Abstract
The paper-and-pencil Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure (ROCF) copy task has been extensively used to assess visuo-constructional skills in children and adults. The scoring systems utilized in clinical practice provide an integrated evaluation of the drawing process, without differentiating between its visuo-constructional, organizational, and motor components. Here, a tablet-based ROCF copy task capable of providing a quantitative assessment of the drawing process, differentiating between visuo-constructional, organizational, and motor skills, is trialed in 94 healthy children, between 7 and 11 years of age. Through previously validated algorithms, 12 indices of performance in the ROCF copy task were obtained for each child. Principal component analysis of the 12 indices identified spatial, procedural, and kinematic components as distinct dimensions of the drawing process. A composite score for each dimension was determined, and correlation analysis between composite scores and conventional paper-and-pencil measures of visuo-constructional, procedural, and motor skills performed. The results obtained confirmed that the constructional, organizational, and motor dimensions underlie complex figure drawing in children; and that each dimension can be measured by a unique composite score. In addition, the composite scores here obtained from children were compared with previsions results from adults, offering a novel insight into how the interplay between the three dimensions of drawing evolves with age.
Keywords: Rey–Osterrieth figure, Movement control, Organizational strategies, Visuo-constructional skills, Tablet-based neuropsychological tests
Subject terms: Cognitive neuroscience, Computational neuroscience, Psychology, Neurology
Introduction
Graphomotor skills comprise a subset of fine motor skills referring to the manual operation of a pencil or pen, typically during handwriting or drawing1. The term graphomotor skills is often used interchangeably with handwriting skills1,2; however, handwriting involves the cognitive knowledge of letters and words in addition to graphomotor skills1. Graphomotor tasks constitute up to 60% of daily activities in school and kindergarten3,4; and children with graphomotor deficits often show lower academic performance than their peers5. Identifying graphomotor problems at the earliest possible stage is crucial and can guide intervention to prevent learning difficulties later in life5. Several graphomotor evaluation tools are available for children; however, they assess either quality or speed of performance, not taking into account many of the components involved in executing graphomotor tasks6.
Recently, a shift from a product-oriented to a process-oriented assessment of the constructional, organizational, and motor abilities required to execute graphomotor tasks has occurred. In this scenario, the introduction of digital boards, such as tablets equipped with pens, has enabled the numerical analysis of dynamic and kinetic aspects of graphomotor tasks7. Tablet-based digital systems allow to obtain a variety of indices of performance that capture the three fundamental dimensions underlying the execution of graphomotor tasks8–12: the spatial dimension provides information on a subject’s visuo-constructional skills, and precisely the success in forming two-dimensional figures; the procedural dimension assesses a subject’s ability to use perceptual organization strategies; lastly, the kinematic dimension captures aspects of movement control that are crucial for handwriting and drawing. There are several studies assessing graphomotor skills using a tablet-based method, but they mainly focus on handwriting abilities7,11,13; by contrast, fewer studies have assessed drawing abilities using a tablet-based method8–10,14.
Drawing can be defined as the ability of combining one-dimensional units to form two-dimensional models; thus, it is a direct measure of a subject’s visuo-constructional skills. For more than 60 years, complex figure drawings have been utilized as a measure of visuo-constructional skills15. Among them is the Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure (ROCF) copy task, first designed by Swiss neuropsychologist André Rey as an assessment tool for measuring visuo-constructional skills and memory in adults with brain damage16,17. This test consists in reproducing the 18 geometrical units 17 that constitute the ROCF by copying it freehand (Fig. 1A).
Since its initial introduction, the ROCF copy task has been extensively used to assess visuo-constructional skills, perceptual organizational strategies, as well as memory in adults and children16–20. In addition, this task has demonstrated the ability to distinguish between control groups and pediatric and adult patients with a wide variety of disorders, suggesting that it requires the integration of multiple processes18–29.
Several scoring methods have been developed to assess the performance in the ROCF copy task. The Rey–Osterrieth 36-Point Accuracy Scoring Method is the most widely used in clinical practice: it qualitatively determines the accuracy in drawing each of the 18 units constituting the ROCF (Fig. 1A) and their relative position compared to the original design presented17,30–34. Each unit is given a score from 0 (lowest) to 2 (highest) based on copy accuracy compared to the original design. This method has several merits, including adequate internal consistency 35,36 and test–retest reliability37,38. In addition, the Rey–Osterrieth 36-Point Accuracy Scoring Method has a strong positive correlation with the performance in other visuospatial perceptual tests, such as Visual Reproduction of the Wechsler Memory Scale35, line orientation35, and Raven's Standard Progressive Matrices39. However, this method doesn’t provide information on perceptual organization strategies21. Several alternative scoring systems have been proposed, including the Boston Qualitative Scoring System40, the system developed by Meyers and Meyers38, the Savage Scoring System41,42 that can assess perceptual organization strategies, and the Developmental Scoring System43; although the correlations obtained vary in strength and by task22. There is no single method capable of providing a comprehensive evaluation while also differentiating between the visuo-constructional, organizational, and motor components required for this copy task; and using more than one scoring method to differentiate between its components might not be routinely done in the clinical practice.
In this scenario, a tablet-based system could provide a more targeted account of a subject’s performance in the ROCF copy task. A tablet-based assessment of the ROCF copy task has been implemented and validated in healthy adults: this system allowed to accurately assess the temporal and kinematic aspects of the drawing process, as well as visuo-constructional accuracy, through the use of specific indices of performance8. Similar tablet-based approaches, effectively applied for other purposes or to other drawing tasks, have also been reported11,12,29,44,45.
The primary aim of this study was to apply the tablet-based assessment of the ROCF copy task previously validated only in adults8 to healthy elementary school children. In particular, it was hypothesized that this system would support the computation of unique and specific indices of performance in the pediatric population; and that three composite scores, each one underlying one of the three dimensions of the drawing process, could be derived from these indices through principal component analysis, as described in adults8.
An additional goal of this study was to analyze the relationship between each composite score and standardized paper-and-pencil measures of complex figure drawing in children. It was hypothesized that composite scores would correlate with corresponding paper-and-pencil measures of the visuo-constructional, organizational, and motor skills required for complex figure drawing in children.
