Abstract
Here, we review recent progress in the diagnosis and management of primary progressive aphasia—the language-led dementias. We pose six key unanswered questions that challenge current assumptions and highlight the unresolved difficulties that surround these diseases. How many syndromes of primary progressive aphasia are there—and is syndromic diagnosis even useful? Are these truly ‘language-led’ dementias? How can we diagnose (and track) primary progressive aphasia better? Can brain pathology be predicted in these diseases? What is their core pathophysiology? In addition, how can primary progressive aphasia best be treated? We propose that pathophysiological mechanisms linking proteinopathies to phenotypes may help resolve the clinical complexity of primary progressive aphasia, and may suggest novel diagnostic tools and markers and guide the deployment of effective therapies.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s00415-023-12030-4.
Keywords: Primary progressive aphasia, Semantic dementia, Logopenic aphasia, Frontotemporal dementia, Alzheimer’s disease
Introduction
In the 40 years since their modern rediscovery [1], the primary progressive aphasias (PPA) or ‘language-led dementias’ have transformed our picture of aphasia and selective neural system vulnerability to degenerative proteinopathies. In 2018 [2], we presented a general overview and clinical approach to PPA and its variant syndromes in this journal. Despite considerable ongoing clinical and research attention, they are still, in many ways, mysterious disorders. Though uncommon (with a collective population prevalence conservatively estimated at around three cases per 100,000 [3, 4]), they are immensely disabling and distressing, tending to affect people in later middle life and wreaking havoc on social and occupational functioning. These diseases of communication failure present unsolved challenges for neurobiological characterisation, diagnosis and management, liable to misinterpretation and delayed recognition [5, 6].
Here, we present an update on PPA, intended as a companion to our earlier paper [2] and directed to practising clinicians who see and care for these patients. We pose a series of ‘six questions in search of an answer’: each represents a key problem that has long bedevilled clinical practice in PPA, continues to provoke controversy and is likely to shape future progress. For each question (we argue), recent developments challenge conventional assumptions about these diseases. We conclude with a prospect of future progress.
How many PPA syndromes are there—and is a syndromic diagnosis even useful?
Core syndromes
PPA remains a quintessentially clinical diagnosis. Three canonical, clinico-anatomical variant syndromes of PPA are codified in the 2011 international consensus criteria developed by Gorno-Tempini et al. [7] (summarised for reference in Table S1). The nonfluent/agrammatic variant (nfvPPA) is led by impaired speech production with articulatory and/or grammatical errors, characteristically associated with predominantly left-sided anterior peri-Sylvian atrophy. The semantic variant (svPPA) is led by loss of vocabulary and impaired word knowledge due to a broader problem with semantic memory, consistently associated with focal, predominantly left-sided anterior, mesial and inferior temporal lobe atrophy. The logopenic variant (lvPPA) is led by word finding pauses, anomia and impaired verbal (phonological) working memory, manifesting as disproportionate difficulty repeating phrases over single words; its key neuroanatomical locus is the left temporo-parietal junction, though the extent and asymmetry of atrophy here varies widely between patients. A practical implementation of the diagnostic criteria is diagrammed as a ‘roadmap’ for rapid bedside diagnosis of PPA in Fig. 1.
Although the consensus diagnostic criteria have largely shaped current clinical and research practice in PPA, both clinical experience and published case series [8–12] indicate that there are frequent and significant exceptions to the standard formulation, variously designated ‘atypical PPA’, ‘mixed PPA’, ‘PPA-unclassifiable’, ‘PPA-extended’ or ‘PPA not otherwise specified’. In up to around a third of cases, the patient’s presentation may not fulfil criteria for a single canonical syndrome (due to fragmentary or mixed features) or prominent non-linguistic symptoms may accompany the language disturbance. Any consideration of the ‘atypicality’ of PPA should acknowledge the dynamic nature of these diseases: the duration of symptoms is important, as the language deficits of PPA syndromes tend to converge over time [13]. However, a mixed PPA phenotype may be evident from the earliest stages of the illness [11, 12]; even otherwise ‘typical’ nfvPPA, for example, can be accompanied by impaired single-word comprehension [14, 15], and lvPPA by agrammatism [16]. Among patients fulfilling criteria for a given canonical syndrome, there is substantial individual variation in the tempo, ordering and salience of particular features, and this heterogeneity is particularly marked for nfvPPA [5].
Responses to this problem reflect broadly dichotomous ‘splitter—lumper’ perspectives.
Syndrome splitting
From a ‘splitter’ perspective, the existence of separable sub-syndromes within the canonical syndromic variants of PPA has been argued for some time. Sub-syndromes are best defined for nfvPPA, splitting the cardinal speech apraxic and agrammatic components of the canonical syndrome. Primary progressive apraxia of speech (PPAOS) [15, 17–19] is characterised by reduced articulatory agility, variable phonemic distortions and ‘groping’ towards the target sound initially without associated linguistic deficits; there is often accompanying apraxia of orofacial movements (such as yawning or whistling to command) disproportionate to any limb apraxia [20]. Further subdivision of PPAOS into phonetic, prosodic and mixed variants has been proposed [21], while other motor speech disturbances (including various forms of dysarthria and stuttering) have been reported in nfvPPA and atypical PPA [22]; however, such fine-grained differentiation is difficult for most clinicians (and of uncertain clinical value). The second, major sub-syndrome of nfvPPA, progressive agrammatic aphasia without apraxia of speech, is less common than PPAOS [23, 24]: both in this variant and PPAOS, other features of nfvPPA tend to manifest later in the illness, though not invariably. Rarer sub-syndromes are also seen (see [2]). While verbal adynamia—impaired generation of propositional language—often accompanies nfvPPA (particularly in the context of parkinsonism [25, 26]), it can present in pure form years before the development of other aphasic or neurological deficits: ‘primary progressive dynamic aphasia’ [27, 28].
Sub-syndromes of lvPPA have also been delineated [15, 29, 30] though less well defined clinically than in nfvPPA: these vary in the severity of anomia, single-word repetition, word comprehension and expressive agrammatism. There is no sharp clinical demarcation between lvPPA, typical memory-led Alzheimer’s disease and its major ‘visual’ variant, posterior cortical atrophy [31–33]. Non-linguistic cognitive problems—including difficulties with episodic memory (forgetfulness, repetitiveness), praxis (e.g. use of household gadgets) and visuospatial awareness (e.g. locating items by sight, finding exits)—are more common at an earlier stage than in other PPA syndromes [12, 31, 34–36], and with disease evolution, most patients will meet criteria for Alzheimer’s dementia [12].
By contrast, svPPA is a highly coherent and distinctive syndrome; however, its demarcation from the closely related syndrome of right temporal lobe atrophy is an active issue [37–41]. Patients presenting with a right-sided atrophy profile mirroring that seen in typical svPPA usually have a clinical picture dominated by socio-emotional behavioural dysfunction, often accompanied by cognitive deficits (such as progressive prosopagnosia) that overlap with those in svPPA and may be at least partly semantically grounded [38, 42]. Indeed, similar behavioural and nonverbal cognitive deficits develop in svPPA as the disease advances; the right anterior temporal lobe is involved early in svPPA [43] and increasingly prominently as the illness progresses [37, 44], suggesting that the ‘left’ and ‘right’ temporal lobe atrophy syndromes lie on the same clinico-anatomical spectrum. On the other hand, there may be phenotypic features uniquely associated with the preferential targeting of the right anterior temporal lobe, linked to impaired homeostatic and hedonic regulation [39].
Beyond syndromes?
A nuanced version of the ‘lumper’ perspective has recently gained ascendancy in the nosology of PPA. The application of multidimensional methods such as principal component analysis to neuropsychological datasets derived from large PPA cohorts has highlighted the extent to which the canonical syndromes overlap and the graded nature of deficits within as well as between diagnostic groups [45]. Whereas (in line with clinical intuition) svPPA forms a distinct diagnostic category (based on within-group homogeneity and strong between-group differences), the categories are less clear for the other syndromes. This approach emphasises trans-syndromic factors that map onto particular cognitive factors (e.g. motor speech, phonological and semantic) over the canonical syndromes enshrined in the Gorno-Tempini criteria. lvPPA has been particularly foregrounded as a multidimensional disorder with highly heterogeneous individual profiles of cognitive impairment, reflecting the conjunction of differentiated linguistic and non-linguistic processes [31, 46]. One important practical message emerging from this work is that non-linguistic markers may assist in the clinical diagnosis of PPA (for example, nonverbal memory impairment discriminates lvPPA from nfvPPA [47]). As clinicians, we see considerable value in syndromic diagnosis (sensitively conveyed) for patients and caregivers. There is no doubt, however, that the diagnosis of PPA, and understanding what that means for the affected individual, entails much more than the application of a label.
Are these truly ‘language-led’ dementias?
Non-linguistic deficits
From a cognitive perspective, the essentially ‘aphasic’ status of each of the canonical PPA syndromes might be challenged. In the case of lvPPA, the appearance of closely linked non-aphasic cognitive deficits very early in the illness may warrant a reformulation of this syndrome to incorporate domain-general and domain-selective cognitive processes beyond language [31]. svPPA might be better described by its older name, ‘semantic dementia’ for this is integrally a disorder of semantic memory [48–50]—the dedicated memory system that mediates our knowledge of words, objects and concepts. Patients with svPPA in our experience invariably lose understanding of nonverbal signals, visual and other sensory objects as well as social concepts and ‘rules’, as the illness evolves—in keeping with a ‘pan-modal’ semantic impairment [51–55]. This syndrome might manifest as a disorder of language because, in general, language makes the most taxing demands on semantic processing in everyday life, or perhaps because a problem with language is relatively easily recognised and characterised. Even within the nfvPPA spectrum, the core deficit in the major variant—PPAOS—may lie principally with motor speech execution rather than with language processing per se [56]; written or typed communication is often initially largely intact, as indeed is verbal ideation [57], though in our experience aphasic deficits do supervene later.
It is well recognised that non-linguistic features commonly develop later in the course of PPA [12, 58]. Many patients with nfvPPA (or PPAOS) will develop atypical parkinsonism with clinical features of progressive supranuclear palsy or corticobasal syndrome [12, 18, 59, 60]. Features of motor neuron disease develop much less frequently, but have been variably reported to show a stronger association with nfvPPA [61] or svPPA [62]. Behavioural changes overlapping those seen in the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia (such as disinhibition, apathy and altered eating behaviour) are common across subtypes, most strikingly in svPPA [2, 12, 63]. Increasingly, however, nonverbal accompaniments are recognised even in early stage PPA which nevertheless fulfils criteria for a canonical syndrome [7]. It is of course understandable that the onset of speech and language difficulties should prompt the patient or their family to seek medical attention; however, this leaves open the possibility that less well defined (or less readily acknowledged) symptoms might actually lead the syndrome, or at least evolve in tandem with language dysfunction. Certain neuropsychiatric symptoms—such as compulsions in svPPA, depression in nfvPPA and anxiety in lvPPA—are exhibited by a substantial proportion of patients even at presentation [6, 12]. Moreover, early behavioural changes may be more subtle, yet still significant for daily life function (for example, an impoverished sense of humour in svPPA; withdrawal from social activities in lvPPA [5, 64]).
