Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2024 Jul 11;13:RP96281. doi: 10.7554/eLife.96281

Chronic activation of a negative engram induces behavioral and cellular abnormalities

Alexandra L Jellinger 1,, Rebecca L Suthard 1,2,, Bingbing Yuan 3, Michelle Surets 1, Evan A Ruesch 1, Albit J Caban 1,2, Shawn Liu 4, Monika Shpokayte 1,2,, Steve Ramirez 1,5,6,
Editors: Adrien Peyrache7, Laura L Colgin8
PMCID: PMC11239178  PMID: 38990919

Abstract

Negative memories engage a brain and body-wide stress response in humans that can alter cognition and behavior. Prolonged stress responses induce maladaptive cellular, circuit, and systems-level changes that can lead to pathological brain states and corresponding disorders in which mood and memory are affected. However, it is unclear if repeated activation of cells processing negative memories induces similar phenotypes in mice. In this study, we used an activity-dependent tagging method to access neuronal ensembles and assess their molecular characteristics. Sequencing memory engrams in mice revealed that positive (male-to-female exposure) and negative (foot shock) cells upregulated genes linked to anti- and pro-inflammatory responses, respectively. To investigate the impact of persistent activation of negative engrams, we chemogenetically activated them in the ventral hippocampus over 3 months and conducted anxiety and memory-related tests. Negative engram activation increased anxiety behaviors in both 6- and 14-month-old mice, reduced spatial working memory in older mice, impaired fear extinction in younger mice, and heightened fear generalization in both age groups. Immunohistochemistry revealed changes in microglial and astrocytic structure and number in the hippocampus. In summary, repeated activation of negative memories induces lasting cellular and behavioral abnormalities in mice, offering insights into the negative effects of chronic negative thinking-like behaviors on human health.

Research organism: Mouse

Introduction

Stress has profound impacts on both physical and mental health. In particular, chronic stress is recognized as a potent risk factor for numerous neuropsychiatric disorders, including major depressive disorder (MDD), generalized anxiety disorder (GAD), and post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). In humans, repeated negative thinking can parallel the effects of chronic stress. These effects include hippocampal hyperactivity and accelerating the onset of mild cognitive impairment and dementias (Marchant et al., 2020). Additionally, the ability of memories to influence cognition and behavior has been well established. For example, our lab previously demonstrated that reactivating ensembles of hippocampal neurons encoding a positive memory is sufficient to induce anti-depressant effects in mice (Ramirez et al., 2015), and studies in rodents have shown that repeated reactivation of negative memories enhances fear responses (Chen et al., 2019). However, it remains unknown whether modulating a chronic negative memory is sufficient to induce aberrant cellular and behavioral states that recapitulate chronic stress.

The ventral hippocampus (vHPC), specifically vCA1, has been extensively studied for its role in emotional processing. As a modulator of stress responses, vCA1 dysfunction is intimately associated with pathological affective states (Fanselow and Dong, 2010), and its structure and function are heavily impacted by stressors. Additionally, vCA1 neurons project to various downstream targets and route target-specific streams of information to the basolateral amygdala, nucleus accumbens, and prefrontal cortex. These diverse connections are integral to the formation of emotional memory, as well as regulating anxiety and the stress response. Within vCA1, stimulation of negative engram cells has been shown to modulate social avoidance behavior and enhance fear responses, but whether chronic stimulation of fearful memories produces enduring effects on brain activity and behavior remains unknown (Chen et al., 2019; Zhang et al., 2019).

Young and old individuals are differentially susceptible to the hippocampal effects of stress and chronic negative thinking behaviors. Epidemiological population-based studies in humans have established adverse childhood experiences as a risk factor for the development of psychopathologies later in life. Notably, the development of depression and PTSD may increase with the number of these adverse events, suggesting an increased susceptibility of the developing, young brain to the perturbations induced by such events (McLaughlin et al., 2010). However, work in older humans and rodents has shown that there are changes in their stress response and cortisol levels following adverse experiences. Aged rodents have increased stress vulnerability, as repeated stress increased anxiety-like behaviors, induced spatial memory impairments, and raised corticosterone levels more compared to younger animals (Wang et al., 2020).

Here, we chronically reactivated negative memory engrams to mimic the effects of stress or chronic negative thinking-related behaviors in mice. We first characterized the genetic landscape of negative and positive memory engrams to search for putative genetic markers involved in healthy and dysfunctional brain states. We then tested whether stimulating negative memory-bearing neurons would be sufficient to induce cellular abnormalities as well as behavioral impairments in young (6 months) and aged (14 months) mice. Our manipulation induced behavioral abnormalities that manifested as increased anxiety, decreased spatial working memory, and increased fear response during extinction and generalization. We also observed cellular alterations, including changes in the number and morphology of microglia and astrocytes. Together, our data suggest that negative memory engrams are a potent means by which to induce cellular and behavioral changes that provide insight into pathological states.

Results

Ventral hippocampal negative and positive engram cells differentially express genes associated with inflammation

The vHPC parses out negative and positive memories into anatomically segregated, functionally distinct, and molecularly diverse neuronal ensembles or ‘engrams’ that have unique transcriptional profiles and DNA methylation landscapes (Shpokayte et al., 2022). As positive and negative memories have been respectively implicated in driving approach and avoidance-related behaviors in mice, we first sought to test if these two vHPC engrams differentially upregulated genes in response to each experience. We previously performed an RNA-seq experiment described in Shpokayte et al., 2022. Briefly, we utilized an activity-dependent labeling technique to visualize and manipulate the cells processing a specific memory in the hippocampus by leveraging the cFos protein as an indicator of neuronal activity (Liu et al., 2012). Our approach combines a doxycycline (DOX)-dependent strategy with a dual-viruses approach consisting of AAV9-c-fos-tTA+AAV9-TRE-ChR2-eYFP or control AAV9-TRE-eYFP, 24 hr prior to a behavioral experience, DOX is removed from the animal’s diet which will allow tTa to bind to TRE and drive the expression of any downstream effector, such as the fluorescent protein eYFP (Figure 1a). After the behavioral experience, that is, contextual fear conditioning (CFC) or male-to-female exposure, the animal is placed back on DOX to close the tagging window for the negative or positive experience. Thus, the cells involved in processing a discrete experience express the eYFP protein that can be used in fluorescence-activated cell sorting (FACS) to isolate only the eYFP+ as ‘tagged’ engram cells for genetic sequencing. To analyze this RNA-seq dataset, we selected specific gene sets and conducted Gene Set Enrichment Analysis (GSEA) to compare the up- and downregulation of these genes in positive or negative engram cells. The GSEA showed that the anti-inflammatory gene set was enriched in positive engram cells, with higher expression compared to negative engram cells (Figure 1c and d). Further, the pro-inflammatory gene set was enriched in negative engram cells, with higher expression compared to positive engram cells (Figure 1e and f). Interestingly, this gene set included Spp1, Ttr, and C1qb1, which are involved in inflammatory processes and oxidative stress. Together, these data provide the start to uncovering the molecular landscape that positive and negative memory-bearing cells may contain that links them to protective or pathological functioning, respectively.

Figure 1. Gene Set Enrichment Analysis (GSEA) of RNA-sequencing data of positive and negative ventral hippocampus (vHPC) engram upregulate genes associated with neuroprotective and neurodegeneration, respectively.

(a) C57BL/6J mice were bilaterally injected with a 1:1 viral cocktail of AAV9-c-Fos-tTA and AAV9-TRE-eYFP into the vHPC. This activity-dependent labeling strategy captures sufficiently active neurons expressing the immediate-early gene, cfos. This system couples the cfos promoter to the expression of the tetracycline transactivator (tTA), which binds to the tetracycline response element (TRE) in its protein form. When doxycycline (DOX) is present, such as in the animal’s diet, this inhibits the binding of tTA to TRE, preventing eYFP labeling of active cells. (b) Experimental schematic used to label, isolate, and analyze the two groups of vHPC cells labeled by eYFP upon shock (negative) or male-to-female interaction (positive). Prior to surgery, mice were placed on a Dox diet to inhibit the labeling of active neurons. On day 0, mice were injected with the activity-dependent viral strategy to enable labeling of active neurons during a salient experience. On day 10 after viral expression and recovery, the mice were taken off of Dox prior to engram tagging to open the labeling window. On day 11, mice were subjected to contextual fear conditioning (CFC) or male-to-female interaction to label negative or positive neurons, respectively. Mice were placed immediately back on Dox to close this tagging window. Then, 24 hr later, mice were sacrificed and brains were obtained for sequencing experiments. (c) List of gene abbreviations and full names used for the GSEA of anti-inflammatory genes. (d) GSEA with the enrichment score for negative vs. untagged cells and positive vs. untagged cells using the anti-inflammatory gene set. (e) List of gene abbreviations and names used for the GSEA of pro-inflammatory genes. (f) GSEA with the enrichment score of negative vs. untagged cells and positive vs. untagged cells using the pro-inflammatory gene set.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Chronic stimulation of the negative engram does not induce neuronal death.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

Representative images and quantification of NeuN+ cells in the (a, b) subiculum, (c, d) vDG, and (e, f) vCA1 of all groups (6-month mCherry and hM3Dq and 14-month mCherry and hM3Dq). Values are given as a mean + SEM. Statistical analysis was performed with two-way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s post hoc test. For cell counts, n = 3 mice/group × 18 single-tile ROIs each brain region (NeuN). No label = not significant, **p>0.001, ***p>0.0001, ****p>0.00001.

Chronic engram stimulation increases anxiety-related behaviors, decreases spatial memory, and impairs fear extinction and generalization

Mouse models of chronic stress display increased anxiety-related behaviors and impairments in cognition and memory-related tasks that are similar to those seen in human patients with MDD, PTSD, and comorbid GAD (Tran and Gellner, 2023). To further understand the impact of negative engram cells, we next asked if chronic stimulation of a negative engram could induce behavioral changes in anxiety and memory-related tasks. To that end, we combined excitatory Designer Receptors Exclusively Activated by Designer Drugs (DREADDs) with a Fos-based transgenic mouse, Fos-tm2.1(iCre-ERT2) raised on a C57BL/6 background (TRAP2), to label active cells under the control of 4-hydroxytamoxifen (4-OHT) (DeNardo et al., 2019). Then, 3- and 11-month-old TRAP2 mice were bilaterally injected with a Cre-dependent AAV-DIO-hM3Dq-mCherry or control AAV-DIO-mCherry into the vHPC. After 2 weeks for viral expression and surgical recovery, all mice were subjected to CFC, consisting of four, 1.5 mA foot shocks in context A (Cxt A). Also, 30 min after CFC, mice received a 40 mg/kg injection of 4-OHT to label cells sufficiently active during this fearful experience (Figure 2a and b). The animals were returned to their home cages, where they received water-soluble deschloroclozapine dihydrochloride (DCZ), a highly potent, selective, and brain-penetrable muscarinic hM3D(Gq) actuator, for 3 months in their daily water (Nagai et al., 2020). DCZ was selected because even at very low concentrations this ligand is able to increase neuronal activity with binding to hM3D(Gq) receptors (Nagai et al., 2020). To avoid unnecessary pain or stress to the animal with chronic administration of DCZ intraperitoneally (IP), we selected the voluntary oral route (PO), which has been utilized in the past for chemogenetic experiments (Zhan et al., 2019) and shows no significant differences in behavior at the same dose (Ferrari et al., 2022).

Figure 2. Experimental timeline for chronic modulation of a negative engram in TRAP2 mice.

Figure 2.

(a) Schematic representation of tagging a negative engram in young (3 months) and old (11 months) TRAP2 mice followed by 3 months of chronic engram stimulation and a battery of behavioral tests. AAV9-hSyn-DIO-hM3Dq-mCherry or AVV9-hSyn-DIO-mCherry control vector was bilaterally injected into the ventral hippocampus (vHPC) of young and old mice. After viral expression and surgical recovery, all mice were subjected to contextual fear conditioning (CFC) in context A and subsequent 4-hydroxytamoxifen (4-OHT) intraperitoneal (IP) injection to induce negative engram tagging. For the next 3 months, mice received the water-soluble Designer Receptors Exclusively Activated by Designer Drugs (DREADDs) agonist deschloroclozapine dihydrochloride (DCZ) in their home cage water. After 3 months of stimulation wherein the young and old groups reached 6 months and 14 months of age, the mice underwent open field, y-maze, zero maze, remote recall, extinction, and generalization in a novel context B. (b) Representative image of hSyn-DIO-hM3Dq-mCherry expression in the vHPC (red) and DAPI+ cells (blue) after 3 months of hM3Dq activation. (c) Weights of all groups were recorded over the 3-month stimulation protocol, once per month. (Left) Young 6-month-old and (right) older 14-month-old mice. Values are given as a mean + SEM. Statistical analysis was performed with two-way (RM) ANOVA followed by Sidak’s post hoc test. n = 9–17 per group after outlier removal. p≤0.05, **p≤0.01, ***p≤0.001, ****p≤0.0001, no label = not significant.

After 3 months of negative engram stimulation, mCherry and hM3Dq mice were aged to 6 and 14 months old at the time of behavioral testing (Figure 2a). The weights of both the young and old mice increased independently of hM3Dq activation across the 3 months of stimulation, suggesting normal weight gain with aging (two-way repeated measures [RM] ANOVA; [6 months] interaction: F(2, 34) = 0.2633, p=0.7701; month: F(2, 34) = 14.84, p<0.0001; group: F(1,17) = 1.361, p=0.2594; subject: F(17,34) = 15.44, p<0.0001. Sidak’s [month]: 1 vs. 2, p=0.0279; 1 vs. 3, p<0.0001; 2 vs. 3, p=0.0323. [14 months] Interaction: F(2,52) = 2.607, p=0.0833; month: F(2,52) = 7.854, p=0.0010; group: F(1,26) = 0.1275, p=0.7239; subject: F(26,52) = 20.74, p<0.0001. Sidak’s [month]: 1 vs. 2, p=0.6942; 1 vs. 3, p=0.0011; 2 vs. 3, p=0.0198) (Figure 2c). However, we note that we did not include a month 0 body weight measurement; thus, it is possible that during the first month of stimulation (months 0–1), there may have been a drop in body weight that rebounded by the first measurement at month 1 that continued to increase normally through months 2–3 as shown in Figure 1.

Importantly, there were no decreases in the number of NeuN+ cells in the subiculum, dentate gyrus, and CA1 of the vHPC due to chronic activation, suggesting that we are not inducing a reduction in neuronal cell health with this constant stimulation, as measured by this method (two-way ANOVA; [subiculum] interaction: F(1, 8) = 0.2516, p=0.6295; age: F(1, 8) = 7.341, p=0.0267; group: F(1,8) = 0.00721, p=0.9344. Tukey’s [group]: 6MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.9904; 14MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.9744; Figure 1—figure supplement 1a and b. [vDG] Interaction: F(1,8) = 0.6806, p=0.4333; age: F(1,8) = 4.423, p=0.0686; group: F(1,8) = 0.0524, p=0.8246; Figure 1—figure supplement 1c and d. [vCA1] Interaction: F(1,8) = 2.824, p=0.1314; age: F(1,8) = 0.5443, p=0.4817; group: F(1,8) = 2.947, p=0.1244) (Figure 1—figure supplement 1e and f).

