Skip to main content
. 2023 Dec 25;16(1):75. doi: 10.3390/nu16010075

Table 1.

Observational study characteristics.

Author/s Population Cognitive Measures Dietary Measures Sugar Type Major Findings
Al-Sabah et al., 2020 a [44] N = 1370 adolescents aged 11–16 Raven’s Standard Progressive Matrices (SPM) FFQ Sugary drinks
(frequency of consumption)
Inverse relationship between sugary drink consumption and cognitive performance (p < 0.001).
Sugary drink consumption a main predictor of cognitive function (p < 0.001).
Baym et al., 2014 a [40] N = 52 children aged 7–9 Memory tasks (paired associated images) Youth–adolescent FFQ (YAQ) Total sugar,
added sugar
(normalised by total daily kcal consumption)
Total and added sugars had no relationship to memory.
Performance impacted negatively by saturated fatty acids and positively by omega-3 fatty acids (ps < 0.05).
Berger et al., 2020 b [46] N = 88 mother–infant pairs (mother M age = 68.1, SD = 6.7) Bayley-III scales of infant development FFQ at 1 and 6 months postnatal Fructose,
SSB (incl. juice),
total sugar,
added sugar
(adjusted for kcal per day)
Maternal fructose (p < 0.01) and SSB (p = 0.02) consumption at 1 month postnatal negatively associated with infant cognitive development at 24 months.
Infant cognition lower in infants of obese mothers (p ≤ 0.001).
No effect of maternal intake at 6 months.
Chong et al., 2019 a [14] N = 1209 older adults aged 60 years and over (M = 68.1, SD = 5.6) RAVLT,
MMSE,
MoCA,
digit symbol (processing speed test, not described),
VR
Dietary history questionnaire (1 week recall) Fructose,
glucose,
total sugar,
added sugar,
SSB,
sugar from cakes and deserts, adjusted for daily calorie intake)
Mild cognitive impairment had higher consumption of fructose (p = 0.004) and glucose (p = 0.032).
MMSE scores lower in the higher percentile of total and free sugar intake (ps < 0.001).
Risk of cognitive impairment increased 3.3-, 3.3-, and 3.6-fold in highest percentile of sucrose, total sugar, and free sugar, respectively (ps < 0.001).
Risk of cognitive impairment increased by 3.7- and 1.8-fold for SSB and sugar from cakes and deserts, respectively (ps < 0.001).
Reduction in risk of cognitive impairment by 35% in highest percentile of fruit consumption (p < 0.05).
Cohen et al., 2018 b [47] N = 1234, mother–child pairs (tested during pregnancy and early childhood) Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-III),
WRAVMA,
KBIT-II,
WRAML
FFQ Maternal (prenatal) SSB consumption,
child SSB consumption
(not adjusted for energy intake)
Maternal sucrose ingestion inversely associated with nonverbal KBIT-II (p = 0.03) and visual memory (p = 0.01) in mid-childhood.
Maternal SSB ingestion inversely associated with nonverbal KBIT-II in mid-childhood (p = 0.03).
Maternal diet soda consumption associated with lower WRAVMA in early childhood (p = 0.03) and verbal KBIT-II in mid-childhood (p < 0.001).
Early childhood SSB consumption inversely associated with verbal KBIT-II in mid-childhood (p = 0.01).
Fructose (p = 0.005) and fruit (p = 0.03) positively associated with PPVT-II in early childhood.
Gui et al., 2021 a [41] N = 6387 children (3410 male) aged 6–12 (M = 8.6, SD = 1.5) BRIEF FFQ HFCS from SSB
(frequency of consumption)
Associated with poor performance on executive function and high risk of executive dysfunction (ps < 0.0001).
Hassevoort et al., 2020 a [39] N = 54 children (31 female) aged 8–12 (M = 9.1, SD = 0.8) TTCT—Verbal form A 3-day FFQ Added sugar
(normalised to intake per 1000 kcal)
Inversely associated with fluency, originality, and overall TTCT score (p < 0.01).
Lester et al., 1982 a [38] N = 184 children (100 male) aged 5–16 WISC-R,
WIPPSI,
WRAT
FFQ—24-h recall Refined carbohydrates
(adjusted for total calories)
Negative relationship with all aspects of cognition (full-scale IQ (p = 0.001), performance IQ (p = 0.025), verbal IQ (p = 0.005), math (p = 0.005), and reading (p = 0.025)) other than spelling. Ratio of refined carbohydrates to total food calories negatively correlated with full-scale IQ (p < 0.015).
Naveed et al., 2020 a [43] N = 487 children (250 male) aged 6–8 Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices (RCPM) FFQ Fructose,
sucrose,
glucose
(adjusted for daily energy intake)
Increased fructose intake associated with higher fluid intelligence scores in all children (p = 0.002) and boys (p ≤ 0.001) but not girls alone; effect disappeared in all children and reduced in boys when fruits and berries were accounted for.
No effect of glucose or sucrose
Øverby et al., 2013 a [45] N = 482 students (236 male, M age = 14.6) Self-reported schooling difficulties (maths, reading, and writing) FFQ SSB,
junk food
(frequency of consumption)
Higher intake of SSB (p = 0.04) and junk food (p ≤ 0.001) associated with increased odds of self-reported math difficulties.
Fruit intake associated with lower odds of math difficulties.
No relationship with reading and writing difficulties.
Ye et al., 2011 c [48] N = 737 adults aged 45–75 MMSE,
word list learning,
digit span,
clock drawing
and figure copying (visual–spatial),
STROOP,
verbal fluency test
FFQ (12-month estimate) Total sugar,
added sugar,
SSB
(adjusted for total energy intake)
Increased sucrose (p = 0.014), glucose (p = 0.032), SSB (p = 0.005), and added fructose (p = 0.028), but not natural fructose, associated with lower MMSE.
Total sugars inversely correlated with letter fluency (p < 0.05), recognition and recall (ps < 0.05), memory (p = 0.01), and MMSE (p = 0.02).
Added sugars inversely associated with letter fluency (p < 0.05), long-term recall (p < 0.05), and MMSE (p = 0.005).
Zhang et al., 2022 a [42] N = 1231 adolescents aged 13–18 (M = 15.77, SD = 1.7) Modified Erikson flanker task,
1-back and 2-back tasks,
more-odd shifting task
FFQ SSB
(frequency of consumption)
Drinking SSBs ≥2 times per week had worse performance for inhibition, working memory, and cognitive flexibility than no SSBs (ps < 0.001).

Note: a = Cross-sectional study design; b = prospective cohort study design; c = retrospective cohort study; M = mean; FFQ = food frequency questionnaire; MMSE = Mini Mental State Exam; SSB = sugar-sweetened beverages; RAVLT = Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Memory Test; MoCA = Montreal Cognitive Assessment; VR = Visual Reproduction Test; WRAVMA = Wide-Range Assessment of Visual Motor Abilities; KBIT-II = Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test; WRAML = Wide-Range Assessment of Memory Learning; BRIEF = Parent-Rated Behavioural Rating Inventory of Executive Function; HFCS = high-fructose corn syrup; TTCT = Torrance Test of Creative Thinking; WISC-R = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children; WIPPSI = Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence; WRAT = Wide-Range Achievement Test.