20). To study the anterior MTL more closely, we examined the volume-defined estimates at a correlation threshold of r > 0.2, following Braga and Buckner 66 and Braga et al.70,119, accounting for the lower tSNR in these regions (Supp. Fig. S1). Fig. 3 shows sagittal views of DN-A and DN-B, along with the overlap map, in participant S2. Confirming our previous findings,70 DN-A displayed a prominent posterior MTL region at or near the parahippocampal cortex and a further smaller region at or near the subiculum. We discovered that DN-B displays multiple regions in the anterior MTL: one putatively in the entorhinal cortex,86 but intriguingly, other regions were also observed that appeared to be in the amygdala (Fig. 3). Regions of DN-A and DN-B were located in mostly non-overlapping locations, even at the lower correlation threshold of 0.2. The putative amygdala regions displayed lower correlation values than the non-amygdala regions in this subject. To confirm these regions were not due to noise, we examined a coronal slice approximately around MNI y = −6 to −9, in each subject (Fig. 4). This revealed that the putative amygdala regions of DN-B were bilateral in 4/6 subjects, and likely not spurious. Subject S1 displayed unilateral regions, while S6 did not show a DN-B region that was distinct from DN-A at this location. For these two subjects, we lowered the threshold to 0.15, after which participant S1 showed clearer evidence of bilateral regions (Fig. 4), but subject S6 still showed no reliable regions (see also replication and triplication analyses in Supp. Fig. S6). Nonetheless, the analyses provided evidence that distributed network DN-B contains regions within the human amygdala that are bilateral and identifiable in multiple individuals.
Figure 3: Network DN-B contains distinct regions in the anterior medial temporal lobe (MTL), including amygdala (a), entorhinal cortex (e), and potentially subiculum (s).
Sagittal views of the volume-based FC maps of DN-A and DN-B from an example subject (S2) are shown, with the mean blood-oxygenation-level-dependent (BOLD) image as an underlay. The correlation maps were thresholded at 0.2 and binarized to display an overlap map. The top row shows the midline, to display the characteristic posteromedial distinctions between the networks. The second row shows a slice along the MTL, where interdigitated regions of DN-A and DN-B can be seen along the long axis, with DN-B displaying regions in anterior MTL. The dashed boxes indicate the location of the zoom-in shown in the lower two rows. The lowest row shows the T1 image as the underlay for better appreciation of the anatomy. The dashed vertical lines indicate the location of the coronal slices shown in Fig. 4. z(r), Fisher’s r-to-z transformed Pearson’s product-moment correlations. Numbers in each panel correspond to MNI slice coordinates. p, parahippocampal cortex.
Figure 4: Basolateral and medial amygdala regions of DN-B are bilateral and replicate across participants.
Volume-based FC maps of DN-B are shown in coronal slices. Numbers refer to MNI coordinate of each slice. On the left are views around y = −6 to −9, where 5 out of 6 subjects (exception: S6) displayed bilateral regions putatively in the basolateral amygdala. On the right are slices around y = −3 to −5, where in all 6 subjects, a distinct set of bilateral (3 out of 6) or unilateral (3 out of 6) regions could be seen putatively near the medial amygdala (Fig. 6). The white solid line in S1, S4 & S7 denotes that left and right hemispheres are from different slices. Arrows denote putative DN-B regions that were distinct from DN-A (see Figs. 5 & 6 for overlap map at these same slices, and replications in Supp. Figs. S6 & S7).
Replication and triplication of amygdala region of DN-B through multiple approaches
We confirmed the results were consistent across multiple analysis approaches. First, in the discovery dataset, a seed voxel was placed directly in the amygdala, targeting the region of DN-B in each subject. The resulting correlation maps were projected to the surface for comparison with the surface-defined networks (Supp. Fig. S3). In all subjects, the amygdala seed produced a correlation map that overlapped with the boundaries of DN-B (see especially subjects S2, S4 and S7), despite the low tSNR found around the amygdala. Participants S1 and S3 displayed relatively weak correlations that still overlapped with DN-B. These analyses confirmed that the amygdala is connected to the full distributed network DN-B, and confirmed that this link is selective, with the amygdala-seeded map showing limited overlap with DN-A.
We also replicated our findings using the independent, left-out data from each participant. We re-estimated DN-A and DN-B in the volume in the replication and triplication datasets using dorsal prefrontal seeds (Supp. Fig. S5). For each subject, we examined the MNI coordinates where we initially discovered amygdala regions and found amygdala regions of DN-B were replicated in all subjects who displayed a region in the discovery dataset (Supp Fig. S6). For one subject, S1, the region replicated in 1 of the 2 left-out datasets. The DN-B regions were located within the same approximate locations, though did vary slightly, with potential reasons discussed in Limitations and Technical Considerations. We therefore report with confidence that DN-B contains bilateral amygdala regions that are reproducible within and across participants and generalize across analysis approaches.
Confirmation that DN-B regions are in the basolateral complex of the amygdala
The amygdala is a complex structure that contains multiple nuclei with distinct roles and connections.10,96,120 Detailed knowledge of how these nuclei relate to the extended cortical network involved in social cognition is important. Although it is difficult to study amygdala nuclei in the human brain using neuroimaging, confidence can be built by using individualized estimates of the amygdala anatomy, and focusing on findings that are consistent across individuals. We overlaid the network maps onto automated, individualized segmentations of the amygdala.112,121 We also compared our findings to a group-defined amygdala atlas,120 which broadly confirmed the results, but with less specificity (not shown). Fig. 5 and Supp. Fig. S8 show a zoom-in on the amygdala, displaying how the individualized amygdala segmentation relates to the anatomical T1 and BOLD images from each participant. The demarcations should be interpreted as approximations. For instance, in subject S2 the boundaries of the lateral nucleus appear to overextend the amygdala (Supp. Fig. S8). Here, our interpretations are based on a combination of the atlas boundaries and T1/BOLD anatomy. For example, note that subject S2’s DN-B regions visibly do not overextend the amygdala in Fig. 5.
Figure 5: Individualized amygdala segmentation confirms regions of DN-B are located in or near the basolateral complex.
Top row shows an example subject’s (S1) T1 and mean BOLD images, zoomed into the right amygdala, showing estimates of the amygdala nuclei from an automated, individualized segmentation.121 The remaining rows display the amygdala segmentation in each subject (S1–4, S6, S7), around MNI slice y = −6 to −9, along with the FC estimates of DN-A and DN-B, and their overlap. The left column shows the full coronal slice with the map of DN-B, with a box indicating the zoom-in location. White arrows indicate putative regions of DN-B which did not overlap with DN-A. In each subject, the regions of DN-B appeared to overlap most prominently with the accessory basal nucleus (AB) in each subject, often crossing the border into the basal nucleus (BA). Evidence for a distinct region can also be seen in ventral portions of the lateral nucleus (LA) in each subject. Further, DN-B regions also extended dorsomedially beyond the basolateral complex (see S1-S3), but notably did not overlap with the central nucleus (CeA). Other abbreviations refer to corticoamygdaloid (Acot), cortical (Cort), and paralaminar (PL) nuclei. Supp. Fig. S8 shows the same views with the T1 as the underlay, to allow observation of each individual’s anatomy.
At a broad scale, the segmentation confirmed that the regions of DN-B were within the amygdala. When considering the nuclei demarcations, noteworthy consistencies were observed. In most subjects, DN-B reliably displayed: (1) a more prominent region at or near the accessory basal nucleus, sometimes extending to the adjacent basal nucleus (see arrows in Fig. 5), and (2) a separate, more lateral region within or near the lateral nucleus. These observations suggest that the DN-B regions were predominantly within the basolateral complex, which includes lateral, basal, and accessory basal nuclei.123 Finally, (3) DN-B regions often extended dorsomedially beyond the boundaries of the basolateral complex, towards the medial or ‘superficial’ aspect of the amygdala. We explore this dorsomedial region with targeted analyses in the next section.
To quantify these observations, we computed the spatial similarity of the DN-B map with the amygdala segmentation (Supp. Fig. S4). This analysis confirmed that DN-B regions overlapped most closely with the deeper, basolateral complex structures, and furthermore were predominantly in the accessory basal nucleus. These quantitative results were replicated and triplicated (Supp. Fig. S4). In contrast, when we repeated this analysis for DN-A, we did not observe the same pattern (Supp. Fig. S4). Hence, the individualized segmentation confirmed that, of the two interdigitated networks within the canonical DN, DN-B selectively includes regions in the amygdala, likely within the basolateral complex.
Discovery of DN-B regions at or near the medial nucleus of the amygdala
In many individuals, DN-B regions extended beyond the dorsomedial boundaries of the basolateral complex, at or near where the central and medial nuclei are located. These protrusions strikingly did not overlap with the central nucleus, neither in the individualized (Fig. 5) nor group-defined segmentations122 (not shown). Comparison to the Mai atlas124 suggested these dorsomedial DN-B regions were at or near the medial nucleus (MeA), but also suggested that the automated parcellation had underemphasized (i.e., ascribed too few voxels to) the MeA. We therefore had an expert (author Q.Y.), who was blinded to the network maps, hand-draw the MeA in each participant in accordance with published procedures.122 In contrast to other amygdala nuclei, the medial nucleus can be more easily defined anatomically based on its location relative to the sulcal/septal anatomy and optic tract. The anatomical images used to delineate the MeA, alongside the hand-drawn estimates of the MeA, can be seen in Supp. Fig. S8. Fig. 6 shows that in every participant, including subject S6, the manually drawn MeA either partially overlapped or bordered a region of DN-B. These medial regions of DN-B were located around MNI y = −3 to −5, and thus are likely distinct from the basolateral regions at y = −6 to −9. The regions were bilateral in 3/6 subjects and unilateral, on left or right, in the remaining 3 subjects (Fig. 4). We confirmed that this medial region was selectively connected to DN-B by placing a seed voxel near to the MeA in each subject. In all subjects, this analysis replicated the main components of DN-B (Fig. 7), often robustly. We also replicated and triplicated this medial amygdala region (Supp. Fig. S7). In two subjects (S4, S6) whose medial amygdala regions were unilateral in the discovery dataset, bilateral regions were found in the replication and/or triplication data. The MeA region was not present in subject S1’s replication dataset but was present in their triplication dataset. Conversely, in subject S4 a much clearer MeA region was detected in their replication compared to discovery dataset. These analyses provide reproducible evidence that network DN-B contains a region at or near the medial nucleus of the amygdala.
Figure 6: Hand-drawn segmentation confirms that DN-B regions are at or near the medial nucleus (MeA) of the amygdala.
Figure formatted according to Fig. 5. The MeA was hand drawn by an expert blinded to the network maps, following Noto et al.122 and Mai et al.124 All subjects displayed a region of DN-B that partially overlapped or was adjacent to the estimate of the MeA, and which did not overlap with DN-A (see overlap map).
Figure 7: Seeds targeted to medial amygdala (MeA) selectively reproduce the distributed network DN-B.
The left column shows a zoom-in on a sagittal view of the MTL showing each subject’s T1 and overlap map (similar to Fig. 3). FC maps (colorbar) from seeds targeting the MeA region of DN-B in each individual (white circles) recapitulated the distributed pattern of DN-B (see black outlines denoting surface-defined DN-B shown in Fig. 1), despite the amygdala showing reduced signal-to-noise ratio (Supp. Fig. S1). The strength of correlation varies across individuals (e.g., compare S7 with S1), which could be due to many factors, including data quality, signal dropout, the size of the amygdala region being targeted, and accuracy of seed selection.
