Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2024 Jun 13.
Published in final edited form as: Cortex. 2022 Oct 29;158:1–3. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2022.10.008

On the replicability of action-verb deficits in Parkinson’s disease

Adolfo M García 1,2,3,4,*, Agustín Ibáñez 1,2,3,5,6,*
PMCID: PMC11170466  NIHMSID: NIHMS1996693  PMID: 36402119

The study of action verb processing in Parkinson’s disease (PD) represents a promising approach to identify specific neurocognitive biomarkers (Birba et al., 2017; Cardona et al., 2013; García et al., 2020; García & Ibáñez, 2018). We appreciate Holm Møller et al.’s effort to replicate and extend our original study (Garcia et al., 2018), which showed that Spanish-speaking PD patients without mild cognitive impairment (PD-nMCI) were selectively impaired in processing actions evoked by naturalistic narratives. This finding reinforced the widely reported observation that motor-system alterations undermine grasping of verbal units denoting bodily motion (García & Ibáñez, 2018). Yet, Holm Møller et al. failed to replicate such a finding on a Danish-speaking cohort when using partly similar action and non-action texts (AT, nAT) and additional materials.

Holm Møller et al.’s effort is timely, as cross-linguistic studies represent a crucial milestone for cognitive research on PD (Calvo, Ibáñez, Muñoz, & García, 2018; García & Orozco-Arroyave, 2021), especially when focused on action verbs (Birba et al., 2017; Calvo et al., 2018). This is all the more valuable when discrepant outcomes arise. Juxtaposed to García et al.’s findings, Holm Møller et al.’s negative results motivate relevant methodological, typological, epistemological, and meta-theoretical considerations, as detailed next.

Discrepancies between studies and the value of replications

Action verbs are predicted to yield deficits in PD due to their reliance on movement-related mechanisms (Birba et al., 2022; Birba et al., 2017; Cardona et al., 2013; Cervetto et al., 2021; Garcia & Ibanez, 2016; García et al., 2019; Pulvermüller, 2013). Thus, the hypothesis presupposes marked motor symptomatology, as captured through part III of the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS-III). Patients in Holm Møller et al.’s study had a mean UPDRS-III score of 14.29, indicating very mild motor compromise. This seems unusually low, considering that large studies (n = 653) on patients in the same age range reveal a mean UPDRS-III score of 27.2 (Shulman et al., 2010), nearly identical to that of PD-nMCI patients in García et al. (2018). Of note, the difference of >10 points relative to Holm Møller et al.’s sample constitutes a large clinically important difference by current standards (Shulman et al., 2010). Therefore, in the absence of marked motor symptom severity, an important requisite of the study’s hypothesis may not have been met.

Another methodological issue concerns the study’s materials. Unlike the AT and the nAT in García et al.’s study, their Danish counterparts were not controlled for concreteness and imageability. As acknowledged by Holm Møller et al., this could represent a non-trivial confound. Non-action verbs, such as those used in nATs, tend to be less concrete and imageable than action verbs, rendering them harder to access. As shown by behavioral and neuroscientific evidence, abstract words are processed less efficiently than concrete words (West & Holcomb, 2000), even in the elderly (Roxbury, McMahon, Coulthard, & Copland, 2016). Indeed, in Holm Møller et al.’s study, the lowest scores for both groups corresponded to nAT verbs. If these factors are not matched between texts, the greater difficulty of nATs could mask between-text dissociations driven by other factors (i.e., action semantics).

Furthermore, Holm Møller et al.’s null results may reflect idiosyncrasies of the Danish language. As the authors note, motion direction and manner may be differently encoded by Danish and Spanish verbs and particles (Slobin, 1996; Talmy, 1985). Unlike Spanish (a verb-framed language with a rich repertoire of verbs and verb inflections), Danish is a satellite-framed language characterized by a small verb vocabulary with high polysemy, no person marking, and complex verbal syntax (Pedersen, 1999; Pederson, 2010). Accordingly, verb processing in Danish may distinctly benefit from the contextual support offered by naturalistic stories. That is, given that Danish verbs’ correct interpretation critically depend on contextual anchorage (Pederson, 2010), full-blown narratives could facilitate their processing irrespective of the disruption of motor-system grounding. This language-specific feature, together with the patients’ very mild motor compromise, could attenuate or nullify predicted disadvantages for action-verb processing.

A few studies align with Holm Møller et al. in failing to detect action-verb deficits following motor-system damage (Aiello et al., 2022; Argiris et al., 2020). Conversely, significant impairments have been amply reported in PD cohorts and other neurodegenerative movement disorders by different teams in several languages, through diverse tasks, and with stimuli of varying complexity (Bak, 2013; Birba et al., 2017; Cardona et al., 2013; García & Ibáñez, 2018; Ibáñez et al., 2022). Moreover, the very paradigm under discussion has revealed selective links between action-verb processing and motor systems in different populations (Birba et al., 2020; Moguilner et al., 2021; Trevisan, Sedeño, Birba, Ibáñez, & García, 2017). Now, rather than signaling correct and incorrect approaches, positive and negative findings might well indicate that neurocognition in general and embodiment in particular are fundamentally situated and context-sensitive (Ibanez, 2022; Ibáñez et al., 2022). Results may be markedly impact by clinical, stimulus-related, linguistic-, and mainly context-specific factors (Ibáñez et al., 2022). Often overlooked, this point could represent an important aspect of the current replicability crisis, which is usually predicated on the unscientific assumption of stability and universality (Ibanez, 2022).

By the same token, we often endorse a ceteris paribus philosophy in conducting and interpreting replications. Yet, at least when dealing with humans, surrounding factors are rarely, if ever, equal. That is, precisely, why we need replications. By reprising some aspects of their antecedents while differing in others, such studies offer a usable amount of diversity to better understand the conditions under which certain effects may hold or not.

Acknowledgments

Adolfo García is an Atlantic Fellow at the Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI) and is supported with funding from GBHI, Alzheimer’s Association, and Alzheimer’s Society (Alzheimer’s Association GBHI ALZ UK-22-865742); ANID, FONDECYT Regular (1210176, 1210195); and Programa Interdisciplinario de Investigación Experimental en Comunicación y Cognición (PIIECC), Facultad de Humanidades, USACH. AI is supported by grants from Takeda CW2680521; CONICET; ANID/FONDECYT Regular (1210195, 1210176, 1220995); ANID/FONDAP/15150012; Alzheimer’s Drug Discovery Foundation and Target ALS, ANID/FONDEF ID20I10152, ANID/PIA/ANILLOS ACT210096, and the Multi-Partner Consortium to Expand Dementia Research In Latin America [ReDLat, supported by National Institutes of Health, National Institutes of Aging (R01 AG057234), CARDS-NIA, Alzheimer’s Association (SG-20–725707), Rainwater Charitable foundation—Tau Consortium, and Global Brain Health Institute)]. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not represent the official views of the institutions.

Footnotes

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Credit Author Statement

Adolfo M. García: Conceptualization, Writing - Original draft, Funding acquisition.

Agustín Ibáñez: Conceptualization, Writing - Review & Editing, Funding acquisition.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Aiello EN, Grosso M, Di Liberto A, Andriulo A, Buscone S, Caracciolo C, . . . Luzzatti C. (2022). Disembodying language: Actionality does not account for verb processing deficits in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 61, 101040. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2021.101040 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  2. Argiris G, Budai R, Maieron M, Ius T, Skrap M, & Tomasino B (2020). Neurosurgical lesions to sensorimotor cortex do not impair action verb processing. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 523. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-57361-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bak TH (2013). The neuroscience of action semantics in neurodegenerative brain diseases. Current Opinion in Neurology, 26(6), 671–677. doi: 10.1097/wco.0000000000000039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Birba A, Fittipaldi S, Cediel Escobar JC, Gonzalez Campo C, Legaz A, Galiani A, . . . Garcia AM. (2022). Multimodal Neurocognitive Markers of Naturalistic Discourse Typify Diverse Neurodegenerative Diseases. Cereb Cortex, 32(16), 3377–3391. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab421 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Birba A, Garcia-Cordero I, Kozono G, Legaz A, Ibanez A, Sedeno L, & Garcia AM (2017). Losing ground: Frontostriatal atrophy disrupts language embodiment in Parkinson’s and Huntington’s disease. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 80, 673–687. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.07.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Birba A, Vitale F, Padron I, Dottori M, de Vega M, Zimerman M, . . . Garcia AM. (2020). Electrifying discourse: Anodal tDCS of the primary motor cortex selectively reduces action appraisal in naturalistic narratives. Cortex, 132, 460–472. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.08.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Calvo N, Ibáñez A, Muñoz E, & García AM (2018). A core avenue for transcultural research on dementia: on the cross-linguistic generalization of language-related effects in Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease. 33(6), 814–823. doi: 10.1002/gps.4712 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cardona JF, Gershanik O, Gelormini-Lezama C, Houck AL, Cardona S, Kargieman L, . . . Manes F. (2013). Action-verb processing in Parkinson’s disease: new pathways for motor–language coupling. Brain Structure and Function, 218(6), 1355–1373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cardona JF, Gershanik O, Gelormini-Lezama C, Houck AL, Cardona S, Kargieman L, . . . Ibanez A. (2013). Action-verb processing in Parkinson’s disease: new pathways for motor-language coupling. Brain Struct Funct, 218(6), 1355–1373. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0510-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cervetto S, Díaz-Rivera M, Petroni A, Birba A, Caro MM, Sedeño L, . . . García AM. (2021). The Neural Blending of Words and Movement: Event-Related Potential Signatures of Semantic and Action Processes during Motor–Language Coupling. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 33(8), 1413–1427. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01732 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. García AM, Bocanegra Y, Birba A, Orozco-Arroyave JR, Sedeño L, & Ibáñez A (2020). Chapter 26 - Disruptions of frontostriatal language functions in Parkinson’s disease. In Martin CR & Preedy VR (Eds.), Genetics, Neurology, Behavior, and Diet in Parkinson’s Disease (pp. 413–430): Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  12. Garcia AM, Bocanegra Y, Herrera E, Moreno L, Carmona J, Baena A, . . . Ibanez A. (2018). Parkinson’s disease compromises the appraisal of action meanings evoked by naturalistic texts. Cortex, 100, 111–126. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.07.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Garcia AM, & Ibanez A (2016). A touch with words: Dynamic synergies between manual actions and language. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 68, 59–95. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.04.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. García AM, & Ibáñez A (2018). When embodiment breaks down: Language deficits as novel avenues into movement disorders. Cortex, 100, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2017.12.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. García AM, Moguilner S, Torquati K, García-Marco E, Herrera E, Muñoz E, . . . Ibáñez A. (2019). How meaning unfolds in neural time: Embodied reactivations can precede multimodal semantic effects during language processing. NeuroImage, 197, 439–449. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.05.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. García AM, & Orozco-Arroyave JR (2021). Reply to: “Does Cognitive Impairment Influence Motor Speech Performance in De Novo Parkinson’s Disease”. Movement Disorders, 36(12), 2982–2983. doi: 10.1002/mds.28831 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Ibanez A (2022). The mind’s golden cage and cognition in the wild. Trends in Cognitve Sciences, 10.1016/j.tics.2022.07.008. doi: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Ibáñez A, Kühne K, Miklashevsky A, Monaco E, Muraki EJ, Ranzini M, . . . Tuena C. (2022). Ecological meanings: A consensus paper on individual differences and contextual influences in embodied language. Journal of Cognition. doi: 10.31219/osf.io/ej5y3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Moguilner S, Birba A, Fino D, Isoardi R, Huetagoyena C, Otoya R, . . . García AM. (2021). Multimodal neurocognitive markers of frontal lobe epilepsy: Insights from ecological text processing. NeuroImage, 235, 117998. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117998 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Pedersen BS (1999). Systematic Verb Polysemy in MT: A Study of Danish Motion Verbs with Comparisons with Spanish. Machine Translation, 14(1), 35–82. doi: 10.1023/a:1008183205171 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  21. Pederson BS (2010). Danish Motion Verbs: Syntactic Alternations and the Hypothesis of Semantic Determination. Nordic Journal of Linguistics, 20(1), 65–90. doi: 10.1017/s0332586500004017 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  22. Pulvermüller F (2013). How neurons make meaning: brain mechanisms for embodied and abstract-symbolic semantics. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(9), 458–470. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Roxbury T, McMahon K, Coulthard A, & Copland DA (2016). An fMRI study of concreteness effects during spoken word recognition in aging. Preservation or attenuation? Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 7, 240. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00240 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Shulman LM, Gruber-Baldini AL, Anderson KE, Fishman PS, Reich SG, & Weiner WJ (2010). The Clinically Important Difference on the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale. Archives of Neurology, 67(1), 64–70. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2009.295 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Slobin DI (1996). Two ways to travel: Verbs of motion in English and Spanish. In Shibatani M & Thompson SA (Eds.), Grammatical constructions: Their form and meaning (pp. 195–220): Oxford: Clarendon Press. [Google Scholar]
  26. Talmy L (1985). Lexicalization patterns: Semantic structure in lexical forms. In Shopen T (Ed.), Language typology and syntactic description (Vol. 3, pp. 36–149): Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  27. Trevisan P, Sedeño L, Birba A, Ibáñez A, & García AM (2017). A moving story: Whole-body motor training selectively improves the appraisal of action meanings in naturalistic narratives. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 12538. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-12928-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. West WC, & Holcomb PJ (2000). Imaginal, semantic, and surface-level processing of concrete and abstract words: an electrophysiological investigation. J Cogn Neurosci, 12(6), 1024–1037. doi: 10.1162/08989290051137558 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES