Abstract
Background
Previous investigations that have examined associations between family history (FH) of alcohol/substance use and adolescent brain development have been primarily cross-sectional. Here, leveraging a large population-based sample of youths, we characterized frontal cortical trajectories among 9- to 13-year-olds with (FH+) versus without (FH−) an FH and examined sex as a potential moderator.
Methods
We used data from 9710 participants in the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study (release 4.0). FH+ was defined as having ≥1 biological parents and/or ≥2 biological grandparents with a history of alcohol/substance use problems (n = 2433). Our primary outcome was frontal cortical structural measures obtained at baseline (ages 9–11) and year 2 follow-up (ages 11–13). We used linear mixed-effects models to examine the extent to which FH status qualified frontal cortical development over the age span studied. Finally, we ran additional interactions with sex to test whether observed associations between FH and cortical development differed significantly between sexes.
Results
For FH+ (vs. FH−) youths, we observed increased cortical thinning from 9 to 13 years across the frontal cortex as a whole. When we probed for sex differences, we observed significant declines in frontal cortical thickness among boys but not girls from ages 9 to 13 years. No associations were observed between FH and frontal cortical surface area or volume.
Conclusions
Having a FH+ is associated with more rapid thinning of the frontal cortex across ages 9 to 13, with this effect driven primarily by male participants. Future studies will need to test whether the observed pattern of accelerated thinning predicts future substance use outcomes.
Keywords: Adolescence, Alcohol use, Cortical thickness, Family history, Frontal development, Substance use
In This Issue
Previous studies have shown associations between a family history of alcohol and/or substance use–related problems and cortical thickness. Here, using data from the ABCD Study, we show that cortical thickness trajectories vary as a function of family history. Children with a positive family history showed steeper declines in frontal cortical thickness across ages 9 to 13 years, which further analyses revealed was driven by males.
Having a family history of alcohol and/or substance use (FH+) is a recognized risk factor for substance initiation, misuse, and the development of alcohol and substance use disorders in youths (1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6). Family history may increase risk for substance use and misuse via a number of overlapping mechanisms, including genetic propensity, increased substance availability, substance-related life disruptions, and negative parenting roles or behaviors (4,5,7, 8, 9, 10, 11).
Having a positive FH has been associated with altered brain development, particularly of the prefrontal cortex (7,8,12, 13, 14, 15, 16, 17). For example, previous cross-sectional studies have shown that FH+ children (ages 9–10 years) had lower whole-brain mean cortical thickness, including thinner cortices in the left precentral and paracentral lobules, and greater cortical area in the right precentral lobule than children without an FH (FH−) (7). FH+ adolescents exhibited thinner frontal cortices, including the pars triangularis aspect of the inferior frontal gyrus as well as the lateral and medial orbital frontal cortices (8). However, differences in orbital frontal cortical volume were not observed in adolescents aged 12 to 14 years when FH+ and FH− individuals were compared (16). Other studies have shown that FH+ individuals exhibit worse executive functioning and higher impulsivity levels than FH− individuals, which could reflect underlying differences in frontal brain structures (8,13,17, 18, 19, 20, 21) and contribute to future substance use and misuse (20, 21, 22, 23, 24, 25, 26, 27).
The above inconsistencies may be a function of the studies largely being cross-sectional (7, 8, 9,12,28,29), with wide age ranges of the children studied [e.g., 13–18 years (8) or 9–23 years (29)], varying degrees of alcohol use among the offspring (12) (which can itself disrupt cortical development and thus serve as a confounder) (30), and small and/or nonrepresentative samples (12,31). Furthermore, because many of the above studies did not specifically control for maternal prenatal use and pubertal development, the overall effects of FH may be partially confounded by the direct in utero effects of prenatal exposure or differences in pubertal stages (32, 33, 34, 35).
Perhaps most importantly, cross-sectional studies are unable to examine potential ties between FH+ and within-participant change in brain structure over time. This point is particularly relevant during periadolescence, when there is rapid maturing of the key frontal circuits that govern executive functioning and reward behaviors (36, 37, 38, 39, 40, 41, 42, 43, 44). Multiple processes play a role in the maturation of the frontal cortex during adolescence. Mechanisms such as dendritic development, synaptic pruning, change in glial cell density, and myelination likely contribute to magnetic resonance imaging (MRI)–assessed thinning of the frontal cortex (36, 37, 38, 39, 40, 41, 42, 43, 44). Additionally, different study designs (cross-sectional vs. longitudinal) can interfere and underestimate age-related brain changes, as reported in a recent study in which different statistical methods were compared using large neuroimage datasets (45). In addition, recent studies have highlighted the benefits of large samples when examining structural brain trajectories (31,46). To date, although robust associations between cognitive functioning, income, and mental health with MRI-assessed cortical structure have been reported, the exact neurobiological mechanism that underpins MRI-assessed trajectories of cortical structures remains understudied (47, 48, 49).
Interestingly, sex assigned at birth (hereafter, “sex”) has not been fully explored because most structural neuroimaging studies have not included sex as a covariate in their models (7,8,12) or have conducted analyses on all-male samples. (9). Evidence suggests that there may be differences in brain development and sex (with males presenting larger brain volume) (40,50,51), differences in rates of alcohol and substance use and sex (with males presenting higher rates of alcohol and other substance use than females) (52), and interactions of frontal cortex, alcohol use, and sex (with declines consistently demonstrated in prefrontal cortex volume among adolescents aged 14–21) (53). Furthermore, previous research has already shown sex-specific transmission of genetic risk factors for alcohol use disorder (e.g., males seem to be mainly affected by genetic factors, and females are more influenced by environmental factors) (54). However, little is known about sex differences in the ways that FH may be associated with different trajectories of frontal development. Therefore, there is a gap in our knowledge about how sex could potentially differentially impact frontal brain trajectories during preadolescent development.
Longitudinal studies that have examined associations between FH and frontal neurodevelopment are limited. While some studies have shown higher impulsivity behavioral trajectories as a function of FH in preadolescents (13,15), to our knowledge, no study has examined frontal cortical trajectories among FH+ preadolescents prior to substance use exposure. In this study, we leveraged the large, diverse, and longitudinal nature of the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) Study cohort to examine the developmental trajectories of the frontal cortex as a function of FH status before the initiation of offspring substance use. Specifically, we tested whether having a positive FH alters the trajectories of development of frontal cortical thickness, surface areas, and gray matter volume from age 9 to 13; whether these trajectories vary by sex; and how individual frontal regions contribute to these trajectories.