To test the hypothesis that chronic activation of a negative memory is sufficient to increase anxiety-like behaviors, we subjected our mice to the open-field and zero-maze tests (Figure 3). In the open field (Figure 3a), 6- and 14-month-old hM3Dq mice spent less time in the center compared to age-matched mCherry mice (two-way ANOVA); interaction: F(1,33) = 0.1438, p=0.7070; age: F(1,33) = 0.5858, p=0.4495; group: F(1,33) = 29.56, p<0.0001; Tukey’s multiple comparisons [group]: 6MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.0049; 14MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.0016) (Figure 3aii). We observed no significant differences in total distance traveled across all groups (two-way ANOVA; interaction: F(1,37) = 0.0016, p=0.9675; age: F(1,37) = 0.6862, p=0.4128; group: F(1,37) = 0.3522, p=0.5565) (Figure 3ai). Further, 6-month-old hM3Dq mice had an increase in average speed in the open field compared to mCherry mice (two-way ANOVA; interaction: F(1,38) = 4.003, p=0.526; age: F(1,38) = 0.2346, p=0.6306; group: F(1,38) = 7.306, p=0.0102; (Tukey’s [group]: 6MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.0150; 14MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.9532) (Figure 3aiv). However, there were no differences in the number of center entries across all groups, indicating that mice in mCherry and hM3Dq groups are entering the center the same number of times, but spending less time per visit (two-way ANOVA; interaction: F(1,41) = 1.236, p=0.2728; age: F(1,41) = 0.4424, p=0.5097; group: F(1,41) = 0.3370, p=0.5648) (Figure 3aiii).

Figure 3. Chronic activation of an aversive engram induces behavioral changes in anxiety and working memory.

Figure 3.

(a) Schematic of open-field test; (i) total distance traveled, (ii) total time spent in center, (iii) number of entries into the center, and (iv) average speed in 6-month mCherry and hM3Dq and 14-month mCherry and hM3Dq after 3 months of stimulation. (b) Schematic of zero-maze test; (i) total distance traveled, (ii) total time spent in open arms, (iii) number of entries into the open area, and (iv) average speed. (c) Schematic of y-maze test; (i) total distance traveled, (ii) percentage of spontaneous alterations, and (iii) total number of entries into arms. Statistical measure used a two-way ANOVA with time point and group as factors. Tukey’s post hoc tests were performed. Error bars indicate SEM. p≤0.05, **p≤0.01, ***p≤0.001, ****p≤0.0001, ns = not significant. n = 8–11 per group after outlier removal.

In the zero maze (Figure 3b), the 6- and 14-month-old hM3Dq mice spent less time in the center compared to age-matched mCherry mice (two-way ANOVA; interaction: F(1,38) = 0.5633, p=0.4575; age: F(1,38) = 0.1709, p=0.6816; group: F(1,38) = 40.22, p<0.0001; Tukey’s [group]: 6MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.0002; 14MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.0007) (Figure 3bii). No significant differences were observed in the total distance traveled (Figure 3bi) or number of entries into the open area across all groups (two-way ANOVA; [total distance traveled] interaction: F(1,36) = 1.915, p=0.1749; age: F(1,36) = 0.0093, p=0.9237; group: F(1,36) = 0.6022, p=0.4428. [Open area entries] Interaction: F(1,38) = 1.082, p=0.3049; age: F(1,38) = 0.3839, p=0.5392; group: F(1,38) = 3.286, p=0.0778) (Figure 3biii). These findings indicate that mCherry and hM3Dq mice entered the open area the same number of times, but hM3Dq mice had shorter visits. We also observed no significant difference in average speed across all groups (two-way ANOVA; interaction: F(1,37) = 0.5118, p=0.4788; age: F(1,37) = 1.659, p=0.2057; group: F(1,37) = 0.7180, p=0.4023) (Figure 3biv). Ultimately, our observations from open field and zero maze support the hypothesis that chronic activation of a negative memory is sufficient to increase anxiety-like behaviors.

The mice also underwent y-maze testing in order to test the hypothesis that chronic activation of a negative memory is sufficient to impair spatial working memory (Figure 3c). In this task, mice were required to remember the most recent arm of the maze that was explored and subsequently navigate to a ‘novel’ arm. Mice freely explored the three arms of the maze (A, B, C) and spatial working memory was assessed by measuring the percentage of spontaneous alternations (i.e., ABC, CBA, CAB), or three consecutive entrances to a novel arm without repetition. A lower percentage of spontaneous alternations indicates a deficit in spatial working memory as they cannot recognize what arm they were in just moments prior. We observed a higher percentage of spontaneous alterations in 14-month-old mCherry than 14-month-old hM3Dq mice (two-way ANOVA); interaction: F(1,40) = 4.088, p=0.0499; age: F(1,40) = 0.3080, p=0.5820; group: F(1,40) = 7.517, p=0.0091; Tukey’s [group]: 14MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.0045 (Figure 3cii). However, no significant difference in the percentage of spontaneous alterations was observed between 6-month-old mCherry and hM3Dq mice (Tukey’s [group]: 6MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.9634) (Figure 3cii). Additionally, we observed no significant differences in the total distance traveled or total number of entries across all groups (two-way ANOVA; [total distance traveled] interaction: F(1,44) = 0.2668, p=0.6081; age: F(1,44) = 0.5106, p=0.4787; group: F(1,44) = 1.181, p=0.2831; Figure 3ci). [Total number of entries] Interaction: F(1,40) = 1.332, p=0.2553; age: F(1,40) = 0.2236, p=0.6389; group: F(1,40) = 0.8247, p=0.3692 (Figure 3ciii). Therefore, our observations suggest an inability of the mice to remember which arms of the y-maze they previously explored and may represent an impairment in working memory in the 14-month cohort.

Finally, mice had a ‘negative’ engram tagged in a fearful CxtA before the initiation of chronic activation (Figure 4a). During this CFC session, mCherry and hM3Dq mice in the 3- and 11-month groups displayed no differences in freezing behavior, as expected (two-way ANOVA; interaction: F(1,42) = 0.1342, p=0.7150; age: F(1,42) = 12.20, p=0.0011; group: F(1,42) = 2.913, p=0.0953) (Figure 4ai–ii). However, to test the hypothesis that chronic negative engram activation is sufficient to induce impaired fear memory recall and extinction, mice were subjected to remote memory recall after 3 months of treatment. The only significant difference in remote recall behavior was an increase in freezing between 6- and 14-month mCherry mice (two-way ANOVA; interaction: F(1,41) = 2.531, p=0.1193; age: F(1,41) = 10.08, p=0.0028; group: F(1,41) = 6.406, p=0.0153; Tukey’s [age]: 6 vs. 14MO mCherry, p=0.0101; 6 vs. 14MO hM3Dq, p=0.6672. [Group] 6MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.0449; 14MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.8890) (Figure 4bi–ii). Mice typically display increased freezing behavior as they age, so these effects during remote recall are expected. For the extinction session in the same context, there were no significant differences across all groups at 6 and 14 months of age (Figure 4ci–ii). However, we observed a strong trend showing an increase in freezing in the 6-month-old hM3Dq mice that was like remote recall (two-way ANOVA; interaction: F(1,41) = 2.618, p=0.1133; age: F(1,41) = 2.335, p=0.1342; group: F(1,41) = 5.763, p=0.0210; Tukey’s [group]: 6MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.0590; 14MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.9269) (Figure 4di–ii). This may suggest that these mice are displaying an increase in fear during remote recall of a fearful experience from 3 months prior and have difficulty appropriately decreasing levels of fear during extinction. Finally, mice were placed in a neutral, novel context B at this remote time point after chronic activation to test for generalization of fear. In this context B, mice in the hM3Dq groups at both 6 and 14 months of age displayed increased freezing in a neutral context compared to mCherry controls (two-way ANOVA; Iinteraction: F(1,41) = 0.09801, p=0.7558; age: F(1,41) = 2.624, p=0.1129; group: F(1,41) = 26.53, p<0.0001; Tukey’s [group]: 6MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.0053; 14MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.0026) (Figure 4g and h). Chronic negative engram activation induced selective deficits in extinction and modestly in fear generalization, which may point to the negative impact of fearful memories in human patients with disorders like PTSD (Maren and Holmes, 2016).

Figure 4. Chronic activation of the negative engram induces behavioral changes in fear memory.

Figure 4.

(a) Schematic of contextual fear conditioning (CFC) in context A wherein 3- and 11-month-old mice received four, 1.5 mA, 2 s foot shocks. Following the CFC session, the negative engram was tagged in all mice by intraperitoneal (IP) injection of 4-hydroxytamoxifen (4-OHT). (i) Average percent freezing levels and total percentage freezing across the 300 s CFC session for (ii) 3-month groups and (iii) 11-month groups (two-way ANOVA RM; [3 months] interaction: F(5,130) = 1.131, p=0.3473; time: F(5,130) = 115.6, p<0.0001; group: F(1,26) = 2.737, p=0.1101; subject: F(26,130) = 3.056, p<0.0001. [11 months] Interaction: F(5,90) = 1.218, p=0.3074; time: F(5,90) = 234.4, p<0.0001; group: F(1,18) = 1.862, p=0.1892; subject: F(18,90) = 2.887, p=0.0005). (b) Schematic of remote recall performed after 3 months of negative engram stimulation. Mice were returned to context A for 5 min in the absence of foot shocks. (i) Average percent freezing levels and total percentage freezing across the 300 s remote recall session for (ii) 6-month groups and (iii) 14-month groups two-way ANOVA RM; [6 months] interaction: F(5,85) = 15.72, p=0.0115; time: F(5,85) = 15.72, p<0.001; group: F(1,17) = 6.588, p=0.0200; subject: F(17,85) = 2.458, p=0.0035. [14 months] Interaction: F(5,130) = 0.9027, p=0.4815; time: F(5,130) = 14.18, p<0.0001; group: F(1,26) = 0.3967, p=0.5343; subject: F(26,130) = 4.107, p<0.0001. (c) Schematic of extinction wherein mice were placed in context A for 30 min in the absence of foot shocks. (i) Average percent freezing levels and total percentage freezing across the 1800s extinction session for (ii) 6-month groups and (iii) 14-month groups (two-way ANOVA RM; [6 months] interaction: F(5,85) = 0.4133, p=0.8383; time: F(5,85) = 1.979, p=0.0899; group: F(1,17) = 5.494, p=0.0315; subject: F(17,85) = 7.352, p<0.0001. [14 months] interaction: F(5,130) = 1,362, p=0.2570; time: F(5,130) = 5.387, p=0.0002; group: F(1,26) = 0.3836, p=0.5411; subject: F(26, 130)=11.16, p<0.0001). (d) Schematic of the novel context B used to assess generalization. (i) Average percent freezing levels and total percentage freezing across the 300 s generalization session for (ii) 6-month groups and (iii) 14-month groups (two-way ANOVA RM; [6 months] interaction: F(4,72) = 2.257, p=0.0713; time: F(4,72) = 3.373, p=0.0138; group: F(1,18) = 4.151, p=0.0566; subject: F(18,72) = 2.537, p=0.0028. [14 months] interaction: F(4,100) = 2.575, p=0.0421; time: F(4,100) = 5.717, p=0.0003; group: F(1,25) = 6.343, p=0.0186; subject: F(25,100) = 3.175, p<0.0001). Statistical analysis utilized a two-way ANOVA with time point and group as factors and two-way repeated measures (RM) ANOVA with time (seconds) and group as factors across 6- and 14-month-old mice. Tukey’s or Sidak’s post hoc tests were performed where applicable. Error bars indicate SEM. p≤0.05, **p≤0.01, ***p≤0.001, ****p≤0.0001, ns = not significant. n = 8–11 per group after outlier removal.

Negative engram stimulation increases glial cell number and changes morphology

We next tested the hypothesis that chronic negative engram activation will induce glial changes related to inflammation by performing cell counts and morphological characterization of both microglia (Iba1+ ) and astrocytes (GFAP+ ) in the hippocampus. For microglia (Figure 5a–j, Figure 5—figure supplement 1), 14-month-old mice in the hM3Dq group displayed an increase in Iba1+ cells, while 6-month-old mice did not (two-way ANOVA; interaction: F(1,8) = 7.615, p=0.0247; age: F(1,8) = 3.259, p=0.1087; group: F(1,8) = 13.42, p=0.0064; Tukey’s [group]: 6MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.9906; 14MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.0113. [Age] 6 vs. 14MO mCherry, p=0.9876; 6 vs. 14MO hM3Dq, p=0.0704) (Figure 5c). Additionally, these 14-month-old hM3Dq mice had microglia with increased ramification index, cell territory volume, and minimum and maximum branch length, suggesting a distinct change in phenotype with negative memory activation (two-way ANOVA; [ramification index] interaction: F(1,8) = 1.908, p=0.2046; age: F(1,8) = 1.950, p=0.2001; group: F(1,8) = 15.11, p=0.0046. Tukey’s [group]: 6MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.3513; 14MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.03242 (Figure 5d). [Cell territory volume] interaction: F(1,8) = 3.494, p=0.0965; age: F(1,8) = 0.05044, p=0.8279; group: F(1,8) = 16.45, p=0.0037. Tukey’s [group]: 6MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.4564; 14MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.0129 (Figure 5e). [Min branch length] interaction: F(A1,8) = 8.287, p=0.0206; age: F(1,8) = 9.619, p=0.0146; group: F(1,8) = 7.470, p=0.0257. Tukey’s [group]: 6MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.9996; 14MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.0174. [Age]: 6 vs. 14MO mCherry, p=0.9985; 6 vs. 14MO hM3Dq, p=0.0141 (Figure 5i). [Max branch length] Interaction: F(1,8) = 4.094, p=0.0777; age: F(1,8) = 1.214, p=0.3026; group: F(1,8) = 8.964, p=0.0172. Tukey’s [group]: 6MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.8994; 14MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.0309 (Figure 5j)). However, there were no significant differences in the average centroid distance, number of endpoints, and average branch length for Iba1+ cells (two-way ANOVA; [Avg centroid distance] interaction: F(1,8) = 0.7707, p=0.4056; age: F(1,8) = 1.155, p=0.3138; group: F(1,8) = 5.459, p=0.0477. Tukey’s [group]: 6MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.7370; 14MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.1838; Figure 5f. [Number of endpoints] Interaction: F(1,8) = 2.110, p=0.1884; age: F(1,8) = 2.158, p=0.1801; group: F(1,8) = 0.0237, p=0.8815; Figure 5g. [Avg branch length] Interaction: F(1,8) = 0.7574, p=0.4095; age: F(1,8) = 0.02109, p=0.8881; group: F(1,8) = 0.1211, p=0.7369 (Figure 5g)).

Figure 5. Chronic modulation of the negative engram induces changes in the number and morphology of microglia and astrocytes.

(a, b) Representative images of Iba1+ microglia (green) in the hippocampus of (a) 6-month mCherry and hM3Dq mice and (b) 14-month mCherry and hM3Dq mice following 3 months of stimulation. (c) Number of Iba1+ cells. (d–j) Quantified morphological analysis of Iba1+ cells; (d) ramification index, (e) cell territory volume, (f) average centroid distance, (g) number of endpoints, (h) average branch length, (i) minimum branch length, and (j) maximum branch length. (k, l) Representative images of GFAP+ astrocytes (red) in the hippocampus of (k) 6-month mCherry and hM3Dq mice and (l) 14-month mCherry and hM3Dq mice following 3 months of stimulation. (m) Number of GFAP+ cells. (n–t) Quantified morphological analysis of GFAP+ cells; (n) ramification index, (o) cell territory volume, (p) average centroid distance, (q) number of endpoints, (r) average branch length, (s) minimum branch length, and (t) maximum branch length. Statistical analysis used a two-way ANOVA with time point and group as factors. Tukey’s post hoc tests were performed. Error bars indicate SEM. p≤0.05, **p≤0.01, ***p≤0.001, ****p≤0.0001, ns = not significant. For cell counts, n = 3 mice/group × 18 single-tile ROIs each (GFAP, Iba1). For morphology, n = 3 mice/group × 3 single-tile ROIs each (GFAP, Iba1), each containing 20–30 glial cells.