Confirmation of DN-B regions in the entorhinal cortex
Our volume-based analyses also revealed a clear region of DN-B at or near the entorhinal cortex (EC; Fig. 3), confirming a recent report.86 This region was observed in five out of six subjects (excluding subject S1 who had noticeably larger dropout in this region) and was confirmed by overlaying the correlation maps with hand-drawn MTL segmentations included in the NSD (not shown). In each subject, the entorhinal regions of DN-B were distinct from adjacent regions of DN-A. The slice locations where entorhinal regions were found ranged from MNI y = −7 to −14. The regions were also bilateral and replicated across datasets (not shown).
Distributed association networks may be interdigitated along the hippocampal formation
In addition to the amygdala and entorhinal regions of DN-B, in two subjects (S2 and S4), we observed evidence that DN-B may include a region in the subiculum (see ‘s’ arrows in Fig. 3 and seed ‘B’ in Fig. 8). This subiculum region of DN-B was adjacent to, but more posterior to a DN-A region in the subiculum that we previously described.70 This was an unexpected finding and raises the prospect that two distributed association networks, DN-A and DN-B, are finely intertwined along the long axis of the MTL. The DN-B subiculum regions were only clearly distinct from DN-A in subjects S2 and S4, and thus need to be replicated in follow-up studies. However, as an exploratory and hypothesis-generating analysis, we targeted seeds to each network along the long axis of the hippocampal formation in these two subjects. Moving from posterior to anterior MTL, Fig. 8 shows that seeds targeting the two networks in parahippocampal cortex (DN-A), subiculum (DN-B), entorhinal cortex (DN-B), subiculum (DN-A), and the basolateral amygdala (DN-B) selectively and often strongly recapitulated the distributed organization of their associated networks (Fig. 8). This analysis emphasizes that the two networks can be defined from multiple parts of the MTL and suggests some level of interdigitation between the networks along the long axis.
Figure 8: Two distributed cortical networks are interdigitated along the long axis of the medial temporal lobe (MTL).
Circles indicate seeds that were hand-selected in two subjects (S2, S4) to target DN-A and DN-B along the MTL. Filled-in circles indicate seeds selected from the slice shown, and hollow circles indicate seeds selected in adjacent or nearby slices. Seeds targeted the (A) parahippocampal cortex (for DN-A), (B) subiculum (DN-B), (C) entorhinal cortex (DN-B), (D) subiculum (DN-A), and (E) basolateral amygdala (DN-B). Surface renderings underneath show the FC maps (colorbar) from the corresponding seeds, with a black border indicating network DN-B as defined in the surface (Fig. 1). In both individuals, seeds A and D reproduced network DN-A, while seeds B, C and E reproduced network DN-B, suggesting that DN-A and DN-B are interdigitated along the MTL. Note that the subiculum region of DN-B (seed B) was more posterior than the subiculum region of DN-A (seed D).
Discussion
This study investigated the connections of the MTL with distributed cortical association networks. We reproduced the observation that the canonical default network comprises two parallel distributed networks, DN-A and DN-B (Fig. 1 and Supp. Fig. S2), and that only one, DN-B, is active during ToM (i.e., thinking about other people’s thoughts; Fig. 2). We discovered that DN-B contains bilateral regions within the amygdala (Figs. 3 & 4) that were previously missed,20,66,70,86,115 and reproduced these findings across (Figs. 4–6) and within participants (Supp. Figs. S6 & S7), and across analysis approaches (Fig. 7 and Supp. Fig. S3). The high-resolution data allowed consideration of network regions in relation to individualized estimates of amygdala nuclei,112,121 which revealed that DN-B regions are located at or near the basolateral complex (Fig. 5), likely at or near the accessory basal nucleus (Supp. Fig. S4).
Potentially distinct regions were also reproducibly observed at or near the lateral nucleus (Fig. 5) and medial nucleus (MeA; Fig. 6), but not the central nucleus (Fig. 5). In contrast, DN-A did not show reliable evidence of containing amygdala regions (Supp. Fig. S4). The results support that the two networks, DN-A and DN-B, can be dissociated by their connections to posterior and anterior MTL. Finally, we observed evidence in 2 participants for closely juxtaposed, alternating regions of DN-A and DN-B along the long axis of the MTL (Figs. 3 & 8), suggesting the networks are interdigitated in the MTL, as they are in the cortex66 (Supp. Fig. S2). The results support that the amygdala and anterior MTL circuitry work with a broader distributed network of association regions (i.e., DN-B) to support socio-cognitive functions.
Social cognitive regions of association cortex are intrinsically connected to the amygdala
Extensive research has described the circuits linking the amygdala, ventral and dorsal medial prefrontal cortex (including orbitofrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex), and temporal pole and their relevance to social cognition110 and psychiatric conditions.125–132 Here, we show that the amygdala-medial prefrontal circuitry is likely a part of a broader distributed association network that includes temporoparietal junction and dorsal posteromedial cortex, regions which are frequently implicated in ToM tasks18,19,24,28 and historically overlap the canonical DN.21,22 Previous human imaging has suggested a connection between the canonical DN (encompassing both DN-A and DN-B) and the amygdala85,133–136 (but see Roy et al.84; Sylvester et al.85; see also fifth figure in Bickart et al.82). Here, we show that it is specifically the portions of the canonical DN that are involved in social cognition (i.e., DN-B; Fig. 2) that are selectively connected to the amygdala. In contrast, the adjacent cortical network, DN-A, which is involved in mental scene construction (Fig. 2), did not show a connection to the amygdala (see overlap maps in Fig. 5 and Supp. Fig. S4), and was instead confirmed to include posterior MTL regions (Figs. 3, 7 & 8). This distinction is notable as DN-A includes regions characteristic of the canonical DN, such as in the ventral posteromedial and retrosplenial cortex, yet here did not show a connection with the amygdala. We propose that these two distributed networks derive their dissociable functions from this differential link to the MTL; anterior MTL regions work selectively with DN-B regions to enable social cognitive functions, while posterior MTL regions work selectively with DN-A regions to enable episodic processes (Fig. 2).
The finding that association areas active during ToM are intrinsically connected to the amygdala, even while participants are at rest in the scanner, is notable. Evidence for amygdala activation during ToM tasks,103–105,137,138 suggests the amygdala is involved in ToM in an “online” capacity (but see Frith & Frith1). In contrast, evidence that only childhood lesions of the amygdala impair ToM100,101 sparked debate regarding whether the amygdala is only necessary during the acquisition of ToM abilities. Here, we show that in healthy adults the amygdala is intrinsically connected to the broader cortical network involved in ToM, supporting an online role. Indeed, our results suggest the connection is observable even in the absence of an active ToM task (i.e., at rest).
DN-B regions are situated at or near the basolateral complex of the amygdala
We observed reproducible evidence (Figs. 5 & 6 and Supp. Fig. S4) supporting that DN-B regions are located within the basolateral complex of the amygdala, which includes lateral, basal, and accessory basal nuclei.123 The basolateral complex is considered the main input structure to the amygdala, receiving widespread connections from the cortex.126,139–142 In contrast to other amygdala nuclei, the basolateral complex has expanded disproportionately in the mammalian lineage, likely due to the disproportionate expansion of neocortical areas providing its input.10,143,144 As DN-B is located deep within association zones that are most expanded in hominins,2,5,145 the presence of DN-B within the basolateral complex might have been predicted from evolutionary expansion patterns alone.
The basolateral complex is densely connected to the entorhinal cortex.142,146 Here, our results accord surprisingly well with this – albeit simplified – architecture: DN-B was shown to be connected to both the basolateral complex (Fig. 5) and entorhinal cortex (Fig. 3). The basolateral DN-B region overlapped most prominently with the accessory basal nucleus (Supp. Fig. S4), often extending into the basal nucleus (Fig. 5), and possibly includes a separate region within ventral portions of the lateral nucleus. Multisensory cortical input to the lateral nucleus is thought to be integrated through internal connections to basal and accessory basal nuclei, before being sent to the central and medial nuclei.120,129,142,147 The observation here of potentially distinct regions in different amygdala nuclei (Figs. 5 & 6) suggests that network DN-B may span multiple stages of the internal amygdala circuitry. Our results also support the conclusions of Aggleton et al.148 that amygdala projections from the orbitofrontal cortex, anterior insula, temporal pole, and anterior cingulate cortex – all of which contain regions of DN-B – predominantly target lateral and accessory basal nuclei (see also eleventh figure in Stefanacci & Amaral147; and sixteenth figure of McDonald142). Notably, in McDonald’s142 diagram, visual areas project to a more dorsal portion of the lateral nucleus, whereas ‘polysensory’ or associative projections are more ventral (Fig. 5). Thus, the specific patterns we observe here in the distribution of DN-B regions across amygdala nuclei appear to have precedent.
DN-B regions are situated at or near the medial nucleus of the amygdala
The central (CeA) and medial (MeA) amygdala are evolutionarily older structures that mature early149 and originate from the subpallium (in contrast to the basolateral complex which is pallial).120 The CeA and MeA contain the main output projections to the autonomic system.120 The medial nucleus is closely linked to the accessory olfactory system in rodents, and projects abundantly to the hypothalamus.150 These projections are important for multiple olfactory-guided social behaviors in rodents, including mating, aggression, and parenting.9,151–153 Though few studies exist on the MeA in humans,122 it likely receives direct projections from the human olfactory bulb,154 suggesting conservation of function across species. Here, our blinded, hand-drawn estimate of the MeA partially overlapped with a DN-B region in every individual (Figs. 6). This region was bilateral (Fig. 4 & Supp. Fig. S7), was confirmed through seeding in the amygdala (Fig. 7), and was replicated and triplicated (Supp. Fig. S7). In contrast, we did not observe regions of DN-B in the CeA, neither using individualized (Fig. 5) nor group-defined (not shown) segmentations. Notably, the locations where we observed MeA regions of DN-B accord surprisingly well with where Brothers et al.92 identified neural responses when monkeys view socially relevant videos (see second figure in Brothers et al.92). Our findings suggest that the medial amygdala is also an important site for social cognitive functions in the human brain. Further supporting this link, in some individuals we detected putative regions of DN-B that may be at or near the ventromedial hypothalamus (see whole-brain images for S2, S3 and S4 in Fig. 6), which is the primary site of hypothalamic projections from the MeA in primates.155 Hence, although these results need to be confirmed in follow-up studies, the specific pattern of DN-B regions appears to recapitulate the proposed circuitry of the MeA from tracer studies.
Networks for social and episodic functions are interdigitated along the medial temporal lobe
Our results support that there are anterior-posterior asymmetries in the network connections of the MTL. Although DN-A and DN-B are adjacent throughout the cortex, DN-B contains visibly larger regions in the anterior midline, whereas DN-A displays larger regions in the posterior midline (Fig. 1 and Supp. Fig. S2). Our results here show that this asymmetry is also evident in the MTL when high-resolution mapping is performed. However, our results do not support a strict anterior-posterior division.36,76,156 Instead, both anterior and posterior MTL regions are connected to two parallel distributed networks that both include regions in posteromedial and anteromedial, as well as posterolateral and anterolateral cortex (Fig. 1).