Methods and Materials
We used the ABCD Study data release 4.0 (https://nda.nih.gov/abcd) and selected youths with structural MRI measures who passed quality control (55) and with complete data on sociodemographic, prenatal exposure, and FH variables at 2 time points (baseline [n = 9710], mean age in years = 9.92, range = 8.92–11.00; 2-year follow-up [n = 4896], mean age = 11.92, range = 10.58–13.50). We used questions answered by the parents at baseline, using a modified version of the Family History Assessment Module Screener (56), to identify individuals with an FH of substance use–related problems (e.g., alcohol/substance use–related separation/divorce, being laid off/fired related to alcohol/substance use problems, arrests/driving under the influence, being suspended or expelled from school 2 or more times, alcohol/substance harming health, being in an alcohol/substance treatment program, and causing arguments or being drunk/intoxicated a lot).
FH+ (n = 2433, 25.1%) was defined as having ≥1 biological parents and/or ≥2 biological grandparents with a history of substance use–related problems. Individuals who had neither parents nor grandparents with a history of substance use–related problems were classified as FH− (n = 5910, 60.9%), consistent with previous studies including those that have used ABCD Study data (7,57). Preadolescents who had only one grandparent with a history of substance problems (n = 1367, 14.1%) (7,57) were not included because these participants would have a minimal genetic load of previous generations with FH; however, they could not be classified as having a FH−. This definition has been used in previous neuroimaging research (7,57), and it considers a broader representation of FH (first- and second-degree relatives).
Our outcome variables were surface area, average cortical thickness, and gray matter volume within 11 frontal regions of interest (ROIs) (caudal middle frontal, frontal pole, lateral orbital and medial orbital frontal, paracentral, pars orbitalis, pars opercularis, pars triangularis, precentral, superior frontal, and rostral middle frontal), based on the Desikan-Killiany cortical parcellation atlas (58). All structural neuroimaging processing was completed according to standardized processing pipelines for the ABCD Study (55). Cortical reconstruction and volumetric segmentation were performed by the ABCD Data Analysis, Informatics and Resource Center using the FreeSurfer image analysis suite (details are described elsewhere) (59, 60, 61, 62).
Statistical Analyses
We ran descriptive analyses of the following baseline variables: sex (male, female), race (Asian, Black, other/mixed, White), Hispanic (yes/no), parental marital status (married: yes/no), household income (≤$50,000, >$50,000 and <$100,000, ≥$100,000), any prenatal tobacco exposure (yes/no), any prenatal alcohol exposure (yes/no), any prenatal cannabis exposure (yes/no), any prenatal substance (i.e., cocaine, crack, opioids, other substances) exposure (yes/no), and Child Behavior Checklist internalizing and externalizing symptoms using T scores, comparing differences among the 3 distinct family history groups (FH+, FH−, and only 1 grandparent).
Linear mixed-effects models were chosen as our primary approach because these models allow using both fixed and random effects, thereby capturing individual-specific variations (random effects) in the data while concurrently modeling the broader trends and relationships (fixed effects). Our primary analyses consisted of 3 separate models: one for surface area, another for cortical thickness, and a third model for volume. The focus of each one of our 3 models was the overall frontal region, which we represented as a new variable (region). This approach involved accounting for the simultaneous variation of individual frontal ROIs, as well as the effects of FH and age, collectively influencing frontal cortical trajectories.
For example, to evaluate the overall frontal surface area (dependent variable), first, we standardized (normalized) each frontal ROI separately (i.e., 11 ROIs, a total of 22 including right and left hemispheres), and then the dataset was stacked so that each row represents 1 ROI of 1 participant at 1 time point (22 rows). A linear mixed model was fit with the triple interaction between the frontal ROIs, age, and FH plus independent main effects for baseline sociodemographic variables (i.e., sex, race/ethnicity, parental marital status, household income) as fixed effects. Given that prenatal exposure has the potential to interfere with brain development (34,35), we controlled for it using 4 variables for any prenatal exposure (self-reported substance use at baseline before and/or after knowledge of pregnancy) for each substance separately—tobacco, alcohol, cannabis, and other substances—and we included a time-varying total intracranial volume as a fixed effect. Random intercepts for family relatedness, MRI scanner device, and participant ID were included, implying a compound symmetry covariance structure for the repeated measures. Categorical variables were parameterized as sum-to-zero contrasts, and the model effects were assessed with type III sum of squares in an analysis of deviance table. Estimated marginal means (i.e., model-based predicted values) were computed for each factor combination in the triple interaction and selected age values to better assess FH and ROI differences at each age; multiplicity correction for these means comparisons was done using Tukey’s method. Model diagnostics were carried out with quantile-quantile plots to evaluate residual and random effects normality and fitted values against the square root of absolute standardized residuals plot to assess homoscedasticity. Finally, we ran separate models without an interaction term and 2-way interactions (Tables S6, S7) and models using nesting structure with individuals within families (i.e., 1 | family relatedness/participant ID).
To probe for potential sex differences, we conducted analyses exploring sex differences and frontal trajectories that included all participants because FH was not the primary exposure in these models (see Table S1 and Figure S1). Next, we used a 4-way interaction (FH status, age, sex, frontal cortical regions) in the same 3 main models, adjusting for the same variables in our main models while including the puberty development scale (63,64). As we did for the aforementioned primary models, for sex differences analyses, we ran a model without interaction terms, with 2-way and 3-way interactions (see Tables S1–S7).
Next, we generated estimated marginal means based on the full model, running pairwise comparisons for each ROI to examine differences in specific frontal cortical regions between FH+ and FH− individuals at ages 9.9 (mean age at baseline) and 11.9 years (mean age at the 2-year follow-up) (see Tables S1–S7). We performed a Bonferroni correction to adjust for each modality (i.e., surface area, cortical thickness, and volume), and an alpha of .017 or lower was considered significant.