Lastly, the performances of healthy children and adults in this tablet-based ROCF copy task system were compared8. The expectation was to identify similarities between children and adults, with one exception: it was hypothesized that age would positively correlate with ROCF copy task performance in children, as compared to adults where aging can be associated with a declining performance.
Methods
Study participants
Study participants were recruited from an elementary school located in the Lombardy region of Northern Italy. To be included in the study, each participant had to meet the following inclusion criteria: (i) age ranging from 7 to 11 years; (ii) lack of neurologic, neuropsychological, or neuropsychiatric disorders, as reported by either parents or teachers; (iii) normal or corrected-to-normal vision as reported by either parents or teachers; and (iv) not having previously completed the ROCF copy task. Prior to testing, informed consent was obtained from a parent or legal guardian. The study was approved by the Local Ethic Committee of Don Gnocchi Foundation IRCCS Santa Maria Nascente (Milan, Italy), and performed in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration.
A total of 98 participants met inclusion criteria, but 4/98 experienced technical difficulties that didn’t allow recording of the ROCF copy task on the graphic tablet. Since repeating the copy task was not permitted based on the study inclusion criteria, ultimately 94 participants were included in this study (Table 1). Mean age was 9.0 years (± 1.3 years). Participants’ gender and mean level of parental education have been previously shown not to exert any significant effect on ROCF copy task performance18. Each participant used their dominant hand to perform the ROCF copy task46.
Table 1.
Study participants | N | Mean age in years (± SD) |
---|---|---|
2nd grade | 20 | 7.5 (± 0.2) |
3rd grade | 31 | 8.4 (± 0.2) |
4th grade | 22 | 9.6 (± 0.2) |
5th grade | 21 | 10.5 (± 0.3) |
Total | 94 | 9.0 (± 1.3) |
Procedure
Each study participant was individually tested in a quiet room. Participants were asked to sit on a chair with a graphic tablet (Wacom Intuos Pro, Germany) placed in front of them. The tablet was equipped with a dedicated ballpoint pen (Ink Pen, Wacom, Germany); and it was wirelessly connected to a laptop computer running on Windows10 and operated by a single experienced examiner. Each participant was asked to copy the ROCF on a A4 size blank paper covering the entire active area of the tablet as accurately as possible, using the dedicated ballpoint pen just like in the conventional paper-and-pencil test16,17; no time limits were given16,17. This allowed to virtually recreate the same texture conditions that characterize the conventional paper-and-pencil method; and also to obtain ROCF copy task conventional scores using the 36-Point Accuracy Scoring Method31, as well as ROCF copy strategy scores using the Savage Scoring System41,42. The reference ROCF was available on a A4 size paper placed next to the tablet. The pen was wirelessly connected to the graphic tablet that was capturing every pen-down event (i.e., pen in contact with the drawing surface of the paper) and recording the pen-tip position compared to the paper’s surface at a rate of 100 Hz with a precision of 0.3 mm. The entire drawing process from the first to the last pen-down event was recorded, and the pen-tip position at any pen-down event was measured over time. Compared to the conventional paper-and-pencil method, this digital system allows to simultaneously assess visuo-constructional skills, perceptual organization strategies, and the degree of movement control involved in the drawing task8,12,13,44,45.
In addition, each study participant was asked to perform the Beery-Buktenica Test of Visual-Motor Integration47 (Beery VMI) and the Standardized Italian Test of Handwriting and Orthographic Competences48 (BVSCO-3) using conventional paper-and-pencil methods. It has to be noted that 2/94 study participants didn’t complete the Beery VMI and BVSCO-3 batteries; when working with young children in a school, sometimes there are unforeseeable circumstances that prevent a child from completing a test battery, as simple as a child having to leave earlier. The Beery VMI is a validated test47 commonly used in clinical practice as a measure of visual-constructional abilities in children. Thus, the study participants’ performance in the Beery VMI was used as an internal control of their normal visuo-constructional skills; and it was compared with the tablet-derived measure of visuo-constructional skills in order to assess its validity. Similarly, the BVSCO-3 is a validated test composed of three batteries48 that is routinely used to assess hand-motor skills in Italian-speaking children; it was used here as an internal control of the participants’ normal motor skills, as well as a measure of the tablet-derived kinematic score’s validity.
All paper-and-pencil tests were administered and scored by an experienced neuropsychologist (Table 2).
Table 2.
Mean score | SD | Description | |
---|---|---|---|
INDEX: sHP |
5.2 (range 0–0 +) |
2.6 | Higher sHP = lower accuracy in positioning basic elements of ROCF on the horizontal axes |
INDEX: sVP |
4.6 (range 0–0 +) |
2.1 | Higher sVP = lower accuracy in positioning basic elements of ROCF on the vertical axes |
INDEX: sLG |
7.7 (range 0–0 +) |
2.9 | Higher sLG = lower accuracy in reproducing the length of the basic elements of the ROCF |
INDEX: sIC |
9.0 (range 0–0 +) |
3.3 | Higher sIC = lower accuracy in reproducing the inclination of the basic elements of the ROCF |
INDEX: pOR |
15.9 (range 0–0 +) |
5.9 | Higher pOR = lower level of organization for the basic elements of the ROCF |
INDEX: pBR |
37.4 (range 0–100) |
12.1 | Higher pBR = lower level of priority given to the base rectangle of the ROCF |
INDEX: pID |
70.1 (range 0–100) |
13.0 | Higher pID = lower level of priority given to the inner details of the ROCF |
INDEX: pFR |
13.1 (range 0–0 +) |
5.9 | Higher pFR = higher degree of fragmentation of the geometrical units of the ROCF |
INDEX: kVL |
30.2 (range 0–0 +) |
6.5 | Higher kVL = higher mean velocity per stroke |
INDEX: kAC |
208.8 (range 0–0 +) |
59.7 | Higher kAC = higher change of rate of velocity during acceleration phases |
INDEX: kDC |
193.5 (range 0–0 +) |
61.0 | Higher kDC = higher change of rate of velocity during deceleration phases |
INDEX: kPK |
5.1 (range 0–0 +) |
1.1 | Higher kPK = higher number of peaks per stroke |
ROCF copy accuracy |
26.4 (range 0–36) |
4.4 | Higher scores = higher copy accuracy |
ROCF copy strategy |
1.9 (range 0–6) |
1.4 | Higher scores = higher ROCF copy strategy |
Beery VMI |
19.5 (range 1–24) |
2.2 | Higher score = higher visual-constructional abilities |
BVSCO-3 (3 batteries) |
59 (range 0–0 +) |
14.1 | Higher score = higher motor control |
54.67 (range 0–0 +) |
17.5 | ||
60.68 (range 0–0 +) |
23.7 |
Tablet-based ROCF copy task data analysis
In order to acquire and analyze data from each study participant’s performance in the tablet-based ROCF copy task, a MATLAB (MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA; www.mathworks.com) software previously described and validated was utilized8. The software employs functions from the following MathWorks toolboxes: Signal Processing Toolbox (Version 7.5), Optimization Toolbox (Version 8.0), Statistics and Machine Learning Toolbox (Version 11.2), and Image Processing Toolbox (Version 10.1). Data analysis was carried out following four main stages (Fig. 2), as previously described8: administration stage; classification stage; pre-processing stage; graphical outputs and performance indices stage. While the first stage can take a variable amount of time depending on how fast the ROCF copy task is completed, the following three stages can take a minimum of 5 min to be completed by a trained operator.