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Analysis of astrocyte and microglial morphology using the MATLAB-based tool 3DMorph.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

(a–d) Representative 3DMorph images of GFAP+ astrocytes in (a) 6-month mCherry, (b) 6-month hM3Dq, (c) 14-month mCherry, and (d) 14-month hM3Dq mice. (e–h) Representative 3DMorph images of Iba1+ microglia in (e) 6-month mCherry, (f) 6-month hM3Dq, (g) 14-month mCherry, and (h) 14-month hM3Dq mice.

For astrocytes (Figure 5k–t, Figure 5—figure supplement 1), there was a similar increase in the number of GFAP+ cells in the hippocampus, but in both the 6- and 14-month groups receiving hM3Dq activation (two-way ANOVA; interaction: F(1,8) = 0.05526, p=0.8201; age: F(1,8) = 6.824, p=0.0310; group: F(1,8) = 26.95, p=0.0008; Tukey’s [group]: 6MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.0328; 14MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.0208) (Figure 5m). Additionally, these astrocytes at both ages had increased average branch length and maximum branch length (two-way ANOVA; [Avg branch length] interaction: F(1,8) = 0.1537, p=0.7052; age: F(1,8) = 0.9883, p=0.3493; group: F(1,8) = 32.75, p=0.0004; Tukey’s [group]: 6MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.0228; 14MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.0109; Figure 5r. [Max branch length] Interaction: F(1,8) = 0.5438, p=0.4819; age: F(1,8) = 0.8447, p=0.3849; group: F(1,8) = 41.82, p=0.0002. Tukey’s [group]: 6MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.0041; 14MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.0155) (Figure 5t). Astrocytes in the 6-month hM3Dq group additionally had increased minimum branch length (two-way ANOVA; interaction: F(1,8) = 1.568, p=0.2459; age: F(1,8) = 0.7927, p=0.3993; group: F(1,8) = 16.26, p=0.0038; Tukey’s [group]: 6MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.0238; 14MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.2759) (Figure 5s). However, astrocytes did not show any significant differences for any group in their ramification index, cell territory volume, average centroid distance, and number of endpoints (two-way ANOVA; [ramification index] interaction: F(1,8) = 0.0003, p=0.9856; age: F(1,8) = 0.08582, p=0.7770; group: F(1,8) = 6.677, p=0.0324. Tukey’s [group]: 6MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.3233; 14MO mCherry vs. hM3Dq, p=0.3339; Figure 5n. [Cell territory volume] Interaction: F(1,8) = 0.0028, p=0.9584; age: F(1,8) = 0.5819, p=0.4674; group: F(1,8) = 1.996, p=0.1954; Figure 5o. [Avg centroid distance] Interaction: F(1,8) = 0.04699, p=0.8338; age: F(1,8) = 4.275, p=0.0725; group: F(1,8) = 3.856, p=0.0852; Figure 5p. [Number of endpoints] Interaction: F(1,8) = 0.5463, p=0.4810; age: F(1,8) = 0.0365, p=0.8532; group: F(1,8) = 0.0071, p=0.9346) (Figure 5q). In summary, chronic activation of a negative engram induces an increase in glial cell number and changes their morphology in both young and older cohorts.

Discussion

We chronically activated negative engrams in the vHPC using chemogenetics over the course of 3 months. This stimulation protocol induced detrimental behavioral effects as evidenced by increased anxiety-like behaviors, decreased working memory, and increased fear responses across extinction and generalization. Notably, our chronic negative engram activation significantly impacted microglial and astrocytic number and morphology within the hippocampus. Together, these findings present evidence for the role of negative memories in producing maladaptive cellular and behavioral responses within a rodent model and further our understanding of the broad impacts of chronic negative thinking-like behaviors that may detrimentally affect human health.

Molecular features of engram cells

First, to characterize the molecular features of engram cells, we performed RNA-sequencing and utilized GSEA to compare gene expression profiles in cells that were labeled during a positive or negative experience (Rao-Ruiz et al., 2019; Fuentes-Ramos et al., 2021; Sardoo et al., 2022). Using a priori gene sets of interest, we found that negative engram cells upregulated genes that were associated with pathways involved in neurodegeneration, proinflammatory processes, and apoptosis. In contrast, positive engram cells upregulated genes involved in neuroprotective and anti-inflammatory processes. Notable genes that were significantly upregulated in the negative engram cells were secreted phosphoprotein 1 (Spp1) and transthyretin (Ttr). SPP1, also known as osteopontin, is an extracellular matrix protein widely expressed in immune cells, such as macrophages and microglia, mediating inflammatory responses. Blocking SPP1 protein expression in cells within the hippocampus has been shown to attenuate inflammation-induced depressive-like behaviors in mice (Li et al., 2023). Furthermore, the Spp1 gene is consistently upregulated after chronic social stress (Stankiewicz et al., 2015), recall of contextual fear (Barnes et al., 2012), and in rats with increased trait anxiety (Díaz-Morán et al., 2013). Mice receiving chronic negative memory stimulation in our study displayed similar increases in anxiety-like behaviors and impairments in fear responses. Further, TTR is a serum transporter for thyroid hormones and retinol binding proteins that has been shown to be detrimental when misfolded and aggregated in various forms of amyloidosis, while also having evidence of protective effects in Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (Buxbaum et al., 2008; Sharma et al., 2019). Interestingly, TTR mRNA and protein were increased in both male and female rats that underwent acute or chronic stress, emphasizing the potential impact of glucocorticoid hormones on TTR protein levels (Martinho et al., 2012). Together, these findings suggest that genes upregulated in negative engram cells are linked to stress-induced changes in anxiety, fear, and depressive-like behaviors.

Strikingly, negative engram cells also significantly upregulated amyloid precursor protein (App) and apolipoprotein E (Apoe) genes. APP plays a central role in AD pathology through generation of amyloid plaques (Lee et al., 2018; Lee et al., 2020). Apoe genotype accounts for the majority of AD risk and pathology (Johnson et al., 2015; Schuff et al., 2009), with the E4 isotype accounting for as much as 50% of AD in the United States (Raber et al., 2004). The upregulation of these genes involved in neurodegeneration and neuroinflammation is in line with human research showing a link between repetitive negative thinking and increased risk of cognitive decline and vulnerability to dementia (Schlosser et al., 2020). Furthermore, negative engram cells downregulated neuroprotective and anti-inflammatory genes, notably brain-derived neurotrophic factor (Bdnf). BDNF is a key molecule involved in memory in the healthy and pathological brain. Interventions such as exercise and antidepressant treatments enhance the expression of BDNF protein (Miranda et al., 2019). Future studies may further investigate the neuroprotective role of positive engrams to test, for instance, if activating a positive engram in a mouse model of AD confers protective effects on the brain and slows the progression of disease. Indeed, evidence of positive memory impact on mice with a depression-like phenotype has already shown promising results in this area of research (Ramirez et al., 2015). Together, these genetic data begin to uncover the molecular landscape that differentiates positive and negative memory-bearing cells, linking them to either protective or pathological brain states, respectively.

Chronic stimulation of engrams alter behavior and cellular responses

Chronic modulation of engram cells in rodents has differential effects of behavior, depending on the brain region and valence of the tagged neurons. Reactivation of positive engram cells in the hippocampus rescued stress-induced depression-like phenotype and increased neurogenesis in mice (Ramirez et al., 2015). Further, chronic stimulation of dorsal or ventral hippocampal fear engrams produced a context-specific reduction or enhancement of fear responses, respectively (Chen et al., 2019). Interestingly, chronic optogenetic activation of dorsal hippocampal fear engrams results in context-specific reduction in fear expression that resembles extinction, even in mice with impaired extinction due to ethanol withdrawal (Cincotta et al., 2021). Activation of negative engram cells labeled during chronic social defeat stress induced social avoidance, a behavior common in human patients with MDD (Zhang et al., 2019). Together, these studies suggest that activity-dependent modulation of hippocampal cells over prolonged periods of time is sufficient to modify behavioral responses and brain states in mice.

Engram studies have predominantly focused on the neuronal phenotypes resulting from activity-dependent manipulations; however, glial cell responses to chronic memory activation and subsequent behavioral changes are unknown. Microglia are the brain’s resident immune cells, actively surveying the environment to maintain homeostasis and protect against insult and disease. Under normal conditions, microglial cells display small cell bodies and fine, highly ramified processes that allow them to detect and respond to threats (Nimmerjahn et al., 2005). Once a threat is detected (i.e., pathogens, debris, injury), microglia retract and exhibit large soma size and thickening, entering an ‘ameboid’ state. Evidence has accumulated that microglial morphology may be more complex than this dichotomy between ‘surveying’ and ‘ameboid’ microglia (Vidal-Itriago et al., 2022; Leyh et al., 2021; Reddaway et al., 2023). Our chronic negative engram activation may induce an ‘insult’ on the brain, as evidenced by an increase in Iba1 expression and hyper-ramified morphology, indicating a complex change in microglial phenotype. In support of these findings, chronic stress similarly induced an increase in Iba1 expression and hyper-ramified microglia in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) that was correlated with impaired spatial working memory (Hinwood et al., 2013). Further supporting these findings, a mouse model of PTSD displayed a similar increase in Iba1+ and hyper-ramified microglial cells in the PFC and CA1 layer of the hippocampus (Smith et al., 2019). Together, the changes in microglial cell number and morphology observed in the context of chronic negative engram stimulation are reminiscent of the alterations seen in chronic stress, PTSD, and related depression-like behaviors.

Further, astrocytes express glial fibrillary acidic protein (GFAP), a critical intermediate filament responsible for maintaining their cytoskeletal structure (Yang and Wang, 2015). Chronic restraint stress has been associated with static GFAP+ astrocytic number and atrophy of astrocytic processes in the hippocampus, amygdala, and retrosplenial cortex (Kassem et al., 2013). Studies have primarily shown a decrease in GFAP+ astrocyte numbers and atrophy in rodent brains subjected to chronic and acute stressors (Tynan et al., 2013; Banasr and Duman, 2008; Gosselin et al., 2009; Miguel-Hidalgo et al., 2000; Bender et al., 2016; Codeluppi et al., 2021). However, recent studies have found a variety of changes in astrocytic morphology and number depending on the type of stressor and brain region. Conversely, others report an increase in GFAP+ cells in the hippocampus (Jang et al., 2008). Additionally, unpredictable chronic mild stress substantially increased GFAP immunoreactivity and promoted a more branched, reactive morphology in the hippocampus, which was correlated with increases in anxiety and anhedonic-like behaviors (Du Preez et al., 2021). This observed phenotype was coupled with similar changes in Iba1+ microglia, featuring increased branching complexity and ramification, longer processes, and increased volume of occupation, as discussed previously (Du Preez et al., 2021; Hinwood et al., 2013). In the case of humans, the discourse on glial changes in chronic stress and depression-like behaviors has been similarly complex. GFAP concentrations in the cerebellum, prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, and anterior cingulate cortex have been reported to decrease in patients with major depression compared to controls (Fatemi et al., 2004; Si et al., 2004; Webster et al., 2005; Cobb et al., 2016). However, a recent study found increased GFAP concentrations in the cerebral spinal fluid of patients with major depression compared to controls (Michel et al., 2021). Our findings that astrocytes increase in number and branch length in the hippocampus after chronic negative memory stimulation mirror the impacts of chronic stress on glial morphology. Further research is needed to understand these nuanced changes across brain regions, animal models, and type of stressors and influence on behavior.

Our previous work demonstrated that global chronic activation of vHPC excitatory neurons or astrocytes for three months was sufficient to induce a variety of behavioral and cellular changes (Suthard et al., 2023). In the current study, we modulated hippocampal cells in an activity-dependent manner, gaining access to the specific impact of chronic negative memory activation on behavioral and cellular responses. Collectively, our data show a similar impact of CaMKII+ neuron and negative fear memory activation on fear extinction behaviors – namely, an increase in freezing responses. However, GFAP+ astrocyte activation induced a mild decrease in anxiety-related behaviors in the open field, while our negative memory activation significantly increased anxiety. Histologically, our previous work had mild impacts on glial number and morphology with non-memory-specific neuron or astrocyte chronic activation (Suthard et al., 2023). The current study demonstrates a much more potent increase in both microglia and astrocyte number, as well as robust changes in morphological characteristics like ramification index, cell territory volume, and branch lengths, which underscore the efficacy of chronic negative engram stimulation. A concern, however, is that activation of vHPC neurons could induce local tissue pathology as evidenced by inflammation and glial response. As this previous work activated all CaMKII+ neurons in vHPC and induced very modest changes in glial cells, we expect that in the current work, stimulating a smaller ensemble of cells would induce less of such ‘off-target’ inflammation. Together, these findings highlight the differences between modulating cells in a cell-type-specific or activity-dependent manner (e.g., vHPC neurons or negative engram cells) and the resulting phenotypes that emerge.

Previous work from our lab and others has shown that there are differential effects of chronic activation of negative, positive, and neutral ensembles within the hippocampus. As discussed earlier, chronic reactivation of a positive valence engram was sufficient to reverse the effects of stress on depression-like behaviors, whereas modulating a neutral ensemble did not (Ramirez et al., 2015). Here, we speculate that chronic activation of a positive engram in healthy, unstressed mice may produce behavioral outcomes similar to those effects seen in chronic environmental enrichment, with improved cognitive flexibility, memory, and even enhanced levels of anti-inflammatory proteins like BDNF (see Bayne, 2018 for review). We further speculate that unlike neutral or negative memories, chronic stimulation of a positive memory may provide prophylactic effects against stress, degeneration, and aging, though this work remains an opportunity for future experiments. Other studies utilizing repeated negative memory activation (Chen et al., 2019; Zhang et al., 2019; Cincotta et al., 2021) showed content- and context-specific effects, such as bi-directional control over the strength of a fear memory in healthy and addiction-related states, with no changes observed in a neutral control group. Although our experiments here do not compare the effects of positive or neutral memory stimulation directly but rather compare a negative memory group to a fluorophore-only control, which is therefore a current limitation, we believe that chronic stimulation of each would produce unique sets of cellular and behavioral phenotypes that raise the intriguing notion of manipulating memories to mimic or ameliorate various pathological states.

Relevance of chronic negative thinking-like behaviors to human health

Chronic memory activation may mimic symptoms of rumination, negative thinking, and ongoing worry shared by individuals with GAD, MDD, and PTSD (Brewin et al., 2010; Newman et al., 2013; Watkins, 2008; McEvoy et al., 2013), but the mechanisms behind this connection are currently unknown. In the healthy brain, ‘inhibitory control’ of the prefrontal cortex on the hippocampus is thought to keep these symptoms at bay. Recent work has suggested a framework where stress may compromise GABAergic signaling in the hippocampus (Alaiyed et al., 2020; Banasr et al., 2017; Albrecht et al., 2021), allowing a state of hippocampal hyperactivity to persist. This elevated hippocampal activity and dysfunction of GABAergic interneuron is evident in human patients with major depression and PTSD, and the activity levels are correlated with behavioral rumination and flashback intensity (Osuch et al., 2001; Rayner et al., 2016). Human functional imaging work has supported this framework by showing that fronto-hippocampal inhibitory control via GABAergic signaling underlies this inability to suppress unwanted thoughts. Further, GABAergic signaling in the hippocampus has been shown to produce impaired extinction of conditioned fear (Müller et al., 2015) and increased anxiety (Crestani et al., 1999). This mirrors our findings of increased anxiety and decreased extinction ability in mice that experienced negative memory activation. Further exploration into how chronic engram activation impacts excitatory-inhibitory (E/I) imbalance may provide a link between memory modulation and induction of effects on the brain similar to those related to rumination, negative thinking, and worry.