We ran exploratory analyses to investigate whether structural brain development was related to FH in other brain regions. We ran similar models, with outcome variables being the average cortical thicknesses of parietal, temporal, and occipital lobes (see Tables S1–S7). In a series of post hoc exploratory analyses, we first added pubertal stage measures [e.g., the puberty development scale (63,64)] as a time-varying covariate. Then, as noted above, because prenatal substance exposure has been associated with atypical cortical development (65, 66, 67), we removed these variables to probe for potential differences that could be attributed to them. Next, we ran analyses excluding participants who self-reported alcohol and/or cannabis use initiation (defined as having 1+ standard drink of alcohol and/or puffing cannabis at the 1-year and/or 2-year follow-up) and any alcohol and cannabis use including sipping alcohol to isolate observed effects of FH on neurodevelopment from youth substance use (20,68,69). These sensitivity analyses were run because previous research had found that 4.1% of the total ABCD sample reported alcohol/substance use initiation (i.e., ≥1 standard drink, > puff/taste cannabis or nicotine, or any other substance use) at the 2-year follow-up, and 12.7% reported alcohol sipping at the 1-year follow-up and 12.6% at the 2-year follow-up (70). In addition, we repeated our primary analyses (linear mixed-effects models 3-way interaction) and sex (4-way interaction) including only participants with complete MRI data (see Tables S1–S7). Finally, we examined nonlinear effects of age and frontal cortical development by modeling age as a quadradic function in our analyses (i.e., age2). All analyses were conducted using the lme4 (71) and emmeans (72) packages, and we used ggseg (73) for visualization and interpretation of our findings in R version 4.1.3.
Results
Sociodemographic Characteristics
Baseline characteristics are shown in Table 1. Overall, groups differed by race, ethnicity, parental marital status, and household income. Lower proportions of individuals who identified as Asian and parents reporting being married as well as higher proportions of those reporting a household income <$50,000 were seen in the FH+ group. FH+ adolescents had greater prenatal exposure to tobacco, alcohol, cannabis, and other substances than those in the FH− group and greater levels of internalizing and externalizing symptomatology at baseline.
Table 1.
Overall, n = 9710 | FH+, n = 2433 | FH−, n = 5910 | Only 1 Grandparent, n = 1367 | p Value | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sex Assigned at Birth, Female | 47.9% | 49.0% | 48.1% | 45.0% | .052 |
Race | |||||
Asian | 2.0% | 0.6% | 2.8% | 0.7% | <.001 |
Black | 13.7% | 14.9% | 13.9% | 10.7% | |
Other/mixed | 16.8% | 20.3% | 15.4% | 16.2% | |
White | 67.6% | 64.2% | 67.8% | 72.4% | |
Hispanic, Yes | 19.0% | 21.4% | 18.4% | 17.3% | .002 |
Parental Marital Status, Married, Yes | 70.4% | 51.8% | 76.8% | 76.3% | <.001 |
Household Income, $ | |||||
<50,000 | 28.3% | 40.4% | 25.2% | 20.2% | <.001 |
≥50,000 and <100,000 | 43.0% | 28.9% | 47.8% | 47.5% | |
≥100,000 | 28.7% | 30.7% | 27.0% | 32.3% | |
Prenatal Exposure to Substances | |||||
Tobacco | 13.2% | 27.9% | 7.5% | 11.7% | <.001 |
Alcohol | 26.1% | 33.0% | 21.9% | 32.0% | <.001 |
Cannabis | 5.7% | 13.3% | 2.8% | 4.5% | <.001 |
Drugs (cocaine, crack, opioids, other drugs) | 1.8% | 5.6% | 0.6% | 0.3% | <.001 |
CBCL Internalizing Symptoms | 48.49 (10.58) | 51.08 (11.13) | 47.33 (10.19) | 48.84 (10.41) | <.001 |
CBCL Externalizing Symptoms | 45.60 (10.25) | 48.78 (11.06) | 44.29 (9.69) | 45.60 (9.82) | <.001 |
Values are presented as % or mean (SD). After adjusting for multiple comparisons, an alpha < .017 is considered significant. FH+ refers to ≥1 biological parents and/or ≥2 biological grandparents with history of alcohol/substance use related problems. FH− refers to no parent or grandparent with history of alcohol/substance use related problems.
CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist; FH, family history.
FH, Age, and Frontal Cortex
We first ran models to test whether the effects of age on average surface area, thickness, or volume might vary as a function of FH. The results are shown in Table 2, with significant age × FH interaction terms indicating that frontal trajectories differed by FH. We found significant interactions for average frontal cortical thickness (p = .002) but not for surface area (p = .31) or volume (p = .30). To visualize this interaction, we plotted the trajectories of frontal cortical thickness across ages 9 to 13 for the FH+ and FH− groups separately. As shown in Figure 1, differences in cortical thickness by FH increased with age. For example, at age 9, the overall mean of frontal cortical thickness (standardized mean) in the FH+ group was 0.144 (95% CI, 0.074 to 0.214) and FH− was 0.146 (95% CI, 0.076 to 0.217). At age 13, the FH+ group had a standardized mean of −0.401 (95% CI, −0.473 to −0.330), while the FH− group’s mean was −0.351 (95% CI, −0.422 to −0.280), indicating a more rapid thinning in FH+ individuals.
Table 2.
Models With a 3-Way Interaction Terma | Frontal Lobe |
||
---|---|---|---|
Area p Value | Thickness p Value | Volume p Value | |
FH+ vs. FH− | .40 | .013 | .54 |
Age | <.001 | <.001 | <.001 |
Region (11 Frontal Parcellations) | <.001 | <.001 | <.001 |
FH × Age | .31 | .002 | .30 |
FH × Region | .98 | .48 | .69 |
Age × Region | <.001 | <.001 | <.001 |
FH × Age × Region | .98 | .21 | .62 |
All the models were adjusted by intracranial volume, and for the following baseline variables as fixed effects: sex assigned at birth, race/ethnicity, parental marital status, household income, and prenatal exposure to tobacco, alcohol, cannabis, and substance use. The models were also adjusted to random effects of magnetic resonance imaging device, family relationship, and participant ID. After adjusting for multiple comparisons, an alpha < .017 is considered significant.
FH, family history.
We ran 3 models with 3-way interaction term: FH × age × region (one for each of the cortical measures: area, thickness, and volume).