Hearing changes
Hearing impairment has emerged as an important non-linguistic issue in PPA, in line with the known close relations between language and auditory brain function [65]. Difficulty hearing in noisy environments is reported early in the illness across PPA syndromes, and all show reduced comprehension of acoustically degraded speech compared with healthy older listeners; these difficulties are most marked in lvPPA and nfvPPA [5, 66, 67]. Abnormal ‘auditory hedonic’ behaviours (strong liking or aversion for music or other sounds) frequently develop in svPPA [68]. Additional, relatively specific auditory cognitive phenotypes have been linked to each of the canonical syndromes. Patients with nfvPPA have reduced detection of sounds [69] and impaired perception of fundamental sound properties such as pitch, timbre and rhythm [53, 70–72] and their elaboration in prosody and music [73–75]. svPPA is associated with environmental sound agnosia [53] and phonagnosia (impaired voice recognition) [76], as well as tinnitus and hyperacusis [77]. lvPPA is particularly associated with impaired perception of phonemes [71, 78], while performance on hearing tasks more generally is modulated by auditory working memory capacity [70]. Atypical PPA presentations with prominent auditory impairments have also been identified, including progressive phonagnosia [79, 80], progressive word deafness (disproportionately impaired comprehension of spoken versus written words) [1, 81–83], and generalised auditory agnosia [84]. Taken together, these diverse non-linguistic auditory deficits suggest that PPA syndromes might be best characterised as pervasive ‘communication’ disorders: language output deficits in all three major PPA syndromes are likely to be influenced by disordered complex sound processing. The linkage between auditory and language output deficits is still poorly understood—here, speech repetition paradigms developed for lvPPA might perhaps serve as a model.
How can we diagnose (and track) PPA better?
Making the diagnosis
Timely diagnosis of PPA resolves uncertainty for patients and families, enables future planning and unlocks access to services and support. The dawning of the age of disease-modifying therapies for neurodegenerative disease [85], with its imperative to diagnose dementias earlier and more accurately, has only amplified this issue. However, the clinical diagnosis of PPA is often challenging even for those with extensive experience of the syndromes. We present some clinical, cognitive and neuroimaging ‘fingerprints’ that we have found particularly useful in the early detection of PPA syndromes in Table 1, an approach to bedside syndromic diagnosis in Fig. 1 and an outline of ancillary investigations in Fig. 2.
Table 1.
Initial suspiciona | Is it primary progressive aphasia? | ||
---|---|---|---|
Speech/language symptoms | Spelling errors (recent onset), work/social activities impacted, progressive decline | ||
Other symptoms | Difficulty using/learning devices, increased difficulty hearing in busy environments, changes in personality and inter-personal interactions (often subtle)b |
Syndromic diagnosis | nfvPPA | svPPA | lvPPA |
---|---|---|---|
Speech/language: history | Hesitant speech with errors of pronunciation and/or grammar, binary (especially ‘yes/no’) confusions | Asking the meaning of familiar words, keeping personal ‘dictionaries’ | Losing thread of sentences, frequent tip-of-tongue hesitations in conversation, occasional jargon (neologisms) |
Speech/language: examination |
Speech apraxia: obviously effortful, distorted speech sounds with ‘groping’ after target sound, reduced articulatory agility (difficulty repeating syllable strings, e.g. ‘pa-ke-ta’) Grammatical errorsc |
Impaired single word comprehensiond: unable to define words/identify items nominated by examiner Regularisation errors on reading aloud, e.g. sounding yacht as ‘yached’ (‘surface dyslexia’) |
Impaired verbal working memory: errors repeating phrases/sentences, more prominent with increasing target length, despite intact repetition of single words; reduced digit span |
Other clinical featurese |
Orofacial apraxia Neurological (especially extrapyramidal) signsf |
Visual agnosiag New interests (e.g. puzzles, numbers, music), clockwatching |
Difficulty with route-finding Limb apraxia Nonverbal episodic memory impairment Visuospatial difficulties |
Brain MRI atrophy signatures | Left anterior peri-Sylvian | Predominantly left anterior, mesial and inferior temporalh | Left posterior temporal/parietal |
In this table, we summarise symptoms, signs and other features we have found useful in corroborating an initial clinical suspicion of primary progressive aphasia (PPA) and in differentiating between the major variant syndromes: nonfluent/agrammatic (nfvPPA), semantic (svPPA) and logopenic (lvPPA) (see also Figs. 1, 2, 3). The table is informed in part by surveys of caregivers for people living with primary progressive aphasia [5, 112]; however, not all patients with a given diagnosis will exhibit every feature, the chronology of deficits varies between individuals and nonverbal cognitive, behavioural and neurological features tend to become more prominent later in the illness (see text). a These features in our experience tend to develop early across syndromes and are useful in distinguishing PPA from other presentations with ‘word finding difficulty’, including health anxiety or functional cognitive disorder; b may include changes in humour, sociability, initiative, etc. as well as mood symptoms; c tends to be less prominent than speech apraxia but may sometimes lead the presentation, more reliably detected in written sentences if speech is very effortful; d this must be distinguished from anomia due to impaired word retrieval, which is very prominent in both svPPA and lvPPA and commonly seen across the PPA spectrum, and in other patients presenting to memory clinics; e by definition, initially subordinate to the language problem but of diagnostic and clinical relevance if present; f mild features of atypical parkinsonism (or less commonly, motor neuron disease) may be evident at presentation; g unable to recognise and/or demonstrate use of familiar items by sight); h we question the diagnosis of svPPA if the characteristic atrophy profile is absent, MRI features in other syndromes are substantially more variable
Early on, the key diagnostic problem is often to decide whether a ‘language-led dementia’ is present [86], and secondarily to make a syndromic diagnosis, as the major syndromes have their own management needs and implications for future care planning. The first symptoms of PPA are insidious, often subtle and tend to be dynamic (often brought out by particular situations, such as speaking before an audience or on the telephone). Some symptoms (such as ‘tip-of-the-tongue’ hesitations in lvPPA [87]) are commonly experienced by normal speakers. Some patients presenting with health anxiety or functional cognitive disorder describe prominent speech and language symptoms: aside from lack of progression (and sometimes, abrupt onset), positive clues to these diagnoses on history include elaborate symptom descriptions and discrepancies between the severity of the subjective complaint and daily life performance [88]. On examination, hallmarks of a functional speech disorder include stuttering or prosodic changes (‘childlike’ or a ‘foreign accent’) with pronounced variability (severe disruption interspersed with segments of normal speech) and distractibility [89]. The differential diagnosis of PPA is fairly limited. Canonical PPA variants do not have close equivalents in the classical syndromes of post-stroke aphasia (though detailed comparisons are surprisingly sparse [45, 90, 91]); this distinction is usually obvious on history, but may be confounded if a PPA syndrome is reported to have begun ‘suddenly’ (usually, this will be misattribution to a sentinel event) or where there are substantial comorbid cerebrovascular changes on brain MRI.
Clinical diagnostic ‘algorithms’ for PPA (see Figs. 1 and 2) tend to be directed towards established disease, though certain clues can be helpful in picking up problems at presentation (Table 1). Detailed and quantitative cognitive profiling by an expert neuropsychologist is invaluable in corroborating the initial clinical impression [92]; however, this is time-consuming and access to a neuropsychology department may be limited. Several cognitive instruments have been designed to facilitate the relatively rapid diagnosis of PPA syndromes in the clinic [93]. These include the Progressive Aphasia Language Scale (PALS) [94], the Sydney Language Battery [95], the Screening for Aphasia in NeuroDegeneration Battery (SAND) [96], the Progressive Aphasia RatIng Scale (PARIS) [97], and most recently, the Mini Linguistic State Examination [98] (which rests on an analysis of error types) and an online calculator based on the Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination version III [99] (which profiles subscores across language and other cognitive domains). These instruments have yet to be assessed head-to-head (or indeed, in combination) but we would emphasise that some clinical experience of PPA is required to use them optimally (interpretation of errors, for example, is often not straightforward). As always in neurology, the way a patient performs a test may be as informative as their score (the sense of effort associated with speaking, for example, is a distinguishing hallmark of nfvPPA (Table 1)). There is growing interest in automated diagnosis based on machine learning approaches [100] and the identification of digital markers that might detect very early stage PPA (based, for example, on daily life conversational language or device use [101, 102]), though their clinical utility has yet to be established.
The value of neuroimaging
As with any dementia, brain imaging (wherever possible, MRI) is central to the diagnosis of PPA. In addition to excluding rare mimic syndromes, the profile of atrophy on MRI can substantiate the clinical impression (see Fig. 2). In our experience, typical svPPA is invariably associated with pronounced, selective (usually left-sided) anterior temporal lobe atrophy, even when the patient presents with only mild deficits. By contrast, in nfvPPA and lvPPA neuroanatomical correlation is less straightforward. Syndromic MRI signatures have been identified—in the nfvPPA spectrum, PPAOS and agrammatic aphasia have separable group-level atrophy profiles, predominantly involving premotor/supplementary motor cortex and inferior frontal/prefrontal cortices, respectively [17, 19, 23, 24]; while in lvPPA, involvement of left temporo-parietal cortex has been widely identified as a critical signature [31, 36, 103]. However, in individual patients MRI findings may be subtle or not in keeping with the level of clinical impairment. Serial MRI to detect progressive regional volume loss after an approximately 12-month interval may be helpful, as may metabolic neuroimaging with fluorodeoxyglucose positron emission tomography or single photon emission computed tomography: in nfvPPA and particularly in lvPPA, these techniques may detect regional neural hypometabolism ahead of any structural brain degeneration [104] but reporting is often not quantitative. Newer MRI techniques such as quantitative analysis of diffusion-weighted sequences to capture microstructural disorganisation show promise as sensitive markers of PPA [105] but have not yet entered clinical practice. Accurate interpretation of MRI and other neuroimaging modalities in PPA benefits from close acquaintance with this disease spectrum.
Prognosis and staging
Related to the early diagnosis of PPA is the problem of prognosis, which remains very imprecise in the individual case. Both prognosis and care planning would be greatly facilitated by a clinical staging system for PPA [58]. However, this is a daunting proposition, due to the heterogeneous and dynamic, nonlinear trajectories of these diseases (patients and their caregivers often describe fluctuations in their ability to perform specific tasks from day to day and periods of relative stability or apparently more rapid decline) as well as the relatively loose mapping between clinical status and objective disease markers. Standard instruments for assessing dementia stage and severity rest primarily on concepts formulated by clinicians (rather than the lived experience of patients and caregivers) and do not assess language and communication functions in detail [106–110]. Clinical severity scales that are most relevant to PPA (such as the Progressive Aphasia Severity Scale (PASS) [111]) have limited coverage of non-linguistic features. Furthermore, even within the domain of language, it is challenging to identify cognitive measures that track disease severity across the PPA spectrum or even within syndromes (naming, for example, falls rapidly to floor in svPPA but this does not reflect daily life functionality).