In humans, persistent worrying and rumination behaviors that are common in neuropsychiatric disorders can affect the health of the entire body. In the context of our findings, the concept of allostatic load refers to the ‘wear and tear’ the body experiences as a result of chronic exposure to stressful events. While the body attempts to produce a physiologically adaptive response through the process of allostasis, it is proposed that repeated cycles of allostasis in response to chronic stressors results in allostatic overload. In this state, the cumulative impact of chronic stress has overcome the body’s ability to cope, ultimately culminating in disease (McEwen and Stellar, 1993; McEwen, 2004). This applies to the nervous system, wherein rumination has been shown to increase the risk of alcohol and substance abuse, especially in young individuals, as a form of compensatory behavior to numb the persistent negative thoughts (Nolen-Hoeksema and Watkins, 2011). Rumination after a stressful event is also linked to insomnia, poor sleep quality, and pre-sleep intrusive thoughts, as well as an increase in the risk of heart disease and cardiovascular issues via autonomic dysregulation (Guastella and Moulds, 2007; Busch et al., 2017). Together, we hope that the current study provides a novel framework in which memory itself may affect, and lead to, the maladaptive brain and body states described here.

Chronic engram reactivation and its differing effects on freezing behavior

Previous work showed that experimental activation of recall-induced populations in the dentate gyrus leads to fear attenuation (Khalaf et al., 2018), while our experimental manipulation produces lasting negative impacts on the brain and behavior. Specifically, this study posits that this fear attenuation occurs in neuronal ensembles through updating or unlearning of the original memory trace via the engagement, rather than suppression, of an original traumatic experience. Notably, one major difference is that our experiment reactivated an ensemble that was tagged during memory encoding, while they activated a remote recall-induced ensemble that was tagged 1 month after encoding. Although there is high overlap between the encoding and recall ensembles when mice are exposed to the conditioning context, these ensembles are not identical and may result in different behavioral phenotypes when chronically reactivated. Further, Khalaf et al. relied on reactivation of the recall-induced ensemble during extinction to facilitate rapid fear attenuation. This differs experimentally from our current work as their reactivation occurred during the extinction process in the previously conditioned context, while we reactivated chronically in the animal’s home cage over the course of a longer time period. It may be necessary that the memory is reactivated, and thus, more liable to re-contextualization, in the original context compared to an unrelated home cage environment where there are no related cues present. Importantly, this previous work tested the attenuation of fear shortly after an extinction process, while we did not extinguish the memory with aid of the memory reactivation. Finally, we tested remote recall (i.e., 3 months post-conditioning), as opposed to a shorter time interval (28 days). Despite these discrepancies, it is indeed possible that our chronic reactivation of a fear acquisition ensemble over time mildly attenuates or changes the original memory in some way. Future work could build on the findings of Khalaf et al. and/or show differences in the activation of a conditioning vs. recall-induced ensemble on fear attenuation during extinction.

Limitations of the study

Limitations of chronic chemogenetic modulation

In this study, we rely on the successful chemogenetic modulation of fearful neuronal ensembles to assess its impact on behavior over a long period of time. This strategy requires that our ligand, DCZ, maintains an effect on neuronal excitability across our 3 months of chronic reactivation. Previous work has shown that CNO administration in drinking water over 1 month consistently inhibited hM4Di+ neurons without altering baseline neuronal excitability as measured by firing rate and potassium currents (Xia et al., 2017). Although this is only for 1 month, it is administered via the same oral route as our DCZ protocol and suggests that at least for that amount of time we are likely producing similar effects. However, this must be experimentally tested as CNO and DCZ are unique ligands for chemogenetic modulation and must be extended out from 1 month to 3 months to assess the longevity of its effects in the brain. If DCZ is only having an effect for 1 vs. 3 months, we are still observing enduring changes that resulted from this shorter-term disturbance, which nonetheless informs our understanding of negative memory’s effects on the brain and behavior. On this same note, assessment of DREADD impact on E/I balance must be quantified utilizing both excitatory and inhibitory markers in future work to understand how chemogenetic modulation of engram cells may truly impact circuit-level E/I dysfunction.

Related to this, other work has shown evidence of DREADD toxicity at high titer levels of AAV2/7-CaMKII-hM4Di-mCherry in the HPC at 5 weeks (Goossens et al., 2021). We mitigated this risk by utilizing a viral strategy that targets a smaller number of engram cells using the AAV9-DIO-Flex-hM3Dq-mCherry virus at a lower titer, unlike this previous work that targeted all CaMKII+ cells in the HPC. Despite these efforts, we wanted to assess signs of decreasing neuronal health that may have resulted from chronic DREADD expression and utilized NeuN counts within multiple hippocampal subregions (Yousef et al., 2017). Here, we note that we did not observe significant decreases in NeuN across active Gq receptor or mCherry control groups in either of the age groups (Figure 1—figure supplement 1). However, immunohistochemistry using an individual marker may not be sufficient to capture the entire health profile of an individual neuron and future work should consider other markers of cell death or inflammation to ensure prolonged health of DREADD+ cells.

Limitations of genetic tagging strategies

Further, the present study relies on use of the Tet-tag and TRAP2 genetic strategies to label engram cells for sequencing (Tet-tag) and chronic activation (TRAP2). While both systems rely on the cFos promoter to drive an effector of interest (fluorophore or DREADD), their efficacy and temporal resolution vary substantially depending on the genetic cell type, brain region, temporal parameters of Dox or 4-OHT delivery, subject-by-subject metabolic variability, and threshold to Dox induction given the promoter sequences inherent to each system. The TRAP2 line labels a subset of endogenously activeCA1 pyramidal cells (e.g., 5–18%) while the DOX system labels 20–40% of CA1 pyramidal cells (DeNardo et al., 2019; Monasterio et al., 2024). Further, the temporal windows for each system range from hours in TRAP2, compared to 24–48 hr in the Tet-tag system, resulting in potentially less-specific labeling in the latter (DeNardo et al., 2019; Denny et al., 2014; Liu et al., 2012). Further, the efficacy of tagging a population of cells with either system will inherently constrain the number of cells that may overlap with cFos+ cells upon re-exposure to a given experience, given the difference in the percentage of initial cells labeled (Kim and Cho, 2020; Ortega-de San Luis et al., 2023; Shpokayte et al., 2022). However, it is important to note that tagging vHPC cells with both strategies is nonetheless sufficient to drive behavioral responses (Shpokayte et al., 2022; Ortega-de San Luis et al., 2023).

Finally, and promisingly, as more studies continue to link the in vivo physiological dynamics of these cell populations tagged using each system (e.g., compare Pettit et al., 2022 with Tanaka et al., 2018) and correlating their activity to behavioral phenotypes, our field is in the prime position to uncover deeper principles governing hippocampus-mediated engrams in the brain. Together, we believe a more comprehensive understanding of these systems is fully warranted, especially in the service of further cataloging cellular similarities and differences within such tagged populations.

Limitations of GSEA within engram cells

Although the gene sets were pre-selected to assess changes canonically involved in ‘neurodegeneration’ or ‘neuroprotection’ such as App, Apoe, or Bdnf, a major limitation of this technique is that we must avoid making strong claims about the actual function of these up- or downregulated genes without performing proper causal studies. We hope these findings provide an unbiased inventory of the changes induced within engram cells related to memory valence that can inspire future work testing the knock-in or knock-down of these genes to assess their direct impact on the brain and behavior.

Limitations of immunohistochemistry analysis

Despite our study’s assessment of changes in astrocytic and microglial morphology and number after our manipulation, using immunohistochemistry does not allow us to capture all changes that may be occurring at the gene or protein level, painting an incomplete picture of the impact on glial cells. Future work utilizing transcriptomic or proteomic profiling of both engram and non-neuronal cells will help the field understand how these cell types are interacting with one another and provide a more complete mechanistic understanding of these glial-related changes.

Materials and methods

Generation of single-cell suspension from mouse hippocampal tissue

As shown in Figure 1 (RNA-sequencing), C57BL/6J mice were bilaterally injected with a 1:1 viral cocktail of AAV9-c-Fos-tTA and AAV9-TRE-eYFP into the vHPC. This activity-dependent labeling strategy captures sufficiently active neurons expressing the immediate-early gene, cfos. This system couples the cfos promoter to the expression of the tetracycline transactivator (tTA), which binds to the tetracycline response element (TRE) in its protein form. When DOX is present, such as in the animal’s diet, this inhibits the binding of tTA to TRE, preventing eYFP labeling of active cells (Liu et al., 2012).

Prior to surgery, mice were placed on a Dox diet to inhibit the labeling of active neurons. After surgery and viral expression, on day 10, mice were taken off of Dox prior to engram tagging to open the labeling window. On day 11, mice were subjected to CFC (negative) or male-to-female interaction (positive) to label active neurons with eYFP. Mice were placed immediately back on Dox to close this tagging window. Then, 24 hr later, mice were sacrificed and brains were rapidly extracted, and the hippocampal regions were isolated by microdissection. Eight mice were pooled by each experimental condition. Single-cell suspension was prepared according to the guideline of the Adult Brain Dissociation Kit (Miltenyi Biotec, Cat# 13-107-677). Briefly, the hippocampal samples were incubated with digestion enzymes in the C Tube placed on the gentleMACS Octo Dissociator with Heaters with gentleMACS Program: 37C_ABDK_01. After termination of the program, the samples were applied through a MACS SmartStrainer (70 μm). Then a debris removal step and a red blood cell removal step were applied to obtain single-cell suspension. Viral injection surgery through RNA-sequencing was performed in Shpokayte et al., 2022, and this dataset was utilized for further analysis in the current study.

Isolation of EYFP-positive single cell by FACS

The single-cell suspension was subject to a BD FACSAria cell sorter according to the manufacturer’s protocol to isolate eYFP-single cell population. This was performed in Shpokayte et al., 2022.

Preparation of RNA-seq library

The RNA of FACS isolated eYFP-positive cells was extracted by using TRIzol (Invitrogen Life Technologies, MA) followed by Direct-zol kit (Zymo Research, CA) according to the manufacturer’s instructions. Then the RNA-seq library was prepared using SMART-Seq v4 Ultra Low Input RNA Kit (TaKaRa Bio, USA). This was performed in Shpokayte et al., 2022.

Analysis of RNA-seq data

The original RNA sequencing data was from Shpokayte et al., 2022. To analyze this data, the resulting reads from Illumina had good quality by checking with FastQC. The first 40 bp of the reads were mapped to mouse genome (mm10) using STAR (Dobin et al., 2013), which was indexed with Ensembl GRCm38.91 gene annotation. The read counts were obtained using featureCounts (Liao et al., 2014) function from the Subread package with an unstranded option. Reads were normalized by library size and differential expression analysis based on negative binomial distribution was done with DESeq2 (Love et al., 2014). Genes with FDR-adjusted p-value<0.00001 plus more than fourfold difference were considered to be differentially expressed. Raw data, along with gene expression levels, are being deposited to the NCBI Gene Expression Omnibus (Liu and Yuan, 2022). For Gene Ontology analysis, we ranked protein-coding genes by their fold changes and used them as the input for the GseaPreranked tool.

GSEA

To further analyze the dataset generated in Shpokayte et al., 2022, we performed further analyses using GSEA (https://www.gsea-msigdb.org/gsea/index.jsp; Subramanian et al., 2005; Mootha et al., 2003). This is a computational method that determines whether a priori-defined set of genes shows statistically significant, concordant differences between two biological states. GSEA is a joint project of UC San Diego and Broad Institute. Gene sets were selected based on existing literature around genes important in neurodegeneration, inflammation, and apoptosis.

Subjects (Figures 25)

Fos-based transgenic mouse, Fos-tm2.1(iCre-ERT2) raised on a C57BL/6 background (TRAP2) (Jackson Laboratories; strain #030323) mice were bred in-house. The mice were housed in groups of 2–5 mice per cage. The animal facilities (vivarium and behavioral testing rooms) were maintained on a 12:12 hr light cycle (07:00–19:00). Mice received food and water ad libitum before surgery. Following surgery, mice were group-housed with littermates and allowed to recover for a minimum of 4 weeks before experimentation for viral expression. All subjects were treated in accordance with protocol 201800579 approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) at Boston University.

DCZ administration

All animals had their home cage water supply replaced with water-soluble DCZ in distilled water (diH2O) every 5 days (HelloBio, #HB9126). Administration was calculated based on each mouse drinking approximately 6 mL of water per day. A stock solution was created of 1–2 mg of water-soluble DCZ into 1 mL of DMSO and vortexed until in solution. 17 µL of stock solution was combined with 1 L of diH2O for a final concentration of 3 µg/kg/day per mouse, as previously described in Suthard et al., 2023, to avoid any potential excitotoxicity resulting through stimulating cells for up to 3 months, and to mimic a more sustained dysregulation that may accumulate in severity over time. Importantly, no obvious signs of distress or seizures were observed with chronic administration of DCZ by experimenters or vivarium staff for the entire duration of the project.

Stereotaxic surgeries

For all surgeries, mice were initially anesthetized with 3.0% isoflurane inhalation during induction and maintained at 1–2% isoflurane inhalation through stereotaxic nose-cone delivery (oxygen 1 L/min). Ophthalmic ointment was applied to the eyes to provide adequate lubrication and prevent corneal desiccation. The hair on the scalp above the surgical site was removed using Veet hair removal cream and subsequently cleaned with alternating applications of betadine solution and 70% ethanol. 2.0% lidocaine hydrochloride (HCl) was injected subcutaneously as local analgesia prior to midsagittal incision of the scalp skin to expose the skull. 0.1 mg/kg (5 mg/kg) subcutaneous (SQ) dose of meloxicam was administered at the beginning of surgery. All animals received bilateral craniotomies with a 0.5–0.6 mm drill-bit for vCA1 injections. A 10 µL airtight Hamilton syringe with an attached 33-gauge beveled needle was slowly lowered to the coordinates of vCA1: –3.16 anteroposterior (AP), ± 3.10 mediolateral (ML) and both −4.25/–4.50 dorsoventral (DV) to cover vertical the axis of vHPC. All coordinates are given relative to bregma (mm). A total volume of 300 nL of AAV9-DIO-Flex-hM3Dq-mCherry or AAV9-DIO-Flex-mCherry was bilaterally injected into the vCA1 at both DV coordinates (150 nL at each site) listed above. The needle remained at the target site for 7–10 min post-injection before removal. Incisions were sutured closed using 4/0 non-absorbable Nylon Monofilament Suture [Brosan]. Following surgery, mice were injected with 0.1 mg/kg IP dose of buprenorphine (volume administered was dependent on the weight of the animal at the time of surgery). They were placed in a recovery cage with a heating pad until fully recovered from anesthesia. Histological assessment verified bilateral viral targeting and data from off-target injections were not included in analyses.

Immunohistochemistry

Mice were overdosed with 3% isoflurane and perfused transcardially with cold (4°C) phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in PBS. Brains were extracted and kept in PFA at 4°C for 24–48 hr and transferred to a 30% sucrose in PBS solution. Long-term storage of the brains consisted of transferring to a 0.01% sodium azide in PBS solution until slicing. Brains were sectioned into 50-µm-thick coronal and sagittal sections with a vibratome and collected in cold PBS or 0.01% sodium azide in PBS for long-term storage. Sections underwent three washes for 10 min each in PBS to remove the 0.01% sodium azide that they were stored in. Sections were blocked for 2 hr at room temperature (RT) in PBS combined with 0.2% Triton X-100 (PBST) and 5% normal goat serum (NGS) on a shaker. Sections were incubated in the primary antibody (1:500 polyclonal guinea pig anti-NeuN/Fox3 [SySy #266-004]; 1:1000 mouse anti-GFAP [NeuroMab #N206A/8]; 1:1000 rabbit polyclonal anti-Iba1 [Wako #019-19741]) diluted in the same PBST-NGS solution at 4°C for 24–48 hr. Sections then underwent three washes for 10 min each in PBST. Sections were incubated in a secondary antibody (1:200 Alexa Fluor 488 anti-guinea [Invitrogen #A-11073], 1:1000 Alexa Fluor 488 anti-rabbit [Invitrogen #A-11008], 1:1000 Alexa Fluor 555 anti-mouse [Invitrogen #A-11001]) for 2 hr at RT. Sections then underwent three more washes in PBST for 10 min each. Sections were then mounted onto microscope slides (VMR International LLC, PA). Vectashield HardSet Mounting Medium with DAPI (Vector Laboratories Inc, CA) was applied and slides were coverslipped and allowed to dry for 24 hr at RT. Once dry, slides were sealed with clear nail polish around the edges and stored in a slide box in 4°C. If not mounted immediately, sections were stored in PBS at 4°C.