We were interested in testing whether the relationship between FH and thinning differed based on sex. As a first step, we plotted the impact of sex on these trajectories independently of FH. We then added an additional sex interaction term to test whether the above trajectories varied by sex (Table S1). Our results showed a significant interaction between sex and age, with females showing more rapid thinning than males (Figure S1). For example, at age 9, the overall mean of frontal cortical thickness (standardized mean) in the female group was 0.187 (95% CI, 0.116 to 0.257) and in the male group was 0.103 (95% CI, 0.034 to 0.173). At age 13, the female group had a standardized mean of −0.334 (95% CI, −0.405 to −0.264) while the male group’s mean was −0.418 (95% CI, −0.488 to −0.348).
Next, we conducted our 4-way interaction models, and the results indicated that the association between FH status, age, and frontal cortical thickness was qualified by sex (Table 3). To examine the nature of this interaction, we plotted the trajectories of frontal cortical thickness across ages 9 to 13 for the male and female groups separately. As shown in Figure 2, effects of FH on thickness were observed in males but not in females. For example, for females, the FH+ group mean was 0.194 (95% CI, 0.118 to 0.270) and the FH− group mean was 0.219 (95% CI, 0.144 to 0.293). At age 13, the FH+ group had a standardized mean of −0.325 (95% CI, −0.405 to −0.246) while the FH− group’s mean was −0.297 (95% CI, −0.374 to −0.220). For males at age 9, the overall mean of frontal cortical thickness (standardized mean) in the FH+ group was 0.097 (95% CI, 0.022 to 0.017) and in the FH− group was 0.075 (95% CI, 0.002 to 0.149). At age 13, the FH+ group had a standardized mean of −0.408 (95% CI, −0.486 to −0.330) while the FH− group’s mean was −0.341 (95% CI, −0.415 to −0.266), indicating a more rapid thinning of the overall frontal thickness among FH+ males (compared with FH+ females, p = .0002 at age 9 and p = .006 at age 13; results were obtained from pairwise comparisons using emmeans between sex at ages 9 and 13 years for participants classified as FH+).
Table 3.
Models With 4-Way Interactiona | Area p Value | Thickness p Value | Volume p Value |
---|---|---|---|
FH, FH+ vs. FH− | .65 | .78 | .78 |
Age | <.001 | <.001 | <.001 |
Region, 11 Frontal Parcellations | .02 | <.001 | .86 |
Sex Assigned at Birth | .05 | <.001 | <.001 |
FH × Age | .47 | .89 | .87 |
FH × Region | .95 | .68 | .58 |
Age × Region | .49 | <.001 | <.001 |
FH × Sex Assigned at Birth | .89 | .007 | .42 |
Age × Sex Assigned at Birth | .10 | <.001 | .14 |
Region × Sex Assigned at Birth | .49 | .18 | .59 |
FH × Age × Region | .97 | .39 | .46 |
FH × Age × Sex Assigned at Birth | .68 | .007 | .61 |
FH × Region × Sex Assigned at Birth | .90 | .96 | .83 |
Age × Region × Sex Assigned at Birth | .86 | .81 | .94 |
FH × Age × Region × Sex Assigned at Birth | .91 | .93 | .81 |
All the models were adjusted by intracranial volume and puberty scale, and for the following baseline variables as fixed effects: sex assigned at birth, race/ethnicity, parental marital status, household income, and prenatal exposure to tobacco, alcohol, cannabis and substance use. The models were also adjusted to random effects of magnetic resonance imaging device, family relationship and participant ID. After adjusting for multiple comparisons, an alpha < .017 is considered significant.
FH, family history.
We ran 3 models with 4-way interaction term: FH × age × region × sex assigned at birth (one for each of the cortical measures: area, thickness, and volume).
Testing Specificity to Frontal Cortical Thickness
To test whether the associations described above were specific to the frontal cortex, we ran analogous models for average cortical thickness for the parietal, temporal, and occipital lobes, but found no associations for the temporal and occipital lobes (Table S3). No significant interactions were noted in any of the 9 models (3 models for each lobe: surface area, cortical thickness, and volume) (Table S3).
Exploratory Post Hoc Examination Analyses
To examine the potential influence of confounding factors, we ran models similar to our primary analyses, with the following modifications: 1) accounting for puberty using a time-varying pubertal scale; 2) removing participants with prenatal tobacco, alcohol, cannabis, and substance exposure variables; 3) excluding children with alcohol and cannabis initiation to rule out the possibility that cortical trajectories were being influenced by substance use; 4) removing participants who reported alcohol/cannabis experimentation; and 5) including internalizing and externalizing symptoms as time-varying variables in the model. Our main findings were similar in all these models (a total of 15 models, 3 for each criterion of inclusion of variables or exclusion of participants). Afterward, our findings from the repeated primary analyses (linear mixed-effects models with 3-way interaction) and sex (4-way interaction) that only included participants with complete MRI data remained the same (Tables S4, S5). Also, when rerunning our primary analyses with nested random effects, our findings were not meaningfully changed (3-way interaction model, p value = .002 [interaction term: FH × age]; 4-way interaction model, p value = .0008 [FH × age × sex assigned at birth]). Finally, our results were not meaningfully altered when frontal cortical trajectories (surface area, cortical thickness, and volume) were modeled as a quadratic function.
Discussion
This is the first large longitudinal study to examine the associations between FH of alcohol and/or substance use and frontal cortical thickness trajectories across a critical periadolescent window (ages 9–13 years). Our findings suggest that from pre- through early adolescence (approximately 9 through 13 years), there is more rapid age-related thinning in the frontal cortex among FH+ than among FH− individuals. Results are consistent with previous research (7,8) indicating that the neurological development of youths may be affected by FH of alcohol/substance use problems. Additionally, thinner cortices in early adolescence were associated with increased risk for initiating alcohol use in a longitudinal study of 137 adolescents, assessed at ages 12 to 14 and again by age 18 (26). In addition, prefrontal cortex thinning has been reported among adults with substance use disorders (74). Therefore, the thinner cortical structures observed among FH+ youths in the current investigation add to the literature and may help explain differences in future alcohol/substance use outcomes.
When testing FH × sex interactions, we observed that FH+ males exhibited a greater rate of age-related prefrontal thinning than FH+ females (Table 3; Figure 2). While examining sex differences, our analyses revealed that females presented a more rapid frontal cortical thinning (than males) when evaluated independently of their FH status (Table S1; Figure S1). These results indicate that our primary findings seem to be primarily driven by males. Interestingly, previous functional MRI and FH investigations have observed similar sex differences in frontal regions (75,76). For example, FH+ individuals had greater activation in the left anterior insula and inferior frontal gyrus during successful inhibitions on the stop-signal task, an effect also driven mainly by males (75). In addition, FH+ males exposed to childhood maltreatment had greater blood oxygen level–dependent response on functional MRI during the stop-signal task in the bilateral middle frontal gyrus, left inferior frontal gyrus, dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, and posterior cingulate cortex; the same effect was not observed in females (76). Although previous research has already shown sex-specific transmission of genetic risk factors for alcohol use disorder (e.g., males seem to be mainly affected by genetic factors, and females are more influenced by environmental factors) (54), more research investigating how FH impacts frontal cortical trajectories, future alcohol use, and development of alcohol use disorder is needed.
Previous studies have reported associations between behavioral traits (e.g., aggression, hyperactivity, and impulsivity) and alterations in frontal cortical trajectories among children and adolescents (77, 78, 79, 80), with mixed findings on sex differences (79,81, 82, 83). Additionally, the impact of FH on behavior and brain development trajectories should be further explored in large longitudinal studies. For example, there is a critical need to understand how neurodevelopmental trajectories may mediate the relationship between an FH of alcohol/substance use and behavioral traits and how sex assigned at birth may moderate these putative associations. A better understanding of these relationships could help inform future intervention strategies.
Results showing differences in cortical structure between FH+ and FH− youths at baseline, as well as evidence of accentuated differences across development, suggest persistent effects of FH+ on the neurodevelopment of youths’ brains. Past research indicates that FH+ youths may be impacted by developmental alterations in their neurological maturation (84). Because the ABCD Study sample is still young, future analyses will examine whether the frontal cortical trajectories continue, stabilize, or normalize and whether they predict future alcohol and/or substance use and misuse as the youths get older.
Taken together with previous research, our results suggest that more longitudinal approaches to studying the effects of FH of substance use on neuroanatomical development of youths are warranted. Incorporating trajectories of frontal cortical changes can offer more comprehensive information about future risk for initiation and escalation of substance use.
This study has significant strengths including a large longitudinal sample of participants, and 2 neuroimaging timepoints scanning a critical period of development using state-of-the-art neuroimaging protocols (31). Some limitations should also be noted. First, data on FH of alcohol and substance use problems were provided by one parent (most often the mother) for all parents and grandparents, leading to potential reporting misclassification. FH variables were created based on problems related to use, and not necessarily a DSM diagnosis. Nonetheless, examining associations between FH and trajectories of frontal cortical development will lay the foundation for subsequent studies to delineate causal pathways by first demonstrating associations between FH and frontal cortical development. Follow-up work can then examine the extent to which genetic, environmental, and interactions between genetic and environmental mechanisms may account for observed associations. Finally, future research could use different thresholds (e.g., severity of substance use, dependence, or other clinical diagnoses) to examine effects on youth neurobiological development and subsequent substance use.
FH effects likely comprise a combination of environmental and genetic influences, and future investigations should be conducted with the aim of disentangling whether the observed associations between cortical thickness and family risk are predominately related to genetic risk factors or psychosocial risk factors (e.g., adverse childhood experiences) or to the interplay between genetics and psychosocial risk factors
Conclusions
This is the largest longitudinal study to date to observe that having a positive FH for alcohol and/or substance-related problems is associated with more thinning of the frontal cortex across ages 9 to 13, a critical period for neurodevelopment, reinforcing the value of examining FH effects over a time continuum in youths.
Acknowledgments and Disclosures
This work is supported by National Institutes of Health, National Institute on Drug Abuse (Grant No. T32DA031099 [to Deborah Hasin and SSM]; Grant No. U01DA051039-02S1 [to HG and AP]; Grant Nos. K01DA052679 [to MEJ]; and R25DA050735-01 [to Kimberly Ann Johnson and MEJ]); National Institute of Mental Health (Grant No. K08 MH121654 [to MDA]); National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (Grant No. T32 AA07459 [to NMG]), and a NARSAD/Brain Behavior Research Foundation grant (to MDA). This manuscript reflects the views of the authors and may not reflect the opinions or views of the National Institutes of Health or ABCD consortium investigators.
PDG, MDA, and AT conceptualized and designed the study. PDG, NMG, and SRR-P managed the literature searches and summaries of previous related work. PDG undertook the statistical analysis, and PDG, MDA, AT, and WKT participated in the interpretation of the data. All authors contributed to the critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content and have approved the final manuscript.
Presented as a poster at the Annual meeting of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, December 2022, Phoenix, Arizona.
Data used in the preparation of this manuscript were obtained from the ABCD Study (https://abcdstudy.org), held in the National Institute of Mental Health Data Archive. This is a multisite, longitudinal study designed to recruit more than 10,000 children ages 9 to 10 years and follow them over 10 years into early adulthood. The ABCD Study is supported by the National Institutes of Health and additional federal partners (Award Nos. U01DA041048, U01DA050989, U01DA051016, U01DA041022, U01DA051018, U01DA051037, U01DA050987, U01DA041174, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041028, U01DA041134, U01DA050988, U01DA051039, U01DA041156, U01DA041025, U01DA041120, U01DA051038, U01DA041148, U01DA041093, U01DA041089, U24DA041123, and U24DA041147). A full list of supporters is available at https://abcdstudy.org/federal-partners.html. A listing of participating sites and a complete listing of the study investigators can be found at https://abcdstudy.org/consortium_members/. ABCD consortium investigators designed and implemented the study and/or provided data but did not necessarily participate in the analysis or writing of this report.
The authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.
Footnotes
Supplementary material cited in this article is available online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsgos.2023.100284.
Supplementary Material
References
- 1.Volkow N.D., Boyle M. Neuroscience of addiction: Relevance to prevention and treatment. Am J Psychiatry. 2018;175:729–740. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.17101174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Squeglia L.M., Cservenka A. Adolescence and drug use vulnerability: Findings from neuroimaging. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2017;13:164–170. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.12.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Dawson D.A., Harford T.C., Grant B.F. Family history as a predictor of alcohol dependence. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1992;16:572–575. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1992.tb01419.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Meyers J.L., Chorlian D.B., Johnson E.C., Pandey A.K., Kamarajan C., Salvatore J.E., et al. Association of polygenic liability for alcohol dependence and EEG connectivity in adolescence and young adulthood. Brain Sci. 2019;9:280. doi: 10.3390/brainsci9100280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.McGovern R., Bogowicz P., Meader N., Kaner E., Alderson H., Craig D., et al. The association between maternal and paternal substance use and child substance use, internalizing and externalizing problems: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Addiction. 2023;118:804–818. doi: 10.1111/add.16127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Schuckit M.A. An overview of genetic influences in alcoholism. J Subst Abuse Treat. 2009;36:S5–S14. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Lees B., Stapinski L.A., Teesson M., Squeglia L.M., Jacobus J., Mewton L. Problems experienced by children from families with histories of substance misuse: An ABCD study®. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2021;218 doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2020.108403. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Henderson K.E., Vaidya J.G., Kramer J.R., Kuperman S., Langbehn D.R., O’Leary D.S. Cortical thickness in adolescents with a family history of alcohol use disorder. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2018;42:89–99. doi: 10.1111/acer.13543. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Holla B., Bharath R.D., Venkatasubramanian G., Benegal V. Altered brain cortical maturation is found in adolescents with a family history of alcoholism. Addict Biol. 2019;24:835–845. doi: 10.1111/adb.12662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Enoch M.A. Genetic and Environmental Influences on the Development of Alcoholism: Resilience vs. risk. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2006;1094:193–201. doi: 10.1196/annals.1376.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Stanger C., Ryan S.R., Fu H., Budney A.J. Parent training plus contingency management for substance abusing families: A Complier Average Causal Effects (CACE) analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2011;118:119–126. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.03.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.McPhee M.D., Claus E.D., Boileau I., Lee A.C.H., Graff-Guerrero A., Hendershot C.S. Does family history of alcohol use disorder relate to differences in regional brain volumes? A descriptive review with new data. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2018;42:2369–2384. doi: 10.1111/acer.13882. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Dougherty D.M., Lake S.L., Mathias C.W., Ryan S.R., Bray B.C., Charles N.E., Acheson A. Behavioral impulsivity and risk-taking trajectories across early adolescence in youths with and without family histories of alcohol and other drug use disorders. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2015;39:1501–1509. doi: 10.1111/acer.12787. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Charles N.E., Ryan S.R., Bray B.C., Mathias C.W., Acheson A., Dougherty D.M. Altered developmental trajectories for impulsivity and sensation seeking among adolescent substance users. Addict Behav. 2016;60:235–241. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.04.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Acheson A., Lake S.L., Bray B.C., Liang Y., Mathias C.W., Ryan S.R., et al. Early adolescent trajectories of impulsiveness and sensation seeking in children of fathers with histories of alcohol and other substance use disorders. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2016;40:2622–2630. doi: 10.1111/acer.13235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Squeglia L.M., Sorg S.F., Jacobus J., Brumback T., Taylor C.T., Tapert S.F. Structural connectivity of neural reward networks in youth at risk for substance use disorders. Psychopharmacol (Berl) 2015;232:2217–2226. doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3857-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Khemiri L., Kaag A.M., Joos L., Dom G., Franck J., Goudriaan A.E., Jayaram-Lindström N. Family history of alcohol abuse associated with higher impulsivity in patients with alcohol use disorder: A multisite study. Eur Addict Res. 2020;26:85–95. doi: 10.1159/000505621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Adkison S.E., Grohman K., Colder C.R., Leonard K., Orrange-Torchia T., Peterson E., Eiden R.D. Impact of fathers’ alcohol problems on the development of effortful control in early adolescence. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2013;74:674–683. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2013.74.674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Kamarajan C., Pandey A.K., Chorlian D.B., Manz N., Stimus A.T., Anokhin A.P., et al. Deficient event-related theta oscillations in individuals at risk for alcoholism: A study of reward processing and impulsivity features. PLoS One. 2015;10 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0142659. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Luciana M. Risks versus consequences of adolescent and young adult substance use: A focus on executive control. Curr Addict Rep. 2020;7:453–463. doi: 10.1007/s40429-020-00341-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Stanger C., Elton A., Ryan S.R., James G.A., Budney A.J., Kilts C.D. Neuroeconomics and adolescent substance abuse: Individual differences in neural networks and delay discounting. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;52:747–755.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.04.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Lees B., Garcia A.M., Debenham J., Kirkland A.E., Bryant B.E., Mewton L., Squeglia L.M. Promising vulnerability markers of substance use and misuse: A review of human neurobehavioral studies. Neuropharmacology. 2021;187 doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2021.108500. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Jones C.B., Meier M.H., Corbin W.E., Chassin L. Adolescent executive cognitive functioning and trait impulsivity as predictors of young-adult risky drinking and alcohol-related problems. Psychol Addict Behav. 2021;35:187–198. doi: 10.1037/adb0000636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Khurana A., Romer D., Betancourt L.M., Hurt H. Working memory ability and early drug use progression as predictors of adolescent substance use disorders. Addiction. 2017;112:1220–1228. doi: 10.1111/add.13792. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Squeglia L.M., Jacobus J., Nguyen-Louie T.T., Tapert S.F. Inhibition during early adolescence predicts alcohol and marijuana use by late adolescence. Neuropsychology. 2014;28:782–790. doi: 10.1037/neu0000083. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Squeglia L.M., Ball T.M., Jacobus J., Brumback T., McKenna B.S., Nguyen-Louie T.T., et al. Neural predictors of initiating alcohol use during adolescence. Am J Psychiatry. 2017;174:172–185. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.15121587. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Elton A., Stanger C., James G.A., Ryan-Pettes S., Budney A., Kilts C.D. Intertemporal decision-making-related brain states predict adolescent drug abuse intervention responses. Neuroimage Clin. 2019;24 doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101968. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Filippi I., Hoertel N., Artiges E., Airagnes G., Guérin-Langlois C., Seigneurie A.S., et al. Family history of alcohol use disorder is associated with brain structural and functional changes in healthy first-degree relatives. Eur Psychiatry. 2019;62:107–115. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2019.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Benegal V., Antony G., Venkatasubramanian G., Jayakumar P.N. Gray matter volume abnormalities and externalizing symptoms in subjects at high risk for alcohol dependence. Addict Biol. 2007;12:122–132. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2006.00043.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Sun D., Adduru V.R., Phillips R.D., Bouchard H.C., Sotiras A., Michael A.M., et al. Adolescent alcohol use is linked to disruptions in age-appropriate cortical thinning: An unsupervised machine learning approach. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2023;48:317–326. doi: 10.1038/s41386-022-01457-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Marek S., Tervo-Clemmens B., Calabro F.J., Montez D.F., Kay B.P., Hatoum A.S., et al. Reproducible brain-wide association studies require thousands of individuals. Nature. 2022;603:654–660. doi: 10.1038/s41586-022-04492-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Vijayakumar N., Youssef G.J., Allen N.B., Anderson V., Efron D., Hazell P., et al. A longitudinal analysis of puberty-related cortical development. Neuroimage. 2021;228 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Pfefferbaum A., Rohlfing T., Pohl K.M., Lane B., Chu W., Kwon D., et al. Adolescent development of cortical and white matter structure in the NCANDA sample: Role of sex, ethnicity, puberty, and alcohol drinking. Cereb Cortex. 2016;26:4101–4121. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Lees B., Mewton L., Jacobus J., Valadez E.A., Stapinski L.A., Teesson M., et al. Association of prenatal alcohol exposure with psychological, behavioral, and neurodevelopmental outcomes in children from the adolescent brain cognitive development study. Am J Psychiatry. 2020;177:1060–1072. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.20010086. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Dwyer J.B., McQuown S.C., Leslie F.M. The dynamic effects of nicotine on the developing brain. Pharmacol Ther. 2009;122:125–139. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2009.02.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Petanjek Z., Judas M., Kostović I., Uylings H.B.M. Lifespan alterations of basal dendritic trees of pyramidal neurons in the human prefrontal cortex: A layer-specific pattern. Cereb Cortex. 2008;18:915–929. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Miller D.J., Duka T., Stimpson C.D., Schapiro S.J., Baze W.B., McArthur M.J., et al. Prolonged myelination in human neocortical evolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109:16480–16485. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1117943109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Alemán-Gómez Y., Janssen J., Schnack H., Balaban E., Pina-Camacho L., Alfaro-Almagro F., et al. The human cerebral cortex flattens during adolescence. J Neurosci. 2013;33:15004–15010. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1459-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Ducharme S., Albaugh M.D., Nguyen T.V., Hudziak J.J., Mateos-Pérez J.M., Labbe A., et al. Trajectories of cortical thickness maturation in normal brain development — The importance of quality control procedures. Neuroimage. 2016;125:267–279. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Vijayakumar N., Allen N.B., Youssef G., Dennison M., Yücel M., Simmons J.G., Whittle S. Brain development during adolescence: A mixed-longitudinal investigation of cortical thickness, surface area, and volume. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016;37:2027–2038. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Kolk S.M., Rakic P. Development of prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2022;47:41–57. doi: 10.1038/s41386-021-01137-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Casey B.J., Getz S., Galvan A. The adolescent brain. Dev Rev. 2008;28:62–77. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Giedd J.N., Blumenthal J., Jeffries N.O., Castellanos F.X., Liu H., Zijdenbos A., et al. Brain development during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal MRI study. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:861–863. doi: 10.1038/13158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Friedman N.P., Robbins T.W. The role of prefrontal cortex in cognitive control and executive function. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2022;47:72–89. doi: 10.1038/s41386-021-01132-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Di Biase M.A., Tian Y.E., Bethlehem R.A.I., Seidlitz J., Alexander-Bloch AaronF, Yeo B.T.T., Zalesky A. Mapping human brain charts cross-sectionally and longitudinally. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2023;120 doi: 10.1073/pnas.2216798120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Liu S., Abdellaoui A., Verweij K.J.H., Van Wingen G.A. Replicable brain–phenotype associations require large-scale neuroimaging data. Nat Hum Behav. 2023;7:1344–1356. doi: 10.1038/s41562-023-01642-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Tomasi D., Volkow N.D. Associations of family income with cognition and brain structure in USA children: Prevention implications. Mol Psychiatry. 2021;26:6619–6629. doi: 10.1038/s41380-021-01130-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Noble K.G., Houston S.M., Brito N.H., Bartsch H., Kan E., Kuperman J.M., et al. Family income, parental education and brain structure in children and adolescents. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18:773–778. doi: 10.1038/nn.3983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Gur R.E., Moore T.M., Rosen A.F.G., Barzilay R., Roalf D.R., Calkins M.E., et al. Burden of environmental adversity associated with psychopathology, maturation, and brain behavior parameters in youths. JAMA Psychiatry. 2019;76:966–975. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.0943. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Mills K.L., Lalonde F., Clasen L.S., Giedd J.N., Blakemore S.J. Developmental changes in the structure of the social brain in late childhood and adolescence. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2014;9:123–131. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Mills K.L., Goddings A.L., Clasen L.S., Giedd J.N., Blakemore S.J. The developmental mismatch in structural brain maturation during adolescence. Dev Neurosci. 2014;36:147–160. doi: 10.1159/000362328. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Johnston L., Miech R., O’Malley P., Bachman J., Schulenberg J., Patrick M. Monitoring the Future national survey results on drug use 1975–2019: Overview, key findings on adolescent drug use. 2020. https://cdn.ymaws.com/www.fadaa.org/resource/resmgr/files/resource_center/mtf-overview2019.pdf Available at: Accessed June 13, 2021.
- 53.Welch K.A., Carson A., Lawrie S.M. Brain structure in adolescents and young adults with alcohol problems: Systematic review of imaging studies. Alcohol. 2013;48:433–444. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agt037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Prescott C.A. Sex differences in the genetic risk for alcoholism. Alcohol Res Health. 2002;26:264–273. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Casey B.J., Cannonier T., Conley M.I., Cohen A.O., Barch D.M., Heitzeg M.M., et al. The adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) study: Imaging acquisition across 21 sites. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2018;32:43–54. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Rice J.P., Reich T., Bucholz K.K., Neuman R.J., Fishman R., Rochberg N., et al. Comparison of direct interview and family history diagnoses of alcohol dependence. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1995;19:1018–1023. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.1995.tb00983.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Cservenka A. Neurobiological phenotypes associated with a family history of alcoholism. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2016;158:8–21. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.10.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Desikan R.S., Ségonne F., Fischl B., Quinn B.T., Dickerson B.C., Blacker D., et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage. 2006;31:968–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.FreeSurfer. Available at: http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/. Accessed January 12, 2023.