Our present findings also accord with the idea that the long axis of the MTL differentiates spatial versus non-spatial (or contextual) information. Here, we show that the posterior MTL is more connected to a network involved in mental (spatial) scene construction (i.e., DN-A; Fig. 2), while the anterior MTL is connected to a separate network involved in social (non-spatial) cognitive processing (i.e., DN-B; Fig. 2). In two participants (S2 and S4), we also saw evidence that DN-A and DN-B are connected to adjacent regions of the subiculum (Fig. 8). Interestingly, the subiculum region of DN-B was located in a more posterior location than that of DN-A (i.e., compare seeds ‘B’ and ‘D’ in Fig. 8), a finding which also contradicts a strict anterior-posterior division of the MTL. These findings need to be confirmed in additional individuals. Speculatively, this fine-scale interdigitation might support the integrative model where contextual information from the anterior MTL, is integrated with spatial information in posterior hippocampus, via the subiculum.157–160
Limitations and Technical Considerations
A limitation of the present study is that the distinction between networks relies on the correlation threshold chosen. Here, we focused on network regions that showed clear separation even at low correlation thresholds (0.15 and 0.20), and on findings that replicated across most individuals. The ideal threshold may vary across participants and datasets depending on data quality and smoothness. Here, we chose thresholds that were as low as possible, to limit noise (i.e., speckling) while still allowing regions of higher correlation (e.g., clear clusters of higher correlation) to be observed. Notably, within-network correlations were as high as 0.6 and above (e.g., note in Fig. 3 how high-correlation regions are generally non-overlapping). Hence, the exact threshold used should not limit the interpretation of high- versus low-correlation regions, i.e., preferential connectivity to one network versus another. We previously noted that the degree of overlap between networks decreases as one moves from lower to higher resolution,70 indicating that overlap is a consequence of technical factors.
The replications of amygdala regions (Supp. Figs. S6 & S7) also showed minor variations. Sometimes, the replication and triplication datasets provided clearer evidence for amygdala regions (e.g., see subjects S3 and S4 in Supp. Fig. S6), and occasionally the replications failed (e.g., see subject S1’s replication data in Supp. Fig. S7). We have previously reported that even within individuals and on the cortical surface, where tSNR is higher, minor differences in FC maps are common when different datasets are analyzed.71 We believe these differences are due to small variations in registration (or data quality), meaning that even within individuals the same voxel might be sampling slightly different brain regions in different datasets. This highlights the difficulties of individualized network estimation when detailed anatomy is targeted. Here, we built confidence in our replications by visualizing the volume-defined networks on the surface, which confirmed that the same distributed networks were being targeted (Supp. Fig. S5).
Conclusions
We show that in the human brain the distributed network associated with social cognitive functions is intrinsically connected to the anterior MTL and includes regions in the amygdala, entorhinal cortex, and potentially subiculum. We further show that the social cognitive network may contain multiple regions within the amygdala itself, putatively spanning different processing stages across the basolateral complex and medial nucleus, the latter of which is extensively linked to social behaviors.9,92,151,152 Our findings suggest that two networks within the canonical DN, DN-A and DN-B, which serve episodic and social functions, respectively, can be distinguished by their connectivity to the posterior and anterior MTL, respectively. These findings indicate that specific regions of association cortex derive their social cognitive functions through interactions with the amygdala and anterior MTL. We propose that network-targeted brain stimulation therapies for psychiatric and neurological conditions including autism, schizophrenia, and anxiety and major depressive disorder may be able to influence the amygdala by targeting stimulation to more accessible regions of DN-B in the cortex or cerebellum.161,162
Methods
Overview
Data processing and analysis procedures were previously reported in Kwon et al.118 and Allen et al.111
Experimental model and study participants details
The study includes participants from two independent datasets. The Natural Scenes Dataset111 (NSD) and the Detailed Brain Network Organization (DBNO) dataset. The NSD is comprised of eight healthy human participants (2 males and 6 females; 19–32 years old) recruited from the University of Minnesota community. Sample size was chosen in accordance with the research team’s goal of collecting extensive data from a small number of individual subjects.164 Participants completed an initial screening session and 30–40 MRI sessions that included 2 resting-state fMRI scans alongside a visual image recognition task. The study ended with two task-based scan sessions measuring responses to other task contexts. This resulted in 33–43 MRI sessions per subject along with additional behavioral assessments. Participants provided informed written consent in compliance with procedures approved by the University of Minnesota Institutional Review Board and were paid for their participation.
The DBNO dataset includes 8 healthy human participants (4 females and 4 males; 22–36 years old). These participants were scanned at Northwestern University (downtown Chicago campus) and recruited from the local community. The sample size for this dataset was chosen based on past precision imaging work focused on estimating networks within extensively sampled individuals, while allowing replication across multiple individuals.66,70 Participants completed eight scanning sessions. During each session, participants provided two 7-min resting-state runs resulting in a total of 112 minutes (2 runs × 8 sessions × 7 min) of resting-state data per participant. In between the two resting-state runs in each session, participants completed nine active tasks targeting different cognitive domains. Participants provided written informed consent in compliance with procedures approved by Northwestern University Institutional Review Board and were paid for their participation.
7T fMRI data and quality control
Each participant was scanned across 30–40 sessions, providing an average of 2.0 hours of resting-state fMRI per participant. We performed rigorous quality control following a two-step process. First, head motion was estimated using FSL’s mcflirt command.165 Whole runs were automatically excluded if 1) maximum frame-wise displacement (FD) was greater than 0.4 mm, and 2) maximum absolute head motion (maxAbs) was greater than 2.0 mm. Second, any runs where FD exceeded 0.2 mm or maxAbs exceeded 1.0 mm were visually inspected for head motion and excluded if any could be clearly seen. Following quality control, two of the eight NSD participants were excluded due to excessive head motion. The remaining NSD participants retained 6–35 resting-state runs per participant (S1, 35 runs; S2, 6; S3, 16; S4, 12; S6, 19; S7, 18). For the two participants who provided 12–16 runs, half the data (6–8 runs) were assigned to a discovery dataset and the remaining half were assigned to validation dataset. For subjects with 18–19 runs, one-third of the data were assigned to discovery dataset and remaining two-thirds of the data were assigned to two validation datasets (6–7 runs each). For subject S1 who provided 35 runs, half of the data (17 runs) were assigned to a discovery dataset and the remaining half were assigned to two validation datasets (9 runs each). This is because we initially divided S1’s data into two datasets and began exploring half the data, before determining that stable estimates could be achieved with 6 runs.
7T MRI data acquisition and processing
As reported in Allen et al.111, 7T MRI data were collected using a 7T Siemens Magnetom scanner at the Center for Magnetic Resonance Research at the University of Minnesota. Blood-oxygenation-level-dependent (BOLD) images were collected using gradient-echo echo-planar imaging (EPI) at 1.8-mm isotropic resolution with whole-brain coverage with the following parameters: TR = 1,600 ms, TE = 22.0 ms, Flip angle 62°, FOV = 216 mm (FE) × 216 mm (PE), slice thickness 1.8 mm, slice gap 0 mm, matrix size 120 × 120, echo spacing 0.66 ms, bandwidth 1,736 Hz per pixel, partial Fourier 7/8, iPAT 2, multiband slice acceleration factor = 3, and 84 slices acquired in the axial plane. Dual-echo fieldmaps were collected periodically in each MRI session, and linearly interpolated over time for post-hoc correction of each EPI volume to account for interactions between small shifts in head position and field inhomogeneities (see further details in Allen et al.111). The accuracy of this fieldmap distortion correction was confirmed through comparison to the magnitude image, which is not distorted. Initial pre-processing as part of the NSD release includes slice timing correction, alignment of each volume to correct for head motion within a run, alignment of data across sessions, and correction for EPI distortion. Registration steps were all performed in one interpolation to reduce smoothing. Functional images were co-registered to the T1 image using a non-linear warp implemented in ANTs 2.1.0166 (BSplineSyN with parameters [0.1, 400, 0, 3]). This transformation was visually confirmed to be accurate based on comparisons to the original T1 volume, as outlined in detail in Allen et al.111
For functional connectivity analysis, we performed additional preprocessing on the resting-state data following the steps outlined in the iProc pipeline.70 The first ten volumes (approximately 12 seconds) were removed to accommodate for T1 attenuation effects, and an individual-specific mean BOLD template was generated from included runs. There is a slight flaring of the brain image at the superior aspect in the meanBOLD image (See Fig. 3). This flaring is a result of fieldmap measurement inaccuracies outside the brain and does not affect measurements within the brain. Nuisance variables, including six parameters to account for head motion, as well as whole-brain, ventricular, and deep white matter signal, and their temporal derivatives, were calculated and regressed out of the data. Nuisance regression was performed using 3dTproject (AFNI version 2016.09.04.1341167) on the native-space-projected BOLD data resampled to 1mm isotropic resolution (i.e., the ‘func1p0mm’ version of the NSD data). Data were then bandpass filtered at 0.01–0.1Hz (using 3dBandpass from AFNI). After pre-processing, we utilized a temporal signal to noise ratio (tSNR) map to examine data quality in the MTL. We found that there was little to no dropout in MTL regions, namely the amygdala, as the majority of dropout observed was in more ventral regions of the temporal lobe (See Supp. Fig. S1).
Surface-based functional connectivity analysis at 7T
For surface-based analysis, preprocessed data were projected onto a standardized cortical surface containing 163,842 vertices (fsaverage) per hemisphere using FreeSurfer’s vol2surf command112 and smoothed along the surface using a 2.5mm full-width at half-minimum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel. The highest-resolution fsaverage cortical mesh was used to minimize blurring and preserve fine-scale distinctions between networks. The smoothing kernel was chosen based on preliminary analyses in one individual to optimize the trade-off between minimizing smoothing, maximizing correlation values, and minimizing noise or ‘speckling’ in the correlation maps. Functional connectivity matrices were calculated in each participant by computing vertex-vertex Pearson’s product-moment correlations for each run, r-to-z transforming, and then averaging across runs within each dataset. These matrices were used for surface-based network estimation using an interactive platform for manual seed-selection and correlation map viewing based in Connectome Workbench.168
Our initial analyses focused on identification of DN-A and DN-B based on characteristic anatomical distinctions between the networks reported in Braga & Buckner66 and Braga et al.70, including the replicated distinction between the networks in posteromedial cortex, inferior parietal lobe, and lateral temporal cortex. Briefly, we aimed to identify in each individual a network that follows the expected pattern for DN-A of containing regions in ventral PMC and/or retrosplenial cortex, posterior parahippocampal cortex, and posterior inferior parietal lobe. We also aimed to identify in each individual a network that followed the expected pattern for DN-B, which occupies adjacent, but distinct regions to DN-A in multiple cortical zones. Specifically, DN-B contains regions within more dorsal PMC at or near the posterior cingulate cortex, and more rostral inferior parietal regions at or near the temporoparietal junction. DN-B also contains prominent regions in lateral temporal cortex and inferior frontal cortex. To define the networks, we used two different approaches: a manual seed-based and a data-driven clustering or ‘parcellation’ approach. In the seed-based approach, initial seed selection targeted the dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex to (i) align with our previous studies and (ii) allow characteristic patterns of functional connectivity to be appreciated in distal zones (e.g., in the MTL and posteromedial cortex) without the confound of local blurring near the seed. To limit the effects of observer bias in the manual seed-selection process, we also employed a multi-session hierarchical Bayesian model (MS-HBM) parcellation method.114 In each individual, we used a k value (i.e., number of clusters) between 14–20 and selected the solution that best matched the networks observed by the seed-based approach (k = 14). The clustering analysis provides a data-driven confirmation of the patterns observed in the manual seed-based approach, offering converging evidence for the distinction between DN-A and DN-B.
Volume-based functional connectivity analysis at 7T
To examine regions of DN-A and DN-B in the MTL, we estimated the networks using a seed-based approach in the volume. We analyzed native-space projected volumetric BOLD data from the NSD that was preprocessed for functional connectivity analysis prior to surface projection. Based on data quality and the strength of correlation maps observed in initial explorations, a 2.5mm FWHM smoothing kernel was applied to five subjects (S1-S4,S6) and a 2mm FWHM smoothing kernel was applied to one subject (S7) using fslmaths (FSL v6.0.3).165 Data were analyzed and visualized using AFNI’s InstaCorr.167,169 3dSetUpGroupInCorr was used to calculate a Pearson’s product-moment correlation coefficient between all voxel pairs within a whole-brain brain mask for each run of resting-state data. Each matrix was Fisher transformed prior to cross-run averaging with 3dGroupInCorr to create a single, cross-run average functional connectivity matrix for each dataset from each individual. We used AFNI to interactively select individual voxels as seeds and observe their associated whole-brain correlation maps. This process was used to define DN-A and DN-B in the discovery dataset. Initially, we converted the surface-selected seed vertex locations that defined DN-A and DN-B into approximate X, Y, Z voxel indices to define the same networks in the volume. Perhaps because the surface projection applies an interpolation which means that a surface vertex may not correspond directly to a single voxel, these converted seed locations did not produce maps with the expected separation of DN-A and DN-B as seen in our prior work.70 We therefore manually explored the surrounding voxels for a seed that better distinguished the two networks. Seed optimization was performed by consulting cortical correlation patterns and focusing on the anatomy of regions within the posteromedial cortex. We explicitly did not consult the MTL during seed selection, and selected candidate DN-A and DN-B seeds based on their degree of overlap with surface-defined maps from the data-driven clustering analysis (e.g., see Figs, 1, 7 & 8).
Overlap maps were created by thresholding the correlation maps at z(r) > 0.3 and then binarizing each network separately. The DN-B binarized map was multiplied by a scalar value of 2, from which we then subtracted from the binarized DN-A map. This created an image where network DN-A’s voxels had a value of −1, network DN-B’s voxels had a value of +2 and the overlap voxels had a value of +1. The overlap maps were visualized in wb_view168 using the PSYCH color bar set to range from −2.5 to 0 and 0 to +20.
To aid the detection of network regions in consistent anatomical location across subjects, we projected the volume-defined functional connectivity maps, mean-BOLD, and T1 images to MNI152 space. Projection was done using the nsd_mapdata commands provided with the NSD, which applied precomputed transforms using nearest neighbor interpolation.111
Replication and Triplication at 7T
To ensure robustness of findings, after analysis of the discovery datasets the results were replicated and triplicated within the same individuals. Initially, the same seed vertices used to define the networks in the discovery dataset were used to define the networks in the left-out datasets. This broadly replicated the definition of the same networks, but sometimes reduced the differentiation between DN-A and DN-B compared to what was seen in the discovery dataset, or in our previous work at 7T.70 Hence, we optimized the seeds in the validation datasets by searching for a nearby seed voxel that maximized correlations within characteristic DN-B regions and minimized correlations in DN-A regions. Again, we explicitly did not consult the MTL in the seed selection procedure, and based seed selection on the distinction between the networks observed in the posteromedial cortex, as well as with the degree of overlap with surface-defined networks from the data-driven clustering analysis. We have previously noted that the optimal seed location to target a given network can change slightly across datasets, even from the same individual, potentially due to small registration differences across datasets.
Determining overlap with amygdala nuclei
To navigate the anatomy of the MTL and quantify overlap of network regions with specific anatomical regions, we utilized (i) a previously published hand-drawn atlas of amygdala subregions in group-averaged MNI152 space,122 (ii) hand-drawn maps of MTL regions that are included in the NSD and were defined within each individual (but do not include the amygdala), (iii) an automated Amygdala segmentation that was calculated using FreeSurfer (7.3.2),121 and (iv) a hand-drawn estimate of the medial nucleus by an expert observer (author Q.Y.) who was blind to the network maps. The FreeSurfer tool uses a procedure where a segmentation of the amygdala into 9 nuclei, defined from super-high-resolution (100–150 μm) 7T imaging in 10 post-mortem specimens, is template-matched to anatomical images from each individual. The tool therefore provided the location of 9 amygdala nuclei in native space T1, including the accessory basal (AB), basal (BA), lateral (LA), medial (fs-MeA), central (CeA), cortical (Cort), cortico-amygdaloid-transition (Acot), anterior amygdaloid area (Ant), and paralaminar (PL). Atlas regions were projected into MNI space for visualization alongside DN-B and DN-A regions using nsdmapdata111 and fslswapdim (FSL).165 The resulting parcellation was cross-checked with the Mai atlas124 for accuracy.
Manual delineation of the medial amygdala nucleus
Because the FreeSurfer amygdala parcellation ascribed few voxels in each individual to the medial nucleus (MeA), we had an expert observer hand-draw the MeA bilaterally in each individual based on procedures and anatomical landmarks reported in Noto et al.122 and Mai et al.124 This observer (author Q.Y.) was blinded to the network maps. The MeA was drawn based on the anatomy captured by each participant’s T1 image. The T1 image projected to 1-mm MNI space was used, to adhere to Noto et al.122 The anterior limit of MeA coincides with the posterior edge of temporal piriform cortex,124 typically around MNI coordinate y = −4. First, the posterior limit of piriform cortex was identified,170 and the anterior edge of the MeA was marked on the next coronal slice, 1.0 mm in the posterior direction from the posterior edge of piriform cortex. In this coronal slice, the surface of the entorhinal sulcus changes from flat and smooth when contiguous with piriform cortex, to angled and exhibiting a distinct small bump, which indicates the location of the cortical amygdala. This bump is visible in conventional T1 images. Based on Mai et al.124, from this anterior edge, the MeA region of interest was extended 3–4 mm into the amygdala for the posterior border of the MeA. The posterior border of the MeA ROI was identified using the optic nerve as a landmark. Specifically, the MeA continues in the posterior direction until the optic nerve nearly touches the amygdala surface, and the anterior border of the hippocampus becomes visible. In the lateral extent, the MeA extends 4mm into the amygdala based on Mai et al.124
Quantifying overlap with amygdala nuclei
To quantify the overlap between the network regions and the amygdala segmentation, in each subject, we restricted the analysis to a mask based on the freesurfer parcellation that included the whole amygdala. Within this mask, we binarized the thresholded correlation maps, using our previously defined thresholds (Fig. 4). We then used the dice similarity function in MATLAB to compare spatial overlap between the binarized correlation maps and each atlas region (i.e., nuclei). This was repeated for the replication and triplication datasets to provide up to three independent estimates of the overlap in each participant. We also assessed overlap with the group-average atlas of the amygdala, obtained from Noto et al.122, which broadly replicated that DN-B regions were at or near the basolateral complex and medial nucleus, but not central nucleus (not shown). The correspondence observed using the group-average amygdala atlas was not as clear as the individualized maps, which showed consistent relationships across participants (Supp. Fig. S4).
Replication of network distinction and functional dissociation at 3T
Analysis of resting-state data from the Detailed Brain Network Organization (DBNO) dataset was previously reported in Kwon et al.118 Eight individuals were scanned at Northwestern University. Participants were native English speakers, neurologically healthy, and had normal or corrected-to-normal vision (4 females, age range 22–36 years, mean age = 26.8). Participants provided written informed consent in compliance with procedures approved by Northwestern University Institutional Review Board and were paid for their participation. The experiment consisted of eight MRI sessions, collected over approximately 18 – 63 days, based on participant availability and avoiding consecutive days. During each session, participants completed two 7-min resting-state runs resulting in a total of 112 minutes (2 runs × 8 sessions × 7 min) of resting-state data per participant. During the resting state runs, participants were asked to fixate their eyes on a cross at the center of the screen. The position of the screen was adjusted for each participant and session to ensure a comfortable viewing angle to minimize head motion. The resting-state runs were collected as the first and last runs in each session.
Acquisition, processing, and quality control of 3T MRI data
Functional images were collected at the Center for Translational Imaging at Northwestern University on a 3T Siemens Prisma scanner. A high-resolution T1-weighted magnetization-prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo (TR = 2,100 ms, TE = 2.9 ms, FOV = 256 mm, flip angle = 8°, slice thickness = 1 mm, 176 sagittal slices parallel to the AC-PC line) was acquired after the first resting-state run. Multi-echo BOLD data171,172 were collected using a 64-channel head coil with the following parameters: TR = 1,355 ms, TE = 12.80 ms, 32.39 ms, 51.98 ms, 71.57 ms, and 91.16 ms, flip angle = 64°, voxel size = 2.4 mm, FOV = 216 mm × 216 mm, slice thickness = 2.4 mm, multiband slice acceleration factor = 6.
Functional MRI data were pre-processed using the iProc pipeline70 with changes implemented to accommodate multi-echo data described below. The iProc pipeline is optimized for alignment of data from different sessions and runs within each individual, using individualized registration templates and with registration steps performed in a single interpolation to minimize blurring and preserve anatomical details. Runs with excessive head motion (a maximum framewise displacement > 0.2 mm or a maximum absolute displacement > 1 mm) were visually inspected and excluded if they exhibited clear movement. This resulted in a total of 10–16 resting-state runs per participant (S1: 16; S2: 16; S3: 16; S4: 6; S5: 10; S6: 15, S7: 14; and S8: 14 runs). For the seven participants providing 12 or more runs, the data were divided into two groups: a discovery dataset (7–8 runs) and a validation dataset (7–8 runs). The DBNO analyses focused on the discovery dataset only.
The first nine volumes (approximately 12 seconds) were removed to account for the T1 attenuation artifact and a mean BOLD template was generated for each individual using the included runs (see details in Braga et al.70). Functional data were registered to a 1.0-mm isotropic resolution T1 image for each individual via this mean BOLD template, including within-run motion correction and across-run and -session alignment. Brain extraction was performed on the T1 using FSL’s Brain Extraction Tool (FSL v6.0.3). White matter and cerebrospinal fluid masks were registered to the skull-stripped mean BOLD template to calculate mean white matter, cerebrospinal fluid, and whole brain signal time series. These mean time series, along with 6 head motion parameters and temporal derivatives, were used to remove nuisance signals through regression. Data were bandpass filtered at 0.01–0.10 Hz. Preprocessed data were then projected onto a standardized cortical surface (fsaverage6: 40,962 vertices per hemisphere) using Freesurfer112 and smoothed along the surface using a 2mm FWHM Gaussian kernel.
Data collection for social cognitive tasks
In each session, DBNO participants completed nine active tasks targeting different cognitive domains. Two tasks targeted social cognitive or ‘theory of mind’ (ToM) processes: False Belief and Emotional Pain,15,16 and were previously described in DiNicola et al.20 Each task lasted 319 seconds per run, with 4 runs of each task collected per subject. Each run included 10 trials. The False Belief task required participants to make inferences about another person’s beliefs in either social (false belief condition) or visual (false photo condition) contexts.15 For the false belief condition, participants responded true or false to prompts, such as: “Lillian titled her novel How to Cook a Grouse and submitted it. Her publisher retitled the novel How to Cook a Pheasant. In the bookstore, Lillian notices the title is How to Cook a Grouse, True or False?” For the false photo condition, participants responded to true or false questions, such as: “A woman’s drawing of her back yard included a blue treehouse. That was before the storm. She built a new treehouse last summer and painted it red. Today the treehouse is blue, True or False?” The Emotional Pain task required participants to provide a rating for another person’s emotional or physical pain. To rate emotional pain participants responded to prompts such as: “Alison lives by herself. Every week her son comes to visit her and they go out to lunch. Her son just lost his job and is arguing with his wife. One day Alison gets a call from her son who tells her that he has been diagnosed with cancer. Rate protagonist pain or suffering.”16 To rate physical pain participants responded to prompts such as: “Suzie was riding in a cab. When she opened the door and began to step out, a child accidentally bumped the door and it close on Suzie, smashing her leg.” Participants were trained on the tasks outside the scanner prior to the first session and were given an example trial in the scanner prior to the start of the first run in each session, to accustom them to the pace of the task. For both tasks, each run started with 18s of fixation to a centrally presented crosshair (‘+’), and each trial included 10s of story presentation, and a 5-second questions presentation. There was 15 seconds of fixation in between each trial. Trial and block order was counterbalanced within and across runs (see DiNicola et al.65).