We have recently created a symptom-led ‘PPA Progression Planning Aid’ (PPA-Squared), based on surveys of caregivers enrolled in PPA support groups in the United Kingdom and Australia about their lived experience of the illness [5, 112]. The proposed staging scheme and functional impairment severity scale cover three key domains of daily living (communication, nonverbal thinking and personality, and personal care and wellbeing) for each of the canonical PPA syndromes. For each syndrome, the scheme delineates six clinical stages covering all three functional domains; within each functional domain, we propose five severity levels (ranging from 0 (Presymptomatic) to 4 (Severe)) and associated functional milestones with implications for care (for example, all patients with later stage PPA are at risk of developing dysphagia; however, this occurs earlier in nfvPPA than other syndromes). This scheme will require prospective corroboration and validation in larger and more diverse patient cohorts, including atypical and mixed PPA cases.
Any staging scheme for PPA should be anchored in disease biology and objective biomarkers of disease evolution. Symptom-led stages should be correlated with neuropsychological scores, structural and functional neuroanatomy and extant rating scales for these diseases [109, 111, 113, 114], as well as CSF and other laboratory indices: the latter may include blood markers such as neurofilament light chain (NfL), which has shown some promise for disease monitoring though has not yet entered clinical practice [115]. Development of quantitative symptom severity scales would facilitate computational modelling approaches, which have been used successfully for tracking of progression in a number of neurodegenerative diseases [116, 117]. Multi-modality indices that integrate clinical measures with neuroimaging and other objective disease biomarkers may provide the most robust signals of disease progression and prognosis [118].
The factors that drive individual variability in the clinical expression and course of PPA are largely unknown, though constitutive genomic and epigenetic differences in language circuit organisation, interacting with specific vulnerabilities such as developmental dyslexia, may play a role [119]. One key question often posed by patients and families concerns life expectancy: this, too, varies widely from patient to patient; however, survival is modulated by language phenotype (on average, substantially longer in svPPA than other syndromes [4, 119, 120]), development of motor features [12] and atypical rightward cerebral lateralisation [121, 122] (probably associated with shorter survival). For reasons presently unclear, the tempo of illness progression seems to be particularly variable in lvPPA [123, 124].
Beyond English
A major limitation of the literature on PPA to date has been its heavy emphasis on native English-speaking (and monolingual) patients. Epidemiological, clinical and neuropathological data on PPA in other languages are presently very incomplete. It is quite unlikely a priori that the clinical experience of English-speaking PPA can be simply or validly generalised to the wider world; rather, the linguistic characteristics and socio-cultural context of particular languages are anticipated to modify the presentation and course of the major syndromes. Indeed, the global spectrum of PPA might include syndromes that are rare or unrecognised in English-speaking patients. Clues to this are already evident: for example, nfvPPA in a Cantonese-speaking patient was associated with distinctive tone production, perception and dysgraphic deficits [125], and it has been suggested that dysgraphia phenotypes in Chinese language users might be used to screen for PPA and to classify PPA subtypes [126]. Word deafness and auditory agnosia appear to be relatively more common presentations of PPA in Japanese language users [84, 127–129]. On the other hand, phenomena such as ‘surface dyslexia/dysgraphia’ (the regularisation of irregularly spelled words when speaking or writing, due to breakdown of vocabulary knowledge, typically in svPPA) may take a different form in fully orthographically regular languages [130] and work is needed to identify equivalent cross-linguistic deficits. Encouragingly, published cohort studies devoted to PPA in languages other than English appear to be on the increase, both for European [12, 131–135] and other world languages [136–138]. In addition, screening instruments for PPA are being applied to other languages, both in translation [139] and as bespoke tools [140]. Looking forward, systematic international collaboration will be essential if we are to synthesise a comprehensive framework for PPA research relevant to diverse linguistic and cultural settings.
Can brain pathology be predicted in PPA?
Signatures of neuropathology
Predicting the underlying proteinopathy in PPA is potentially relevant to prognosis [119] and care planning, and will be increasingly required for the rational targeting of disease-modifying therapies. At present, however, this is far from straightforward.
Several phenotypic associations with neuropathology have been consistently observed across PPA series, albeit with varying estimates of their sensitivity and specificity [119, 141–144]; moreover, group-level associations potentially allow for considerable individual variation, so their value as ‘phenotypic biomarkers’ in clinical practice is uncertain [119]. The strongest and most consistent association links svPPA to TAR-DBA-binding-protein (TDP)-43 type C pathology, in line with the striking clinico-anatomical coherence of this syndrome [48, 49]; cases with predominant right-sided temporal lobe atrophy have more diverse neuropathological associations, supporting a distinct neurobiological status [38, 39, 145, 146]. lvPPA is widely regarded as a predictor of Alzheimer pathology and this association provides a neuropathological substrate for its extensive phenotypic overlap with typical amnestic and ‘visual’ variant Alzheimer syndromes [31, 32]. However, the strength of the association varies between series [8, 16]: an important minority of lvPPA cases have non-Alzheimer pathologies [30, 119, 147–149], while Alzheimer’s disease also presents as nfvPPA or ‘mixed’ PPA [141, 143]. In keeping with its clinical diversity, the pathology underpinning nfvPPA is heterogeneous: most patients will have a primary tauopathy such as progressive supranuclear palsy, corticobasal degeneration or (uncommonly) Pick’s disease, but a sizeable proportion of cases have TDP-43 or Alzheimer pathology [119, 141, 142]. These neuropathological associations of nfvPPA may partition according to the sub-syndromic variant phenotype: PPAOS is strongly associated with underlying tauopathy [19, 56], whereas progressive agrammatic aphasia (without apraxia of speech) may be more likely, based on limited evidence, to have TDP-43 pathology [23, 149]. The development of associated neurological features may provide a strong signal of the causative brain pathology, progressive supranuclear palsy syndrome reliably predicting tauopathy and motor neuron features TDP-43 pathology [62, 142, 150].
Most cases of PPA are sporadic [151, 152]; however, a significant minority are genetically mediated, with autosomal dominant inheritance. The responsible pathogenic mutation usually resides in one of the three major genes causing frontotemporal dementia: progranulin (GRN) (the most frequent genetic cause), chromosome 9 opening reading frame 72 (C9orf72) or (rarely) microtubule-associated protein tau (MAPT) [151–153]. Among the three canonical PPA syndromes, nfvPPA is the most likely to be hereditary; causative mutations in GRN, MAPT and (particularly in association with motor neuron features) C9orf72 have been identified [148, 150, 153, 154]. Cases of more or less typical lvPPA associated with GRN mutations have been identified in a number of series [119, 148, 149, 152, 155]. svPPA is rarely genetic but MAPT mutations appear more likely to cause this than other PPA syndromes; pathogenic mutations in C9orf72 and other genes have also been reported [4, 141, 156, 157].
Pursuing the proteinopathy
After a clinico-anatomical syndromic diagnosis of PPA is reached, further investigations to determine the underlying proteinopathy may be appropriate (see Fig. 2). At present, this is mainly to assess for Alzheimer biomarkers in CSF (elevated phospho-tau, total tau and tau:beta-amyloid1–42 ratio; reduced beta-amyloid42:40 ratio) or on radio-ligand positron emission tomography with Pittsburgh compound B or fluorine 18-labelled tracers (florbetapir, florbetaben, and flutemetamol) (increased uptake with binding of beta-amyloid plaques), as this may help guide a trial of symptomatic therapy for Alzheimer’s disease. Genetic screening should be considered in younger patients, particularly where there is a suggestive family history of younger onset dementia in a first-degree relative (especially frontotemporal dementia or motor neuron disease), the PPA phenotype is atypical (as a genetic basis may be somewhat more likely in this scenario [148, 149, 153, 158, 159]) and/or there is strikingly asymmetric fronto-temporo-parietal atrophy on MRI (as this may signal a GRN mutation [142, 160]). However, the decision whether to broach genetic testing should always be weighed carefully: this is chiefly of clinical relevance for potentially at-risk family members (especially children). It should also be kept in mind that even where Alzheimer biomarkers are positive, conjoint (sporadic or genetic) pathologies may also be present and may drive the phenotype [30, 119, 143].
On balance, the value of both clinical characterisation and biomarker studies in predicting brain pathology across the PPA spectrum remains limited. Given the dearth of clear phenotypic predictors of neuropathology, and the issue of mixed pathologies [161], reliable and practical imaging and/or fluid biomarkers of non-Alzheimer tau and TDP-43 proteinopathies are sorely needed, particularly with a view to trials of disease-modifying therapies in PPA and other frontotemporal dementias. To date, however, these have remained elusive [162, 163].
What is the core pathophysiology of PPA?
From a clinical perspective, a quest for fundamental neural mechanisms driving PPA syndromes may seem rather theoretical. We would argue, however, that clinicians have a stake in this enterprise—on account of the very complexity of PPA and the difficulties that surround diagnosis and neuropathological correlation. There are several motivations to move beyond traditional neurolinguistic accounts of these syndromes to address core pathophysiological mechanisms of PPA [164]. Neurolinguistic functions conventionally sampled in syndrome characterisation such as naming, grammar or speech repetition encompass a number of component processes. A pathophysiological account might dissect these and also suggest links between language dysfunction and the apparently disparate, non-linguistic cognitive and behavioural impairments that together constitute PPA syndromes. Further, by addressing the roots of these syndromes in neural circuit dysfunction, such an account might move closer to the underlying proteinopathies that target these circuits, open up fresh avenues for characterising PPA using physiologically informed methods (such as autonomic recordings, animal models, artificial neural networks, and functional neuroimaging [71, 75, 165–170]) and inform the identification of new physiological biomarkers. These (in contrast to conventional biomarkers) would provide a read-out of neuronal function, sufficiently dynamic to detect very early disease and to assess the impact of therapies, including novel indices such as perceptual learning of degraded speech (an index of retained cerebral plasticity [67]).
The neural networks implicated in the major PPA syndromes have been delineated in some detail, and network changes in these syndromes extend widely beyond the ‘hubs’ of maximal atrophy seen on clinical brain MRI scans [171, 172]. Each of the canonical PPA phenotypes is predominantly associated with a particular neural network and underlying proteinopathy (svPPA with the anterior temporal semantic appraisal network and TDP-43 type C; PPAOS with the dorsal prefrontal motor network and tauopathy; lvPPA with the temporo-parietal ‘default mode’ network and Alzheimer’s disease). This implies that the neural circuitry mediating the phenotype is preferentially vulnerable to that proteinopathy: a concept relevant to a broad range of neurodegenerative diseases, which we have termed ‘molecular nexopathy’ [172, 173]. Why and how pathogenic protein properties map onto neural circuit characteristics to create a molecular nexopathy is still poorly understood; however, this formulation has several important implications relevant to our understanding of PPA.
First, the neural circuits targeted in these PPA ‘nexopathies’ are engaged in a number of other cognitive operations besides language. Language functions depend on iterative transformations of sensory signals to meaning and/or actions, characterised neurophysiologically by spatio-temporal integration, nonlinear coding, plasticity and reciprocal interactions between processing stages [164, 174]; thus, language is likely a priori to be exquisitely vulnerable to the effects of neurodegenerative pathologies that disrupt neural circuit integrity. However, the physiological ‘lesion’ is likely to disrupt a number of additional, more generic cognitive and behavioural functions, potentially explaining why neurodegenerative pathologies that target this circuitry are heralded by speech and language deficits as part of a matrix of associated impairments.
Second, the speech and language deficits caused by proteinopathies need not closely resemble post-stroke aphasic syndromes (which, among other factors, are dictated by vascular anatomy and represent a sudden interruption rather than insidious erosion of neural network function [91]). Thus, we should anticipate that PPA syndromes will differ from the neurolinguistic syndromes of classical aphasiology. Relatedly, proteinopathies may target particular neural elements or connection types, rather than brain regions per se: PPA syndromes might, therefore, encompass deficits arising from different functional brain networks, potentially accounting for ‘atypical’ phenotypes (such as the aphasic syndrome associated with GRN mutations [15, 103, 159]). Vulnerability to proteinopathies may be modulated by neurodevelopmental vulnerabilities and genomic profiles, though any relationship is likely to be complex [119, 175–179].
We have previously proposed candidate core pathophysiological processes that might underpin each of the canonical PPA syndromes [164] (Fig. 3). These rest on a very general model of brain (neural circuit) operation, ‘predictive coding’, according to which stored predictions about incoming sensory data are compared iteratively with actual incoming data and updated so as to minimise error [180]. Perception as well as complex output behaviours such as speech can be understood using a predictive coding framework [169, 181]. In the case of the brain circuitry mediating language, we conceptualise predictive coding as a series of neural ‘template-matching’ operations, whereby incoming data are reconciled with stored representations (for phonemes, objects, motor routines, etc.) to generate an output. How neurodegenerative proteinopathies might degrade neural circuit operation to disrupt representations and thus ultimately language output and other behaviours has not been worked out in detail for PPA. In the case of svPPA—arguably the best defined of the canonical PPA syndromes clinically and pathologically—there is evidence to suggest how a complex disease phenotype with puzzling neuroanatomical focality [44] might be deconstructed to its pathophysiological building blocks, at the level of histopathological effects on neural circuit function [71, 166]. The semantic appraisal network—anchored in the anterior temporal lobes—depends for its operation on strongly convergent integration of sensory data regularities into multimodal semantic representations [49, 182]; such integration is supported by short range interneuronal inhibitory (GABA-ergic) connections that ‘channel’ neural information flow [183]. These connections are targeted by TDP-43 type C in svPPA [166], leading to an erosion of semantic representations that predicts the cognitive and behavioural manifestations of svPPA.
How can PPA best be treated?
Our last question may surprise many neurologists. In fact, despite the present lack of effective disease-modifying therapies for PPA, there is much that can be done to help these patients live as well as possible with their illness. Management begins with the diagnostic process, building up a picture of the impact of the illness on this patient and their family, their social and occupational functioning, the profile of impairments and retained competencies. Once made, the diagnosis and its implications must be carefully and unhurriedly discussed, the general practitioner and appropriate local services and supports should be engaged and future planning should begin. We find it useful to review disease impacts under the broad categories of communication, nonverbal cognition, social and emotional behaviour and physical neurological features [5]: these require periodic review to address dynamically changing needs, as the illness unfolds. Psychological as well practical support should be offered to patients and caregivers, and many will benefit from contact with a local or national support group [184, 185]. A problem-based approach to symptoms might to some extent be applied in any patient with PPA [31, 45], though we find that the major syndromes have specific needs and capacities that tend to dictate management [5].
Non-pharmacological strategies
Speech and language therapy has an integral part to play in the management of PPA [186], though there remain significant inequities and barriers to accessing it (not least a lack of knowledge about its potential benefits in PPA [187–189]). The speech and language therapist can assist with the diagnostic assessment [92], which will also facilitate the design of tailored interventions to support communication. Rather than one-size-fits-all, deficit-focussed or rehabilitative cognitive ‘re-training’ approaches developed for post-stroke aphasia, personalised ‘functional communication interventions’ that promote participation in activities and life situations by engaging a communication partner (usually, the patient’s primary caregiver) are more likely to be effective in PPA [186, 190–192]. Script training has been shown to improve speech fluency and grammar in nfvPPA [193] while interventions focussed on naming and word retrieval can improve communication in all PPA syndromes, particularly svPPA and lvPPA [194, 195]. Such interventions are most beneficial when introduced early in the illness, when they are customised to be personally relevant to the patient and if they can be scaled to the level of natural discourse [196–198]. Communication partner training such as Better Conversations with PPA addresses the essential role of those close to the patient in everyday social interactions: such training can identify communication barriers and promote strategies to overcome them across the PPA spectrum [189, 199–201], while addressing a key everyday management need identified by people with PPA and their care partners [202]. Therapists can also provide guidance in the use of ancillary communication devices, which may be a communication lifeline where the patient’s chief limitation is motor speech production (such as PPAOS [57]) Equally, however, ‘low tech’ communication aids should not be overlooked: a card, booklet or bracelet that the patient can carry may be crucial in maintaining independence. Interventions should, of course, be dynamically customised for evolving needs at different stages of PPA [5]. Later in the course of PPA, the speech and language therapist brings expertise in the management of dysphagia: anticipating problems (particularly in nfvPPA), assessing swallowing safety, and advising on appropriate eating and dietary strategies and assisted nutrition, including percutaneous endoscopic gastrostomy.
There is increasing interest in non-invasive neuromodulation with transcranial direct current or magnetic brain stimulation to improve language function in PPA, with reports of benefit in case studies and small cohorts [203–205]: coupled with behavioural interventions, such approaches might constitute a practical application of pathophysiological principles; however, larger trials are awaited. Similar considerations apply to the development of ‘smart’ assistive hearing devices and interventions to address central auditory dysfunction in PPA [206, 207].
Non-pharmacological strategies for other management issues that commonly arise in people with PPA, such as challenging behaviours, parkinsonism and mood disturbance, are similar to the wider frontotemporal dementia spectrum [208].
Pharmacological treatment
On grounds of clinical pragmatism, we tend to offer a trial of symptomatic therapy with acetylcholinesterase inhibitors and/or memantine in patients with lvPPA and nfvPPA (where Alzheimer pathology is a consideration), although any benefit is usually modest and care is needed to avoid exacerbating concomitant behavioural or extrapyramidal features with acetylcholinesterase inhibitors [209, 210]. Behavioural and psychological symptoms such as depression, anxiety, impulsivity and aggression may be treated with serotonergic antidepressants or new-generation atypical antipsychotics, in parallel with non-pharmacologic approaches [211, 212]. Particular care is needed in the use of neuroleptic medications, as patients with PPA tend to be sensitive to extrapyramidal side effects.
The recent exciting developments towards effective disease modification based on phase III clinical trials of anti-amyloid monoclonal antibody therapy with lecanemab and donanemab in Alzheimer’s disease are a signal of hope for people living with PPA [85]. Patients with atypical Alzheimer variant syndromes (including lvPPA) may be eligible for these new disease-modifying treatments, though current guidelines also emphasise that safety and efficacy in these syndromes have not been specifically studied [213]. This also highlights the importance of early, expert diagnosis to determine whether an individual presenting with speech or language symptoms has a clinico-radiological phenotype consistent with primary Alzheimer pathology—with a view both to obtaining biomarker support and (if positive) determining whether this might be incidental to a concomitant culprit proteinopathy [30, 143]. There has been relatively less progress in disease-modifying therapy for non-Alzheimer pathologies in the frontotemporal dementia spectrum, but this is an increasingly active area of research, with a number of candidate agents in early stage trials targeting sporadic tau and TDP-43 pathologies as well as the pathogenic pathways of GRN and C9orf72 mutations [214]. As well as specific protein biomarkers, bespoke staging and outcome measures appropriate for tracking change in PPA are urgently needed to support clinical trials [5]. Evidence for retained neural plasticity across the PPA spectrum [67, 167, 215] bodes well for the eventual deployment of disease-modifying agents in these diseases, potentially coupled with neurorehabilitation interventions to harness brain repair mechanisms.
The next 40 years
The future of PPA research and clinical practice and ultimately, effective disease modification for people living with PPA will depend on finding answers to these and other questions. A reappraisal of these diseases seems in order—existing diagnostic criteria do not capture their complexity and heterogeneity, and this issue is practically relevant both for counselling of patients and caregivers and for the development of new diagnostic tools and markers. These are highly dynamic and often protean disorders—in the deficits they manifest, their time course, their sensitivity to developmental and contextual factors acting on the language system, and their care needs and response to interventions. The determinants of individual variation are still largely unknown but will likely be crucial in the design of personalised therapy. PPA is rare, meaning that no single centre can build up a comprehensive picture of its full clinical diversity and the common neurobiological threads that may bind syndromes—the field stands in urgent need of international, multi-centre collaboration, across the barriers of language, culture and socioeconomic status. We need systematic assessment protocols that encompass non-linguistic dimensions of nonverbal auditory communication, behaviour and physical disability; these often determine how well someone lives with the illness. We also need a much clearer picture of how illness evolves in the individual person—so that needs can be anticipated and supports engaged proactively, and to detect earliest stage disease reliably when opportunities for effective intervention are maximal. Staging in turn will support larger scale clinical trials in PPA. However, the promise of disease-modifying therapies targeting specific proteinopathies will only be realised with more detailed neurobiological models of PPA and how molecular pathologies link to phenotypes—in this enterprise, genetic forms of PPA might serve as instructive test cases, and mixed phenotypes warrant more attention, as these often signal underlying Alzheimer pathology [141, 143]. Designing rational interventions for PPA—both pharmacological and non-pharmacological—will entail an appreciation of preserved capacities as well as impairments [57]. This speaks to the wider theme of pathophysiology, which we would argue underwrites all of the questions posed here and offers the best prospect of a transformative solution—both by providing rapid and specific readouts of neural dysfunction to guide therapeutic decision making, and by informing the search for novel intervention targets and brain repair and compensatory mechanisms [67, 216]. Clinical observation will remain essential, not least in revealing fresh facets of these kaleidoscopic diseases [217]. There are grounds for optimism that the next 40 years will see the ‘language-led dementias’ rendered treatable.
Supplementary Information
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the Alzheimer’s Society, Alzheimer’s Research UK, the UK Dementia Research Institute at UCL, the National Institute for Health Research University College London Hospitals Biomedical Research Centre and the National Brain Appeal (Frontotemporal Dementia Research Studentship in Memory of David Blechner). CJDH and CRM are supported by a NIHR [Invention for Innovation (NIHR204280)] grant and CJDH received a Wellcome Institutional Strategic Support Fund Award (204841/Z/16/Z). AV is supported by a NIHR Advanced Fellowship NIHR302240. JDR has been supported by a Miriam Marks Brain Research UK Senior Fellowship, a Medical Research Council Clinician Scientist Fellowship (MR/M008525/1) and the National Institute for Health Research Rare Disease Translational Research Collaboration (BRC149/NS/MH). JDW is supported by the Alzheimer’s Society, Alzheimer’s Research UK, the Royal National Institute for Deaf People (Discovery Grant G105_WARREN) and the National Institute for Health Research University College London Hospitals Biomedical Research Centre. This research was funded in part by UKRI and Wellcome Trust (Grant 204841/Z/16/Z). For the purpose of Open Access, the authors have applied a Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) public copyright licence to any Author Accepted Manuscript version arising from this submission. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NIHR or the Department of Health and Social Care.
Data availability
Not applicable.
Declarations
Conflicts of interest
The authors declare they have no conflicts of interest.