- 60.Dale A.M., Fischl B., Sereno M.I. Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage. 1999;9:179–194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Fischl B., Sereno M.I., Dale A.M. Cortical surface-based analysis. II: Inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system. Neuroimage. 1999;9:195–207. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Paulus M.P., Squeglia L.M., Bagot K., Jacobus J., Kuplicki R., Breslin F.J., et al. Screen media activity and brain structure in youth: Evidence for diverse structural correlation networks from the ABCD study. Neuroimage. 2019;185:140–153. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.10.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Herting M.M., Sowell E.R. Puberty and structural brain development in humans. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2017;44:122–137. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2016.12.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Goddings A.L., Beltz A., Peper J.S., Crone E.A., Braams B.R. Understanding the role of puberty in structural and functional development of the adolescent brain. J Res Adolesc. 2019;29:32–53. doi: 10.1111/jora.12408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Treit S., Zhou D., Lebel C., Rasmussen C., Andrew G., Beaulieu C. Longitudinal MRI reveals impaired cortical thinning in children and adolescents prenatally exposed to alcohol. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35:4892–4903. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22520. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Treit S., Chen Z., Zhou D., Baugh L., Rasmussen C., Andrew G., et al. Sexual dimorphism of volume reduction but not cognitive deficit in fetal alcohol spectrum disorders: A combined diffusion tensor imaging, cortical thickness and brain volume study. Neuroimage Clin. 2017;15:284–297. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Hendrickson T.J., Mueller B.A., Sowell E.R., Mattson S.N., Coles C.D., Kable J.A., et al. Two-year cortical trajectories are abnormal in children and adolescents with prenatal alcohol exposure. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2018;30:123–133. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.02.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Ewing S.W., Sakhardande A., Blakemore S.J. The effect of alcohol consumption on the adolescent brain: A systematic review of MRI and fMRI studies of alcohol-using youth. Neuroimage Clin. 2014;5:420–437. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.06.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Luciana M., Collins P.F., Muetzel R.L., Lim K.O. Effects of alcohol use initiation on brain structure in typically developing adolescents. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2013;39:345–355. doi: 10.3109/00952990.2013.837057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Sullivan R.M., Wade N.E., Wallace A.L., Tapert S.F., Pelham W.E., Brown S.A., et al. Substance use patterns in 9 to 13-year-olds: Longitudinal findings from the adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) study. Drug Alcohol Depend Rep. 2022;5 doi: 10.1016/j.dadr.2022.100120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Bates D., Mächler M., Bolker B., Walker S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw. 2015;67:1–48. [Google Scholar]
- 72.R package emmeans: Estimated marginal means. 2023. https://www.rdocumentation.org/packages/emmeans/versions/1.8.8 Available at: Accessed August 26, 2023.
- 73.Mowinckel A.M., Vidal-Piñeiro D. Visualisation of Brain Statistics with R-packages ggseg and ggseg3d, version 1. 2019. Accessed August 23, 2023. [DOI]
- 74.Navarri X., Afzali M.H., Lavoie J., Sinha R., Stein D.J., Momenan R., et al. How do substance use disorders compare to other psychiatric conditions on structural brain abnormalities? A cross-disorder meta-analytic comparison using the ENIGMA consortium findings. Hum Brain Mapp. 2022;43:399–413. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.DeVito E.E., Meda S.A., Jiantonio R., Potenza M.N., Krystal J.H., Pearlson G.D. Neural correlates of impulsivity in healthy males and females with family histories of alcoholism. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013;38:1854–1863. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Elton A., Allen J.H., Yorke M., Khan F., Xu P., Boettiger C.A. Sex moderates family history of alcohol use disorder and childhood maltreatment effects on an fMRI stop-signal task. Hum Brain Mapp. 2023;44:2436–2450. doi: 10.1002/hbm.26221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Shaw P., Eckstrand K., Sharp W., Blumenthal J., Lerch J.P., Greenstein D., et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is characterized by a delay in cortical maturation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007;104:19649–19654. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0707741104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Yu G., Liu Z., Wu X., Becker B., Zhang K., Fan H., et al. Common and disorder-specific cortical thickness alterations in internalizing, externalizing and thought disorders during early adolescence: An Adolescent Brain and Cognitive Development study. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2023;48:E345–E356. doi: 10.1503/jpn.220202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Whittle S., Vijayakumar N., Simmons J.G., Allen N.B. Internalizing and externalizing symptoms are associated with different trajectories of cortical development during late childhood. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2020;59:177–185. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2019.04.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Albaugh M.D., Hudziak J.J., Spechler P.A., Chaarani B., Lepage C., Jeon S., et al. Conduct problems are associated with accelerated thinning of emotion-related cortical regions in a community-based sample of adolescents. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2023;330 doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2023.111614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Almeida Montes L.G., Prado Alcántara H., Martínez García R.B., De La Torre L.B., Avila Acosta D., Duarte M.G. Brain cortical thickness in ADHD: Age, sex, and clinical correlations. J Atten Disord. 2013;17:641–654. doi: 10.1177/1087054711434351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Ducharme S., Albaugh M.D., Hudziak J.J., Botteron K.N., Nguyen T.V., Truong C., et al. Anxious/depressed symptoms are linked to right ventromedial prefrontal cortical thickness maturation in healthy children and young adults. Cereb Cortex. 2014;24:2941–2950. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Newman E., Thompson W.K., Bartsch H., Hagler D.J., Chen C.H., Brown T.T., et al. Anxiety is related to indices of cortical maturation in typically developing children and adolescents. Brain Struct Funct. 2016;221:3013–3025. doi: 10.1007/s00429-015-1085-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Spadoni A.D., Simmons A.N., Yang T.T., Tapert S.F. Family history of alcohol use disorders and neuromaturation: A functional connectivity study with adolescents. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2013;39:356–364. doi: 10.3109/00952990.2013.818680. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.