Data collection for mental scene construction task
In each DBNO session, participants also completed a mental imagery task. Participants were asked to covertly imagine scenarios based on a specific prompt provided on a screen. Each run comprised 25 trials. In each trial, participants saw a written description of a scenario, such as “Imagine a nature trail in the summer,” and were instructed to imagine the scenario in their mind. The imagination period lasted 7s. After the imagining period, participants were given three seconds to rate the visual vividness of their imagined scenario on a four-point scale (“Nothing”, “Vague”, “Moderate”, and “Vivid”), followed by an additional three seconds to rate the auditory vividness of their imagining on the same scale. In half of the runs, the auditory vividness rating preceded the visual vividness rating. After both ratings, a fixation cross was presented for 10–11 seconds (jittered), followed by the next trial. Each run began with 18s of fixation to a crosshair (‘+’), and the mental imagery task was always the second-to-last task of the session (followed by a final resting-state run). Each run lasted 614 seconds and eight runs were collected from each subject.
After the MRI scanning session, participants immediately completed an out-of-scanner follow-up task on a computer. In this task, participants were re-presented with the 25 scenarios they had seen during the in-scanner mental imagery task. For each scenario, participants were asked to provide ratings to questions regarding their experience imagining inside the scanner. First, participants repeated their visual and auditory vividness ratings (using the same scale), which confirmed that participants were able to replicate their in-scanner responses in the out-of-scanner test (correlation between in- and out-of-scanner vividness ratings: S1, 0.81; S2, 0.93; S3, 0.81; S4, 0.79; S5, 0.69; S6, 0.92; S7, 0.61; S8, 0.7). Participants then provided ratings on 15 additional questions using a 9-point endorsement scale (ranging from Strongly Disagree to Strongly Agree). These questions probed additional aspects of the imaging experience such as the degree to which they thought about spatial dimensions (i.e., “I envisioned the location of objects, people or places”), their level of immersion (i.e., “I felt immersed in my thoughts, as if I was experiencing the item(s) in real life”), and how successful they were at imagining the scenario (i.e., “I was able to imagine what was asked successfully”). Participants were given unlimited time to provide their responses for each scenario and question, and on average spent 21.9 minutes on each follow-up session (range: 12.3 – 48.6 minutes).
Resting-State Functional Connectivity Analysis at 3T
Within each surface-projected resting-state run, vertex-wise Pearson’s product-moment correlations were computed to generate a correlation matrix. Correlation matrices were r-to-z transformed before averaging across runs within each participant. We defined DN-A and DN-B in the 3T data using the same process as described in the 7T data, including seed-based and data-driven clustering approaches. The MS-HBM parcellation method was again applied to this dataset to define networks,163 which is supposed to improve stability in the network estimation procedure. In each individual, we used a k value between 14–20 and selected the solution (k = 14) that best matched the networks observed by the seed-based approach.
Task-Based Analysis at 3T
Task data were quality controlled for head motion and tSNR using the same thresholds and procedures used for the resting-state data. Runs that passed quality control for MRI data were then assessed for behavioral performance. Behavioral performance was computed for each participant and task. Error rates include invalid responses (e.g., multiple button presses), non-responses, and incorrect responses. For the False Belief task, runs with <80% accuracy were excluded from further analysis resulting in the following remaining runs for each subject: S1, 4 runs; S2, 4; S3, 2; S4, 0; S5, 4; S6, 3; S7, 1. Error rates across the remaining runs were S1, 7.5%; S2, 5.0%; S3, 0.0%; S4, n/a; S5, 10.0%; S6, 6.7%; S7, 15.0%; S8, 0.0%). For the Pain Task, which involved a subjective rating, error rates included invalid and non-responses, and were as follows: S1, 0%; S2, 0%; S3, 2.5%; S4, 0%; S5, 0%; S6, 0%; S7, 2.5%; S8, 0%). No runs were excluded from the Pain task. An initial analysis that included all runs across all subjects (i.e., including runs with high error rate) produced very similar results to those in Fig. 2.
For the mental imagery task, after exclusion due to head motion and tSNR criteria each participant retained 6–8 out of 8 runs (S1, 8 runs; S2, 8; S3, 6; S4, 8; S5, 6; S7, 6; S8, 7). As this task was analyzed in a trial-wise manner, error trials (missed or invalid responses) were excluded trial-wise. Participants on average provided both visual and vividness ratings for 96.4% of trials per run (S1, 100%; S2, 100%; S3, 100%; S4, 92%; S5, 95%; S6, 100%; S7, 86%; S8, 97%). Any trial missing at least one response was excluded from analyses, but was still modelled in FEAT.
Task BOLD data were analyzed using a general linear model (GLM) as implemented in FSL’s FEAT.173 For the ToM tasks, each condition was modelled as a single explanatory variable that included both the story and question periods of each trial. For the mental imagery task, each trial element was modeled individually, with separate explanatory variables for each trial’s imagining, response, and post-response fixation periods. Explanatory variables were convolved with a double-gamma hemodynamic response function. In the False Belief task, the False Belief condition was contrasted against the False Photo condition. In the Pain task, the Emotional Pain condition was contrasted against the Physical Pain condition. In the mental imagery task, each imagining period was contrasted against its preceding fixation period to create a beta estimate for each trial.
Task contrast maps for the ToM tasks were converted to t-statistics and then to a z-statistic, then averaged together across runs within each participant using fslmaths.165 Next, we averaged together the contrast maps from the False Belief and Emotional Pain tasks using fslmaths (FSL v6.0.3)165 to create a single map of ToM-related activity per subject. We overlaid this map against the outline of the functional connectivity-defined estimate of DN-B to assess the extent of overlap between the two maps. The close correspondence between maps (Fig. 2) replicated the findings of DiNicola et al.20 that DN-B, as defined using functional connectivity, captures brain regions involved in social cognitive processes.
For the mental imagery task, a behavioral summed composite metric was created for each trial based on the participant’s responses to the visual vividness (in-scanner) and spatial (out-of-scanner) ratings. These closely align with the “Scene Construction” composite reported in DiNicola et al.65 using a similar approach. For each subject, trials were identified with the highest and lowest Scene Construction scores (top 20% of usable trials and bottom 20% of usable trials; if multiple trials at the cut-off margins had the same score, we randomly selected which were included. Beta values for trials in the bottom 20% of composite scores were multiplied by −1, and then all trials (top and bottom 20%) were averaged together to create a contrast map between high-Scene Construction and low-Scene Construction trials. The contrast (beta) map was then z-scored to create a z-stat map. We compared this map to the functional connectivity-defined estimate of DN-A to determine how Scene Construction-related activity overlaps with the anatomy of DN-A. The close correspondence between maps (Fig. 2) confirms that DN-A, as defined using functional connectivity, can delineate brain regions involved in mental scene construction (i.e. “episodic projection” or EP).
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We thank Randy Buckner and Lauren DiNicola for helpful discussions. This work was supported in part by National Institute of Mental Health grant R00 MH117226 (R.M.B.) and an Alzheimer’s Disease Core Center grant (P30 AG013854) pilot award (R.M.B.) from the National Institute on Aging to Northwestern University, Chicago, Illinois.; a training award T32 NS047987 (to J.J.S. and N.A.); the William Orr Dingwall Foundations of Language Fellowship (J.J.S); and NSF GFRP award DGE-2234667 (D.E.). Collection of the NSD dataset was supported by NSF IIS-1822683 and NSF IIS-1822929 (to K.K.). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health or National Science Foundation. This research was supported in part through the computational resources and staff contributions provided for the Quest high performance computing facility at Northwestern University which is jointly supported by the Office of the Provost, the Office for Research, and Northwestern University Information Technology, and the Center for Translational Imaging at Northwestern University.
Footnotes
Declaration of Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
References
- 1.Frith C.D., and Frith U. (2012). Mechanisms of Social Cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 63, 287–313. 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Hill J., Inder T., Neil J., Dierker D., Harwell J., and Van Essen D. (2010). Similar patterns of cortical expansion during human development and evolution. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 107, 13135–13140. 10.1073/pnas.1001229107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Chaplin T.A., Yu H.-H., Soares J.G.M., Gattass R., and Rosa M.G.P. (2013). A Conserved Pattern of Differential Expansion of Cortical Areas in Simian Primates. J. Neurosci. 33, 15120–15125. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2909-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Amlien I.K., Fjell A.M., Tamnes C.K., Grydeland H., Krogsrud S.K., Chaplin T.A., Rosa M.G.P., and Walhovd K.B. (2016). Organizing Principles of Human Cortical Development—Thickness and Area from 4 to 30 Years: Insights from Comparative Primate Neuroanatomy. Cereb. Cortex 26, 257–267. 10.1093/cercor/bhu214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.DiNicola L.M., and Buckner R.L. (2021). Precision estimates of parallel distributed association networks: evidence for domain specialization and implications for evolution and development. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 40, 120–129. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.03.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Dunbar R.I.M. (2016). The Social Brain Hypothesis and Human Evolution. Oxf. Res. Encycl. Psychol. 10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.44. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Klüver H., and Bucy P.C. (1939). Preliminary Analysis of Functions of the Temporal Lobes in Monkeys. Arch. Neurol. Psychiatry 42, 979–1000. 10.1001/archneurpsyc.1939.02270240017001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Brown S., and Sharpey-Schafer E.A. (1888). XI. An investigation into the functions of the occipital and temporal lobes of the monkey’s brain. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 179, 303–327. 10.1098/rstb.1888.0011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Raam T., and Hong W. (2021). Organization of neural circuits underlying social behavior: A consideration of the medial amygdala. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 68, 124–136. 10.1016/j.conb.2021.02.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Janak P.H., and Tye K.M. (2015). From circuits to behaviour in the amygdala. Nature 517, 284–292. 10.1038/nature14188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kwong K.K., Belliveau J.W., Chesler D.A., Goldberg I.E., Weisskoff R.M., Poncelet B.P., Kennedy D.N., Hoppel B.E., Cohen M.S., and Turner R. (1992). Dynamic magnetic resonance imaging of human brain activity during primary sensory stimulation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 89, 5675–5679. 10.1073/pnas.89.12.5675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Ogawa S., Tank D.W., Menon R., Ellermann J.M., Kim S.G., Merkle H., and Ugurbil K. (1992). Intrinsic signal changes accompanying sensory stimulation: functional brain mapping with magnetic resonance imaging. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 89, 5951–5955. 10.1073/pnas.89.13.5951. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Wimmer H., and Perner J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition 13, 103–128. 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Baron-Cohen S., Leslie A.M., and Frith U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind” ? Cognition 21, 37–46. 10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Dodell-Feder D., Koster-Hale J., Bedny M., and Saxe R. (2011). fMRI item analysis in a theory of mind task. NeuroImage 55, 705–712. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Jacoby N., Bruneau E., Koster-Hale J., and Saxe R. (2016). Localizing Pain Matrix and Theory of Mind networks with both verbal and non-verbal stimuli. NeuroImage 126, 39–48. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Saxe R., and Kanwisher N. (2003). People thinking about thinking people: The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind.” NeuroImage 19, 1835–1842. 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00230-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.van Veluw S.J., and Chance S.A. (2014). Differentiating between self and others: an ALE meta-analysis of fMRI studies of self-recognition and theory of mind. Brain Imaging Behav. 8, 24–38. 10.1007/s11682-013-9266-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Schurz M., Radua J., Aichhorn M., Richlan F., and Perner J. (2014). Fractionating theory of mind: a meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 42, 9–34. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.DiNicola L.M., Braga R.M., and Buckner R.L. (2020). Parallel distributed networks dissociate episodic and social functions within the individual. J. Neurophysiol. 123, 1144–1179. 10.1152/jn.00529.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Buckner R.L., Andrews-Hanna J.R., and Schacter D.L. (2008). The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1124, 1–38. 10.1196/annals.1440.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Mars R., Neubert F.-X., Noonan M., Sallet J., Toni I., and Rushworth M. (2012). On the relationship between the “default mode network” and the “social brain.” Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Vogeley K., Bussfeld P., Newen A., Herrmann S., Happé F., Falkai P., Maier W., Shah N.J., Fink G.R., and Zilles K. (2001). Mind Reading: Neural Mechanisms of Theory of Mind and Self-Perspective. NeuroImage 14, 170–181. 10.1006/nimg.2001.0789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Saxe R. (2006). Uniquely human social cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 16, 235–239. 10.1016/j.conb.2006.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Svoboda E., McKinnon M.C., and Levine B. (2006). The functional neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 44, 2189–2208. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.05.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.D’Argembeau A., Collette F., Van der Linden M., Laureys S., Del Fiore G., Degueldre C., Luxen A., and Salmon E. (2005). Self-referential reflective activity and its relationship with rest: a PET study. NeuroImage 25, 616–624. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.11.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Gusnard D.A., and Raichle M.E. (2001). Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 685–694. 10.1038/35094500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Amodio D.M., and Frith C.D. (2006). Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 268–277. 10.1038/nrn1884. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Schacter D.L., Addis D.R., Hassabis D., Martin V.C., Spreng R.N., and Szpunar K.K. (2012). The Future of Memory: Remembering, Imagining, and the Brain. Neuron 76, 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.11.001. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Wheeler M.E., and Buckner R.L. (2004). Functional-anatomic correlates of remembering and knowing. NeuroImage 21, 1337–1349. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Wagner A.D., Shannon B.J., Kahn I., and Buckner R.L. (2005). Parietal lobe contributions to episodic memory retrieval. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 445–453. 10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Raichle M.E., MacLeod A.M., Snyder A.Z., Powers W.J., Gusnard D.A., and Shulman G.L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 98, 676–682. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Buckner R.L., and DiNicola L.M. (2019). The brain’s default network: updated anatomy, physiology and evolving insights. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 20, 593–608. 10.1038/s41583-019-0212-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Biswal B., Zerrin Yetkin F., Haughton V.M., and Hyde J.S. (1995). Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar mri. Magn. Reson. Med. 34, 537–541. 10.1002/mrm.1910340409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Greicius M.D., Krasnow B., Reiss A.L., and Menon V. (2003). Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 100, 253–258. 10.1073/pnas.0135058100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Kahn I., Andrews-Hanna J.R., Vincent J.L., Snyder A.Z., and Buckner R.L. (2008). Distinct Cortical Anatomy Linked to Subregions of the Medial Temporal Lobe Revealed by Intrinsic Functional Connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 100, 129–139. 10.1152/jn.00077.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Hassabis D., and Maguire E.A. (2009). The construction system of the brain. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 364, 1263–1271. 10.1098/rstb.2008.0296. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Shulman G.L., Fiez J.A., Corbetta M., Buckner R.L., Miezin F.M., Raichle M.E., and Petersen S.E. (1997). Common Blood Flow Changes across Visual Tasks: II. Decreases in Cerebral Cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 9, 648–663. 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Spreng R.N., McKinnon M.C., Mar R.A., and Levine B. (2009). The Toronto Empathy Questionnaire. J. Pers. Assess. 91, 62–71. 10.1080/00223890802484381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Andreasen N.C., O’Leary D.S., Cizadlo T., Arndt S., Rezai K., Watkins G.L., Ponto L.L., and Hichwa R.D. (1995). Remembering the past: two facets of episodic memory explored with positron emission tomography. Am. J. Psychiatry 152, 1576–1585. 10.1176/ajp.152.11.1576. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Andrews-Hanna J.R., Smallwood J., and Spreng R.N. (2014). The default network and self-generated thought: component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1316, 29–52. 10.1111/nyas.12360. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Buckner R.L., and Carroll D.C. (2007). Self-projection and the brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 49–57. 10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Laird A.R., Fox P.M., Eickhoff S.B., Turner J.A., Ray K.L., McKay D.R., Glahn D.C., Beckmann C.F., Smith S.M., and Fox P.T. (2011). Behavioral Interpretations of Intrinsic Connectivity Networks. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 4022–4037. 10.1162/jocn_a_00077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Schilbach L., Eickhoff S.B., Rotarska-Jagiela A., Fink G.R., and Vogeley K. (2008). Minds at rest? Social cognition as the default mode of cognizing and its putative relationship to the “default system” of the brain. Conscious. Cogn. 17, 457–467. 10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Li W., Mai X., and Liu C. (2014). The default mode network and social understanding of others: what do brain connectivity studies tell us. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Smallwood J., Bernhardt B.C., Leech R., Bzdok D., Jefferies E., and Margulies D.S. (2021). The default mode network in cognition: a topographical perspective. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 22, 503–513. 10.1038/s41583-021-00474-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Hassabis D., and Maguire E.A. (2007). Deconstructing episodic memory with construction. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 299–306. 10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Fedorenko E., Behr M.K., and Kanwisher N. (2011). Functional specificity for high-level linguistic processing in the human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 108, 16428–16433. 10.1073/pnas.1112937108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Steinmetz H., and Seitz R.J. (1991). Functional anatomy of language processing: Neuroimaging and the problem of individual variability. Neuropsychologia 29, 1149–1161. 10.1016/0028-3932(91)90030-C. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Gratton C., Kraus B.T., Greene D.J., Gordon E.M., Laumann T.O., Nelson S.M., Dosenbach N.U.F., and Petersen S.E. (2020). Defining Individual-Specific Functional Neuroanatomy for Precision Psychiatry. Biol. Psychiatry 88, 28–39. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.10.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Fedorenko E., Hsieh P.-J., Nieto-Castañón A., Whitfield-Gabrieli S., and Kanwisher N. (2010). New Method for fMRI Investigations of Language: Defining ROIs Functionally in Individual Subjects. J. Neurophysiol. 104, 1177–1194. 10.1152/jn.00032.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Andrews-Hanna J.R., Reidler J.S., Sepulcre J., Poulin R., and Buckner R.L. (2010). Functional-Anatomic Fractionation of the Brain’s Default Network. Neuron 65, 550–562. 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Leech R., Braga R., and Sharp D.J. (2012). Echoes of the Brain within the Posterior Cingulate Cortex. J. Neurosci. 32, 215–222. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3689-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Braga R.M., Sharp D.J., Leeson C., Wise R.J.S., and Leech R. (2013). Echoes of the Brain within Default Mode, Association, and Heteromodal Cortices. J. Neurosci. 33, 14031–14039. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0570-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Kernbach J.M., Yeo B.T.T., Smallwood J., Margulies D.S., Thiebaut de Schotten M., Walter H., Sabuncu M.R., Holmes A.J., Gramfort A., Varoquaux G., et al. (2018). Subspecialization within default mode nodes characterized in 10,000 UK Biobank participants. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 115, 12295–12300. 10.1073/pnas.1804876115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Rabin J.S., Gilboa A., Stuss D.T., Mar R.A., and Rosenbaum R.S. (2010). Common and Unique Neural Correlates of Autobiographical Memory and Theory of Mind. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 1095–1111. 10.1162/jocn.2009.21344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Spreng R.N., and Grady C.L. (2010). Patterns of Brain Activity Supporting Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, and Theory of Mind, and Their Relationship to the Default Mode Network. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 1112–1123. 10.1162/jocn.2009.21282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Wen T., Duncan J., and Mitchell D.J. (2020). Hierarchical Representation of Multistep Tasks in Multiple-Demand and Default Mode Networks. J. Neurosci. 40, 7724–7738. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0594-20.2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Kurczek J., Wechsler E., Ahuja S., Jensen U., Cohen N.J., Tranel D., and Duff M. (2015). Differential contributions of hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex to self-projection and self-referential processing. Neuropsychologia 73, 116–126. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.05.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Fairhall S.L., and Caramazza A. (2013). Brain Regions That Represent Amodal Conceptual Knowledge. J. Neurosci. 33, 10552–10558. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0051-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Peer M., Salomon R., Goldberg I., Blanke O., and Arzy S. (2015). Brain system for mental orientation in space, time, and person. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 112, 11072–11077. 10.1073/pnas.1504242112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Silson E.H., Steel A., Kidder A., Gilmore A.W., and Baker C.I. (2019). Distinct subdivisions of human medial parietal cortex support recollection of people and places. eLife 8, e47391. 10.7554/eLife.47391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Deen B., and Freiwald W.A. (2021). Parallel systems for social and spatial reasoning within the cortical apex. Preprint at bioRxiv, 10.1101/2021.09.23.461550 10.1101/2021.09.23.461550. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Schacter D.L., and Addis D.R. (2007). The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: remembering the past and imagining the future. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 362, 773–786. 10.1098/rstb.2007.2087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.DiNicola L.M., Ariyo O.I., and Buckner R.L. (2023). Functional specialization of parallel distributed networks revealed by analysis of trial-to-trial variation in processing demands. J. Neurophysiol. 129, 17–40. 10.1152/jn.00211.2022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Braga R.M., and Buckner R.L. (2017). Parallel Interdigitated Distributed Networks within the Individual Estimated by Intrinsic Functional Connectivity. Neuron 95, 457–471.e5. 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Laumann T.O., Gordon E.M., Adeyemo B., Snyder A.Z., Joo S.J., Chen M.-Y., Gilmore A.W., McDermott K.B., Nelson S.M., Dosenbach N.U.F., et al. (2015). Functional System and Areal Organization of a Highly Sampled Individual Human Brain. Neuron 87, 657–670. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.06.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Gordon E.M., Laumann T.O., Gilmore A.W., Newbold D.J., Greene D.J., Berg J.J., Ortega M., Hoyt-Drazen C., Gratton C., Sun H., et al. (2017). Precision Functional Mapping of Individual Human Brains. Neuron 95, 791–807.e7. 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.07.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Fedorenko E. (2021). The early origins and the growing popularity of the individual-subject analytic approach in human neuroscience. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 40, 105–112. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.02.023. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Braga R.M., Van Dijk K.R.A., Polimeni J.R., Eldaief M.C., and Buckner R.L. (2019). Parallel distributed networks resolved at high resolution reveal close juxtaposition of distinct regions. J. Neurophysiol. 121, 1513–1534. 10.1152/jn.00808.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Salvo J.J., Holubecki A.M., and Braga R.M. (2021). Correspondence between functional connectivity and task-related activity patterns within the individual. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 40, 178–188. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.05.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Striedter G.F. (2005). Principles of brain evolution (Sinauer Associates; ). [Google Scholar]
- 73.Strange B.A., Witter M.P., Lein E.S., and Moser E.I. (2014). Functional organization of the hippocampal longitudinal axis. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 655–669. 10.1038/nrn3785. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Poppenk J., Evensmoen H.R., Moscovitch M., and Nadel L. (2013). Long-axis specialization of the human hippocampus. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 230–240. 10.1016/j.tics.2013.03.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Ryan L., Lin C.-Y., Ketcham K., and Nadel L. (2010). The role of medial temporal lobe in retrieving spatial and nonspatial relations from episodic and semantic memory. Hippocampus 20, 11–18. 10.1002/hipo.20607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Ranganath C., and Ritchey M. (2012). Two cortical systems for memory-guided behaviour. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 713–726. 10.1038/nrn3338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Ojemann J.G., Akbudak E., Snyder A.Z., McKinstry R.C., Raichle M.E., and Conturo T.E. (1997). Anatomic Localization and Quantitative Analysis of Gradient Refocused Echo-Planar fMRI Susceptibility Artifacts. NeuroImage 6, 156–167. 10.1006/nimg.1997.0289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Merboldt K.-D., Fransson P., Bruhn H., and Frahm J. (2001). Functional MRI of the Human Amygdala? NeuroImage 14, 253–257. 10.1006/nimg.2001.0802. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Sladky R., Baldinger P., Kranz G.S., Tröstl J., Höflich A., Lanzenberger R., Moser E., and Windischberger C. (2013). High-resolution functional MRI of the human amygdala at 7T. Eur. J. Radiol. 82, 728–733. 10.1016/j.ejrad.2011.09.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Maass A., Berron D., Libby L.A., Ranganath C., and Düzel E. (2015). Functional subregions of the human entorhinal cortex. eLife 4, e06426. 10.7554/eLife.06426. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Gorka A.X., Torrisi S., Shackman A.J., Grillon C., and Ernst M. (2018). Intrinsic functional connectivity of the central nucleus of the amygdala and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis. NeuroImage 168, 392–402. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Bickart K.C., Hollenbeck M.C., Barrett L.F., and Dickerson B.C. (2012). Intrinsic Amygdala–Cortical Functional Connectivity Predicts Social Network Size in Humans. J. Neurosci. 32, 14729–14741. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1599-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Bzdok D., Langner R., Hoffstaedter F., Turetsky B.I., Zilles K., and Eickhoff S.B. (2012). The Modular Neuroarchitecture of Social Judgments on Faces. Cereb. Cortex 22, 951–961. 10.1093/cercor/bhr166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Roy A.K., Shehzad Z., Margulies D.S., Kelly A.M.C., Uddin L.Q., Gotimer K., Biswal B.B., Castellanos F.X., and Milham M.P. (2009). Functional connectivity of the human amygdala using resting state fMRI. NeuroImage 45, 614–626. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.11.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Sylvester C.M., Yu Q., Srivastava A.B., Marek S., Zheng A., Alexopoulos D., Smyser C.D., Shimony J.S., Ortega M., Dierker D.L., et al. (2020). Individual-specific functional connectivity of the amygdala: A substrate for precision psychiatry. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 117, 3808–3818. 10.1073/pnas.1910842117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Reznik D., Trampel R., Weiskopf N., Witter M.P., and Doeller C.F. (2023). Dissociating distinct cortical networks associated with subregions of the human medial temporal lobe using precision neuroimaging. Neuron, S0896–6273(23)00402–6. 10.1016/j.neuron.2023.05.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Adolphs R. (2010). What does the amygdala contribute to social cognition? Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1191, 42–61. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05445.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.LeDoux J. (2007). The amygdala. Curr. Biol. 17, R868–R874. 10.1016/j.cub.2007.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Rosvold H.E., Mirsky A.F., and Pribram K.H. (1954). Influence of amygdalectomy on social behavior in monkeys. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 47, 173–178. 10.1037/h0058870. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Adolphs R. (2010). What does the amygdala contribute to social cognition? Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1191, 42–61. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05445.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Machado C.J., and Bachevalier J. (2006). The impact of selective amygdala, orbital frontal cortex, or hippocampal formation lesions on established social relationships in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Behav. Neurosci. 120, 761–786. 10.1037/0735-7044.120.4.761. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Brothers L., Ring B., and Kling A. (1990). Response of neurons in the macaque amygdala to complex social stimuli. Behav. Brain Res. 41, 199–213. 10.1016/0166-4328(90)90108-Q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Gothard K.M., Battaglia F.P., Erickson C.A., Spitler K.M., and Amaral D.G. (2007). Neural Responses to Facial Expression and Face Identity in the Monkey Amygdala. J. Neurophysiol. 97, 1671–1683. 10.1152/jn.00714.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Hoffman K.L., Gothard K.M., Schmid M.C., and Logothetis N.K. (2007). Facial-Expression and Gaze-Selective Responses in the Monkey Amygdala. Curr. Biol. 17, 766–772. 10.1016/j.cub.2007.03.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Leonard C.M., Rolls E.T., Wilson F.A.W., and Baylis G.C. (1985). Neurons in the amygdala of the monkey with responses selective for faces. Behav. Brain Res. 15, 159–176. 10.1016/0166-4328(85)90062–2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Rutishauser U., Tudusciuc O., Neumann D., Mamelak A.N., Heller A.C., Ross I.B., Philpott L., Sutherling W.W., and Adolphs R. (2011). Single-Unit Responses Selective for Whole Faces in the Human Amygdala. Curr. Biol. CB 21, 1654–1660. 10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Grabenhorst F., Báez-Mendoza R., Genest W., Deco G., and Schultz W. (2019). Primate Amygdala Neurons Simulate Decision Processes of Social Partners. Cell 177, 986–998.e15. 10.1016/j.cell.2019.02.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Mosher C.P., Zimmerman P.E., and Gothard K.M. (2014). Neurons in the monkey amygdala detect eye-contact during naturalistic social interactions. Curr. Biol. CB 24, 2459. 10.1016/j.cub.2014.08.063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Winston J.S., O’Doherty J., and Dolan R.J. (2003). Common and distinct neural responses during direct and incidental processing of multiple facial emotions. NeuroImage 20, 84–97. 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00303–3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Shaw P., Lawrence E.J., Radbourne C., Bramham J., Polkey C.E., and David A.S. (2004). The impact of early and late damage to the human amygdala on ‘theory of mind’ reasoning. Brain 127, 1535–1548. 10.1093/brain/awh168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Fine C., Lumsden J., and Blair R.J. (2001). Dissociation between “theory of mind” and executive functions in a patient with early left amygdala damage. Brain J. Neurol. 124, 287–298. 10.1093/brain/124.2.287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Sergerie K., Chochol C., and Armony J.L. (2008). The role of the amygdala in emotional processing: A quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 811–830. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Stein M.B., Simmons A.N., Feinstein J.S., and Paulus M.P. (2007). Increased Amygdala and Insula Activation During Emotion Processing in Anxiety-Prone Subjects. Am. J. Psychiatry 164, 318–327. 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.2.318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Santos S., Almeida I., Oliveiros B., and Castelo-Branco M. (2016). The Role of the Amygdala in Facial Trustworthiness Processing: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analyses of fMRI Studies. PLOS ONE 11, e0167276. 10.1371/journal.pone.0167276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Spunt R.P., Elison J.T., Dufour N., Hurlemann R., Saxe R., and Adolphs R. (2015). Amygdala lesions do not compromise the cortical network for false-belief reasoning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 112, 4827–4832. 10.1073/pnas.1422679112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Brown S.S.G., Rutland J.W., Verma G., Feldman R.E., Alper J., Schneider M., Delman B.N., Murrough J.M., and Balchandani P. (2019). Structural MRI at 7T reveals amygdala nuclei and hippocampal subfield volumetric association with Major Depressive Disorder symptom severity. Sci. Rep. 9, 10166. 10.1038/s41598-019-46687-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Klumpp H., Keutmann M.K., Fitzgerald D.A., Shankman S.A., and Phan K.L. (2014). Resting state amygdala-prefrontal connectivity predicts symptom change after cognitive behavioral therapy in generalized social anxiety disorder. Biol. Mood Anxiety Disord. 4, 14. 10.1186/s13587-014-0014-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Rosenfeld A.J., Lieberman J.A., and Jarskog L.F. (2011). Oxytocin, Dopamine, and the Amygdala: A Neurofunctional Model of Social Cognitive Deficits in Schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 37, 1077–1087. 10.1093/schbul/sbq015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Schultz R.T. (2005). Developmental deficits in social perception in autism: the role of the amygdala and fusiform face area. Int. J. Dev. Neurosci. 23, 125–141. 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.12.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Brothers L. (1990). The neural basis of primate social communication. Motiv. Emot. 14, 81–91. 10.1007/BF00991637. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Allen E.J., St-Yves G., Wu Y., Breedlove J.L., Prince J.S., Dowdle L.T., Nau M., Caron B., Pestilli F., Charest I., et al. (2022). A massive 7T fMRI dataset to bridge cognitive neuroscience and artificial intelligence. Nat. Neurosci. 25, 116–126. 10.1038/s41593-021-00962-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Fischl B., Sereno M.I., and Dale A.M. (1999). Cortical Surface-Based Analysis: II: Inflation, Flattening, and a Surface-Based Coordinate System. NeuroImage 9, 195–207. 10.1006/nimg.1998.0396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Kong R., Li J., Orban C., Sabuncu M.R., Liu H., Schaefer A., Sun N., Zuo X.-N., Holmes A.J., Eickhoff S.B., et al. (2019). Spatial Topography of Individual-Specific Cortical Networks Predicts Human Cognition, Personality, and Emotion. Cereb. Cortex 29, 2533–2551. 10.1093/cercor/bhy123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Thomas Yeo B.T., Krienen F.M., Sepulcre J., Sabuncu M.R., Lashkari D., Hollinshead M., Roffman J.L., Smoller J.W., Zöllei L., Polimeni J.R., et al. (2011). The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 106, 1125–1165. 10.1152/jn.00338.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Braga R.M., DiNicola L.M., Becker H.C., and Buckner R.L. (2020). Situating the left-lateralized language network in the broader organization of multiple specialized large-scale distributed networks. J. Neurophysiol. 124, 1415–1448. 10.1152/jn.00753.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Toro-Serey C., Tobyne S.M., and McGuire J.T. (2020). Spectral partitioning identifies individual heterogeneity in the functional network topography of ventral and anterior medial prefrontal cortex. NeuroImage 205, 116305. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Du J., DiNicola L.M., Angeli P.A., Saadon-Grosman N., Sun W., Kaiser S., Ladopoulou J., Xue A., Yeo B.T.T., Eldaief M.C., et al. (2023). Within-Individual Organization of the Human Cerebral Cortex: Networks, Global Topography, and Function. Preprint at bioRxiv, 10.1101/2023.08.08.552437 10.1101/2023.08.08.552437. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Kwon Y., Salvo J.J., Anderson N., Holubecki A.M., Lakshman M., Yoo K., Kay K., Gratton C., and Braga R.M. (2023). Situating the parietal memory network in the context of multiple parallel distributed networks using high-resolution functional connectivity. Preprint at bioRxiv, 10.1101/2023.08.16.553585 10.1101/2023.08.16.553585. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Braga R.M., and Buckner R.L. (2017). Parallel Interdigitated Distributed Networks within the Individual Estimated by Intrinsic Functional Connectivity. Neuron 95, 457–471.e5. 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Moreno N., and González A. (2007). Evolution of the amygdaloid complex in vertebrates, with special reference to the anamnio-amniotic transition. J. Anat. 211, 151–163. 10.1111/j.1469-7580.2007.00780.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Saygin Z.M., Kliemann D., Iglesias J.E., van der Kouwe A.J.W., Boyd E., Reuter M., Stevens A., Van Leemput K., McKee A., Frosch M.P., et al. (2017). High-resolution magnetic resonance imaging reveals nuclei of the human amygdala: manual segmentation to automatic atlas. NeuroImage 155, 370–382. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.04.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Noto T., Zhou G., Yang Q., Lane G., and Zelano C. (2021). Human Primary Olfactory Amygdala Subregions Form Distinct Functional Networks, Suggesting Distinct Olfactory Functions. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Price J., Russchen F., and Amaral D.G. (1987). The limbic region. II. The amygdaloid complex. In Handbook of Chemical Neuroanatomy, Hökfelt T., Björklund A., and Swanson L. W., eds. (Elsevier; ), pp. 279–388. [Google Scholar]