During the administration stage, the entire drawing process from the first to the last pen-down event was recorded using the graphic tablet that was connected to a computer. In the classification stage, a semi-manual classification of the segmented strokes into the 48 basic elements of the ROCF (Fig. 1B) was performed, as previously described8. The digital recordings of the ROCF drawings were transformed in the pre-processing stage in order to eliminate global distortion of the copy outputs8. In the last stage, three graphical outputs for each study participant were generated: a graphic output of the ROCF drawn; a velocity output with a visual representation of the velocity profile in drawing the ROCF; and a procedural output of the drawing sequence of each ROCF’s basic element. Additionally, during the last stage the analysis software provided 12 indices of performance for each study participant. These indices, that were previously demonstrated to capture the spatial, procedural, and kinematic dimensions underlying the execution of the ROCF copy task in adults8, are presented in the following paragraphs.
The spatial indices provided information on a participant’s ability to accurately reproduce the basic elements that constitute the ROCF and included: the horizontal placement accuracy (sHP) index; the vertical placement accuracy (sVP) index; the length accuracy (sLG) index; and the inclination accuracy (sIC) index. Each index was calculated as a measure of the deviation from the reference placement (mean) of each ROCF’s basic element. The accuracy in reproducing the spatial arrangement of the ROCF's basic elements decreased, as the deviation from the reference range increased. sHP and sVP indicated the level of precision in maintaining the horizontal and vertical placement of the ROCF's basic elements, respectively; sLG represented the degree of accuracy in preserving the relative size of each ROCF’s basic element compared to the others; sIC represented the degree of accuracy in preserving the relative inclination of each ROCF’s basic elements compared to the others.
The procedural indices provided information on participants’ constructional strategies and included: base rectangle priority (pBR) index; inner details priority (pID) index; organization by relevance (pOR) index; fragmentation (pFR) index. Altogether the procedural indices measured the ability to use an organized strategy when copying the ROCF. The ROCF is made of 4 constitutive structures49: the base rectangle (Fig. 1C, in green), the main substructure (Fig. 1C, in orange), the outer configuration (Fig. 1C, in grey), and the inner details (Fig. 1C, in dark blue). The base rectangle and the main substructure represent the most important organizational units and the guiding structures when copying the ROCF. Thus, highest scores were given when the ROCF copy started from its constitutive elements, namely the base rectangle and the main substructure, and ended with its secondary elements, namely the inner details and the outer configuration (Fig. 1C)8,21. Specifically, the pBR and pID indices measured the level of priority given to the base rectangle and to the inner details, respectively, on a scale from 0 to 100; 0 represented the first pen-surface contact, while 100 represented the last contact. Subsequently, the higher the pBR or pID, the lower the priority given. The pOR index measured the number of times a participant interrupted drawing a constitutive element of the ROCF and proceeded to draw a secondary element. The higher the pOR, the higher the number of interruptions. The pFR index measured the number of times a participant interrupted drawing a basic element of the ROCF to start another element. The higher the pFR, the higher the number of interruptions and therefore fragmentation of the ROCF’s basic elements.
Kinematic indices were obtained for the entire drawing process, from the first to the last pen-down event, using a method previously described8. Each subject’s velocity profile was smoothed using a fourth-order Butterworth filter with a cut-off of 7 Hz8. A pen-down event between two velocity throughs (< 5 mm/s) of 5 mm/s or less was considered a pen-stroke; only pen-strokes longer than 10 mm were included in the analysis and used to extract kinematic indices8. The kinematic indices provided information on participants’ movement control and included: mean velocity (kVL) index; mean acceleration (kAC) index; mean deceleration (kDC) index; and number of peak velocity (kPK) index. The kVL index provided information on pen-tip’s position changes over time; the higher the kVL, the higher the participant’s movement control. The kAC and kDC indices measured velocity changes during acceleration and deceleration, respectively. Specifically, these indices measured the mean acceleration per stroke and the average velocity change across all strokes. The higher the kAC and kDC, the higher the movement control. The kPK index measured movements’ fluency by averaging the number of velocity peaks across all pen strokes. The velocity profile of a perfectly fluent stroke is characterized by a single velocity peak. The higher the number of velocity peaks, the lower the movements fluency.
The mean and standard deviation for each index in the study participants is summarized in Table 2.