Ethical standard
Research studies relevant to this work have been approved by the London—Queen Square Research Ethics Committee and conducted in accordance with the ethical standards laid down in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments. All research participants gave informed consent prior to their inclusion in the studies.
References
- 1.Mesulam Marsel M. Slowly progressive aphasia without generalized dementia. Ann Neurol. 1982;11:592–598. doi: 10.1002/ana.410110607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Marshall CR, Hardy CJD, Volkmer A, et al. Primary progressive aphasia: a clinical approach. J Neurol. 2018;265:1474–1490. doi: 10.1007/s00415-018-8762-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Magnin E, Démonet JF, Wallon D, et al. Primary progressive aphasia in the network of French Alzheimer plan memory centers. J Alzheimer’s Dis. 2016;54:1459–1471. doi: 10.3233/JAD-160536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Coyle-Gilchrist ITS, Dick KM, Patterson K, et al. Prevalence, characteristics, and survival of frontotemporal lobar degeneration syndromes. Neurology. 2016;86:1736–1743. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002638. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hardy CJ, Taylor-Rubin C, Taylor B, et al. Symptom-led staging for primary progressive aphasia. medRxiv. 2023 doi: 10.1101/2023.03.13.23286972. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Magnin E, Chopard G, Ferreira S, et al. Initial neuropsychological profile of a series of 20 patients with logopenic variant of primary progressive aphasia. J Alzheimer’s Dis. 2013;36:799–808. doi: 10.3233/JAD-122335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Gorno-Tempini ML, Hillis AE, Weintraub S, et al. Classification of primary progressive aphasia and its variants. Neurology. 2011;76:1006–1014. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31821103e6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Louwersheimer E, Keulen MA, Steenwijk MD, et al. Heterogeneous language profiles in patients with primary progressive aphasia due to Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimer’s Dis. 2016;51:581–590. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150812. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Sajjadi SA, Patterson K, Arnold RJ, et al. Primary progressive aphasia: a tale of two syndromes and the rest. Neurology. 2012;78:1670–1677. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182574f79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Wicklund MR, Duffy JR, Strand EA, et al. Quantitative application of the primary progressive aphasia consensus criteria. Neurology. 2014;82:1119–1126. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Mesulam MM, Wieneke C, Thompson C, et al. Quantitative classification of primary progressive aphasia at early and mild impairment stages. Brain. 2012;135:1537–1553. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Ulugut H, Stek S, Wagemans LEE, et al. The natural history of primary progressive aphasia: beyond aphasia. J Neurol. 2022;269:1375–1385. doi: 10.1007/s00415-021-10689-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Rogalski E, Cobia D, Harrison TM, et al. Progression of language decline and cortical atrophy in subtypes of primary progressive aphasia. Neurology. 2011;76:1804–1810. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31821ccd3c. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Schaeverbeke J, Gabel S, Meersmans K, et al. Single-word comprehension deficits in the nonfluent variant of primary progressive aphasia. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2018 doi: 10.1186/S13195-018-0393-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Rohrer JD, Rossor MN, Warren JD. Syndromes of nonfluent primary progressive aphasia: a clinical and neurolinguistic analysis. Neurology. 2010;75:603–610. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181ed9c6b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Mazzeo S, Polito C, Padiglioni S, et al. Linguistic profiles, brain metabolic patterns and rates of amyloid-β biomarker positivity in patients with mixed primary progressive aphasia. Neurobiol Aging. 2020;96:155–164. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2020.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Josephs KA, Duffy JR, Strand EA, et al. Characterizing a neurodegenerative syndrome: primary progressive apraxia of speech. Brain. 2012;135:1522–1536. doi: 10.1093/brain/aws032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Josephs KA, Duffy JR, Strand EA, et al. The evolution of primary progressive apraxia of speech. Brain. 2014;137:2783–2795. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Josephs KA, Duffy JR, Strand EA, et al. Clinicopathological and imaging correlates of progressive aphasia and apraxia of speech. Brain. 2006;129:1385–1398. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl078. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Morihara K, Ota S, Kakinuma K, et al. Buccofacial apraxia in primary progressive aphasia. Cortex. 2023;158:61–70. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2022.10.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Utianski RL, Duffy JR, Clark HM, et al. Prosodic and phonetic subtypes of primary progressive apraxia of speech. Brain Lang. 2018;184:54–65. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2018.06.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Staiger A, Schroeter ML, Ziegler W, et al. Speech motor profiles in primary progressive aphasia. Am J Speech-Lang Pathol. 2023;32:1296–1321. doi: 10.1044/2023_AJSLP-22-00319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Tetzloff KA, Duffy JR, Clark HM, et al. Progressive agrammatic aphasia without apraxia of speech as a distinct syndrome. Brain. 2019;142:2466–2482. doi: 10.1093/BRAIN/AWZ157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Tetzloff KA, Duffy JR, Clark HM, et al. Longitudinal structural and molecular neuroimaging in agrammatic primary progressive aphasia. Brain. 2018;141:302–317. doi: 10.1093/BRAIN/AWX293. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Rohrer JD, Paviour D, Bronstein AM, et al. Progressive supranuclear palsy syndrome presenting as progressive nonfluent aphasia: a neuropsychological and neuroimaging analysis. Mov Disord. 2010;25:179–188. doi: 10.1002/mds.22946. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Magdalinou NK, Golden HL, Nicholas JM, et al. Verbal adynamia in parkinsonian syndromes: behavioral correlates and neuroanatomical substrate. Neurocase. 2018;24:204–212. doi: 10.1080/13554794.2018.1527368. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Warren JD, Warren JE, Fox NC, Warrington EK. Nothing to say, something to sing: primary progressive dynamic aphasia. Neurocase. 2003;9:140–155. doi: 10.1076/neur.9.2.140.15068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Berthier ML, Hoet F, Beltrán-Corbellini Á, et al. Case Report: Barely able to speak, can’t stop echoing: echolalic dynamic aphasia in progressive supranuclear palsy. Front Aging Neurosci. 2021 doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2021.635896. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Leyton CE, Hodges JR, McLean CA, et al. Is the logopenic-variant of primary progressive aphasia a unitary disorder? Cortex. 2015;67:122–133. doi: 10.1016/J.CORTEX.2015.03.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Chokesuwattanaskul A, Marshall CR, Van Harskamp N, et al. Primary progressive aphasia: ReADing the clinical GRANularity. Pract Neurol. 2022;22:509–514. doi: 10.1136/practneurol-2022-003460. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Ramanan S, Irish M, Patterson K, et al. Understanding the multidimensional cognitive deficits of logopenic variant primary progressive aphasia. Brain. 2022;145:2955–2966. doi: 10.1093/brain/awac208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Warren JD, Fletcher PD, Golden HL. The paradox of syndromic diversity in Alzheimer disease. Nat Rev Neurol. 2012;8:451–464. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2012.135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Crutch SJ, Lehmann M, Warren JD, Rohrer JD. The language profile of posterior cortical atrophy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2013;84:460–466. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2012-303309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Piguet O, Leyton CE, Gleeson LD, et al. Memory and emotion processing performance contributes to the diagnosis of non-semantic primary progressive aphasia syndromes. J Alzheimer’s Dis. 2015;44:541–547. doi: 10.3233/JAD-141854. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Butts AM, Machulda MM, Duffy JR, et al. Neuropsychological profiles differ among the three variants of primary progressive aphasia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2015;21:429–435. doi: 10.1017/S1355617715000399. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Conca F, Esposito V, Giusto G, et al. Characterization of the logopenic variant of primary progressive aphasia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Res Rev. 2022;82:101760. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2022.101760. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Kumfor F, Landin-Romero R, Devenney E, et al. On the right side? A longitudinal study of left-versus right-lateralized semantic dementia. Brain. 2016;139:986–998. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Younes K, Borghesani V, Montembeault M, et al. Right temporal lobe and socioemotional semantics: semantic behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia. Brain. 2022 doi: 10.1093/BRAIN/AWAC217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Belder CRS, Chokesuwattanaskul A, Marshall CR, et al. The problematic syndrome of right temporal lobe atrophy: unweaving the phenotypic rainbow. Front Neurol. 2023;13:2951. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2022.1082828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Campos DF, Rocca AR, Caixeta LF. Right temporal lobe variant of frontotemporal dementia: systematic review. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2022;36:272–279. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000511. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Ulugut Erkoyun H, Groot C, Heilbron R, et al. A clinical-radiological framework of the right temporal variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain. 2020;143:2831–2843. doi: 10.1093/brain/awaa225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Woollams AM, Patterson K. Cognitive consequences of the left-right asymmetry of atrophy in semantic dementia. Cortex. 2018;107:64–77. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.11.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Bocchetta M, Iglesias JE, Russell LL, et al. Segmentation of medial temporal subregions reveals early right-sided involvement in semantic variant PPA. Alzheimer’s Res Ther. 2019 doi: 10.1186/s13195-019-0489-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Borghesani V, Battistella G, Mandelli ML, et al. Regional and hemispheric susceptibility of the temporal lobe to FTLD-TDP type C pathology. NeuroImage Clin. 2020 doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102369. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Ingram RU, Halai AD, Pobric G, et al. Graded, multidimensional intra- and intergroup variations in primary progressive aphasia and post-stroke aphasia. Brain. 2020;143:3121–3135. doi: 10.1093/brain/awaa245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Ramanan S, Roquet D, Goldberg ZL, et al. Establishing two principal dimensions of cognitive variation in logopenic progressive aphasia. Brain Commun. 2020 doi: 10.1093/BRAINCOMMS/FCAA125. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Ramanan S, Flanagan E, Leyton CE, et al. Non-verbal episodic memory deficits in primary progressive aphasias are highly predictive of underlying amyloid pathology. J Alzheimer’s Dis. 2016;51:367–376. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150752. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Hodges JR, Patterson K. Semantic dementia: a unique clinicopathological syndrome. Lancet Neurol. 2007;6:1004–1014. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(07)70266-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Fletcher PD, Warren JD. Semantic dementia: a specific network-opathy. J Mol Neurosci. 2011;45:629–636. doi: 10.1007/s12031-011-9586-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Warrington EK. The selective impairment of semantic memory. Q J Exp Psychol. 1975;27:635–657. doi: 10.1080/14640747508400525. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Omar R, Mahoney CJ, Buckley AH, Warren JD. Flavour identification in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2013;84:88–93. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2012-303853. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Omar R, Henley SMD, Bartlett JW, et al. The structural neuroanatomy of music emotion recognition: evidence from frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Neuroimage. 2011;56:1814–1821. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.03.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Goll JC, Crutch SJ, Loo JHY, et al. Non-verbal sound processing in the primary progressive aphasias. Brain. 2010;133:272–285. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Mayberry EJ, Sage K, Lambon Ralph MA. At the edge of semantic space: the breakdown of coherent concepts in semantic dementia is constrained by typicality and severity but not modality. J Cogn Neurosci. 2011;23:2240–2251. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21582. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Zahn R, Green S, Beaumont H, et al. Frontotemporal lobar degeneration and social behaviour: dissociation between the knowledge of its consequences and its conceptual meaning. Cortex. 2017;93:107–118. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.05.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Duffy JR, Utianski RL, Josephs KA. Primary progressive apraxia of speech: from recognition to diagnosis and care. Aphasiology. 2020;35:1–32. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2020.1787732. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Barnes RJ, Warren JD. Shouting from far away: three poems about living with speechlessness. Pract Neurol. 2023;23:176–177. doi: 10.1136/pn-2022-003651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Rogalski EJ, Mesulam MM. Clinical trajectories and biological features of primary progressive aphasia (PPA) Curr Alzheimer Res. 2009;6:331–336. doi: 10.2174/156720509788929264. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Graff-Radford J, Duffy JR, Strand EA, Josephs KA. Parkinsonian motor features distinguish the agrammatic from logopenic variant of primary progressive aphasia. Park Relat Disord. 2012;18:890–892. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2012.04.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Kremen SA, Mendez MF, Tsai PH, Teng E. Extrapyramidal signs in the primary progressive aphasias. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. 2011;26:72. doi: 10.1177/1533317510391239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Aiello EN, Feroldi S, De Luca G, et al. Primary progressive aphasia and motor neuron disease: a review. Front Aging Neurosci. 2022;14:1003792. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.1003792. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Vinceti G, Olney N, Mandelli ML, et al. Primary progressive aphasia and the FTD-MND spectrum disorders: clinical, pathological, and neuroimaging correlates. Amyotroph Lateral Scler Frontotemporal Degener. 2019;20:146–158. doi: 10.1080/21678421.2018.1556695. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Rohrer JD, Warren JD. Phenomenology and anatomy of abnormal behaviours in primary progressive aphasia. J Neurol Sci. 2010;293:35–38. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2010.03.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Clark CN, Nicholas JM, Gordon E, et al. Altered sense of humor in dementia. J Alzheimer’s Dis. 2015;49:111–119. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Warren JE, Wise RJS, Warren JD. Sounds do-able: Auditory-motor transformations and the posterior temporal plane. Trends Neurosci. 2005;28:636–643. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Jiang J, Johnson JC, Requena-Komuro M-C, et al. Comprehension of acoustically degraded speech in Alzheimer’s disease and primary progressive aphasia. Brain. 2023;139:16–17. doi: 10.1093/BRAIN/AWAD163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Hardy CJD, Marshall CR, Bond RL, et al. Retained capacity for perceptual learning of degraded speech in primary progressive aphasia and Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2018 doi: 10.1186/S13195-018-0399-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Fletcher PD, Downey LE, Golden HL, et al. Auditory hedonic phenotypes in dementia: a behavioural and neuroanatomical analysis. Cortex. 2015;67:95–105. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.03.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Hardy CJD, Frost C, Sivasathiaseelan H, et al. Findings of impaired hearing in patients with nonfluent/agrammatic variant primary progressive aphasia. JAMA Neurol. 2019;76:607–611. doi: 10.1001/JAMANEUROL.2018.4799. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Goll JC, Kim LG, Hailstone JC, et al. Auditory object cognition in dementia. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:2755–2765. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.06.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Hardy CJD, Agustus JL, Marshall CR, et al. Functional neuroanatomy of speech signal decoding in primary progressive aphasias. Neurobiol Aging. 2017;56:190–201. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.04.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Grube M, Bruffaerts R, Schaeverbeke J, et al. Core auditory processing deficits in primary progressive aphasia. Brain. 2016;139:1817–1829. doi: 10.1093/BRAIN/AWW067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Rohrer JD, Sauter D, Scott S, et al. Receptive prosody in nonfluent primary progressive aphasias. Cortex. 2012;48:308–316. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.09.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Golden HL, Clark CN, Nicholas JM, et al. Music perception in dementia. J Alzheimer’s Dis. 2017;55:933–949. doi: 10.3233/JAD-160359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Benhamou E, Zhao S, Sivasathiaseelan H, et al. Decoding expectation and surprise in dementia: the paradigm of music. Brain Commun. 2021 doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcab173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Hailstone JC, Ridgway GR, Bartlett JW, et al. Voice processing in dementia: a neuropsychological and neuroanatomical analysis. Brain. 2011;134:2535–2547. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Mahoney CJ, Rohrer JD, Goll JC, et al. Structural neuroanatomy of tinnitus and hyperacusis in semantic dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2011;82:1274–1278. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2010.235473. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Johnson JCS, Jiang J, Bond RL, et al. Impaired phonemic discrimination in logopenic variant primary progressive aphasia. Ann Clin Transl Neurol. 2020;7:1252–1257. doi: 10.1002/acn3.51101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Hailstone JC, Crutch SJ, Vestergaard MD, et al. Progressive associative phonagnosia: a neuropsychological analysis. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:1104–1114. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.12.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Didic M, Aglieri V, Tramoni-Nègre E, et al. Progressive phonagnosia in a telephone operator carrying a C9orf72 expansion. Cortex. 2020;132:92–98. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.05.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Utianski RL, Duffy JR, Clark HM, et al. Prominent auditory deficits in primary progressive aphasia: a case study. Cortex. 2019;117:396–406. doi: 10.1016/J.CORTEX.2019.01.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Mesulam MM, Nelson MJ, Hyun J, et al. Preferential disruption of auditory word representations in primary progressive aphasia with the neuropathology of FTLD-TDP type A. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2019;32:46–53. doi: 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Buciuc M, Josephs KA, Jones DT, et al. Progressive auditory verbal agnosia secondary to Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2021;97:908–909. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000012783. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Watanabe H, Ikeda M, Mori E. Non-fluent/agrammatic variant of primary progressive aphasia with generalized auditory agnosia. Front Neurol. 2020;11:519. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2020.00519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.van Dyck CH, Swanson CJ, Aisen P, et al. Lecanemab in early Alzheimer’s disease. N Engl J Med. 2022 doi: 10.1056/NEJMOA2212948/SUPPL_FILE/NEJMOA2212948_DATA-SHARING.PDF. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Johnson JCS, McWhirter L, Hardy CJD, et al. Suspecting dementia: canaries, chameleons and zebras. Pract Neurol. 2021;21:300–312. doi: 10.1136/practneurol-2021-003019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Teichmann M, Kas A, Boutet C, et al. Deciphering logopenic primary progressive aphasia: a clinical, imaging and biomarker investigation. Brain. 2013;136:3474–3488. doi: 10.1093/brain/awt266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Kemp S, Kapur N, Graham CD, Reuber M. Functional cognitive disorder: differential diagnosis of common clinical presentations. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2022;37:1158–1176. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acac020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Chung DS, Wettroth C, Hallett M, Maurer CW. Functional speech and voice disorders: case series and literature review. Mov Disord Clin Pract. 2018;5:312–316. doi: 10.1002/mdc3.12609. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Grossman M, Irwin DJ. Primary progressive aphasia and stroke aphasia. Contin Lifelong Learn Neurol. 2018;24:745–767. doi: 10.1212/CON.0000000000000618. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Silveri MC, Di Tella S, Magni E, et al. Degenerative and vascular fluent aphasia: looking for differences. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2019;32:225–235. doi: 10.1097/WNN.0000000000000207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Henry ML, Grasso SM. Assessment of individuals with primary progressive aphasia. Semin Speech Lang. 2018;39:231–241. doi: 10.1055/s-0038-1660782. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Matias-Guiu JA, Grasso SM. Primary progressive aphasia: in search of brief cognitive assessments. Brain Commun. 2022;4(5):fcac227. doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcac227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Leyton CE, Villemagne VL, Savage S, et al. Subtypes of progressive aphasia: application of the international consensus criteria and validation using β-amyloid imaging. Brain. 2011;134:3030–3043. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Savage S, Hsieh S, Leslie F, et al. Distinguishing subtypes in primary progressive aphasia: application of the Sydney language battery. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2013;35:208–218. doi: 10.1159/000346389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Catricalà E, Gobbi E, Battista P, et al. SAND: a screening for aphasia in neurodegeneration. Development and normative data. Neurol Sci. 2017;38:1469–1483. doi: 10.1007/s10072-017-3001-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Epelbaum S, Saade YM, Flamand Roze C, et al. A reliable and rapid language tool for the diagnosis, classification, and follow-up of primary progressive aphasia variants. Front Neurol. 2021;11:571657. doi: 10.3389/FNEUR.2020.571657/FULL. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Patel N, Peterson KA, Ingram RU, et al. A “mini linguistic state examination” to classify primary progressive aphasia. Brain Commun. 2022 doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcab299. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Foxe D, Hu A, Cheung SC, et al. Utility of the Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination III online calculator to differentiate the primary progressive aphasia variants. Brain Commun. 2022 doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcac161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Matias-Guiu JA, Díaz-Álvarez J, Cuetos F, et al. Machine learning in the clinical and language characterisation of primary progressive aphasia variants. Cortex. 2019;119:312–323. doi: 10.1016/J.CORTEX.2019.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Zimmerer VC, Hardy CJD, Eastman J, et al. Automated profiling of spontaneous speech in primary progressive aphasia and behavioral-variant frontotemporal dementia: an approach based on usage-frequency. Cortex. 2020;133:103–119. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.08.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Plonka A, Mouton A, Macoir J, et al. Primary progressive aphasia: use of graphical markers for an early and differential diagnosis. Brain Sci. 2021 doi: 10.3390/brainsci11091198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Rohrer JD, Ridgway GR, Crutch SJ, et al. Progressive logopenic/phonological aphasia: erosion of the language network. Neuroimage. 2010;49:984–993. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Routier A, Habert MO, Bertrand A, et al. Structural, microstructural, and metabolic alterations in primary progressive aphasia variants. Front Neurol. 2018 doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Illán-Gala I, Montal V, Borrego-Écija S, et al. Cortical microstructure in primary progressive aphasia: a multicenter study. Alzheimer’s Res Ther. 2022 doi: 10.1186/s13195-022-00974-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12:189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Reisberg B, Ferris SH, De Leon MJ, Crook T. The global deterioration scale for assessment of primary degenerative dementia. Am J Psychiatry. 1982;139:1136–1139. doi: 10.1176/ajp.139.9.1136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Shay KA, Duke LW, Conboy T, et al. The clinical validity of the Mattis Dementia Rating Scale in staging Alzheimer’s dementia. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 1991;4:18–25. doi: 10.1177/089198879100400104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Mioshi E, Hsieh S, Savage S, et al. Clinical staging and disease progression in frontotemporal dementia. Neurology. 2010;74:1591–1597. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181e04070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Knopman DS, Weintraub S, Pankratz VS. Language and behavior domains enhance the value of the clinical dementia rating scale. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2011;7:293–299. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2010.12.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Sapolsky D, Domoto-Reilly K, Dickerson BC. Use of the Progressive Aphasia Severity Scale (PASS) in monitoring speech and language status in PPA. Aphasiology. 2014;28:993–1003. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2014.931563. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Hardy CJ., Taylor-Rubin C, Taylor B, et al (2023) Symptom-led staging for semantic and nonfluent/agrammatic variants of primary progressive aphasia. Alzheimer Dement (in press). 