Cell quantification and morphological analysis (NeuN, Iba1, GFAP)

Only animals that had accurate bilateral injections were selected for quantification. Images were acquired using a confocal microscope (Zeiss LSM800, Germany) with a ×20 objective. 6–8 single-tile images were taken in the brain regions of interest across 3–5 coronal slices, giving us a total of 18 single-tile images per mouse × 3–4 mice in each group. When imaging NeuN z-stacks, intervals were taken at 4 µm, whereas Iba1 and GFAP z-stacks were taken at 1 µm intervals for detailed morphological analysis. Ilastik (Berg et al., 2019) was used to quantify the total number of cells and 3DMorph (York et al., 2018) was used to assess the morphology of GFAP and Iba1 cells. Images of NeuN, GFAP, and Iba-1 were captured in a 300 µ 300 single-tile (micrometers) that contained matched ROIs across groups for hippocampal subregions.

For 3DMorph analysis, we selected a total of three single-tile images from the same ROI per mouse per group. This gave a total of ~20–30 cells per image. Therefore, we conducted morphological analysis on a total of 755 Iba1+ cells and 842 GFAP+ cells across all groups. Acquired image files (.czi) were opened in ImageJ. Processing of images in Figures 1, 2 and 5 and Figure 1—figure supplement 1 involved maximizing intensity, removing outlier noise (despeckling), and adjusting contrast of images for improved detection of glial cells in 3DMorph.

Cell volume (µm3) is calculated by converting the number of voxels occupied by the cell object to a real-world unit based on defined scaling factors. Territorial volume (µm3) is estimated by creating a polygon around the outside points of each cell and measuring the volume inside. Ramification index is a measure of the complexity of astrocyte and microglial morphology, with a higher ramification index indicating a more complex and extensive branching structure. This is computed by dividing the cell territorial volume by the cell volume. Average centroid distance (µm) helps us understand the distribution of cells in the brain, with a larger centroid distance indicating the glial cells are further apart. Using a 3D reconstruction of each cell skeleton, branches are labeled as primary, secondary, tertiary, and quaternary. The number of branch points (#) is calculated by the intersections of each branch type and endpoints (#) are counted at the termination of these branches. Finally, each of these branches are measured in length and utilized for the minimum, maximum, and average branch length (µm).

Behavioral assays

All behavioral assays were conducted during the light cycle of the day (07:00–19:00) on animals. Mice were handled for 3–5 days, 2 min per day, before all behavioral experiments began. Behavioral assays include open field, zero maze, y-maze, fear conditioning, recall, extinction, and generalization. As described in our previous work in greater detail (Suthard et al., 2023), DCZ water was removed 1–2 hr from the home cage to allow for a decline in ligand concentration in the brain and to ensure there were no acute effects of the drug on behavior. When administered IP in mice, DCZ peaks 5–10 min after injection, declining to baseline levels around 2 hr after (Nagai et al., 2020).

Open-field test

An open (61 cm × 61 cm) arena with black plastic walls was used for the open-field test, with a red-taped area in the middle delineating ‘center’ from ‘edges’. A camera was placed above the open field in order to record video of the session. Mice were individually placed into the center of the chamber and allowed to explore freely for 10 min. At the end of the session, each mouse was placed into a separate cage until all of its cage mates had also gone through the behavioral test. They were all placed back into the home cage once this occurred. An automated video-tracking system, AnyMaze v4.3 (Stoelting Co, Wood Dale, IL), was used to measure total distance traveled, number of entries into the center, and total time spent in the center.

Zero-maze test

The zero-maze test is a pharmacologically validated assay of anxiety in animal models that is based on the natural aversion of mice to elevated, open areas. It is composed of a 44-cm-wide ring with an outer diameter of 62 cm. It contained four equal zones of walled (closed) and unwalled (open) areas. The entire ring is 8 cm in height and sits at an elevation of 66 cm off the ground. All animals at each time point were tested on the same day. Mice were placed in the closed area at the start of a 10 min session. The following parameters were analyzed: time spent in the open area, number of entries into the open area, etc. The maze was cleaned with 70% ethanol between mice. During the behavioral testing, the lighting levels remained constant and there were no shadows over the surface of the maze.

y-maze test

The y-maze is a hippocampal-dependent spatial working memory task that requires mice to use external cues to navigate the identical internal arms. The apparatus consisted of a clear plexiglass maze with three arms (36.5 cm length, 7.5 cm width, 12.5 cm height) that were intersected at 120°. A mouse was placed at the end of one arm and allowed to move freely through the maze for 10 min without any reinforcements, such as food or water. Entries into all arms were noted (center of the body must cross into the arm for a valid entry) and a spontaneous alternation was counted if an animal entered three different arms consecutively. Percentage of spontaneous alternation was calculated according to the following formula: [(number of alterations)/(total number of arm entries - 1) × 100]. To prevent bias in data analysis, the test was carried out in a blind manner by the experimenter and behavior was analyzed blindly in AnyMaze. Additionally, total distance traveled was calculated for the test to rule out changes in locomotion.

Fear conditioning, recall, and extinction

For CFC, all time points took place in mouse conditioning chambers (Coulbourn Instruments, Holliston, MA). The grid floor was connected to a precision animal shocker (Coulbourn Instruments), which delivered a total of four foot shocks (2 s duration, 1.5 mA) at the 120, 180, 240, and 300 s time points during the 360 s session. This mouse conditioning chamber (18.5 × 18.5 × 21.5 cm) was composed of metal walls, plexiglass front and back walls, and a stainless-steel grid floor (16 grid bars). For recall, all animals were placed back into the same chambers they were originally fear conditioned in for a total of 300 s in the absence of foot shocks. For extinction, all animals were placed back into the same chambers they were originally conditioned in for a total of 1800s in the absence of foot shocks. To record the animal’s freezing activity, a video camera was mounted to the ceiling of the chamber and fed into a computer running FreezeFrame software (Actimetrics, Wilmette, IL). The program determined movement as changes in pixel luminance over a set period of time. Freezing was defined as a bout of 1.25 s or longer without changes in pixel luminance and verified by an experimenter blind to treatment groups. The chambers were cleaned with 70% ethanol solution prior to each animal placement.

Context B exposure

For generalization (context B), after 5 days of extinction, the animals were taken into a different room from the original conditioning and placed in a new larger chamber. During the experiment, the floor was changed to a smooth bottom, the walls were changed to have striped backgrounds, the odor was changed to a neutral almond scent, and the lights were turned off during the procedure. Red lights were turned on in the room for the experimenter. The entire exposure to context B was a total of 10 min. Activity recording and freezing level analysis were assessed as described above using FreezeFrame software. The chambers were cleaned with 70% ethanol solution prior to each animal placement.

Statistics and data analysis

Data were analyzed using GraphPad Prism (GraphPad Software v9.4.1, La Jolla, CA). Data were tested for normality using the Shapiro–Wilk and Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests. Outliers were removed using the ROUT method, which uses identification from nonlinear regression. We chose a ROUT coefficient Q value of 10% (false discovery rate), making the threshold for outliers less-strict and allowing for an increase in power for outlier detection. No more than 1–2 mice were removed in any group as outliers for any behavioral measure. To analyze differences between groups (mCherry and hM3Dq) and across age groups (3 and 11 months old [fear conditioning]; 6 and 14 months old [all other behavioral tasks]), we used a two-way ANOVA (between-subject factor: group; within-subject factor: age). To analyze the differences between groups and across time within a single session (i.e., fear conditioning) or across months (i.e., body weight), we used two-way RM ANOVAs (between-subjects factor: group; within-subjects factor: time/month). Post hoc analyses were performed using Tukey’s and Sidak’s multiple comparisons test, where applicable. Alpha was set to 0.05 for all tests.

Acknowledgements

This material was based on the work supported by an NIH Early Independence Award DP5 OD023106-01, NIH Transformative Award, the Ludwig Family Foundation, the Air Force Office of Scientific Research award (FA9550-21-1-0310), the Pew Scholars Program in the Biomedical Sciences, the Chan-Zuckerberg Initiative, and the Center for Systems Neuroscience and Neurophotonics Center at Boston University.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Monika Shpokayte, Email: mshpokay@bu.edu.

Steve Ramirez, Email: dvsteve@bu.edu.

Adrien Peyrache, McGill University, Canada.

Laura L Colgin, University of Texas at Austin, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Institutes of Health Transformative Award to Steve Ramirez.

  • Ludwig Family Foundation to Rebecca L Suthard.

  • Air Force Office of Scientific Research FA9550-21-1-0310 to Steve Ramirez.

  • Pew Scholars Program in Biomedical Sciences to Steve Ramirez.

  • Chan-Zuckerberg Initiative to Steve Ramirez.

  • Center for Systems Neuroscience and Neurophotonics Center at Boston University to Steve Ramirez.

  • Nationai Institutes of Health Early Independence Award DP5 OD023106-01 to Steve Ramirez.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Formal analysis, Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Methodology.

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Supervision, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Project administration, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Project administration, Writing – review and editing.

Ethics

All subjects were treated in accord with protocol 201800579 approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) at Boston University.

Additional files

MDAR checklist

Data availability

Raw data along with gene expression levels are deposited to NCBI Gene Expression Omnibus under accession code GSE198731.

The following dataset was generated:

Liu S, Yuan B. 2022. Hippocampal cells multiplex positive and negative engrams. NCBI Gene Expression Omnibus. GSE198731