Statistical analysis
For each of the 94 study participants, 12 indices of performance were obtained from the tablet-based ROCF copy task. For comparability, the same analyses previously conducted in adults using the same graphic tablet and data analysis software (i.e., R studio, version 4.3.2) were applied8. In order to reduce the set of indices into a smaller set of composite scores, the indices were standardized and Principal Component Analysis (PCA) with orthogonal Varimax rotation of the loading matrix applied50–53; subsequently, Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy54 was performed to examine the appropriateness of the datasets for PCA, as previously described8. The communalities of each index were evaluated, with items poorly accounted for by the factor solution indicated by a communality below 0.5. Primary loadings were deemed meaningful if they exceeded 0.50; while indices were considered as cross-loading indices if their loadings exceeded ± 0.30 on two or more components. In order to simplify the model structure, cross-loading indices were not retained. A cumulative scale constructed by averaging the indices’ loadings for the spatial, procedural, and kinematic components, respectively, was used to extract a composite score for each component. Before creating the composite scores, negative loadings were reverse scored. The reliability of the cumulative scales was measured by Cronbach’s alpha. Values that exceed 0.70 were deemed above the recommended level of acceptability. Composite scores were subsequently correlated with conventional paper-and-pencil measures of ROCF copy accuracy (as measured by the 36-Point Accuracy Scoring Method31), ROCF copy strategy (as measured by the Savage Scoring System41,42), visuo-constructional skills (as measured by Beery VMI47), and motor skills (as measured by BVSCO-348), as well as participants’ age. In regard to participants’ age, it has to be noted that children with a normal neurodevelopment attend grades based on their chronological age. Since children in the same grade are approximately the same age, age and educational level can be considered overlapping in the pediatric population here presented. By contrast, age and educational level don’t overlap in adults, where a higher level of education is generally associated with a higher ROCF copy task performance8, while higher age is associated with a decline in performance due physiological aging8.
Results
Principal component analysis
The minimum amount of data for PCA analysis was met, with a sample size of 94 study participants and a calculated ratio of 7.9 observations per variable53. PCA was deemed appropriate in reducing the original 12 indices into a smaller number of composite scores, based on the following three criteria: a level of correlation between two or more variables of at least 0.3, suggesting a high level of redundancy in the data (Fig. 3A); a significant correlation among several indices, as shown by the statistically significant result of Bartlett’s test of sphericity55 (χ2(55) = 853.06, p < 0.001); and lastly, a minimum MSA level of 0.553. Only the MSA for pID was slightly below the accepted level (pID MSA: 0.48), in line with the analysis performed in healthy adults8. Ultimately, pID exclusion from the analysis resulted in an overall MSA level of 0.75, with a minimum MSA for individuals indices of 0.58 (Fig. 3B). PCA was then performed on the 11 remaining indices; and Horn’s parallel analysis for PCA indicated the existence of three components, in line with previous assumptions8 (observed eigenvalues: C1 = 3.84, C2 = 3.15, and C3 = 1.73; simulated eigenvalues: C1 = 1.59, C2 = 1.41, and C3 = 1.28). The observed eigenvalues for the first three components were significantly higher than those for the simulated randomly generated datasets (ΔC1 = 2.25; ΔC2 = 1.74; ΔC3 = 0.45). The Kaiser's criterion54 was used to confirm the existence of three components, that together accounted for 79.3% of the variance in total scores. Specifically, the first three components explained 34.9%, 28.6%, and 15.7% of the variance, respectively.
The component matrix results highlighted commonalities for all indices higher than 0.5, with no index exhibiting cross-loading (criteria for cross-loading: two loadings greater than ± 0.30). With primary loadings greater than ± 0.50 for each of the indices, and a combined loading greater than ± 0.70, the PCA confirmed its very robust structure. Standardized loadings (pattern matrix) based upon the correlation matrix for the three components (RC1, RC2, and RC3 for 1, 2, and 3, respectively) are shown in Table 3. Overall, the structure of relationships between the various dimensions of drawing overlapped what previously described in healthy adults8 (Fig. 4 and Table 3).
Table 3.
RC1 | RC2 | RC3 | h2 | u2 | com | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
sHP | 0.19 | 0.85 | 0.08 | 0.77 | 0.23 | 1.10 |
sVP | 0.14 | 0.80 | 0.18 | 0.69 | 0.31 | 1.20 |
sLG | 0.03 | 0.90 | 0.06 | 0.82 | 0.18 | 1.00 |
sIC | − 0.06 | 0.76 | 0.06 | 0.59 | 0.41 | 1.00 |
kVL | 0.93 | 0.05 | − 0.21 | 0.91 | 0.09 | 1.10 |
kAC | 0.97 | 0.04 | − 0.12 | 0.96 | 0.04 | 1.00 |
kDC | 0.93 | 0.18 | − 0.11 | 0.91 | 0.09 | 1.10 |
kPK | − 0.87 | − 0.04 | − 0.10 | 0.78 | 0.22 | 1.00 |
pOR | − 0.01 | 0.11 | 0.88 | 0.78 | 0.22 | 1.00 |
pBR | − 0.17 | 0.18 | 0.75 | 0.63 | 0.37 | 1.20 |
pFR | − 0.07 | 0.03 | 0.93 | 0.87 | 0.13 | 1.00 |
The first component (RC1) showed significant positive primary loadings for kVL, kAC, kDC, but a significant negative loading for kPK. This composite score, renamed “Kinematic” (KIN), provided information on the kinematic aspects of the ROCF copy task; lower values of KIN were associated with greater movement control. The second component (RC2) showed high positive primary loadings for sHP, sVP, sLG, and sIC. This composite score, renamed “Spatial” (SPA), provided information on the accuracy in reproducing the spatial relationships of the ROCF’s basic elements; lower values of SPA were associated with greater accuracy in reproducing the spatial relationship of the ROCF’s basic elements. Lastly, the third component (RC3), renamed "Procedural" (PRO), demonstrated positive primary loadings with pOR, pBR, and pFR, indicating that lower values of PRO were associated with better organizational strategy in drawing the ROCF. Of note, due to its negative loading the kPK index was reverse-scored prior to the creation of the KIN composite score. The SPA and the PRO composite scores were also reversed scored so that higher scores represented better performances for all the composite measures. The assumption of dimensionality for each composite score was supported by their clean interpretation in the model’s results (i.e., indices were strongly interrelated and represented a single component distinctly). For all scales, Cronbach’s alpha exceeded the recommended threshold of 0.70, indicating satisfactory internal consistency (0.86 for the scale of SPA; 0.95 for the scale of the KIN; 0.83 for the scale of PRO).
