Table 2.
Author/s | Population | Cognitive Measures | Intervention/Control | Fasting | Major Findings |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Adan and Serra-Grabulosa, 2010 [51] | N = 72 young adults (36 male) aged 18–25 (M = 21.07, SD = 1.70) | RAVLT, Purdue–Pegboard, Benton Judgement of Line Orientation Test (JoLO), WCST, California Computerized Assessment Package (CalCAP), digit span of WAIS |
|
8 h | Water performed worse than treatment groups (p = 0.026). Glucose performed better on Purdue pegboard assembly than placebo or caffeine (p = 0.039). No effect of treatment on reaction time, WAIS, WCST, and RAVLT. |
Allen et al., 1996 [62] | N = 28 elderly adults (6 male) aged 61–87 (M = 73) | Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure, Taylor Complex Figure, dichotic listening, TMT, verbal fluency, Boston naming test, Meier visual test, grooved pegboard, figural fluency |
|
9 h | Glucose enhanced delayed recall and verbal and figure fluency (ps < 0.001). No effect of glucose on other measures. Poor glucose regulation associated with worse performance in dichotic listening (p < 0.005) and verbal fluency (p < 0.05). |
Azari, 1993 [63] |
N = 18 young male adults aged 19–25 (M = 21, SD = 1.65) |
Word recall |
|
4.5 h | No effect of glucose or BGL on memory. |
Benton et al., 1987 [64] | N = 60 children (30 male) aged 6 or 7 years old. | Paradigm of Shakow |
|
2–3 h | Improved reaction time after glucose (p ≤ 0.05). Glucose group reported increased quiet concentration (p < 0.001) and less likely to fidget (p < 0.04). |
Benton, 1990 [65] | T1: N = 20 male students (M age = 20.3 and 20.5 per group) T2: N = 40 undergraduate students (20 male; M age = 21.2 and 20.9) |
T1: choice reaction time task T2: long arithmetic |
|
4 h | T1: Glucose associated with fewer errors (p < 0.05). T2: No effect of glucose on coordination or arithmetic. Males performed better on arithmetic (p < 0.001) and coordination (p < 0.01). |
Benton and Owens, 1993 [66] | T1: N = 153 undergraduate students (100 male, M age = 21.6, SD = 4.8; 53 female, M age = 21.8, SD = 5.2) T2: N = 53 female undergraduates (M = 21.5, SD = 5.0) |
T1: word list, pattern recognition T2: word list, Weschler memory scale |
T1:
|
No | T1: No effect of glucose on recall or spatial memory. Increased BGL associated with more words remembered (p < 0.002). T2: No effect of glucose on Weschler or word list. Falling BGL in glucose group associated with improved memory; falling BGL in placebo group associated with worse memory. |
Benton et al., 1994 [67] | T1: N = 70 female undergraduates (M age = 21.46) T2: N = 50 male undergraduates (M age = 21.7) |
T1: RIPT T2: STROOP |
|
No | Glucose associated with more errors than placebo in RIPT (p < 0.031). No effect of glucose on memory, reaction time, or STROOP. |
Benton and Stevens, 2008 [68] | N = 16 children (7 male) aged 9 or 10 years | Recall of objects test, pattern recognition, paradigm of Shakow |
|
No | More pictures recalled after glucose consumption (p < 0.025). No effect of glucose on spatial memory. |
Best et al., 2008 [69] | N = 45 adults (19 male) aged 40–63 (M = 52.1, SD = 5.9) | RAVLT, Self-Ordered Pointing Task (SOPT), digit span, WAIS matrix reasoning |
|
2 h | No effect of glucose on any outcome. |
Birnie et al., 2015 [70] | N = 16 adults (8 male) aged 18–45 (M = 23.7, SD = 5.0) | SART, Short Imaginal Processes Inventory (SIPI) |
|
2 h | No effect of glucose on any outcome. |
Brandt et al., 2006 [71] | T1: N = 40 undergraduate students (20 male) aged 18–25 T2: N = 40 undergraduate students (14 male) aged 18–36 (M = 22) |
Word list |
|
2 h | No effect of glucose on emotional memory enhancement. BGL were negatively associated with positive items remembered (p < 0.05). Better glucoregulation associated with improved memory for negative items and fewer errors (ps < 0.05). |
Brandt et al., 2010 [72] | T1: N = 40 undergraduate students (5 male) aged 18–34 (M = 19.1) T2: N = 40 undergraduate students (27 male) aged 18–37 (M = 21) |
Word list | T1:
|
2 h | Greater false alarm rate after 25 g glucose condition (p < 0.05) Recognition memory was marginally improved in the aspartame group (p = 0.05). |
Brandt et al., 2013 [73] |
N = 60 undergraduate students (14 male; M age = 19.7) |
STROOP |
|
Overnight | Shorter reaction times in congruent and incongruent conditions after glucose consumption (ps < 0.05). Greater facilitative effect of glucose in incongruent (higher cognitive load) task. |
Brandt, 2015 [74] |
N = 41 undergraduate students (9 male; M age = 19.47) |
Process dissociation procedure |
|
Overnight | Exclusion (high effort condition) was superior in glucose condition (p < 0.05). No effect of treatment on inclusion. Aspartame scored higher in familiarity (p < 0.01) (low effort condition). No effect of treatment on recollection. |
Brody and Wolitzky, 1983 [75] |
N = 59 undergraduate students (28 male) aged 16–24 (M = 18.7) |
Serial sevens |
|
8 h | No effect of treatment. |
Brown and Riby, 2013 [76] | N = 35 young adults (14 male) aged 18–35 (M = 22.17, SD = 5.97) | Item recognition task, STROOP |
|
2 h | Glucose facilitation effect in more difficult task conditions, but glucose impaired performance on new items (low cognitive load) (p = 0.02). No effect of glucose on attention. |
Craft et al., 1994 [77] | N = 59 (27 younger adults aged 19–28, M = 20.8; 32 older adults aged 58–77, M = 68.5) | Paced Serial Addition Test, paragraph recall, modified CVLT, pattern recall and recognition, serial reaction time, word generation |
|
Overnight | Glucose improved declarative memory in older males with good glycaemic recovery (p < 0.01). Glucose improved recall in younger men with poor glycaemic recovery (p < 0.01). Younger men with good glycaemic recovery saw memory deterioration after glucose consumption (p < 0.001). No effect of glucose on procedural memory, working memory, or verbal fluency. |
Donohoe and Benton, 1999a [78] | T1: N = 67 female undergraduate students (M age = 21.8, SD = 5.1) T2: N = 69 female undergraduates (M age = 20.2, SD = 2.1) |
T1: water jar test, Finding Embedded Figures Test, Baddeley Logical Reasoning Task T2: Controlled Oral Word Association Test, WAIS—Block design subtest, Porteus maze—adults/14 year olds |
|
No | T1: No effect of glucose on outcomes. T2: Improved verbal fluency after glucose consumption (p < 0.001). Faster time to solve Porteus maze for 14 year olds after glucose (p < 0.002). No effect of glucose on block design. |
Donohoe and Benton, 1999b [79] | N = 188 female undergraduate students (M age 21, SD = 4) | T1: RIPT, word list |
|
No | Faster recall for glucose compared to control (p < 0.001). No effect of glucose on recall. More errors in the placebo group at 2, 4 (ps < 0.01), and 6 min (p < 0.05) (but not 8 or 10 min) of RIPT vigilance task. |
Flint and Turek, 2003 [80] |
N = 67 Undergraduate students (15 male) aged 18–50 (M = 19.49, SD = 4.35) |
Test of Variable of Attention (TOVA) |
|
8 h | 100 mg/kg showed impaired impulsivity (greater postcommission response time variability) (p < 0.01). No effect of treatment on any other measure. |
Ford et al., 2002 [81] | N = 20 undergraduate students aged 20–23 | Tailored version of CDR Assessment Battery |
|
Overnight | No effect of glucose on memory. |
Foster et al, 1998 [52] | N = 30 female young adults aged 18–22 years (M = 19.5) | Modified CVLT, ROCF, digit span |
|
9 h | Improved delayed recall after glucose consumption (p < 0.05). No effect of treatment on any other outcome. |
Giles et al., 2018 [82] | N = 105 young adults (74 female; M age = 22.5, SD = 6.6) | Immediate and delayed recall, STROOP, N-back task, continuous performance task |
|
12 h | Improved performance on sustained attention after sugar intake (p < 0.05). No effect of treatment on selective attention, verbal memory, or working memory. |
Ginieis et al., 2018 [83] | N = 49 young adults (26 fasted (15 female, M age = 22.6, SD = 4.2) or 23 nonfasted (13 female, M age = 24.3, SD = 4.9)) | Simple response time task, arithmetic task, STROOP |
|