10.1002/alz.13415 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 113.Bocchetta M, Iglesias Espinosa MDM, Lashley T, et al. In vivo staging of frontotemporal lobar degeneration TDP-43 type C pathology. Alzheimer’s Res Ther. 2020 doi: 10.1186/s13195-020-00600-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Onyike CU, Sloane KL, Smyth SF, et al. Estimating severity of illness and disability in frontotemporal dementia: preliminary analysis of the Dementia Disability Rating (DDR) Acta Neuropsychol. 2011;9:141–153. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Steinacker P, Semler E, Anderl-Straub S, et al. Neurofilament as a blood marker for diagnosis and monitoring of primary progressive aphasias. Neurology. 2017;88:961–969. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003688. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Young AL, Marinescu RV, Oxtoby NP, et al. Uncovering the heterogeneity and temporal complexity of neurodegenerative diseases with subtype and stage inference. Nat Commun. 2018;9:1–16. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-05892-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Oxtoby NP, Leyland LA, Aksman LM, et al. Sequence of clinical and neurodegeneration events in Parkinson’s disease progression. Brain. 2021;144:975–988. doi: 10.1093/brain/awaa461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Mazzeo S, Polito C, Lassi M, et al. Loss of speech and functional impairment in Alzheimer’s disease-related primary progressive aphasia: predictive factors of decline. Neurobiol Aging. 2022;117:59–70. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2022.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Mesulam MM, Coventry CA, Bigio EH, et al. Neuropathological fingerprints of survival, atrophy and language in primary progressive aphasia. Brain. 2022;145:2133–2148. doi: 10.1093/brain/awab410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Tastevin M, Lavoie M, de la Sablonnière J, et al. Survival in the three common variants of primary progressive aphasia: a retrospective study in a tertiary memory clinic. Brain Sci. 2021 doi: 10.3390/brainsci11091113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Frings L, Blazhenets G, Binder R, et al. More extensive hypometabolism and higher mortality risk in patients with right- than left-predominant neurodegeneration of the anterior temporal lobe. Alzheimer’s Res Ther. 2023 doi: 10.1186/s13195-022-01146-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Buciuc M, Duffy JR, Machulda MM, et al. Clinical, imaging, and pathologic characteristics of patients with right vs left hemisphere-predominant logopenic progressive aphasia. Neurology. 2021;97:E523–E534. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000012322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Matias-Guiu JA, Cabrera-Martín MN, Moreno-Ramos T, et al. Clinical course of primary progressive aphasia: clinical and FDG-PET patterns. J Neurol. 2015;262:570–577. doi: 10.1007/s00415-014-7608-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Sebastian R, Thompson CB, Wang NY, et al. Patterns of decline in naming and semantic knowledge in primary progressive aphasia. Aphasiology. 2018;32:1010–1030. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2018.1490388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Tee BL, Deleon J, Chen Li Ying LK, et al. Tonal and orthographic analysis in a Cantonese-speaking individual with nonfluent/agrammatic variant primary progressive aphasia. Neurocase. 2022;28:1–10. doi: 10.1080/13554794.2021.1925302. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Tee BL, Lorinda Kwan-Chen LY, Chen TF, et al. Dysgraphia phenotypes in native Chinese speakers with primary progressive aphasia. Neurology. 2022;98:E2245–E2257. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000200350. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Otsuki M, Soma Y, Sato M, Homma A, Tsuji S. Slowly progressive pure word deafness. Eur Neurol. 1998;39:135–140. doi: 10.1159/000007923. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Kuramoto S, Hirano T, Uyama E, et al. A case of slowly progressive aphasia accompanied with auditory agnosia. Rinsho Shinkeigaku. 2002;42:299–303. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Iizuka O, Suzuki K, Endo K, et al. Pure word deafness and pure anarthria in a patient with frontotemporal dementia. Eur J Neurol. 2007;14:473–475. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-1331.2007.01671.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Billette OV, Preiß D, Nestor PJ. The concept of regularization: resolving the problem of surface dyslexia in semantic variant primary progressive aphasia across different languages. Neuropsychology. 2020;34:298–307. doi: 10.1037/neu0000611. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Chapman CA, Polyakova M, Mueller K, et al. Structural correlates of language processing in primary progressive aphasia. Brain Commun. 2023;5:fcad76. doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcad076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Dos Reis TG, Machado TH, Caramelli P, et al. First symptoms of primary progressive aphasia and Alzheimer’s disease in Brazilian individuals. Front Neurol. 2021;12:628406. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2021.628406. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Ferrari C, Polito C, Vannucchi S, et al. Primary progressive aphasia: natural history in an Italian cohort. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2019;33:42–46. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.González R, Rojas M, Rosselli M, Ardila A. Linguistic profiles of variants of primary progressive aphasia. J Commun Disord. 2022;97:106202. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2022.106202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Maruta C, Pereira T, Madeira SC, et al. Classification of primary progressive aphasia: do unsupervised data mining methods support a logopenic variant? Amyotroph Lateral Scler Frontotemporal Degener. 2015;16:147–159. doi: 10.3109/21678421.2015.1026266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Watanabe H, Hikida S, Ikeda M, Mori E. Unclassified fluent variants of primary progressive aphasia: distinction from semantic and logopenic variants. Brain Commun. 2022;4:fcac015. doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcac015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Kang SH, Cho H, Shin J, et al. Clinical characteristic in primary progressive aphasia in relation to Alzheimer’s disease biomarkers. J Alzheimer’s Dis. 2021;84:633–645. doi: 10.3233/JAD-210392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Dong L, Wang J, Liu C, et al. Genetic spectrum and clinical heterogeneity of Chinese frontotemporal dementia patients: data from PUMCH dementia cohort. J Alzheimer’s Dis. 2022;89:893–901. doi: 10.3233/JAD-220594. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Matias-Guiu JA, Pytel V, Hernández-Lorenzo L, et al. Spanish version of the mini-linguistic state examination for the diagnosis of primary progressive aphasia. J Alzheimer’s Dis. 2021;83:771–778. doi: 10.3233/JAD-210668. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Sellami L, Meilleur-Durand S, Chouinard AM, et al. The Dépistage Cognitif de Québec: A new clinician’s tool for early recognition of atypical dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2019;46:310–321. doi: 10.1159/000494348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Spinelli EG, Mandelli ML, Miller ZA, et al. Typical and atypical pathology in primary progressive aphasia variants. Ann Neurol. 2017;81:430–443. doi: 10.1002/ana.24885. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Rohrer JD, Lashley T, Schott JM, et al. Clinical and neuroanatomical signatures of tissue pathology in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Brain. 2011;134:2565–2581. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143.Santos-Santos MA, Rabinovici GD, Iaccarino L, et al. Rates of amyloid imaging positivity in patients with primary progressive aphasia. JAMA Neurol. 2018;75:342–352. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2017.4309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Rohrer JD, Rossor MN, Warren JD. Alzheimer’s pathology in primary progressive aphasia. Neurobiol Aging. 2012;33:744–752. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.05.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145.Ulugut Erkoyun H, Dijkstra AA, Scarioni M, et al. Right temporal variant frontotemporal dementia is pathologically heterogeneous: a case-series and a systematic review. Acta Neuropathol Commun. 2021 doi: 10.1186/s40478-021-01229-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146.Erkoyun HU, van der Lee SJ, Nijmeijer B, et al. The right temporal variant of frontotemporal dementia is not genetically sporadic: a case series. J Alzheimer’s Dis. 2021;79:1195–1201. doi: 10.3233/JAD-201191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147.Buciuc M, Whitwell JL, Kasanuki K, et al. Lewy body disease is a contributor to logopenic progressive aphasia phenotype. Ann Neurol. 2021;89:520–533. doi: 10.1002/ana.25979. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148.Saracino D, Ferrieux S, Noguès-Lassiaille M, et al. Primary progressive aphasia associated with GRN mutations. Neurology. 2021;97:e88–e102. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000012174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Harris JM, Gall C, Thompson JC, et al. Classification and pathology of primary progressive aphasia. Neurology. 2013;81:1832–1839. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000436070.28137.7b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150.Tan RH, Guennewig B, Dobson-Stone C, et al. The underacknowledged PPA-ALS: a unique clinicopathologic subtype with strong heritability. Neurology. 2019;92:E1354–E1366. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000007146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151.Ramos EM, Dokuru DR, Van Berlo V, et al. Genetic screen in a large series of patients with primary progressive aphasia. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2019;15:553–560. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.10.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152.Flanagan EP, Baker MC, Perkerson RB, et al. Dominant frontotemporal dementia mutations in 140 cases of primary progressive aphasia and speech apraxia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2015;39:281–286. doi: 10.1159/000375299. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 153.Samra K, MacDougall AM, Bouzigues A, et al. Genetic forms of primary progressive aphasia within the GENetic frontotemporal dementia Initiative (GENFI) cohort—comparison with sporadic primary progressive aphasia. Brain Commun. 2023;14:39. doi: 10.1093/BRAINCOMMS/FCAD036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 154.Greaves CV, Rohrer JD. An update on genetic frontotemporal dementia. J Neurol. 2019;266:2075–2086. doi: 10.1007/s00415-019-09363-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 155.Josephs KA, Duffy JR, Strand EA, et al. Progranulin-associated PiB-negative logopenic primary progressive aphasia. J Neurol. 2014;261:604–614. doi: 10.1007/s00415-014-7243-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 156.Caroppo P, Camuzat A, De Septenville A, et al. Semantic and nonfluent aphasic variants, secondarily associated with amyotrophic lateral sclerosis, are predominant frontotemporal lobar degeneration phenotypes in TBK1 carriers. Alzheimer’s Dement Diagn Assess Dis Monit. 2015;1:481–486. doi: 10.1016/j.dadm.2015.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 157.Floris G, Borghero G, Cannas A, et al. Clinical phenotypes and radiological findings in frontotemporal dementia related to TARDBP mutations. J Neurol. 2015;262:375–384. doi: 10.1007/s00415-014-7575-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 158.Snowden JS, Pickering-Brown SM, du Plessis D, et al. Progressive anomia revisited: Focal degeneration associated with progranulin gene mutation. Neurocase. 2007;13:366–377. doi: 10.1080/13554790701851510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 159.Rohrer JD, Crutch SJ, Warrington EK, Warren JD. Progranulin-associated primary progressive aphasia: a distinct phenotype? Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:288–297. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROPSYCHOLOGIA.2009.09.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 160.Fumagalli GG, Basilico P, Arighi A, et al. Distinct patterns of brain atrophy in Genetic Frontotemporal Dementia Initiative (GENFI) cohort revealed by visual rating scales. Alzheimer’s Res Ther. 2018 doi: 10.1186/s13195-018-0376-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 161.Forrest SL, Kovacs GG. Current concepts of mixed pathologies in neurodegenerative diseases. Can J Neurol Sci. 2022;50:329–345. doi: 10.1017/cjn.2022.34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 162.Zetterberg H, van Swieten JC, Boxer AL, Rohrer JD. Review: Fluid biomarkers for frontotemporal dementias. Neuropathol Appl Neurobiol. 2019;45:81–87. doi: 10.1111/nan.12530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 163.Brooks DJ. Future imaging in dementia. Semin Nucl Med. 2021;51:303–308. doi: 10.1053/j.semnuclmed.2020.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 164.Ruksenaite J, Volkmer A, Jiang J, et al. Primary progressive aphasia: toward a pathophysiological synthesis. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. 2021;21:1–12. doi: 10.1007/s11910-021-01097-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 165.Marshall CR, Hardy CJD, Russell LL, et al. The functional neuroanatomy of emotion processing in frontotemporal dementias. Brain. 2019;142:2873–2887. doi: 10.1093/brain/awz204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 166.Benhamou E, Marshall CR, Russell LL, et al. The neurophysiological architecture of semantic dementia: spectral dynamic causal modelling of a neurodegenerative proteinopathy. Sci Rep. 2020;10:1–13. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-72847-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 167.Cope TE, Shtyrov Y, MacGregor LJ, et al. Anterior temporal lobe is necessary for efficient lateralised processing of spoken word identity. Cortex. 2020;126:107–118. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.12.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 168.Fletcher PD, Nicholas JM, Shakespeare TJ, et al. Physiological phenotyping of dementias using emotional sounds. Alzheimer’s Dement. 2015;1:170–178. doi: 10.1016/j.dadm.2015.02.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 169.Cope TE, Sohoglu E, Sedley W, et al. Evidence for causal top-down frontal contributions to predictive processes in speech perception. Nat Commun. 2017;8:2154. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-01958-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 170.Ahmed RM, Irish M, van Eersel J, et al. Mouse models of frontotemporal dementia: a comparison of phenotypes with clinical symptomatology. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;74:126–138. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2017.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 171.Grossman M. Linguistic aspects of primary progressive aphasia. Annu Rev Linguist. 2018;4:377–403. doi: 10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011516-034253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 172.Tee BL, Gorno-Tempini ML. Primary progressive aphasia: a model for neurodegenerative disease. Curr Opin Neurol. 2019;32:255–265. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0000000000000673. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 173.Warren JD, Rohrer JD, Schott JM, et al. Molecular nexopathies: a new paradigm of neurodegenerative disease. Trends Neurosci. 2013;36:561–569. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2013.06.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 174.Jiang J, Benhamou E, Waters S, et al. Processing of degraded speech in brain disorders. Brain Sci. 2021 doi: 10.3390/brainsci11030394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 175.Nassan M, Piras I, s, Rogalski E, , et al. Evaluating the association between genetically proxied neurodevelopmental language phenotypes and the risk for primary progressive aphasia. Neurology. 2023 doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000207136.10.1212/wnl.0000000000207136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 176.de la Sablonnière JL, Tastevin M, Lavoie M, Laforce R. Longitudinal changes in cognition, behaviours, and functional abilities in the three main variants of primary progressive aphasia: a literature review. Brain Sci. 2021;11:1209. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11091209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 177.Weintraub S, Rader B, Coventry C, et al. Familial language network vulnerability in primary progressive aphasia. Neurology. 2020;95:E847–E855. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000009842. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 178.Miller ZA, Mandelli ML, Rankin KP, et al. Handedness and language learning disability differentially distribute in progressive aphasia variants. Brain. 2013;136:3461. doi: 10.1093/BRAIN/AWT242. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 179.Miller ZA, Spina S, Pakvasa M, et al. Cortical developmental abnormalities in logopenic variant primary progressive aphasia with dyslexia. Brain Commun. 2019 doi: 10.1093/BRAINCOMMS/FCZ027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 180.Friston K, Kiebel S. Predictive coding under the free-energy principle. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2009;364:1211–1221. doi: 10.1098/RSTB.2008.0300. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 181.Botvinick MM. Multilevel structure in behaviour and in the brain: a model of Fuster’s hierarchy. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2007;362:1615. doi: 10.1098/RSTB.2007.2056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 182.Lambon Ralph MA, Sage K, Jones RW, Mayberry EJ. Coherent concepts are computed in the anterior temporal lobes. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107:2717–2722. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0907307107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 183.Jung JY, Williams SR, Sanaei Nezhad F, Lambon Ralph MA. GABA concentrations in the anterior temporal lobe predict human semantic processing. Sci Rep. 2017 doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-15981-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 184.Taylor-Rubin C, Azizi L, Croot K, Nickels L. Primary progressive aphasia education and support groups: a clinical evaluation. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. 2019 doi: 10.1177/1533317519895638. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 185.Rare Dementia Support: Primary Progressive Aphasia (PPA). https://www.raredementiasupport.org/primary-progressive-aphasia/. Accessed 29 May 2023
- 186.Volkmer A, Cartwright J, Ruggero L, et al. Principles and philosophies for speech and language therapists working with people with primary progressive aphasia: an international expert consensus. Disabil Rehabil. 2023;45:1063–1078. doi: 10.1080/09638288.2022.2051080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 187.Volkmer A, Spector A, Warren JD, Beeke S. Speech and language therapy for primary progressive aphasia: referral patterns and barriers to service provision across the UK. Dementia. 2020;19:1349–1363. doi: 10.1177/1471301218797240. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 188.Volkmer A, Spector A, Warren JD, Beeke S. Speech and language therapy for primary progressive aphasia across the UK: a survey of current practice. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2019;54:914–926. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 189.Volkmer A, Rogalski E, Henry M, et al. Speech and language therapy approaches to managing primary progressive aphasia. Pract Neurol. 2020;20:154–162. doi: 10.1136/practneurol-2018-001921. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 190.Carthery-Goulart MT, da Silveira AC, Machado TH, et al. Nonpharmacological interventions for cognitive impairments following primary progressive aphasia: a systematic review of the literature. Dement Neuropsychol. 2013;7:122. doi: 10.1590/S1980-57642013DN70100018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 191.Nickels L, Croot K. Understanding and living with primary progressive aphasia: current progress and challenges for the future. Aphasiology. 2014;28:885–899. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2014.933521. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 192.Volkmer A, Spector A, Meitanis V, et al. Effects of functional communication interventions for people with primary progressive aphasia and their caregivers: a systematic review. Aging Ment Health. 2020;24:1381–1393. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2019.1617246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 193.Henry ML, Hubbard HI, Grasso SM, et al. Retraining speech production and fluency in non-fluent/agrammatic primary progressive aphasia. Brain. 2018;141:1799–1814. doi: 10.1093/brain/awy101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 194.Jokel R, Graham NL, Rochon E, Leonard C. Word retrieval therapies in primary progressive aphasia. Aphasiology. 2014;28:1038–1068. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2014.899306. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 195.Henry ML, Hubbard HI, Grasso SM, et al. Treatment for word retrieval in semantic and logopenic variants of primary progressive aphasia: immediate and long-term outcomes. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 2019;62:2723–2749. doi: 10.1044/2018_JSLHR-L-18-0144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 196.Croot K. Treatment for lexical retrieval impairments in primary progressive aphasia: a research update with implications for clinical practice. Semin Speech Lang. 2018;39:242–256. doi: 10.1055/s-0038-1660783. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 197.Cadório I, Lousada M, Martins P, Figueiredo D. Generalization and maintenance of treatment gains in primary progressive aphasia (PPA): a systematic review. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2017;52:543–560. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 198.Whitworth A, Cartwright J, Beales A, et al. Taking words to a new level: a preliminary investigation of discourse intervention in primary progressive aphasia. Aphasiology. 2018;32:1284–1309. doi: 10.1080/02687038.2017.1390543. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 199.Volkmer A, Spector A, Warren JD, Beeke S. The “better conversations with primary progressive aphasia (BCPPA)” program for people with PPA (primary progressive aphasia): protocol for a randomised controlled pilot study. Pilot Feasibility Stud. 2018 doi: 10.1186/s40814-018-0349-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 200.Volkmer A, Spector A, Swinburn K, et al. Using the Medical Research Council framework and public involvement in the development of a communication partner training intervention for people with primary progressive aphasia (PPA): better conversations with PPA. BMC Geriatr. 2021 doi: 10.1186/s12877-021-02561-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 201.Taylor-Rubin C, Croot K, Power E, et al. Communication behaviors associated with successful conversation in semantic variant primary progressive aphasia. Int Psychogeriatr. 2017;29:1619–1632. doi: 10.1017/S1041610217000813. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 202.Loizidou M, Brotherhood E, Harding E, et al. ‘Like going into a chocolate shop, blindfolded’: what do people with primary progressive aphasia want from speech and language therapy? Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2022 doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12818. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 203.Cotelli M, Manenti R, Ferrari C, et al. Effectiveness of language training and non-invasive brain stimulation on oral and written naming performance in primary progressive aphasia: a meta-analysis and systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2020;108:498–525. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 204.Nissim NR, Moberg PJ, Hamilton RH. Efficacy of noninvasive brain stimulation (tDCS or TMS) paired with language therapy in the treatment of primary progressive aphasia: an exploratory meta-analysis. Brain Sci. 2020;10:1–18. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10090597. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 205.Bereau M, Magnin E, Nicolier M, et al. Left prefrontal repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in a logopenic variant of primary progressive aphasia: a case report. Eur Neurol. 2016;76:12–18. doi: 10.1159/000447399. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 206.Johnson JCS, Marshall CR, Weil RS, et al. Hearing and dementia: from ears to brain. Brain. 2021;144:391–401. doi: 10.1093/brain/awaa429. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 207.Hardy CJD, Marshall CR, Golden HL, et al. Hearing and dementia. J Neurol. 2016;263:2339–2354. doi: 10.1007/s00415-016-8208-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 208.Sivasathiaseelan H, Marshall CR, Agustus JL, et al. Frontotemporal dementia: a clinical review. Semin Neurol. 2019;39:251–263. doi: 10.1055/s-0039-1683379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 209.Kertesz A, Morlog D, Light M, et al. Galantamine in frontotemporal dementia and primary progressive aphasia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2008;25:178–185. doi: 10.1159/000113034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 210.Kaye ED, Petrovic-Poljak A, Verhoeff NPLG, Freedman M. Frontotemporal dementia and pharmacologic interventions. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2010;22:19–29. doi: 10.1176/jnp.2010.22.1.19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 211.Hsu TW, Stubbs B, Liang CS, et al. Efficacy of serotonergic antidepressant treatment for the neuropsychiatric symptoms and agitation in dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Ageing Res Rev. 2021;69:101362. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2021.101362. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 212.Prodan CI, Monnot M, Ross ED. Behavioural abnormalities associated with rapid deterioration of language functions in semantic dementia respond to sertraline. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2009;80:1416–1417. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2009.173260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 213.Cummings J, Apostolova L, Rabinovici GD, et al. Lecanemab: appropriate use recommendations. J Prev Alzheimer’s Dis. 2023 doi: 10.14283/JPAD.2023.30/TABLES/11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 214.Panza F, Lozupone M, Seripa D, et al. Development of disease-modifying drugs for frontotemporal dementia spectrum disorders. Nat Rev Neurol. 2020;16:213–228. doi: 10.1038/S41582-020-0330-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 215.Cope TE, Wilson B, Robson H, et al. Artificial grammar learning in vascular and progressive non-fluent aphasias. Neuropsychologia. 2017;104:201–213. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.08.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 216.Pascual B, Funk Q, Zanotti-Fregonara P, et al. Neuroinflammation is highest in areas of disease progression in semantic dementia. Brain. 2021;144:1565–1575. doi: 10.1093/brain/awab057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 217.Mulroy E, Core LB, Chokesuwattanaskul A et al (2023) Binary reversals: a diagnostic sign in primary progressive aphasia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (in press). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Not applicable.