Correlation analyses for the composite scores highlighted a significant correlation between SPA and PRO (r = 0.22; p = 0.03); by contrast, there was no significant correlation between SPA and KIN (r = − 0.16; p = 0.12), and between PRO and KIN (r = 0.17; p = 0.10).
Correlations between tablet-based ROCF copy task and conventional paper-and-pencil tests
As a final step in the analysis, the relationship between composite scores and age, as well as between composite scores and conventional paper-and-pencil measures of ROCF copy accuracy (as measured by the 36-Point Accuracy Scoring Method31), ROCF copy strategy (as measured by the Savage Scoring System41,42), visuo-constructional skills (as measured by Beery VMI47), and motor skills (as measured by BVSCO-348) were assessed through partial correlation analysis using Pearson correlation coefficient. All statistical analyses were conducted through the program R (version 4.3.1) and the packages psych56, dplyr57, corrplot58, qgraph59, ltm60. Correlation results are presented in Table 4.
Table 4.
SPA | AGE (N = 94) |
ROCF copy accuracy (N = 94) | ROCF copy strategy (N = 94) | Beery VMI (N = 921) |
BVSCO-31 (N = 922) |
||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
r = 0.29** | r = 0.77*** | r = 0.04 | r = 0.33** | r = 0.04 | r = − 0.07 | r = 0.08 | |
PRO | r = 0.16 | r = 0.25* | r = 0.64*** | r = 0.18 | r = − 0.09 | r = − 0.14 | r = − 0.15 |
KIN | r = 0.19 | r = − 0.17 | r = 0.10 | r = − 0.23* | r = 0.12 | r = 0.00 | r = 0.17 |
1Scores obtained from participants’ performance in the three paper-and-pencil tests that are part of the BVSCO-3 battery are reported.
2For 92/94 study participants, performance in the conventional paper-and-pencil Beery VMI and BVSCO-3 tests was compared with each composite score (SPA, PRO, and KIN).
SPA demonstrated a positive and significant correlation with age (r = 0.29; p < 0.01); similarly, there was a positive although not significant trend between KIN and age (r = 0.19; p = 0.06). By contrast, there was no significant correlation between PRO and age (r = 0.16; p = 0.11). Both SPA (r = 0.77; p < 0.001) and PRO (r = 0.25; p < 0.05) showed a positive and significant correlation with paper-and-pencil ROCF copy accuracy assessed using the conventional 36-Point Accuracy Scoring Method31; while KIN did not correlate significantly with it (r = − 0.17; p = 0.11). Paper-and-pencil ROCF copy strategy assessed with the conventional Savage Scoring System41,42 correlated significantly with PRO (r = 0.64; p < 0.001), but not with SPA (r = 0.04; p = 0.70) or KIN (r = 0.10; p = 0.35). The performance in the Beery VMI47 demonstrated a positive and significant correlation with SPA (r = 0.29; p < 0.01), a negative and significant correlation with KIN (r = − 0.23; p < 0.05), but no significant correlation with PRO (r = 0.18; p = 0.09). Lastly, the performance in the BVSCO-348 did not correlate with any of the three composite scores, nor demonstrated a trend toward any of them.
Discussion
The use of a tablet-based digital system for analyzing graphomotor functions has several advantages compared to conventional paper-and-pencil methods. Through the computation of three composite scores, this system allows to simultaneously capture and differentiate the spatial, procedural, and kinematic dimensions of the drawing process8,12,13,44,45. The spatial dimension provides information on a subject’s visuo-constructional skills, and precisely the success in forming a two-dimensional figure; the procedural dimension provides information on a subject’s use of perceptual organization strategies; lastly, the kinematic dimension allows to capture aspects of movement control in drawing. These three separate dimensions cannot be derived from conventional paper-and-pencil scoring methods, such as the Rey–Osterrieth 36-Point Accuracy Scoring Method. Even when larger paper-and-pencil testing batteries are performed in order to provide an evaluation of all three components, the process is laborious and time-consuming in both the administration and evaluation stages.
The tablet-based ROCF copy task system here proposed and trialed for the first time in healthy children demonstrated to be a tool capable of comprehensively assess visuo-constructional, procedural, and motor skills, while also differentiating between these three components. The PCA’s results here presented in a pediatric population further support the use of separate scores to assess each dimension of the drawing process in the ROCF copy task (Figs. 3B and 4): SPA for the spatial dimension; PRO for the procedural dimension; and KIN for the kinematic dimension. Interestingly, the PCA results highlighted how the spatial and procedural dimensions of the drawing process have a higher degree of independence from each other in healthy children (Fig. 3B); by contrast, a greater interplay between these two components has been described in adults8. This phenomenon can be at least partially explained by an acquired integration between visuo-constructional skills and perceptual organization strategies, represented by SPA and PRO respectively, that is present in adults but has yet to develop in children. In adults, it has been postulated that the integration between SPA and PRO allows to adopt effective procedural strategies, with the goal of simplifying the constructional process8.
In order to provide further insights into the aspects underlying complex figures drawing in healthy children, as captured by the composite scores SPA, PRO, and KIN, correlation analyses were performed. Each composite score was initially correlated with participants’ age. Age showed a positive and significant correlation with SPA, indicating that visuo-constructional skills improve with age, as expected (Table 4). This is in contrast with what observed in adults, where the correlation between SPA and age is significant but negative, indicating that physiological aging is associated with a decline in visuo-constructional skills8. Age showed a positive although not significant trend toward KIN; this highlights how movement control physiologically improves with age, but eventually plateaus in adults8. There was no significant correlation between PRO and age, neither in the pediatric population here presented nor in adults8; while a positive correlation between PRO and educational level has been described in adults8. These findings suggest that PRO might correlate with cognitive changes associated to long-term education; thus, it’s postulated that perceptual organizational strategies are not yet fully developed during elementary school, and they consolidate later in the school years.