10 h | Slower reaction time after glucose consumption in the fasting group for simple response task and arithmetic task (p < 0.05). STROOP response time was impaired in the glucose and sucrose conditions, independent of fasting (p < 0.001). |
Gonder-Frederick et al., 1987 [84] |
N = 11 elderly adults aged 58–76 (M = 67.4, SD = 5.7) |
WAIS memory subscales |
|
9 h | Glucose group had improved performance on narrative memory (p = 0.024) and total Weschler scale (p = 0.009) (one-tailed). BGL at 30 and 60 min after beverage consumption was negatively associated with narrative memory, visual memory, and total Weschler scale (ps < 0.05). |
Hope et al., 2013 [85] | T1: N = 12 young adults (6 male; M age = 25.1, SD = 2.1). T2: N = 24 young adults (3 male; M age = 20.1, SD = 0.7) |
(1) Flanker task, simple version (2) Flanker task, demanding version |
|
No | T1: Slower reaction time after glucose consumption (p = 0.03) only when glucose administered in session 1 and placebo in session 2. T2: Slower reaction time after glucose consumption (p = 0.045). No effect of glucose on error rates. |
Jones et al., 2012 [32] | N = 18 young adults (5 male) aged 18–37 (M age = 19) | Tailored version of CDR Assessment Battery |
|
12 h | Enhancements in attention (p < 0.01) and speed (p < 0.05) 15 min after glucose ingestion. Impairments in working memory (p < 0.05) 60 min after glucose ingestion. Speed enhanced 15 min after fat consumption (p < 0.05). Working memory enhanced 15 min following protein ingestion (p < 0.05). Episodic memory and memory quality enhanced 60 min following protein ingestion (p < 0.01). |
Kaplan et al., 2000 [31] | N = 20 older adults (10 male) aged 60–82 | Tailored version of RAVLT, TMT, attention task (television recall) |
|
10–12 h | No effect of treatment on cognitive performance. Significant predictors of declarative memory were glycaemic regulation, BMI, and beta-cell function. |
Kaplan et al., 2001 [33] | N = 22 older adults (11 male) aged 61–79 (M = 71.2, SD = 1.3) | Tailored version of RAVLT, paragraph recall, TMT, attention task (television recall) |
|
10–12 h | Protein, glucose, and fat (ps < 0.001) improved delayed recall at 15 min compared to placebo. Protein (p = 0.04), glucose (p = 0.02), and fat (p = 0.008) improved immediate recall at 15 min compared to placebo. No effect of treatment on recall or TMT at 60 min. Fat ingestion improved attention at 60 min. |
Kennedy and Scholey, 2000 [86] | N = 20 young adults (6 male) aged 19–30 (M = 20.4) | Serial threes and sevens, word retrieval |
|
9 h | Improved performance in serial sevens after glucose ingestion (p < 0.01). Performance in serial threes and sevens positively associated with fall in BGL during task (ps < 0.05). No significant effect of glucose on word retrieval. |
Maben and Smith, 1996 [49] | N = 48 young adults (24 male) aged 18–32 | Word list, Baddeley Logical Reasoning Task, Semantic memory task (not described) |
|
9 h | Sugar and aspartame conditions performed more accurately on logical reasoning than control (p < 0.05) but more slowly (p < 0.001). No effect of sugar or aspartame on free recall, semantic memory, or recognition memory. |
Macpherson et al., 2015 [87] | N = 48 (24 young adults aged 18–23 (M = 20.6, SD = 1.4) and 24 older adults aged 65–85 (M = 72.5, SD = 5.1)) | Auditory word recognition, target tracking task |
|
12 h | Tracking precision improved in older adults after glucose ingestion (p = 0.05) after controlling for BMI, IQ, and glucose regulation. No effect of glucose in younger adults. |
Mantantzis et al., 2018 [88] | N = 112 (54 undergraduate students aged 18–27 and 58 older adults aged 65–82) | Choice reaction time task |
|
2 h | Glucose improved speed (p = 0.001) and accuracy (p = 0.007) in the older adult group only. |
Martin and Benton, 1999 [89] | N = 80 female undergraduates (M age = 22.6) | Consonant trigrams |
|
Overnight fast vs. no fast | Performance improved over time for fasting plus glucose (p < 0.001), breakfast plus glucose (p < 0.03), and breakfast without glucose (p < 0.001). Falling BGL during the task was associated with better recall (p < 0.001). |
Meikle et al., 2004 [53] | N = 25 adults (17 female) aged 18–52 (M = 28.4, SD = 9.3). Younger group (N = 14, M age = 21.8, SD = 3.3) and middle-aged group (N = 11, M age = 38.4, SD = 6.7) | Choice reaction time task, TMT, letter cancelation test, word retrieval, word list |
|
9 h | Older adults saw improvement in reaction time for high memory load tasks after 25 g (p < 0.05) or 50 g (p < 0.01) of glucose. All participants had improved delayed free recall after 25 g (p < 0.05) and 50 g (p < 0.01) of glucose. |
Meikle et al., 2005 [54] | T1: 37 young adults (29 female aged 17–48 (M = 28.3) T2: N = 24 young adults (20 female) aged 18–20 (M = 18.9) |
Word list | T1:
|
9 h | T1: No main effect of treatment post hoc; for higher difficulty task, placebo forgot more words than glucose prelearning (p < 0.01) and postlearning groups (p < 0.05) T2: Glucose performed better on high memory load tasks (p < 0.01) but not on high cognitive demand tasks. |
Messier et al., 1998 [90] | N = 100 female undergraduate students aged 17–48 (M = 21.3, SD = 4.6) | Word list |
|
12 h | Increased primacy word recall after 10, 300, 500, 800, and 1000 mg/kg compared to one or both controls. Increased recency word performance after 500 mg/kg glucose. Impaired performance on recency word recall after 10 and 300 mg/kg glucose. |
Miller et al., 2013 [91] | N = 36 adults (11 male; M age = 23.3, SD = 7.0) | Anagram problem solving |
|
3 h | Fructose (p = 0.01) and glucose (p < 0.01) solved more problems than placebo. |
Mohanty and Flint, 2001 [92] | N = 70 undergraduate students (22 male; M age = 20.6, SD = 4.3) | Recall of object location task (pattern recognition) |
|
8 h | In the emotional condition, more errors were made following glucose ingestion (no p values given). In the neutral condition, fewer errors were made following 100 mg/kg glucose compared to placebo. Response time was slower following 50 g glucose. More errors were made in spatial memory retention following glucose ingestion. |
Owen et al., 2010 [55] | N = 90 undergraduate students (29 male) aged 18–30 (M = 21) | Word list, face recognition test |
|
12 h | Improvement in implicit memory following 60 g glucose (p < 0.01). More false alarms after 25 g glucose compared to placebo or 60 g glucose (ps = 0.03). |
Owen et al., 2012 [56] | N = 30 adults aged 18–25 (M = 20) | Word list, serial threes and sevens, Corsi block-tapping, STROOP, simple response time task |
|
2 h vs. 12 h | 60 g glucose increased working memory (speed of recognition (p < 0.05) and serial threes (p < 0.01)) after fasting. Reaction time impaired with 25 g glucose after fasting. |
Owen et al., 2013 [57] | N = 24 young adults aged 18–30 (M = 20) | Word list, serial threes and sevens, Corsi block-tapping |
|
12 h | Improved working memory and declarative memory after 25 g and 60 g glucose (ps < 0.05). Improved spatial working memory and word recognition after 25 g (ps < 0.05) and 60 g (ps < 0.01) glucose. No effect of glucose on accuracy. Participants with poor glycaemic control performed better on recall after 25 g glucose (p < 0.05). Improved response time after 25 g (p < 0.05) and 60 g (p < 0.01) of glucose. |
Parker and Benton, 1995 [93] | N = 100 females (M age = 20.15) | Dichotic listening task, word list, choice reaction time task, auditory word recognition |
|
No | No effect of glucose on recognition Glucose drinkers recalled more when they nominated their right ear than left (p < 0.005), whereas placebo drinkers did not. Recall from attended (p < 0.013) or unattended (p < 0.034) right ear was better when BGL falling rather than rising. |
Peters et al., 2020a [30] | N = 32 adults (16 younger adults aged 21–30 (8 female, M age = 25.8, SD = 3.2) and 16 older adults aged 55–78 (8 female, M age = 68.6, SD = 6.5)) | Serial sevens, Virtual Morris Water Maze |
|
12 h | Older adults performed worse than younger in placebo condition (p = 0.02) but not glucose condition. Older adults had poorer glucose regulation (p = 0.002) and a greater response to glucose (p = 0.006) than younger adults. |
Riby et al., 2004 [94] | N = 20 older adults (M age = 68.75, SD = 6.0) | Verbal Paired Associates, digit symbol substitution, digit span |
|
Unclear | Greater recall in glucose compared to placebo group (p < 0.05); immediate recall most sensitive to glucose (p < 0.01). |
Riby et al., 2008 [95] | N = 33 middle-aged adults (19 females) aged 35–55) | Word list, national adult reading test, digit symbol substitution, letter cancellation, TMT, digit span, category fluency |
|
2 h | 50 g glucose scored higher on word recall accuracy than 25 g or placebo (p < 0.001). Good glucose regulators performed better on trail making, except after 50 g glucose (p = 0.05). Consumption of “sugar, calories, sweets and drinks” were related to poor glucose control (p < 0.05). |
Riby et al., 2011 [96] | N = 56 adults (25 male) aged 17—80 (M = 34.4, SD = 17.0) | SART |
|
3 h | Increased speed of response after glucose (p < 0.05). No effect of glucose on accuracy or sustained attention. Good glucose regulators had quicker responses compared to poor regulators (p < 0.05). |
Scholey et al., 2001 [97] | N = 30 adults (11 male) aged 20–30 (M = 27.7) | Serial sevens, word retrieval, word list |
|
9 h | Glucose ingestion led to more responses in serial sevens (p < 0.05). BGL fell more during serial sevens (high cognitive load task) regardless of treatment (p = 0.009). No effect of glucose on verbal fluency or memory. |
Scholey et al., 2009 [98] | N = 120 adults (77 female; M age = 21.6, SD = 4.9) | Word recognition (auditory), tracking task |
|
11 h | Improved tracking after glucose ingestion (p = 0.045). No effect of glucose on recognition accuracy or reaction time. |
Scholey et al., 2014 [58] | N = 160 adults aged 18–55 | Arithmetic task, STROOP, memory search task, target tracker |
|
12 h | No difference between glucose groups and placebo. Improved scores due to caffeine + glucose. |
Serra-Grabulosa et al., 2010 [59] | N = 40 students (20 male) aged 18–25 (M = 19.6, SD = 1.7) | Continuous Performance Test—Identical Pairs (CPT-IP) |
|
8 h | No effect of treatment on performance. |
Smith and Foster, 2008 [99] |
N = 32 adolescents (12 male) aged 14–17 (mean = 15.6, SD = 0.9) N = 10 in glucose second test condition |
Modified CVLT-II |
|
9.5–10.5 h | No effect of glucose on recall. Treatment x treatment order effect—improved performance for glucose ingestion only when glucose ingested in second session after placebo trial. |
Smith et al., 2011b [100] | N = 58 adolescent males aged 14–17 (M = 15.5, SD = 1.0) | Modified CVLT-II |
|
9.5–11 h | More items recalled after glucose ingestion on 4th (p < 0.05) and 5th (p < 0.01) trial. Glucose only improved memory in those reporting higher trait anxiety (p < 0.05). |
Spiers et al., 1998 [101] | N = 48 adults (24 male) aged 18–35 | Word list, digit span, Corsi block test, TMT, Go-No-Go, Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT), STROOP |
|
No | No effect of treatment on cognitive performance. |
Stollery and Christian, 2013 [102] | M = 93 adults (35 male) aged 18–35 (M = 20.7) | Word list recall, spatial location recognition, category verification |
|
Unclear | No effect of glucose on any outcome. |
Stollery and Christian, 2015 [103] | N = 80 adults (26 male) aged 18–51 (M = 22.4) | PAL | T1:
|
9.5 h | T1: No effect of glucose on performance. T2: Glucose ingestion at retrieval led to improved retention (p = 0.016) and lower omission errors (p = 0.008). |
Stollery and Christian, 2016 [104] | N = 31 adults (9 male; glucose M age 22.5, SD = 1.5; placebo M age = 26.5, SD = 4.0) | Object-location binding task |
|
9.5 h | Improved location memory (p = 0.029) and object-location binding memory (p = 0.006) after glucose ingestion. No effect of glucose on object memory, retrieval time, or errors. Higher BGL associated with better location memory (p = 0.027) and binding memory (p = 0.012). |
Sünram-Lea et al., 2001 [105] | N = 60 adults aged 18–28 (M age = 21) | CVLT, Rey–Osterrieth complex figure drawing modified digit Span |
|
9 h or 2 h | Superior performance of glucose on immediate recall interference list (p < 0.01), no effect of fasting condition. Short-delay free recall improved after glucose (p < 0.001) and after breakfast compared to lunch and 9 h fasted (ps < 0.001). Short-delay cued recall, long-delay free recall, and long-delay recognition improved after glucose (ps < 0.001); no effect of fasting condition. Long-delay cued recall improved after glucose (p < 0.001), and breakfast outperformed lunch (p < 0.05). Rey–Osterrieth performance superior after glucose (p < 0.005), no effect of condition. No effect of drink or condition on digit span. |
Sünram-Lea et al., 2002a [106] | N = 80 (18 male) adults aged 18–29 (M age = 20) | Modified CVLT, modified Rey–Osterrieth, complex figure drawing, modified serial sevens |
|
2 h | Superior performance of glucose on immediate recall interference list and long-delay recognition (p < 0.01). Aspartame improved long-delay cued recall in conditions with no interference (low cognitive load) (p < 0.05). Glucose facilitation effect in other short- and long-delay cued and free recall seen only in interference conditions (high cognitive load). Rey–Osterrieth performance superior after glucose (p < 0.05). Serial sevens performance superior after glucose (p < 0.005). |
Sünram-Lea et al., 2002b [50] | N = 60 adults (26 male) aged 19–34 (M = 21) | Modified CVLT, serial sevens, ROCF (Delivered at baseline, 15 min, and 24 h after treatment) |
|
2 h | Improved performance on delayed free recall after glucose ingestion (p < 0.001). Improved 24 h delayed free recall (p < 0.001) and recognition (p = 0.007) after glucose ingestion. After 30 min, glucose performed better on delayed reproduction of figure (p = 0.03). No effect of glucose on immediate free recall or serial sevens performance. |
Sünram-Lea et al., 2008 [107] | N = 56 young adults aged 18–25 (M = 20) | Word list |
|
2 h | Increased recognition responses (recalling words associated with memories or experiences) following glucose ingestion (p = 0.04). |
Sünram-Lea et al., 2011 [108] | N = 30 young adults (6 male) aged 18–25 (M = 20) | Serial threes and sevens, word list |
|
12 h | Improved spatial working memory (Corsi block task) after 25 g glucose ingestion (p < 0.02). Improved immediate free recall after 25 g glucose ingestion (p < 0.01). Improved recognition performance after 25 g glucose ingestion (p < 0.05). No effect of glucose on numeric working memory (serial threes and sevens) or delayed free recall. Good glycaemic control associated with improved performance after 60 g glucose. Poor glycaemic control associated with improved performance after 15 g glucose. |
van der Zwaluw and et al., 2014 [61] |
N = 43 older adults (16 male; M age = 77.7, SD = 5.6) |
RAVLT, PAL, story recall, verbal fluency, digit span, STROOP, Test for Attentional Performance (TAP) |
|
10–12 h | Improved attention, working memory, and information processing after sucrose compared to placebo (p = 0.04). Improved tap flexibility. |
Walk et al., 2017 [60] | N = 113 children aged 9–10 | Erikson flanker task |
|
10 h | No effect of treatment on cognitive performance. When glycaemic regulation was adjusted for, only Maltodextrin had improved reaction time (p = 0.044). |
Winder and Borrill, 1998 [109] | 104 adults (52 male) aged 18–55 (mean = 29.2, SD = 9.23) | Name–face association task, selective reminding task |
|
No | No effect of glucose on performance. |
M = mean; RAVLT = Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Memory Test; WCST = Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; WAIS = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; TMT = trail making tests; RIPT = rapid information processing task; SART = sustained attention to response task; CVLT = California Verbal Learning Test; CDR = Cognitive Drug Research; ROCF = Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure; PAL = paired-associate learning; BGL = blood glucose level.