- 124.Mai J.K., Majtanik M., and Paxinos G. (2015). Atlas of the Human Brain (Academic Press; ). [Google Scholar]
- 125.Aggleton J.P., Wright N.F., Rosene D.L., and Saunders R.C. (2015). Complementary Patterns of Direct Amygdala and Hippocampal Projections to the Macaque Prefrontal Cortex. Cereb. Cortex 25, 4351–4373. 10.1093/cercor/bhv019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Amaral D.G., and Price J.L. (1984). Amygdalo-cortical projections in the monkey (Macaca fascicularis). J. Comp. Neurol. 230, 465–496. 10.1002/cne.902300402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Jalbrzikowski M., Larsen B., Hallquist M.N., Foran W., Calabro F., and Luna B. (2017). Development of White Matter Microstructure and Intrinsic Functional Connectivity Between the Amygdala and Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex: Associations With Anxiety and Depression. Biol. Psychiatry 82, 511–521. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.01.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Motzkin J.C., Philippi C.L., Wolf R.C., Baskaya M.K., and Koenigs M. (2015). Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Is Critical for the Regulation of Amygdala Activity in Humans. Biol. Psychiatry 77, 276–284. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.02.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Rutishauser U., Mamelak A.N., and Adolphs R. (2015). The primate amygdala in social perception – insights from electrophysiological recordings and stimulation. Trends Neurosci. 38, 295–306. 10.1016/j.tins.2015.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Johansen-Berg H., Gutman D.A., Behrens T.E.J., Matthews P.M., Rushworth M.F.S., Katz E., Lozano A.M., and Mayberg H.S. (2008). Anatomical Connectivity of the Subgenual Cingulate Region Targeted with Deep Brain Stimulation for Treatment-Resistant Depression. Cereb. Cortex 18, 1374–1383. 10.1093/cercor/bhm167. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Phelps E.A., Delgado M.R., Nearing K.I., and LeDoux J.E. (2004). Extinction Learning in Humans: Role of the Amygdala and vmPFC. Neuron 43, 897–905. 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Allsop S.A., Wichmann R., Mills F., Burgos-Robles A., Chang C.-J., Felix-Ortiz A.C., Vienne A., Beyeler A., Izadmehr E.M., Glober G., et al. (2018). Corticoamygdala Transfer of Socially Derived Information Gates Observational Learning. Cell 173, 1329–1342.e18. 10.1016/j.cell.2018.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Fox M.D., Buckner R.L., Liu H., Chakravarty M.M., Lozano A.M., and Pascual-Leone A. (2014). Resting-state networks link invasive and noninvasive brain stimulation across diverse psychiatric and neurological diseases. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 111, E4367–E4375. 10.1073/pnas.1405003111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.Mishra A., Rogers B.P., Chen L.M., and Gore J.C. (2014). Functional connectivity-based parcellation of amygdala using self-organized mapping: A data driven approach. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 1247–1260. 10.1002/hbm.22249. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Zheng A., Montez D.F., Marek S., Gilmore A.W., Newbold D.J., Laumann T.O., Kay B.P., Seider N.A., Van A.N., Hampton J.M., et al. (2021). Parallel hippocampal-parietal circuits for self- and goal-oriented processing. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 118, e2101743118. 10.1073/pnas.2101743118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Bickart K.C., Dickerson B.C., and Feldman Barrett L. (2014). The amygdala as a hub in brain networks that support social life. Neuropsychologia 63, 235–248. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.08.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Baron-Cohen S., Ring H.A., Wheelwright S., Bullmore E.T., Brammer M.J., Simmons A., and Williams S.C.R. (1999). Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 11, 1891–1898. 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00621.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Stone V.E., Baron-Cohen S., Calder A., Keane J., and Young A. (2003). Acquired theory of mind impairments in individuals with bilateral amygdala lesions. Neuropsychologia 41, 209–220. 10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00151-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.LeDoux J.E., Farb C.R., and Romanski L.M. (1991). Overlapping projections to the amygdala and striatum from auditory processing areas of the thalamus and cortex. Neurosci. Lett. 134, 139–144. 10.1016/0304-3940(91)90526-Y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Whitlock D.G., and Nauta W.J.H. (1956). Subcortical projections from the temporal neocortex in Macaca mulatta. J. Comp. Neurol. 106, 183–212. 10.1002/cne.901060107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Turner B.H., Mishkin M., and Knapp M. (1980). Organization of the amygdalopetal projections from modality-specific cortical association areas in the monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 191, 515–543. 10.1002/cne.901910402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142.McDonald A.J. (1998). Cortical pathways to the mammalian amygdala. Prog. Neurobiol. 55, 257–332. 10.1016/S0301-0082(98)00003-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143.Chareyron L.J., Banta Lavenex P., Amaral D.G., and Lavenex P. (2011). Stereological analysis of the rat and monkey amygdala. J. Comp. Neurol. 519, 3218–3239. 10.1002/cne.22677. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Stephan H., and Andy O.J. (1977). Quantitative comparison of the amygdala in insectivores and primates. Acta Anat. (Basel) 98, 130–153. 10.1159/000144789. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145.Margulies D.S., Ghosh S.S., Goulas A., Falkiewicz M., Huntenburg J.M., Langs G., Bezgin G., Eickhoff S.B., Castellanos F.X., Petrides M., et al. (2016). Situating the default-mode network along a principal gradient of macroscale cortical organization. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 113, 12574–12579. 10.1073/pnas.1608282113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146.Pitkänen A., Kelly J.L., and Amaral D.G. (2002). Projections from the lateral, basal, and accessory basal nuclei of the amygdala to the entorhinal cortex in the macaque monkey. Hippocampus 12, 186–205. 10.1002/hipo.1099. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147.Stefanacci L., and Amaral D.G. (2002). Some observations on cortical inputs to the macaque monkey amygdala: An anterograde tracing study. J. Comp. Neurol. 451, 301–323. 10.1002/cne.10339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148.Aggleton J.P., Burton M.J., and Passingham R.E. (1980). Cortical and subcortical afferents to the amygdala of the rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta). Brain Res. 190, 347–368. 10.1016/0006-8993(80)90279-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Schumann C.M., Bauman M.D., and Amaral D.G. (2011). Abnormal structure or function of the amygdala is a common component of neurodevelopmental disorders. Neuropsychologia 49, 745–759. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.09.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150.Pardo-Bellver C., Cadiz-Moretti B., Novejarque A., Martinez-Garcia F., and Lanuza E. (2012). Differential efferent projections of the anterior, posteroventral, and posterodorsal subdivisions of the medial amygdala in mice. Front. Neuroanat. 6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151.Newman S.W. (2006). The Medial Extended Amygdala in Male Reproductive Behavior A Node in the Mammalian Social Behavior Network. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 877, 242–257. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1999.tb09271.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152.Atzil S., Touroutoglou A., Rudy T., Salcedo S., Feldman R., Hooker J.M., Dickerson B.C., Catana C., and Barrett L.F. (2017). Dopamine in the medial amygdala network mediates human bonding. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 114, 2361–2366. 10.1073/pnas.1612233114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 153.Keshavarzi S., Sullivan R.K.P., Ianno D.J., and Sah P. (2014). Functional Properties and Projections of Neurons in the Medial Amygdala. J. Neurosci. 34, 8699–8715. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1176-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 154.Allison A.C. (1954). The secondary olfactory areas in the human brain. J. Anat. 88, 481–488.2. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 155.Veazey R.B., Amaral D.G., and Cowan W.M. (1982). The morphology and connections of the posterior hypothalamus in the cynomolgus monkey (Macaca fascicularis). II. Efferent connections. J. Comp. Neurol. 207, 135–156. 10.1002/cne.902070204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 156.Libby L.A., Ekstrom A.D., Ragland J.D., and Ranganath C. (2012). Differential Connectivity of Perirhinal and Parahippocampal Cortices within Human Hippocampal Subregions Revealed by High-Resolution Functional Imaging. J. Neurosci. 32, 6550–6560. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3711-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 157.Lavenex P., and Amaral D.G. (2000). Hippocampal-neocortical interaction: A hierarchy of associativity. Hippocampus 10, 420–430. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 158.Manns J.R., and Eichenbaum H. (2006). Evolution of declarative memory. Hippocampus 16, 795–808. 10.1002/hipo.20205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 159.Liang J.C., Wagner A.D., and Preston A.R. (2013). Content Representation in the Human Medial Temporal Lobe. Cereb. Cortex 23, 80–96. 10.1093/cercor/bhr379. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 160.Roesler R., and McGaugh J.L. (2022). The Entorhinal Cortex as a Gateway for Amygdala Influences on Memory Consolidation. Neuroscience 497, 86–96. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2022.01.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 161.Xue A., Kong R., Yang Q., Eldaief M.C., Angeli P.A., DiNicola L.M., Braga R.M., Buckner R.L., and Yeo B.T.T. (2021). The detailed organization of the human cerebellum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity within the individual. J. Neurophysiol. 125, 358–384. 10.1152/jn.00561.2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 162.Nair A., Jolliffe M., Lograsso Y.S.S., and Bearden C.E. (2020). A Review of Default Mode Network Connectivity and Its Association With Social Cognition in Adolescents With Autism Spectrum Disorder and Early-Onset Psychosis. Front. Psychiatry 11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 163.Kong R., Yang Q., Gordon E., Xue A., Yan X., Orban C., Zuo X.-N., Spreng N., Ge T., Holmes A., et al. (2021). Individual-Specific Areal-Level Parcellations Improve Functional Connectivity Prediction of Behavior. Cereb. Cortex 31, 4477–4500. 10.1093/cercor/bhab101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 164.Naselaris T., Allen E., and Kay K. (2021). Extensive sampling for complete models of individual brains. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 40, 45–51. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.12.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 165.Smith S.M., Jenkinson M., Woolrich M.W., Beckmann C.F., Behrens T.E.J., Johansen-Berg H., Bannister P.R., De Luca M., Drobnjak I., Flitney D.E., et al. (2004). Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. NeuroImage 23, S208–S219. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 166.Avants B.B., Tustison N.J., Stauffer M., Song G., Wu B., and Gee J.C. (2014). The Insight ToolKit image registration framework. Front. Neuroinformatics 8, 44. 10.3389/fninf.2014.00044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 167.Cox R.W. (1996). AFNI: Software for Analysis and Visualization of Functional Magnetic Resonance Neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. 29, 162–173. 10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 168.Marcus D., Harwell J., Olsen T., Hodge M., Glasser M., Prior F., Jenkinson M., Laumann T., Curtiss S., and Van Essen D. (2011). Informatics and Data Mining Tools and Strategies for the Human Connectome Project. Front. Neuroinformatics 5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 169.Jo H.J., Saad Z.S., Simmons W.K., Milbury L.A., and Cox R.W. (2010). Mapping sources of correlation in resting state FMRI, with artifact detection and removal. NeuroImage 52, 571–582. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 170.Zelano C., Montag J., Johnson B., Khan R., and Sobel N. (2007). Dissociated Representations of Irritation and Valence in Human Primary Olfactory Cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 97, 1969–1976. 10.1152/jn.01122.2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 171.Kundu P., Voon V., Balchandani P., Lombardo M.V., Poser B.A., and Bandettini P.A. (2017). Multi-echo fMRI: A review of applications in fMRI denoising and analysis of BOLD signals. NeuroImage 154, 59–80. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 172.Lynch C.J., Power J.D., Scult M.A., Dubin M., Gunning F.M., and Liston C. (2020). Rapid Precision Functional Mapping of Individuals Using Multi-Echo fMRI. Cell Rep. 33, 108540. 10.1016/j.celrep.2020.108540. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 173.Woolrich M.W., Ripley B.D., Brady M., and Smith S.M. (2001). Temporal Autocorrelation in Univariate Linear Modeling of FMRI Data. NeuroImage 14, 1370–1386. 10.1006/nimg.2001.0931. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.