As a next step in the analysis, composite scores were correlated with conventional paper-and-pencil measures of ROCF copy accuracy (36-Point Accuracy Scoring Method31), ROCF copy strategy (Savage Scoring System41,42), visuo-constructional skills (Beery VMI47), and motor skills (BVSCO-348). Higher SPA scores were associated with a better performance in conventional paper-and-pencil measures of spatial accuracy, namely the 36-Point Accuracy Scoring Method31 and the Beery VMI47. Of note, the highest correlation was observed between SPA and ROCF copy task accuracy (p < 0.001 vs p < 0.01 between SPA and Beery VMI): this is in line with what observed in adults8, further supporting the validity of SPA as a measure of visuo-constructional skills. The correlation between SPA and the other conventional paper-and-pencil measures of drawing were low and not significant. By contrast, PRO exhibited its highest correlation with organizational skills as measured by ROCF copy strategy using the Savage Scoring System41,42 (p < 0.001). In this regard, it has to be noted that PRO also correlated with ROCF copy accuracy (p < 0.05), although at a lower degree compared to ROCF copy strategy (p < 0.001). However, PRO did not show a significant correlation with the other measure of spatial accuracy considered in this study, namely the Beery VMI. This discrepancy can be attributed to a low specificity of the 36-Point Accuracy Scoring Method31, that is known to be influenced by perceptual organizational strategies in addition to visuo-constructional skills61. In contrast, the Beery VMI47 likely provides a purer measure of visuo-constructional skills, unaffected by organizational strategies. Lastly, KIN exhibited only a significant negative correlation with Beery VMI47 (p < 0.05). Previous research has shown a correlation between KIN and copy accuracy in drawing tasks administered to healthy adults; specifically, higher KIN was associated with a less meticulous and careful drawing style (lower spatial precision due to increased drawing velocity)8. A similar rationale may apply in the context of this study; in fact, the Beery VMI47 is a lengthy test, and can represent a challenge for elementary school children not used to keep a high level of concentration for a long period of time. Thus, it’s possible that the negative correlation between Beery VMI47 and KIN was dictated by the adoption of a rapid and careless drawing style, with subsequent negative repercussions on drawing accuracy. This pattern suggests that healthy individuals could trade off accuracy for a fastest execution time. Contrary to expectations, the BVSCO-348 test battery did not correlate with the KIN or with any other composite score. The BVSCO-348 is a paper-and-pencil Standardized Italian Test of Handwriting and Orthographic Competences, and was used here as an internal control to assess participants’ hand-motor skills. However, the test is influenced by orthographic competences in addition to hand-motor skills, and this contamination is most certainly responsible for the lack of correlation with the three composite scores.
Compared to conventional paper-and-pencil scoring methods that provide an integrated evaluation of the drawing process without differentiating between its components, the results presented here demonstrated that this tablet-based system is capable of extracting unique scores (SPA, PRO, and KIN) correlating with the skills required for drawing complex figures in healthy children. In this study 12 indices of performance were utilized and 3 composite scores derived from them, as previously described8. Nonetheless, the method here described has the potential of capturing additional indices of performance assessing other aspects of the drawing process. For instance, over thirty different temporal, kinematic, and dynamic indices examining handwriting movement using graphic tablets have been described11, as well as additional indices of visuo-constructive abilities for the early detection of cognitive impairment and dementia12. Considering different measures in the future could potentially improve the tools used in this field, and allow to determine which metrics better explain performance in the ROCF copy task in children.
Additional tablet-based system assessing the performance in the ROCF copy task have been developed over the years with different goals, including identifying a digital biomarker for Alzheimer disease9, and differentiating between healthy adults and patients with either mild cognitive impairment or dementia29. The authors speculate that this system could potentially identify and differentiate between deficits in specific areas of the drawing process in children, similarly to what has been reported for adults29. While additional studies in a patient population are required in order to prove this hypothesis, it’s important to note that being able to diagnose deficits in organizational, fine motor, and/or visuo-constructional skills could be used to build treatment plans tailored to patients’ specific needs.
Lastly, although the scoring algorithm here described is fully automated, the classification stage (Fig. 2) requires a semi-manual classification of the segmented strokes into the 48 basic elements of the ROCF (Fig. 1B). This process takes about 5 min per ROCF drawing. A novel automated classification algorithm capable of automatically identifying the drawing parts of a complex figure has been implemented44; thus, future studies could aim at integrating a similar algorithm into the tablet-based system here utilized, with the goal of cutting operator-depended analysis time and allowing to obtain almost instant results.
Author contributions
All authors contributed to the study concept and design. M.F., S.G., M.P., M.R., and C.Z. contributed to data acquisition. M.F., S.G., and M.P. performed the analysis. M.F., S.G., M.P., and A.C. interpreted the data. M.F. wrote the main manuscript. All authors contributed to the critical revision of the manuscript.
Data availability
The dataset generated and analyzed during the current study is available from the corresponding author upon request.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Suggate, S., Pufke, E. & Stoeger, H. The effect of fine and grapho-motor skill demands on preschoolers’ decoding skill. J. Exp. Child. Psychol.141, 34–48. 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.012 (2016). 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Ratzon, N. Z., Efraim, D. & Bart, O. A short-term graphomotor program for improving writing readiness skills of first-grade students. Am. J. Occup. Ther.61, 399–405. 10.5014/ajot.61.4.399 (2007). 10.5014/ajot.61.4.399 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Marr, D., Cermak, S., Cohn, E. S. & Henderson, A. Fine motor activities in Head Start and kindergarten classrooms. Am. J. Occup. Ther.57, 550–557. 10.5014/ajot.57.5.550 (2003). 10.5014/ajot.57.5.550 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.McHale, K. & Cermak, S. A. Fine motor activities in elementary school: Preliminary findings and provisional implications for children with fine motor problems. Am. J. Occup. Ther.46, 898–903. 10.5014/ajot.46.10.898 (1992). 10.5014/ajot.46.10.898 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Knaier, E. et al. Integration of speed and quality in measuring graphomotor skills: The Zurich graphomotor test. Am. J. Occup. Ther.10.5014/ajot.2022.049242 (2022). 10.5014/ajot.2022.049242 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Feder, K. P. & Majnemer, A. Children’s handwriting evaluation tools and their psychometric properties. Phys. Occup. Ther. Pediatr.23, 65–84 (2003). 10.1080/J006v23n03_05 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Gerth, S. et al. Adapting to the surface: A comparison of handwriting measures when writing on a tablet computer and on paper. Hum. Mov. Sci.48, 62–73. 10.1016/j.humov.2016.04.006 (2016). 10.1016/j.humov.2016.04.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Petilli, M. A., Daini, R., Saibene, F. L. & Rabuffetti, M. Automated scoring for a Tablet-based Rey Figure copy task differentiates constructional, organisational, and motor abilities. Sci. Rep.11, 14895. 10.1038/s41598-021-94247-9 (2021). 10.1038/s41598-021-94247-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Kim, K. W. et al. A comprehensive evaluation of the process of copying a complex figure in early- and late-onset Alzheimer disease: A quantitative analysis of digital pen data. J. Med. Internet. Res.22, e18136. 10.2196/18136 (2020). 10.2196/18136 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Zhang, X. et al. A tablet-based multi-dimensional drawing system can effectively distinguish patients with amnestic MCI from healthy individuals. Sci. Rep.14, 982. 10.1038/s41598-023-46710-y (2024). 10.1038/s41598-023-46710-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Danna, J., Paz-Villagran, V. & Velay, J. L. Signal-to-noise velocity peaks difference: A new method for evaluating the handwriting movement fluency in children with dysgraphia. Res. Dev. Disabil.34, 4375–4384. 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.09.012 (2013). 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.09.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Muller, S., Preische, O., Heymann, P., Elbing, U. & Laske, C. Diagnostic value of a tablet-based drawing task for discrimination of patients in the early course of Alzheimer’s disease from healthy individuals. J. Alzheimers Dis.55, 1463–1469. 10.3233/JAD-160921 (2017). 10.3233/JAD-160921 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Accardo, A. P., Genna, M. & Borean, M. Reprint of “Development, maturation and learning influence on handwriting kinematics”. Hum. Mov. Sci.32, 999–1009. 10.1016/j.humov.2013.08.002 (2013). 10.1016/j.humov.2013.08.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Savickaite, S., Morrison, C., Lux, E., Delafield-Butt, J. & Simmons, D. R. The use of a tablet-based app for investigating the influence of autistic and ADHD traits on performance in a complex drawing task. Behav. Res. Methods54, 2479–2501. 10.3758/s13428-021-01746-8 (2022). 10.3758/s13428-021-01746-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Hubley, A. M. & Jassal, S. Comparability of the Rey-Osterrieth and Modified Taylor Complex Figures using total scores, completion times, and construct validation. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol.28, 1482–1497. 10.1080/13803390500434441 (2006). 10.1080/13803390500434441 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Rey, A. L’examen psychologique dans les cas d’encéphalopathie traumatique (Les problems) [The psychological examination in cases of traumatic encepholopathy Problems]. Archives de Psychologie28, 215–285 (1941). [Google Scholar]
- 17.Le Osterrieth, P.A. Test de copie d’une figure complexe; contribution a l’etude de la perception et de la memoire, Vol. 20 (1944).
- 18.Conson, M. et al. Normative data of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure for Italian-speaking elementary school children. Neurol. Sci.40, 2045–2050. 10.1007/s10072-019-03929-w (2019). 10.1007/s10072-019-03929-w [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Rabin, L. A., Barr, W. B. & Burton, L. A. Assessment practices of clinical neuropsychologists in the United States and Canada: A survey of INS, NAN, and APA Division 40 members. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol.20, 33–65. 10.1016/j.acn.2004.02.005 (2005). 10.1016/j.acn.2004.02.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Rabin, L. A., Paolillo, E. & Barr, W. B. Stability in test-usage practices of clinical neuropsychologists in the United States and Canada over a 10-year period: A follow-up survey of INS and NAN members. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol.31, 206–230. 10.1093/arclin/acw007 (2016). 10.1093/arclin/acw007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Zhang, X. et al. Overview of the complex figure test and its clinical application in neuropsychiatric disorders, including copying and recall. Front Neurol12, 680474. 10.3389/fneur.2021.680474 (2021). 10.3389/fneur.2021.680474 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Weber, R. C., Riccio, C. A. & Cohen, M. J. Does Rey Complex Figure copy performance measure executive function in children?. Appl. Neuropsychol. Child2, 6–12. 10.1080/09084282.2011.643964 (2013). 10.1080/09084282.2011.643964 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Cardillo, R., Lievore, R. & Mammarella, I. C. Do children with and without autism spectrum disorder use different visuospatial processing skills to perform the Rey-Osterrieth complex figure test?. Autism Res.15, 1311–1323. 10.1002/aur.2717 (2022). 10.1002/aur.2717 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Sami, N., Carte, E. T., Hinshaw, S. P. & Zupan, B. A. Performance of girls with ADHD and comparison girls on the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure: Evidence for executive processing deficits. Child. Neuropsychol.9, 237–254. 10.1076/chin.9.4.237.23514 (2003). 10.1076/chin.9.4.237.23514 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Seidman, L. J., Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Weber, W. & Ouellette, C. Toward defining a neuropsychology of attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder: Performance of children and adolescents from a large clinically referred sample. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol.65, 150–160. 10.1037/0022-006X.65.1.150 (1997). 10.1037/0022-006X.65.1.150 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Russell, C. et al. A deficit of spatial remapping in constructional apraxia after right-hemisphere stroke. Brain133, 1239–1251. 10.1093/brain/awq052 (2010). 10.1093/brain/awq052 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Salvadori, E., Dieci, F., Caffarra, P. & Pantoni, L. Qualitative evaluation of the immediate copy of the Rey–Osterrieth complex figure: comparison between vascular and degenerative MCI patients. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol.34, 14–23. 10.1093/arclin/acy010 (2019). 10.1093/arclin/acy010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Slapik, M. et al. Visuospatial organization and recall in cerebellar ataxia. Cerebellum18, 33–46. 10.1007/s12311-018-0948-z (2019). 10.1007/s12311-018-0948-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Di Febbo, D. et al. A decision support system for Rey-Osterrieth complex figure evaluation. Expert Syst. Appl.10.1016/j.eswa.2022.119226 (2023). 10.1016/j.eswa.2022.119226 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Meyers, J.E.M., Meyers, K.R. Rey Complex Figure Test and Recognition Trial (RCFT). (1995).
- 31.Awad, N. et al. Explicit and objective scoring criteria for the taylor complex figure test. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol.26, 405–415. 10.1080/13803390490510112 (2004). 10.1080/13803390490510112 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Taylor, L. J. O. Scoring criteria for the rey-osterrieth complex figure test 350–351 (Oxford University Press, New York, 1998). [Google Scholar]
- 33.Knight, J. A. ROCF administration procedures and scoring systems, 57–192 (2003).
- 34.Knight, J.A., Kaplan, E., & Ireland, L.D. Survey findings of Rey-Osterrieth complex figure usage, pp. 45–56 (2003).
- 35.Berry, D. T. R., Allen, R. S. & Schmitt, F. A. Rey-Osterrieth complex figure: Psychometric characteristics in a geriatric sample. Clin. Neuropsychol.5, 143–153. 10.1080/13854049108403298 (1991). 10.1080/13854049108403298 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Fastenau, P. S., Bennett, J. M. & Denburg, N. L. Application of psychometric standards to scoring system evaluation: is “new” necessarily “improved”?. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol.18, 462–472. 10.1080/01688639608409003 (1996). 10.1080/01688639608409003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Levine, A. J., Miller, E. N., Becker, J. T., Selnes, O. A. & Cohen, B. A. Normative data for determining significance of test-retest differences on eight common neuropsychological instruments. Clin. Neuropsychol.18, 373–384. 10.1080/1385404049052420 (2004). 10.1080/1385404049052420 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Meyers, J.E. Meyers Scoring System for the Rey Complex Figure Test and the Recognition Trial, Vol. 20 (1994).
- 39.Biesbroek, J. M. et al. The anatomy of visuospatial construction revealed by lesion-symptom mapping. Neuropsychologia62, 68–76. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.07.013 (2014). 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.07.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Stern, R. A. et al. The Boston Qualitative Scoring System for the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure: Description and interrater reliability. Clin. Neuropsychol.8, 309–322 (1994). 10.1080/13854049408404137 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Savage, C. R. et al. Organizational strategies mediate nonverbal memory impairment in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol. Psychiatry45, 905–916. 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00278-9 (1999). 10.1016/s0006-3223(98)00278-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Deckersbach, T. et al. Reliability and validity of a scoring system for measuring organizational approach in the Complex Figure Test. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol.22, 640–648. 10.1076/1380-3395(200010)22:5;1-9;FT640 (2000). 10.1076/1380-3395(200010)22:5;1-9;FT640 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Holmes, J.H., & Waber, D.P. The Developmental Scoring System for the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (1996).
- 44.Webb, S. S. et al. Validation of an automated scoring program for a digital complex figure copy task within healthy aging and stroke. Neuropsychology35, 847–862. 10.1037/neu0000748 (2021). 10.1037/neu0000748 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Hyun, G. J. et al. Visuospatial working memory assessment using a digital tablet in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Comput. Methods Programs Biomed.157, 137–143. 10.1016/j.cmpb.2018.01.022 (2018). 10.1016/j.cmpb.2018.01.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Yamashita, H. Right- and left-hand performance on the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure: A preliminary study in non-clinical sample of right handed people. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol.25, 314–317. 10.1093/arclin/acq019 (2010). 10.1093/arclin/acq019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Beery, K. E., Buktenica, N. A. & Beery, N. A. The Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration: administration, scoring, and teaching manual 6th edn. (Pearson, 2010). [Google Scholar]
- 48.Cornoldi, C. M. F., R.; Re A.M. BVSCO-3 - Batteria per la Valutazione clinica della Scrittura e della Competenza Ortografica. (Giunti, 2022).
- 49.Trojano, L., De Cicco, G. & Grossi, D. Copying procedures in focal brain-damaged patients. Ital. J. Neurol. Sci.14, 23–33. 10.1007/BF02339039 (1993). 10.1007/BF02339039 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.DiStefano, C., Zhu, M. & Mindrila, D. Understanding and using factor scores: Considerations for the applied researcher. Pract. Assess. Res. Eval.14, 20. 10.7275/da8t-4g52 (2019). 10.7275/da8t-4g52 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Andy Field, J. M. Zoë Field. Discovering Statistics Using R (SAGE, Berlin, 2012). [Google Scholar]
- 52.Kabacoff, R. I. R in Action, 3rd edn. (Manning, 2022).
- 53.Hair, J. F. B., Babin, B. J. & Anderson, R. E. Multivariate Data Analysis (Pearson, Harlow, 2014). [Google Scholar]
- 54.Kaiser, H. F. An index of factorial simplicity. Psychometrika10.1007/BF02291575 (1974). 10.1007/BF02291575 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Bartlett, M. S. The Effect of Standardization on a χ2 Approximation in Factor Analysis Vol. 38, 337–344 (Oxford University Press, 1951). [Google Scholar]
- 56.Revelle, W. psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research (Evanston, Illinois, 2013). [Google Scholar]
- 57.Wickham, H., François, R., Henry, L., Müller, K., & Wickham, M. H. Package ‘dplyr’. A Grammar of Data Manipulation. (2019).
- 58.Wei, T., Simko, V., Levy, M., Xie, Y., Jin, Y., & Zemla, J. Package ‘corrplot’ (2017).
- 59.Epskamp, S., Cramer, A. O., Waldorp, L. J., Schmittmann, V. D., & Borsboom, D. qgraph: Network visualizations of relationships in psychometric data. (2012).
- 60.Rizopoulos, D. ltm: An R package for latent variable modeling and item response analysis. J. Stat. Softw.10.18637/jss.v017.i05 (2007). 10.18637/jss.v017.i05 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Studeny, J. et al. Executive and nonexecutive demands of constructional measures within a children’s psychiatric inpatient setting. Appl. Neuropsychol. Child.8, 40–49. 10.1080/21622965.2017.1381098 (2019). 10.1080/21622965.2017.1381098 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Cohen, J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences 2nd edn. (Erlbaum, 1988). [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The dataset generated and analyzed during the current study is available from the corresponding author upon request.