References

  1. Alaiyed S, McCann M, Mahajan G, Rajkowska G, Stockmeier CA, Kellar KJ, Wu JY, Conant K. Venlafaxine stimulates an mmp-9-dependent increase in excitatory/inhibitory balance in a stress model of depression. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2020;40:4418–4431. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2387-19.2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Albrecht A, Redavide E, Regev-Tsur S, Stork O, Richter-Levin G. Hippocampal gabaergic interneurons and their co-localized neuropeptides in stress vulnerability and resilience. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2021;122:229–244. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Banasr M, Duman RS. Glial loss in the prefrontal cortex is sufficient to induce depressive-like behaviors. Biological Psychiatry. 2008;64:863–870. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.06.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Banasr M, Lepack A, Fee C, Duric V, Maldonado-Aviles J, DiLeone R, Sibille E, Duman RS, Sanacora G. Characterization of GABAergic marker expression in the chronic unpredictable stress model of depression. Chronic Stress. 2017;1:247054701772045. doi: 10.1177/2470547017720459. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Barnes P, Kirtley A, Thomas KL. Quantitatively and qualitatively different cellular processes are engaged in CA1 during the consolidation and reconsolidation of contextual fear memory. Hippocampus. 2012;22:149–171. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bayne K. Environmental enrichment and mouse models: current perspectives. Animal Models and Experimental Medicine. 2018;1:82–90. doi: 10.1002/ame2.12015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bender CL, Calfa GD, Molina VA. Astrocyte plasticity induced by emotional stress: a new partner in psychiatric physiopathology? Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry. 2016;65:68–77. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2015.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Berg S, Kutra D, Kroeger T, Straehle CN, Kausler BX, Haubold C, Schiegg M, Ales J, Beier T, Rudy M, Eren K, Cervantes JI, Xu B, Beuttenmueller F, Wolny A, Zhang C, Koethe U, Hamprecht FA, Kreshuk A. ilastik: interactive machine learning for (bio)image analysis. Nature Methods. 2019;16:1226–1232. doi: 10.1038/s41592-019-0582-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brewin CR, Gregory JD, Lipton M, Burgess N. Intrusive images in psychological disorders: characteristics, neural mechanisms, and treatment implications. Psychological Review. 2010;117:210–232. doi: 10.1037/a0018113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Busch LY, Pössel P, Valentine JC. Meta-analyses of cardiovascular reactivity to rumination: a possible mechanism linking depression and hostility to cardiovascular disease. Psychological Bulletin. 2017;143:1378–1394. doi: 10.1037/bul0000119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Buxbaum JN, Ye Z, Reixach N, Friske L, Levy C, Das P, Golde T, Masliah E, Roberts AR, Bartfai T. Transthyretin protects Alzheimer’s mice from the behavioral and biochemical effects of Abeta toxicity. PNAS. 2008;105:2681–2686. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0712197105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Chen BK, Murawski NJ, Cincotta C, McKissick O, Finkelstein A, Hamidi AB, Merfeld E, Doucette E, Grella SL, Shpokayte M, Zaki Y, Fortin A, Ramirez S. Artificially enhancing and suppressing hippocampus-mediated memories. Current Biology. 2019;29:1885–1894. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.04.065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cincotta C, Murawski NJ, Grella SL, McKissick O, Doucette E, Ramirez S. Chronic activation of fear engrams induces extinction-like behavior in ethanol-exposed mice. Hippocampus. 2021;31:3–10. doi: 10.1002/hipo.23263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Cobb JA, O’Neill K, Milner J, Mahajan GJ, Lawrence TJ, May WL, Miguel-Hidalgo J, Rajkowska G, Stockmeier CA. Density of GFAP-immunoreactive astrocytes is decreased in left hippocampi in major depressive disorder. Neuroscience. 2016;316:209–220. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.12.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Codeluppi SA, Chatterjee D, Prevot TD, Bansal Y, Misquitta KA, Sibille E, Banasr M. Chronic stress alters astrocyte morphology in mouse prefrontal cortex. The International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2021;24:842–853. doi: 10.1093/ijnp/pyab052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Crestani F, Lorez M, Baer K, Essrich C, Benke D, Laurent JP, Belzung C, Fritschy JM, Lüscher B, Mohler H. Decreased GABAA-receptor clustering results in enhanced anxiety and a bias for threat cues. Nature Neuroscience. 1999;2:833–839. doi: 10.1038/12207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. DeNardo LA, Liu CD, Allen WE, Adams EL, Friedmann D, Fu L, Guenthner CJ, Tessier-Lavigne M, Luo L. Temporal evolution of cortical ensembles promoting remote memory retrieval. Nature Neuroscience. 2019;22:460–469. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0318-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Denny CA, Kheirbek MA, Alba EL, Tanaka KF, Brachman RA, Laughman KB, Tomm NK, Turi GF, Losonczy A, Hen R. Hippocampal memory traces are differentially modulated by experience, time, and adult neurogenesis. Neuron. 2014;83:189–201. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Díaz-Morán S, Palència M, Mont-Cardona C, Cañete T, Blázquez G, Martínez-Membrives E, López-Aumatell R, Sabariego M, Donaire R, Morón I, Torres C, Martínez-Conejero JA, Tobeña A, Esteban FJ, Fernández-Teruel A. Gene expression in hippocampus as a function of differential trait anxiety levels in genetically heterogeneous NIH-HS rats. Behavioural Brain Research. 2013;257:129–139. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.09.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Dobin A, Davis CA, Schlesinger F, Drenkow J, Zaleski C, Jha S, Batut P, Chaisson M, Gingeras TR. STAR: ultrafast universal RNA-seq aligner. Bioinformatics. 2013;29:15–21. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/bts635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Du Preez A, Onorato D, Eiben I, Musaelyan K, Egeland M, Zunszain PA, Fernandes C, Thuret S, Pariante CM. Chronic stress followed by social isolation promotes depressive-like behaviour, alters microglial and astrocyte biology and reduces hippocampal neurogenesis in male mice. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 2021;91:24–47. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2020.07.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Fatemi SH, Laurence JA, Araghi-Niknam M, Stary JM, Schulz SC, Lee S, Gottesman II. Glial fibrillary acidic protein is reduced in cerebellum of subjects with major depression, but not schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research. 2004;69:317–323. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2003.08.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Ferrari LL, Ogbeide-Latario OE, Gompf HS, Anaclet C. Validation of DREADD agonists and administration route in a murine model of sleep enhancement. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2022;380:109679. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2022.109679. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Fuentes-Ramos M, Alaiz-Noya M, Barco A. Transcriptome and epigenome analysis of engram cells: next-generation sequencing technologies in memory research. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2021;127:865–875. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.06.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Goossens MG, Larsen LE, Vergaelen M, Wadman W, Haute C, Brackx W, Proesmans S, Desloovere J, Christiaen E, Craey E, Vanhove C, Vonck K, Boon P, Raedt R. Level of hm4d(gi) dreadd expression determines inhibitory and neurotoxic effects in the hippocampus. eNeuro. 2021;8:ENEURO.0105-21.2021. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0105-21.2021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gosselin RD, Gibney S, O’Malley D, Dinan TG, Cryan JF. Region specific decrease in glial fibrillary acidic protein immunoreactivity in the brain of a rat model of depression. Neuroscience. 2009;159:915–925. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.10.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Guastella AJ, Moulds ML. The impact of rumination on sleep quality following a stressful life event. Personality and Individual Differences. 2007;42:1151–1162. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.04.028. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. Hinwood M, Tynan RJ, Charnley JL, Beynon SB, Day TA, Walker FR. Chronic stress induced remodeling of the prefrontal cortex: structural re-organization of microglia and the inhibitory effect of minocycline. Cerebral Cortex. 2013;23:1784–1797. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Jang S, Suh SH, Yoo HS, Lee YM, Oh S. Changes in iNOS, GFAP and NR1 expression in various brain regions and elevation of sphingosine-1-phosphate in serum after immobilized stress. Neurochemical Research. 2008;33:842–851. doi: 10.1007/s11064-007-9523-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Johnson LA, Zuloaga DG, Bidiman E, Marzulla T, Weber S, Wahbeh H, Raber J. Apoe2 exaggerates ptsd-related behavioral, cognitive, and neuroendocrine alterations. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:2443–2453. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.95. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kassem MS, Lagopoulos J, Stait-Gardner T, Price WS, Chohan TW, Arnold JC, Hatton SN, Bennett MR. Stress-induced grey matter loss determined by MRI is primarily due to loss of dendrites and their synapses. Molecular Neurobiology. 2013;47:645–661. doi: 10.1007/s12035-012-8365-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Khalaf O, Resch S, Dixsaut L, Gorden V, Glauser L, Gräff J. Reactivation of recall-induced neurons contributes to remote fear memory attenuation. Science. 2018;360:1239–1242. doi: 10.1126/science.aas9875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kim WB, Cho JH. Encoding of contextual fear memory in hippocampal-amygdala circuit. Nature Communications. 2020;11:1382. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-15121-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Lee MH, Siddoway B, Kaeser GE, Segota I, Rivera R, Romanow WJ, Liu CS, Park C, Kennedy G, Long T, Chun J. Somatic APP gene recombination in Alzheimer’s disease and normal neurons. Nature. 2018;563:639–645. doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0718-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Lee SH, Kang J, Ho A, Watanabe H, Bolshakov VY, Shen J. APP family regulates neuronal excitability and synaptic plasticity but not neuronal survival. Neuron. 2020;108:676–690. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.08.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Leyh J, Paeschke S, Mages B, Michalski D, Nowicki M, Bechmann I, Winter K. Classification of microglial morphological phenotypes using machine learning. Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience. 2021;15:701673. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2021.701673. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Li T, Yuan L, Zhao Y, Jiang Z, Gai C, Xin D, Ke H, Guo X, Chen W, Liu D, Wang Z, Ho CSH. Blocking osteopontin expression attenuates neuroinflammation and mitigates LPS-induced depressive-like behavior in mice. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2023;330:83–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2023.02.105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Liao Y, Smyth GK, Shi W. featureCounts: an efficient general purpose program for assigning sequence reads to genomic features. Bioinformatics. 2014;30:923–930. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btt656. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Liu X, Ramirez S, Pang PT, Puryear CB, Govindarajan A, Deisseroth K, Tonegawa S. Optogenetic stimulation of a hippocampal engram activates fear memory recall. Nature. 2012;484:381–385. doi: 10.1038/nature11028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Liu S, Yuan B. 2022. Hippocampal cells multiplex positive and negative engrams. NCBI Gene Expression Omnibus, GSE198731. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/geo/query/acc.cgi?acc=GSE198731
  41. Love MI, Huber W, Anders S. Moderated estimation of fold change and dispersion for RNA-seq data with DESeq2. Genome Biology. 2014;15:550. doi: 10.1186/s13059-014-0550-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Marchant NL, Lovland LR, Jones R, Pichet Binette A, Gonneaud J, Arenaza‐Urquijo EM, Chételat G, Villeneuve S. Repetitive negative thinking is associated with amyloid, tau, and cognitive decline. Alzheimer’s & Dementia. 2020;16:1054–1064. doi: 10.1002/alz.12116. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Maren S, Holmes A. Stress and fear extinction. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2016;41:58–79. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Martinho A, Gonçalves I, Costa M, Santos CR. Stress and glucocorticoids increase transthyretin expression in rat choroid plexus via mineralocorticoid and glucocorticoid receptors. Journal of Molecular Neuroscience. 2012;48:1–13. doi: 10.1007/s12031-012-9715-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. McEvoy PM, Watson H, Watkins ER, Nathan P. The relationship between worry, rumination, and comorbidity: evidence for repetitive negative thinking as a transdiagnostic construct. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2013;151:313–320. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.06.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. McEwen BS, Stellar E. Stress and the individual: mechanisms leading to disease. Archives of Internal Medicine. 1993;153:2093–2101. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. McEwen BS. Protection and damage from acute and chronic stress: allostasis and allostatic overload and relevance to the pathophysiology of psychiatric disorders. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2004;1032:1–7. doi: 10.1196/annals.1314.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. McLaughlin KA, Conron KJ, Koenen KC, Gilman SE. Childhood adversity, adult stressful life events, and risk of past-year psychiatric disorder: a test of the stress sensitization hypothesis in a population-based sample of adults. Psychological Medicine. 2010;40:1647–1658. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709992121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Michel M, Fiebich BL, Kuzior H, Meixensberger S, Berger B, Maier S, Nickel K, Runge K, Denzel D, Pankratz B, Schiele MA, Domschke K, van Elst LT, Endres D. Increased GFAP concentrations in the cerebrospinal fluid of patients with unipolar depression. Translational Psychiatry. 2021;11:308. doi: 10.1038/s41398-021-01423-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Miguel-Hidalgo JJ, Baucom C, Dilley G, Overholser JC, Meltzer HY, Stockmeier CA, Rajkowska G. Glial fibrillary acidic protein immunoreactivity in the prefrontal cortex distinguishes younger from older adults in major depressive disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2000;48:861–873. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00999-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Miranda M, Morici JF, Zanoni MB, Bekinschtein P. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor: a key molecule for memory in the healthy and the pathological brain. Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience. 2019;13:363. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2019.00363. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Monasterio A, Lienkaemper C, Coello S, Ocker GK, Ramirez S, Scott BB. CA1 engram cell dynamics before and after learning. Neuroscience. 2024;01:e9790. doi: 10.1101/2024.04.16.589790. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  53. Mootha VK, Lindgren CM, Eriksson KF, Subramanian A, Sihag S, Lehar J, Puigserver P, Carlsson E, Ridderstråle M, Laurila E, Houstis N, Daly MJ, Patterson N, Mesirov JP, Golub TR, Tamayo P, Spiegelman B, Lander ES, Hirschhorn JN, Altshuler D, Groop LC. PGC-1alpha-responsive genes involved in oxidative phosphorylation are coordinately downregulated in human diabetes. Nature Genetics. 2003;34:267–273. doi: 10.1038/ng1180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Müller I, Çalışkan G, Stork O. The GAD65 knock out mouse - a model for GABAergic processes in fear- and stress-induced psychopathology. Genes, Brain, and Behavior. 2015;14:37–45. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12188. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Nagai Y, Miyakawa N, Takuwa H, Hori Y, Oyama K, Ji B, Takahashi M, Huang XP, Slocum ST, DiBerto JF, Xiong Y, Urushihata T, Hirabayashi T, Fujimoto A, Mimura K, English JG, Liu J, Inoue KI, Kumata K, Seki C, Ono M, Shimojo M, Zhang MR, Tomita Y, Nakahara J, Suhara T, Takada M, Higuchi M, Jin J, Roth BL, Minamimoto T. Deschloroclozapine, a potent and selective chemogenetic actuator enables rapid neuronal and behavioral modulations in mice and monkeys. Nature Neuroscience. 2020;23:1157–1167. doi: 10.1038/s41593-020-0661-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Newman MG, Llera SJ, Erickson TM, Przeworski A, Castonguay LG. Worry and generalized anxiety disorder: a review and theoretical synthesis of evidence on nature, etiology, mechanisms, and treatment. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2013;9:275–297. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185544. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Nimmerjahn A, Kirchhoff F, Helmchen F. Resting microglial cells are highly dynamic surveillants of brain parenchyma in vivo. Science. 2005;308:1314–1318. doi: 10.1126/science.1110647. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Watkins ER. A heuristic for developing transdiagnostic models of psychopathology: explaining multifinality and divergent trajectories. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2011;6:589–609. doi: 10.1177/1745691611419672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Ortega-de San Luis C, Pezzoli M, Urrieta E, Ryan TJ. Engram cell connectivity as a mechanism for information encoding and memory function. Current Biology. 2023;33:5368–5380. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2023.10.074. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Osuch EA, Benson B, Geraci M, Podell D, Herscovitch P, McCann UD, Post RM. Regional cerebral blood flow correlated with flashback intensity in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2001;50:246–253. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01107-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Pettit NL, Yap EL, Greenberg ME, Harvey CD. Fos ensembles encode and shape stable spatial maps in the hippocampus. Nature. 2022;609:327–334. doi: 10.1038/s41586-022-05113-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Raber J, Huang Y, Ashford JW. ApoE genotype accounts for the vast majority of AD risk and AD pathology. Neurobiology of Aging. 2004;25:641–650. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2003.12.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Ramirez S, Liu X, MacDonald CJ, Moffa A, Zhou J, Redondo RL, Tonegawa S. Activating positive memory engrams suppresses depression-like behaviour. Nature. 2015;522:335–339. doi: 10.1038/nature14514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Rao-Ruiz P, Couey JJ, Marcelo IM, Bouwkamp CG, Slump DE, Matos MR, van der Loo RJ, Martins GJ, van den Hout M, van IJcken WF, Costa RM, van den Oever MC, Kushner SA. Engram-specific transcriptome profiling of contextual memory consolidation. Nature Communications. 2019;10:2232. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-09960-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Rayner G, Jackson G, Wilson S. Cognition-related brain networks underpin the symptoms of unipolar depression: evidence from a systematic review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2016;61:53–65. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.09.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Reddaway J, Richardson PE, Bevan RJ, Stoneman J, Palombo M. Microglial morphometric analysis: so many options, so little consistency. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics. 2023;17:1211188. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2023.1211188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Sardoo AM, Zhang S, Ferraro TN, Keck TM, Chen Y. Decoding brain memory formation by single-cell RNA sequencing. Briefings in Bioinformatics. 2022;23:bbac412. doi: 10.1093/bib/bbac412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Schlosser M, Demnitz-King H, Whitfield T, Wirth M, Marchant NL. Repetitive negative thinking is associated with subjective cognitive decline in older adults: a cross-sectional study. BMC Psychiatry. 2020;20:500. doi: 10.1186/s12888-020-02884-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Schuff N, Woerner N, Boreta L, Kornfield T, Shaw LM, Trojanowski JQ, Thompson PM, Jack CR, Weiner MW. MRI of hippocampal volume loss in early Alzheimer’s disease in relation to ApoE genotype and biomarkers. Brain. 2009;132:1067–1077. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Sharma M, Khan S, Rahman S, Singh LR. The extracellular protein, transthyretin is an oxidative stress biomarker. Frontiers in Physiology. 2019;10:5. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2019.00005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Shpokayte M, McKissick O, Guan X, Yuan B, Rahsepar B, Fernandez FR, Ruesch E, Grella SL, White JA, Liu XS, Ramirez S. Hippocampal cells segregate positive and negative engrams. Communications Biology. 2022;5:1009. doi: 10.1038/s42003-022-03906-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Si X, Miguel-Hidalgo JJ, O’Dwyer G, Stockmeier CA, Rajkowska G. Age-dependent reductions in the level of glial fibrillary acidic protein in the prefrontal cortex in major depression. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29:2088–2096. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300525. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Smith KL, Kassem MS, Clarke DJ, Kuligowski MP, Bedoya-Pérez MA, Todd SM, Lagopoulos J, Bennett MR, Arnold JC. Microglial cell hyper-ramification and neuronal dendritic spine loss in the hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex in a mouse model of PTSD. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 2019;80:889–899. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2019.05.042. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Stankiewicz AM, Goscik J, Majewska A, Swiergiel AH, Juszczak GR. The effect of acute and chronic social stress on the hippocampal transcriptome in mice. PLOS ONE. 2015;10:e0142195. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0142195. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Subramanian A, Tamayo P, Mootha VK, Mukherjee S, Ebert BL, Gillette MA, Paulovich A, Pomeroy SL, Golub TR, Lander ES, Mesirov JP. Gene set enrichment analysis: a knowledge-based approach for interpreting genome-wide expression profiles. PNAS. 2005;102:15545–15550. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0506580102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Suthard RL, Jellinger AL, Surets M, Shpokayte M, Pyo AY, Buzharsky MD, Senne RA, Dorst K, Leblanc H, Ramirez S. Chronic Gq activation of ventral hippocampal neurons and astrocytes differentially affects memory and behavior. Neurobiology of Aging. 2023;125:9–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2023.01.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Tanaka KZ, He H, Tomar A, Niisato K, Huang AJY, McHugh TJ. The hippocampal engram maps experience but not place. Science. 2018;361:392–397. doi: 10.1126/science.aat5397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Tran I, Gellner AK. Long-term effects of chronic stress models in adult mice. Journal of Neural Transmission. 2023;130:1133–1151. doi: 10.1007/s00702-023-02598-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Tynan RJ, Beynon SB, Hinwood M, Johnson SJ, Nilsson M, Woods JJ, Walker FR. Chronic stress-induced disruption of the astrocyte network is driven by structural atrophy and not loss of astrocytes. Acta Neuropathologica. 2013;126:75–91. doi: 10.1007/s00401-013-1102-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Vidal-Itriago A, Radford RAW, Aramideh JA, Maurel C, Scherer NM, Don EK, Lee A, Chung RS, Graeber MB, Morsch M. Microglia morphophysiological diversity and its implications for the CNS. Frontiers in Immunology. 2022;13:997786. doi: 10.3389/fimmu.2022.997786. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Wang W, Qin X, Wang R, Xu J, Wu H, Khalid A, Jiang H, Liu D, Pan F. EZH2 is involved in vulnerability to neuroinflammation and depression-like behaviors induced by chronic stress in different aged mice. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2020;272:452–464. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.03.154. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Watkins ER. Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin. 2008;134:163–206. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Webster MJ, O’Grady J, Kleinman JE, Weickert CS. Glial fibrillary acidic protein mRNA levels in the cingulate cortex of individuals with depression, bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Neuroscience. 2005;133:453–461. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.02.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Xia F, Richards BA, Tran MM, Josselyn SA, Takehara-Nishiuchi K, Frankland PW. Parvalbumin-positive interneurons mediate neocortical-hippocampal interactions that are necessary for memory consolidation. eLife. 2017;6:e27868. doi: 10.7554/eLife.27868. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Yang Z, Wang KKW. Glial fibrillary acidic protein: from intermediate filament assembly and gliosis to neurobiomarker. Trends in Neurosciences. 2015;38:364–374. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2015.04.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. York EM, LeDue JM, Bernier LP, MacVicar BA. 3Dmorph automatic analysis of microglial morphology in three dimensions from ex vivo and in vivo imaging. eNeuro. 2018;5:ENEURO. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0266-18.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Yousef A, Robinson JL, Irwin DJ, Byrne MD, Kwong LK, Lee EB, Xu Y, Xie SX, Rennert L, Suh E, Van Deerlin VM, Grossman M, Lee VMY, Trojanowski JQ. Neuron loss and degeneration in the progression of TDP-43 in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. Acta Neuropathologica Communications. 2017;5:68. doi: 10.1186/s40478-017-0471-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Zhan J, Komal R, Keenan WT, Hattar S, Fernandez DC. Non-invasive strategies for chronic manipulation of dreadd-controlled neuronal activity. Journal of Visualized Experiments. 2019;01:e9439. doi: 10.3791/59439. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Zhang TR, Larosa A, Di Raddo ME, Wong V, Wong AS, Wong TP. Negative memory engrams in the hippocampus enhance the susceptibility to chronic social defeat stress. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2019;39:7576–7590. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1958-18.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

eLife assessment

Adrien Peyrache 1

This useful study reports the behavioral and physiological effects of the longitudinal activation of neurons associated with negative experiences. The main claims of the article are supported by solid experimental evidence, although the specificity of the long-term manipulation could have benefitted from additional validation. This study will be of interest to neuroscientists working on memory.

Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

Anonymous

Summary:

In this study, Jellinger et al. performed engram-specific sequencing and identified genes that were selectively regulated in positive/negative engram populations. In addition, they performed chronic activation of the negative engram population over 3 months and observed several effects on fear/anxiety behavior and cellular events such as upregulation of glial cells and decreased GABA levels.

Strengths:

They provide useful engram-specific GSEA data and the main concept of the study, linking negative valence/memory encoding to cellular level outcomes including upregulation of glial cells, is interesting and valuable.

Comments on the revised manuscript:

The revised manuscript still does not adequately address the primary technical concern regarding long-term DREADD manipulation. The authors reference their previous work (Suthard et al., 2023) as evidence; however, this earlier paper only presents fluorescence intensity in a non-quantitative manner with merely three samples (Supplementary Figure 7). This limited evidence does not sufficiently support the claim of potent long-term activation. The discussion in the revision stating "...even if our manipulation is only working for 1 month, rather than 3 months..." is unconvincing, particularly given that the title and abstract still claims "chronic activation of...". To substantiate the technical validity of the study, at least cFos staining at various time points is necessary, which is less burdensome compared to more direct demonstrations such as slice physiology. Thus, although I believe it could be an interesting study for some audiences, I cannot support the strength of the evidence presented in the study.

Furthermore, in response to all reviewers' concerns regarding the quantification of GABA, the authors have removed the data from the study rather than providing properly acquired images or quantified data. This action diminishes the significance of the study.

Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

Anonymous

Summary:

Jellinger, Suthard, et al. investigated the transcriptome of positive and negative valence engram cells in the ventral hippocampus, revealing anti- and pro-inflammatory signatures of these respective valences. The authors further reactivated the negative valence engram ensembles to assay the effects of chronic negative memory reactivation in young and old mice. This chronic re-activation resulted in differences in aspects of working memory, fear memory, and caused morphological changes in glia. Such reactivation-associated changes are putatively linked to GABA changes and behavioral rumination.

Strengths:

Much the content of of this manuscript is of benefit to the community, such as the discovery of differential engram transcriptomes dependent on memory valence. The chronic activation of neurons, and the resultant effects on glial cells and behavior, also provide the community with important data. Laudable points of this manuscript include the comprehensiveness of behavioral experiments, as well as the cross-disciplinary approach.

Weaknesses:

Weaknesses noted in the previous version of the manuscript have been accounted for.

Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

Anonymous

Summary:

The authors note that negative ruminations can lead to pathological brain states and mood/anxiety dysregulation. They test this idea by using mouse engram-tagging technology to label dentate gyrus ensembles activated during a negative experience (fear conditioning). They show that chronic chemogenetic activation of these ensembles leads to behavioral (increased anxiety, increased fear generalization, reduced fear extinction) and neural (increases in neuroinflammation, microglia and astrocytes).

Strengths:

The question the authors ask here is an intriguing one, and the engram activation approach is a powerful way to address the question. Examination of a wide range of neural and behavioral dependent measures is also a strength.

Weaknesses:

The major weakness is that the authors have found a range of changes that are correlates of chronic negative engram reactivation. However, they do not manipulate these outcomes to test whether microglia, astrocytes, neuroinflammation are causally linked to the dysregulated behaviors.

eLife. 2024 Jul 11;13:RP96281. doi: 10.7554/eLife.96281.3.sa4

Author response

Alexandra L Jellinger 1, Rebecca L Suthard 2, Bingbing Yuan 3, Michelle Surets 4, Evan A Ruesch 5, Albit J Caban 6, X Shawn Liu 7, Monika Shpokayte 8, Steve Ramirez 9

The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

Public Reviews:

Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

Summary:

In this study, Jellinger et al. performed engram-specific sequencing and identified genes that were selectively regulated in positive/negative engram populations. In addition, they performed chronic activation of the negative engram population over 3 months and observed several effects on fear/anxiety behavior and cellular events such as upregulation of glial cells and decreased GABA levels.

Strengths:

They provide useful engram-specific GSEA data and the main concept of the study, linking negative valence/memory encoding to cellular level outcomes including upregulation of glial cells, is interesting and valuable.

Weaknesses:

A number of experimental shortcomings make the conclusion of the study largely unsupported. In addition, the observed differences in behavioral experiments are rather small, inconsistent, and the interpretation of the differences is not compelling.

Major points for improvement:

(1) Lack of essential control experiments

With the current set of experiments, it is not certain that the DREADD system they used was potent and stable throughout the 3 months of manipulations. Basic confirmatory experiments (e.g., slice physiology at 1m vs. 3m) to show that the DREADD effects on these vHP are stable would be an essential bottom line to make these manipulation experiments convincing.

In previous work from our lab performing long-term activation of Gq DREADD receptors in the vHPC, we quantify the presence of Gq receptor expression over 3-, 6- and 9-month timepoints and show that there is no decrease in receptor expression, as measured via fluorescence intensity (Suthard et al., 2023). In this study, we also address that even if our manipulation is only working for 1 month, rather than 3 months, we are observing the long-term effects of this shorter-term stimulation. This is still relevant, and only changes how we interpret these findings, as shorter-term stimulation or disruption of neuronal activity can still have detrimental effects on behavior.

Furthermore, although the authors use the mCherry vector as a control, they did not have a vehicle/saline control for the hM3Dq AAV. Thus, the long-term effects such as the increase in glial cells could simply be due to the toxicity of DREADD expression, rather than an induced activity of these cells.

For chemogenetic studies, our experimental rationale utilized a standard approach in the field, which includes one of two control options: (1) active receptor vs. control vector + ligand or (2) active receptor + ligand or saline control. We chose the first option, as this more properly controls for the potential off-target effects of the ligand itself, as shown in other previous work (Xia et al., 2017). This is particularly important for studies using CNO, as many off-target effects have been noted as a limitation (Manvich et al., 2018). We chose to use DCZ as it is closely related to CNO and newer ligands, but comes with added benefits of high specificity, low off-target effects, high potency and brain penetrance (Nagai et al., 2020), but any potential off-target effects of DCZ are yet to be completely investigated as this ligand is very new.

Evidence of DREADD toxicity has been shown at high titer levels of AAV2/7- CamKIIα-hM4D(Gi)-mCherry in the hippocampus at 5 weeks, as the reviewer pointed out in their above comment (Goossens et al., 2021). Our viral strategy is targeted to a much smaller number of cells using AAV9-DIO-Flex-hM3Dq-mCherry at a lower titer, unlike expression within a much larger population of CaMKII+ excitatory neurons in this study. Additionally, visual comparison of their viral load and expression with ours shows much more intense expression that spans a larger area of the hippocampus (Goossens et al, 2021; Figure 1D), whereas ours is isolated to a smaller region of vHPC (see Figure 1B).

Further, we attempted to quantify a decrease in neuronal health (Yousef et al., 2017) resulting from DREADD expression via NeuN counts within multiple hippocampal subregions for the 6- and 14-month groups across active Gq receptor and mCherry conditions and did not observe significant decreases in NeuN as a result (Supplemental Figure 1). However, immunohistochemistry of an individual marker may not be sufficient to capture the entire health profile of an individual neuron and future work should consider other markers of cell death or inflammation, which we have added to the Limitations & Future Work section of our Discussion.

(2) Figure 1 and the rest of the study are disconnected

The authors used the cFos-tTA system to label positive/negative engram populations, while the TRAP2 system was used for the chronic activation experiments. Although both genetic tools are based on the same IEG Fos, the sensitivity of the tools needs to be validated. In particular, the sensitivity of the TRAP2 system can be arbitrarily altered by the amount of tamoxifen (or 4OHT) and the administration protocols. The authors should at least compare and show the percentage of labeled cells in both methods and discuss that the two experiments target (at least slightly) different populations. In addition, the use of TRAP2 for vHP is relatively new; the authors should confirm that this method actually captures negative engram populations by checking for reactivation of these cells during recall by overlap analysis of Fos staining or by artificial activation.

We thank the reviewer for their comments and opportunity to discuss the marked differences between TRAP2 and DOX systems. In particular, we agree that while both systems rely on the the Fos promoter to drive an effector of interest, their efficacy and temporal resolution vary substantially depending on genetic cell-type, brain region, temporal parameters of Dox or 4-OHT delivery, subject-by-subject metabolic variability, and threshold to Fos induction given the promoter sequences inherent to each system. For example, recent studies have reported the following:

- The TRAP2 line labels a subset of endogenously activeCA1 pyramidal cells (e.g. 5-18%) while the DOX system labels 20-40% of CA1 pyramidal cells (DeNardo et al, 2019; Monasterio et al, BioRxiv 2024 ).

- The temporal windows for each range from hours in TRAP2 to 24-48 hours for DOX (DeNardo et al, 2019; Denny et al, 2014; Liu & Ramirez et al, 2012).

- The efficacy of “tagging” a population of cells with TRAP2 vs. with DOX will constrain the number of possible cells that may overlap with cFos upon re-exposure to a given experience e.g. see the observed overlaps in vCA1 - BLA circuits (Kim & Cho, 2020), compared to vCA1 in general (Ortega-de San Luis et al, 2023) and valence-specific vCA1 populations (Shpokayte et al, 2022).

- Tagging vCA1 cells with both the TRAP2 and DOX systems are nonetheless sufficient to drive corresponding behaviors (e.g. vCA1 terminal stimulation drives behavioral changes with the DOX and TRAP2 system (Shpokayte et al, 2022) and vCA1 stimulation of an updated fear-linked ensemble drives light-induced freezing in a neutral context utilizing the TRAP2 and DOX systems (Ortega-de San Luis et al, 2023)).

Finally, and promisingly, as more studies continue to link the in vivo physiological dynamics of these cell populations tagged using each system (e.g. compare Pettit et al, 2022 with Tanaka et al, 2018) and correlating their activity to behavioral phenotypes, our field is in the prime position to uncover deeper principles governing hippocampus-mediated engrams in the brain. Together, we believe a more comprehensive understanding of these systems is fully warranted, especially in the service of further cataloging cellular similarities and differences within such tagged populations.

(3) Interpretation of the behavior data

In Figures 3a and b, the authors show that the experimental group showed higher anxiety based on time spent in the center/open area. However, there were no differences in distance traveled and center entries, which are often reduced in highly anxious mice. Thus, it is not clear what the exact effect of the manipulation is. The authors may want to visualize the trajectories of the mice's locomotion instead of just showing bar graphs.

Our findings show that our experimental group displays higher levels of anxiety-like behaviors as measured via time spent in center/open area, while there are no differences in distance traveled or center entries. For distance traveled, our interpretation is in line with complementary research (Jimenez et al, 2018; Kheirbek et al, 2013) that shows no changes in distance traveled/distance traveled in the center coupled with changes in anxiety levels as a result of manipulation within anxiety-related circuits. More broadly, any locomotion-related deficit could cause a change in distance traveled that is unrelated to anxiety-like behaviors alone. For example, a reduction in distance traveled could be coupled with a decrease in time spent in the center, but could also result only from motor or exploratory deficits. We hope that this explanation clarifies our interpretation of the open field and elevated plus maze findings in light of other literature.

In addition, the data shown in Figure 4b is somewhat surprising - the 14MO control showed more freezing than the 6MO control, which can be interpreted as "better memory in old". As this is highly counterintuitive, the authors may want to discuss this point. The authors stated that "Mice typically display increased freezing behavior as they age, so these effects during remote recall are expected" without any reference. This is nonsense, as just above in Figure 4a, older mice actually show less freezing than young mice. Overall, the behavioral effects are rather small and random. I would suggest that these data be interpreted more carefully.

In Figure 4B, we present our findings from remote recall and observe increased freezing levels in control mice with age, as mentioned by the reviewer, indicating increased memory. This is in line with previous work from Shoji & Miyakawa, 2019 which has been added as a reference for the quotation described above; we thank the reviewer for pointing this error out. As the reviewer has pointed out, above in Figure 4A, we measured freezing levels across all groups during contextual fear conditioning before the start of chronic stimulation, as this was the session we ‘tagged’ a negative memory in. Although it appears that there may be slightly lower levels of freezing in older (14-month old) mice, our findings do not determine statistical significance for difference between age group, only effects of time and subject which are expected as freezing increases within the session and animals display high levels of variability in freezing levels across many experiments (Figure 4A i-iii). We also find in previous work that control mice receiving 3-, 6- and 9-months of chronic DCZ stimulation in the vHPC with empty vector (mCherry) receptor show an increase in freezing with age (Suthard et al, 2023; Figure 2A ii).

(4) Lack of citation and discussion of relevant study

Khalaf et al. 2018 from Gräff lab showed that experimental activation of recall-induced populations leads to fear attenuation. Despite the differences in experimental details, the conceptual discrepancy should be discussed.

As mentioned by the reviewer, Khalaf et al. 2018 showed that experimental activation of recall-induced populations in the dentate gyrus leads to fear attenuation. Specifically, they pose that this fear attenuation occurs in these ensembles through updating or unlearning of the original memory trace via the engagement, rather than suppression, of an original traumatic experience. Despite the differences in experimental details with our current study and this work, we agree that the conceptual discrepancy should be discussed. First, one major difference is that we are reactivating an ensemble that was tagged during fear memory encoding, while Khalaf et al. are activating a remote recall-induced ensemble that was tagged one month after encoding. Although there is high overlap between the encoding and recall ensembles when mice are exposed to the conditioning context, these ensembles are not identical and may result in different behavioral phenotypes when chronically reactivated. Further, Khalaf et al rely on reactivation of the recall-induced ensemble during extinction to facilitate rapid fear attenuation. This differs from our current work, as their reactivation is occurring during the extinction process in the previously conditioned context, while we are reactivating chronically in the animal’s home cage over the course of a longer time period. It may be necessary that the memory is first reactivated, and thus, more liable to re-contextualization, in the original context compared to an unrelated homecage environment where there are presumably no related cues present. Importantly, this previous work tests the attenuation of fear shortly after an extinction process, while we are not traditionally extinguishing the context with aid of the memory reactivation. Finally, we are testing remote recall (3 months post-conditioning), while they are testing at a shorter time interval (28 days). In line with these ideas, future work may seek to tease out the mechanistic differences between recent and remote memory extinction both in terms of natural memory recall and chronically manipulated memory-bearing cells.

Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

Summary:

Jellinger, Suthard, et al. investigated the transcriptome of positive and negative valence engram cells in the ventral hippocampus, revealing anti- and pro-inflammatory signatures of these respective valences. The authors further reactivated the negative valence engram ensembles to assay the effects of chronic negative memory reactivation in young and old mice. This chronic re-activation resulted in differences in aspects of working memory, and fear memory, and caused morphological changes in glia. Such reactivation-associated changes are putatively linked to GABA changes and behavioral rumination.

Strengths:

Much of the content of this manuscript is of benefit to the community, such as the discovery of differential engram transcriptomes dependent on memory valence. The chronic activation of neurons, and the resultant effects on glial cells and behavior, also provide the community with important data. Laudable points of this manuscript include the comprehensiveness of behavioral experiments, as well as the cross-disciplinary approach.

Weaknesses:

There are several key claims made that are unsubstantiated by the data, particularly regarding the anthropomorphic framing of "rumination" on a mouse model and the role of GABA. The conclusions and inferences in these areas need to be carefully considered.

(1) There are many issues regarding the arguments for the behavioural data's human translation as "rumination." There is no definition of rumination provided in the manuscript, nor how rumination is similar/different to intrusive thoughts (which are psychologically distinct but used relatively interchangeably in the manuscript), nor how rumination could be modelled in the rodent. The authors mention that they are attempting to model rumination behaviours by chronically reactivating the negative engram ("To understand if our experimental model of negative rumination..."), but this occurs almost at the very end of the results section, and no concrete evidence from the literature is provided to attempt to link the behavioural results (decreased working memory, increased fear extinction times) to rumination-like behaviours. The arguments in the final paragraph of the Discussion section about human rumination appear to be unrelated to the data presented in the manuscript and contain some uncited statements. Finally, the rumination claims seem to be based largely upon a single data figure that needs to be further developed (Figure 6, see also point 2 below).

(2) The staining and analysis in Figure 6 are challenging to interpret, and require more evidence to substantiate the conclusions of these results. The histological images are zoomed out, and at this resolution, it appears that only the pyramidal cell layer is being stained. A GABA stain should also label the many sparsely spaced inhibitory interneurons existing across all hippocampal layers, yet this is not apparent here. Moreover, both example images in the treatment group appear to have lower overall fluorescence intensity in both DAPI and GABA. The analysis is also unclear: the authors mention "ROIs" used to measure normalized fluorescence intensity but do not specify what the ROI encapsulates. Presumably, the authors have segmented each DAPI-positive cell body and assessed fluorescence however, this is not explicated nor demonstrated, making the results difficult to interpret.

Based on the collective discussion from all reviewers on the completeness of our GABA quantification and its implications, we have decided to remove this figure and perform more substantive analysis of this E/I imbalance in future work.

(3) A smaller point, but more specific detail is needed for how genes were selected for GSEA analysis. As GSEA relies on genes to be specified a priori, to avoid a circular analysis, these genes need to be selected in a blind/unbiased manner to avoid biasing downstream results and conclusions. It's likely the authors have done this, but explicitly noting how genes were selected is an important context for this analysis.

As mentioned in our Methods section, gene sets were selected based on pre-existing biology and understanding of genes canonically involved in “neurodegeneration” such as those related to apoptotic pathways and neuroinflammation or “neuroprotection” such as brain-derived neurotrophic factor, to name a few. A limitation of this method is that we must avoid making strong claims about the actual function of these up- or down-regulated genes without performing proper knock-in or knock-out studies, but we hope that this provides an unbiased inventory for future experiments to perform causal manipulations.

Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

Summary:

The authors note that negative ruminations can lead to pathological brain states and mood/anxiety dysregulation. They test this idea by using mouse engram-tagging technology to label dentate gyrus ensembles activated during a negative experience (fear conditioning). They show that chronic chemogenetic activation of these ensembles leads to behavioral (increased anxiety, increased fear generalization, reduced fear extinction) and neural (increases in neuroinflammation, microglia, and astrocytes).

Strengths:

The question the authors ask here is an intriguing one, and the engram activation approach is a powerful way to address the question. Examination of a wide range of neural and behavioral dependent measures is also a strength.

Weaknesses:

The major weakness is that the authors have found a range of changes that are correlates of chronic negative engram reactivation. However, they do not manipulate these outcomes to test whether microglia, astrocytes, or neuroinflammation are causally linked to the dysregulated behaviors.

Recommendations For The Authors:

Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

- Figure 2c should include Month0, the BW before the start of the manipulation.

Regrettably, we do not have access to the Month 0 body weights at this time as this project changed hands over the course of the past year or so. This is an inherent limitation that we missed during analysis and we pose this as a limitation in the Results section after describing this finding. Therefore, it is possible that over the first month of stimulation (Month 0-1), there may have been a drop in body weight that rebounded by the first measurement at Month 1 that continued to increase normally through Months 2-3, as shown in our Figure 1. Thank you for this note.

- Figure 6a looks confusing - the background signal in the green channel is very different between control and experimental groups. Were representative images taken with different microscope settings?

The representative images were taken with the same microscope power settings, but were adjusted in brightness/contrast within FIJI for clarity in the Figure – we apologize that this was misleading in any way and thank the reviewer for their feedback. Further, based on the collective discussion from all reviewers on the completeness of our GABA quantification and its implications, we have decided to remove this figure and perform more substantive analysis of this E/I imbalance in future work.

- Typo mChe;try

This typo was fixed

- "During this contextual... mice in the 6- and 14- month groups..." Isn't it 3- and 11- month respectively at the time of fear conditioning? Throughout the manuscript, this point was written very confusingly.

Yes, we thank the reviewer for pointing this out. It has been corrected to 3- and 11-month old mice at the timing of fear conditioning and clarified throughout the manuscript where applicable.

- "GABAergic eYFP fluorescence" Where does the eYFP come from? The methods state that GABA quantification is based on IHC staining.

Based on the collective discussion from all reviewers on the completeness of our GABA quantification and its implications, we have decided to remove this figure and perform more substantive analysis of this

E/I imbalance in future work. We discuss this E/I balance not being directly assessed in the Limitations & Future Directions section of our Discussion, noting the importance of detailed quantification of both excitatory and inhibitory markers within the hippocampus.

Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

(1) There is a full methods section ("Analysis of RNA-seq data") that mostly describes RNA-seq analysis that seemingly does not appear in the paper. This section should be reviewed.

We have included this portion of the methods that explain the previous workflow from Shpokayte et al., 2022 where this dataset was generated and this has been noted in the “Analysis of RNA-seq data” section of the methods.

(2) Figure 6: GABA staining should be more critically analyzed, as discussed above, and validated with another GABA antibody for rigor. From the representative images provided in Figure 6, it looks possibly as though the hM3Dq images were simply not fully in the focal plane when being imaged or were over-washed, as DAPI staining also appears to be lower in these images.

Based on the collective discussion from all reviewers on the completeness of our GABA quantification and its implications, we have decided to remove this figure and perform more substantive analysis of this E/I imbalance in future work. Specifically, it will be necessary to rigorously investigate both excitatory and inhibitory markers within this region to ensure these claims are substantiated. Thank you for this suggestion.

(3) The first claim that human GABAergic interneurons cause rumination is uncited. (Page 19, first sentence beginning with: "Evidence from human studies suggests...").

Based on the collective discussion from all reviewers on the completeness of our GABA quantification and its implications, we have decided to remove this figure and perform more substantive analysis of this E/I imbalance in future work. Apologies for the lack of citation in-text, the proper citation for this finding is Schmitz et al, 2017.

(4) Gene names throughout the manuscript and figure are written in the wrong format for mice (eg: Page 13, second line: SPP1, TTR, and C1QB1 instead of Spp1, Ttr, C1qb1).

This was corrected throughout the manuscript.

(5) Tense on Page 15 third sentence of the second paragraph: "...spatial working memory was assessed...".

This was corrected throughout the manuscript.

(6) Supplemental Figure 1 would benefit from normalization of the NeuN+ cell counts. The inclusion of an excitatory and inhibitory neuron marker in this figure might benefit the argument that there is a change in the excitation/inhibition of the hippocampus - as the numbers of excitatory neurons outweigh the numbers of inhibitory neurons that would be assayed here.

In an effort to normalize the NeuN+ cell counts, for each of our ROIs (6-8 single tiles for each brain region (DG, vCA1, vSub) x 3-5 coronal slices = ~18 single tiles per mouse x 3-4 mice) we captured a 300 x 300 micrometer, single-tile z-stack at 20x magnification. These ROIs were matched for dimensions and brain regions across all groups for each hippocampal subregion quantified. We initially proposed to normalize these NeuN counts over DAPI, but because DAPI includes all nuclei (microglia, oligodendrocytes, astrocytes and neurons), we weren’t sure this was the most optimal tool. We do agree that further quantification of excitatory and inhibitory cell markers would be vital to more concrete interpretation of our findings and we have added this to our Limitations & Future Work section of the Discussion.

Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors):

(1) The DOX tagging window lacks temporal precision. I suggest the authors note this as a limitation.

We thank the reviewer for noting this, and we have added this limitation to the Methods section with the context of the 24-48 hour DOX window being longer than other methods like TRAP.

(2) Is there a homeostatic response to chronic engram stimulation? That is, is DCZ as effective in increasing neuronal excitability on day 90 as it is on day 1. This could be addressed with electrophysiology, or with IEG induction. Alternatively, the authors could refer to previous literature-- for example, Xia et al (2017) eLife-- that examined whether there was any blunting of the effects of DREADD ligands after sustained delivery via drinking water. There, of course, may be other papers as well.

As noted by the reviewer, it is important to determine if DCZ maintains its effects on neuronal excitability throughout the 3 month administration period. To address this, previous work has shown that CNO administration in drinking water over one month consistently inhibited hM4Di+ neurons without altering baseline neuronal excitability as measured by firing rate and potassium currents (Xia et al, 2017). Although this is only for one month, it is administered via the same oral route as our DCZ protocol and suggests that at least for that amount of time we are likely producing consistent effects. In our reply above to Reviewer #1’s comment, we also note that even if DCZ is only having an effect for one month, rather than 3 months, we are still observing enduring changes that resulted from this short-term disturbance.

(3) Please double check there is no group effect on weight in 6-month-old mice in Figure 2C.

Two-way RM ANOVA showed no main effect of Group within the 6-month-old control and hM3Dq groups.

Group: F(1,17) = 1.361, p=0.2594.

(4) The shock intensity is much higher than is typical for fear conditioning studies in mice. Why was this the case?

Yes, we do agree that this shock intensity is on the higher side of typical paradigms in mice, however, our lab has utilized 0.75mA to 1.5mA intensity foot shocks for contextual fear conditioning in the past (Suthard & Senne et al, 2023; 2024; Dorst & Senne et al, 2023; Grella et al., 2022; Finkelstein et al., 2022) and we maintained this protocol for internal consistency. However, it would be interesting to systematically investigate how differing intensities of foot shock, subsequent tagging of this ensemble and reactivation would uniquely impact behavioral state acutely and chronically in mice.

(5) Remote freezing is very low. The authors should comment on this-- perhaps repeated testing has led to some extinction?

A reviewer above suggested a similar phenomenon may be occuring, specifically fear attenuation as a result of chronic stimulation. They referenced previous work from Khalaf et al. 2018, where they reactivated a recall-induced ensemble, while we reactivated an ensemble tagged during encoding. We expand upon this work in light of our findings within the Limitations & Future Work section of our Discussion. However, we do appreciate the lower levels of freezing observed in remote recall and sought out other literature to understand the typical range of remote freezing levels. One thing that we note is that our remote recall is occurring 3 months after conditioning, which is much longer than typical 14-28 day protocols. However, we find that freezing levels at remote timepoints from 21-45 days results in contextual freezing levels of between 20-50% approximately (Kol et al., 2020), as well as 40-75% approximately in a variety of 28 day remote recall experiments (Lee et al., 2023). This information, together with our current experimental protocol demonstrates a wide range of remote freezing levels that may depend heavily on the foot shock intensity, duration of days after conditioning, and animal variability.

(6) "mice display increased freezing with age": please add a reference.

Apologies, we missed the citation for that claim and it has been added in-text and in the references list (Shoji & Miyakawa, 2019).

(7) Related to the low freezing levels for remote memory, why is generalization minimal? Many studies have shown that there is a time-dependent emergence of generalized fear, yet here this is not seen. Is it linked to extinction (as above)? Or genetic background?

Previous work has shown that rats receiving multiple foot shocks during conditioning displayed a time-dependent generalization of context memory, while those receiving less shocks did not (Poulos et al., 2016), as the reviewer noted in their comment. In our current study, we observe low levels of generalization in all of our groups compared to freezing levels displayed in the conditioned context at the remote timepoint, in opposition to this time-dependent enhancement of generalization. It is possible that the genetic background of our C57BL/6J mice compared to the Long-Evans rat strain in this previous work accounts for some of this difference. In addition, it is possible that the longer duration of time (3 months) compared to their remote timepoint (28 days) resulted in time-dependent decrease in generalization that decreases with greater durations of time from original conditioning. As noted above, it is indeed plausible that the reactivation of a contextual fear ensemble over time is attenuating freezing levels for both the original and similar contexts (Khalaf et al, 2018). We discuss the differences in our study and this 2018 work more comprehensively above.

(8) Morphological phenotypes of astrocytes/microglia. Would be great to do some transcriptomic profiling of microglia/astrocytes to couple with the morphological characterization (but appreciate this is beyond the scope of current work).

We thank the reviewer this suggestion, we agree that would be an incredibly informative future experiment and have added this to our Limitations & Future Experiments section of the Discussion.

(9) The authors could consider including a limitations section in their discussion which discusses potential future directions for this work:

- causal experiments.

- E/I balance is not assessed directly (interestingly, in this regard, expanded engrams are linked to increased generalization [e.g., Ramsaran et al 2023]).

Thank you for this suggestion, we have added a Limitations & Future Directions section to our Discussion and have expanded upon these suggested points.

(10) For Figure 10, consider adding an experimental design/timeline.

We are making the assumption that the reviewer meant Figure 1 instead of Figure 10 here, but note that there is a description of the viral expression duration (D0-D10), followed by an off Dox period of 48 hours (D10-D12), with subsequent engram tagging of a negative (foot shock) or positive (male-to-female exposure) on D12. In our experiments (Shpokayte et al., 2022), Dox was administered for 24 hours (D12-D13), which was followed by sacrificing the animal for cell suspension and sequencing of the positive and negative engram populations. This figure also shows the viral strategy for the Tet-tag system (Figure 1A), as well as representative viral expression in vHPC (Figure 1B). We are happy to add additional experimental design/timeline information to this figure that would be helpful to the reviewer.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Liu S, Yuan B. 2022. Hippocampal cells multiplex positive and negative engrams. NCBI Gene Expression Omnibus. GSE198731
    2. Liu S, Yuan B. 2022. Hippocampal cells multiplex positive and negative engrams. NCBI Gene Expression Omnibus, GSE198731. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/geo/query/acc.cgi?acc=GSE198731

    Supplementary Materials

    MDAR checklist

    Data Availability Statement

    Raw data along with gene expression levels are deposited to NCBI Gene Expression Omnibus under accession code GSE198731.

    The following dataset was generated:

    Liu S, Yuan B. 2022. Hippocampal cells multiplex positive and negative engrams. NCBI Gene Expression Omnibus. GSE198731


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES