Abstract
Some recent research and commentary have suggested that most or all the effects reported by people who microdose psychedelics may be explained by expectations or placebo effects. In this rapid review, we aimed to evaluate the strength of evidence for a placebo explanation of the reported effects of microdosing. We conducted a PubMed search for all studies investigating psychedelic microdosing with controlled doses and a placebo comparator. We identified 19 placebo-controlled microdosing studies and summarised all positive and null findings across this literature. Risk of bias was assessed using the Cochrane risk-of-bias tool for randomised trials. The reviewed papers indicated that microdosing with LSD and psilocybin leads to changes in neurobiology, physiology, subjective experience, affect, and cognition relative to placebo. We evaluate methodological gaps and challenges in microdosing research and suggest eight reasons why current claims that microdosing is predominately a placebo are premature and possibly wrong: (1) there have been only a small number of controlled studies; (2) studies have had small sample sizes; (3) there is evidence of dose-dependent effects; (4) studies have only investigated the effects of a small number of doses; (5) the doses investigated may have been too small; (6) studies have looked only at non-clinical populations; (7) studies so far have been susceptible to selection bias; and (8) the measured impact of expectancy is small. Considering the available evidence, we conclude that it is not yet possible to determine whether microdosing is a placebo.
Keywords: Microdosing, psychedelics, hallucinogen, LSD, psilocybin, low dose, placebo, expectation
Is microdosing a placebo?
Microdosing, the practice of regular ingestion of low doses of psychedelic substances, gained widespread awareness around 2015, with a barrage of positive news stories describing a wide range of potential benefits (e.g., Leonard, 2015). Information about the specific dose that constituted a microdose varied, but the common claim was that microdosers were taking doses that did not result in marked alterations to their state of consciousness. Questionnaire data indicates that microdosing quickly became a popular phenomenon, with many thousands of individuals experimenting with this novel way of using psychedelics (Winstock et al., 2020). Widespread and increasing use initially occurred against a backdrop of almost no scientific knowledge about the effects, mechanisms, or risks of regular use of psychedelic drugs at low doses.
From 2018, academic studies of microdosing began to appear in the literature (e.g., Johnstad, 2018; Prochazkova et al., 2018). Early microdosing studies were predominately self-report survey studies, qualitative interviews, or observational prospective studies. Previously, we comprehensively reviewed all microdosing research up to April 2021 and found that these early studies predominately reported positive benefits of microdosing in the domains of mental health, wellbeing, cognition, personality, changes in conscious state, and physiological changes (Polito and Liknaitzky, 2022).
However, not all studies have indicated benefits of microdosing. One influential study used a ‘self-blinded’ prospective design, whereby individuals prepared their own placebo or genuine dosing materials, mixed them up so they were unaware of the contents of each specific dose, and then completed a 4-week microdosing regimen, providing regular reports to the investigators (Szigeti et al., 2021). That study found little difference between the placebo and active dosing conditions and also found that participants’ guesses about whether they had consumed a placebo or genuine microdose had a strong influence on outcomes. Another prospective study reported that wellbeing outcomes were predicted by microdosers’ expectations (Kaertner et al., 2021).
There have also now been 19 lab-based studies of microdosing that have administered controlled doses of either LSD or psilocybin. Our earlier review included details on eight of these lab studies (i.e., eight lab studies specifically investigating microdosing, published between 2018 and 2021). Since completing that review, there have been a further 11 publications reporting either controlled lab studies of microdosing or field studies that have used a measured and controlled dose (see Table 1 for all microdosing studies with controlled doses). These studies, where the quantity of psychedelic substances is known and controlled, provide a more rigorous standard of evidence than studies based on self-administration.
Table 1.
Significant and null findings from microdosing studies with controlled doses.
![]() |
Papers reporting on the same data are displayed in a single row. Table is ordered alphabetically, based on the first published paper for each dataset.
ARCI: Addiction Research Centre Inventory; ASC: altered states of consciousness scale; AUT: alternative uses test; BFI: big five inventory; BIEPS: psychological wellbeing scale; BTrack: balance management system; CANTAB: Cambridge neuropsychological test automated battery; CFS: cognitive flexibility scale; CPS: creative personality scale; DASS: depression anxiety stress scale; DEQ: drug effects questionnaire; Dflex: detail and flexibility questionnaire; DSST: digit symbol substitution test; FFMQ: five facets of mindfulness questionnaire; FSS: flow state scale; LTP: long-term potentiation; MAIA: multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness; MEG: magnetoencephalography; MODTAS: modified tellegen absorption scale; MWQ: mind wandering questionnaire; NLP: natural language processing; PANAS: positive and negative affect scale; POMS: profile of mood states; PSS: perceived stress scale; RAT: remote associates test; SSS: short susceptibility scale; STAI: state-trait anxiety inventory; TAS: tellegen absorption scale; TECA: cognitive and affective empathy test; VAS: visual analogue scales (exact items vary across studies); W-KT: Wallach-Kogan test.
Some LSD studies used LSD tartrate. Doses reported in this table show the equivalent dose in LSD base.
In Murphy et al. (2023), measures marked with ‘*’ remained significant when only analysing participants who were unsure of which condition they were in.
Overall, studies with controlled doses have provided mixed evidence about the effectiveness of microdosing. This appears to have led to a shift in sentiment in media reporting and attitudes among some scientists, with emerging claims that microdosing may be largely driven by placebo effects and expectations (Dolan, 2022; Haridy, 2022; Siebert, 2021; Smith, 2022). In this paper, we review all the microdosing studies with known and controlled doses, with a particular focus on what this body of evidence can tell us about the role of placebo in explaining the outcomes that are reported by people who microdose. Whereas our earlier review aimed to comprehensively describe all studies with low-dose psychedelics, here we specifically evaluate what the most rigorous studies reveal about the mechanisms underlying microdosing.
Method
This rapid review followed a similar search procedure to Polito and Liknaitzky (2022). Specifically, we conducted a database search that targeted all papers investigating mental health or cognitive enhancement outcomes related to ingestion of a psychedelic compound with controlled doses in the microdose range (see criteria below). We aimed to identify papers with a term related to any psychedelic substance in the title, plus a term indicating low doses in the title or abstract. Notably, in this study, we included only studies where microdoses were administered along with a placebo comparison control. This meant that we excluded papers where a microdose was itself used as a comparator in a study investigating the effects of higher doses of psychedelics (e.g., Griffiths et al., 2018), and studies where participants reported on naturalistic microdosing experiences. We restricted our search to papers published after 2018, which is when the first controlled microdosing study was published.
The search was conducted on PubMed on 12 February 2024 with the following syntax: ((psychedelic[Title] OR hallucinogen[Title] OR lsd[Title] OR psilocybin[Title] OR psilocin[Title] OR “Lysergic acid diethylamide”[Title] OR “Magic mushroom”[Title] OR dmt[Title] OR mescaline[Title] OR trimethoxyphenethylamine[Title] OR peyote[Title] OR “San pedro”[Title] OR dimethyltryptamine[Title] OR “2C-B”[Title] OR iboga[Title] OR ibogaine[Title]) AND ((“low dose”[Title/Abstract] OR “low doses”[Title/Abstract] OR dose-related[Title/Abstract] OR microdose[Title/Abstract] OR microdosing[Title/Abstract] OR “Mini dose”[Title/Abstract] OR “Small dose”[Title/Abstract] OR “Sub-threshold”[Title/Abstract] OR “Sub-perceptual”[Title/Abstract] OR “Sub-acute”[Title/Abstract]) or “dose”[Title])) AND ((“2018”[Date – Publication]: “3000”[Date – Publication])).
Inclusion criteria were: (1) use of ‘classical’ or serotonergic psychedelics; (2) controlled doses within a microdose range (see Table 1 in Polito and Liknaitzky, 2022); (3) inclusion of a placebo comparator condition; (4) reporting of primary empirical data; (5) use of human subjects; and (6) peer-reviewed publications. Papers were screened by each author independently, and any disagreements were resolved through discussion and consensus. The initial search resulted in 131 items. After duplicates were removed, 127 titles and abstracts were screened. Full-text screening was conducted on 29 papers, which led to final sample of 19 papers, as shown in Figure 1. Risk of bias was assessed using the revised Cochrane risk-of-bias tool for randomised trials (Higgins et al., 2019).
Figure 1.
PRISMA diagram, indicating the number of publications at each stage of the review process.
Results
In this section, we comprehensively summarise all empirical findings and null results from the microdosing literature. We identify five broad categories of findings: neurobiological, physiological, phenomenological, affective and cognitive. Here we focus on synthesising findings across these domains. In the following section, we evaluate what this evidence tells us about the likely mechanisms driving microdosing’s effects.
Table 1 summarises the design and key findings of all microdosing studies with controlled doses. This summary shows a large number of variables have been investigated, with numerous findings that differentiate microdosing from placebo, and also numerous reported null effects. 13/19 (68%) of these papers reported pre-registration in clinical trials databases (Bershad et al., 2019, 2020; Cavanna et al., 2022; Holze et al., 2021; Hutten et al., 2020, 2021; Marschall et al., 2022; Molla et al., 2023; Murphy et al., 2024; Ramaekers et al., 2021; Sanz et al., 2022; van Elk et al., 2022).
Key findings
Neurobiological: There have been six neuroimaging studies to date. These show fairly consistent evidence of neural changes related to microdosing. Murphy et al. (2024) failed to find direct evidence that LSD changed evoked responses to stimuli in an EEG visual long-term potentiation paradigm but did find that dynamic causal modelling of cortical activity showed changes in inhibitory feedforward responses consistent with enhanced neural plasticity. In an fMRI study, Bershad et al. (2020) showed that microdosing LSD led to changes in neural connectivity across amygdala and cerebellum regions that may be implicated in depression. In an EEG experiment, Murray et al. (2022, 2024) showed that low doses of LSD led to reduced resting state activity in the default mode network and increased neural complexity in a manner consistent with findings from studies high-dose psychedelics (Gattuso et al., 2022). Murray et al. (2022) also reported reduced error rates in a visual oddball paradigm utilising face stimuli, suggesting that microdoses of LSD may lead to changes in facial or emotional processing. Cavanna et al. (2022) showed comparable reduced EEG resting state power following microdoses of psilocybin but did not find differences in auditory oddball ERP responses. Finally, in an ERP study, Glazer et al. (2022) showed increased neural responses to reward processing in an LSD microdosing condition compared to placebo.
Physiological: Studies also showed that microdosing impacts other physiological and biological processes. In particular, microdosing LSD appears to increase both pain tolerance (Ramaekers et al., 2021) and levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF; Hutten et al., 2021). There was no evidence that microdosing impacts subjective sleep quality (Hutten et al., 2020), general levels of physical activity measured by an app-based fitness tracker (Cavanna et al., 2022), or balance (Family et al., 2020). Findings related to blood pressure and heart rate were mixed: several LSD studies found no changes (Bershad et al., 2020; de Wit et al., 2022; Family et al., 2020; Murphy et al., 2023; Ramaekers et al., 2021), but three LSD studies did report either increased blood pressure or heart rate (Bershad et al., 2019; Molla et al., 2023; Murray et al., 2022). There were no data on blood pressure or heart rate changes in psilocybin studies. We note that no studies have investigated the long-term safety of microdosing. There are concerns that chronic use of 5-HT2B receptor agonists, even at low doses, may have negative impacts on cardiac health (Rouaud et al., 2024; Tagen et al., 2023), and so this is an important question to address.
Phenomenological: There is consistent evidence showing that microdosing of both LSD and psilocybin changes individuals’ acute conscious state. In particular, VAS ratings of feeling ‘under the influence’, ‘good drug effects’ ‘subjective intensity’, ‘happy’ and ‘productive’ were reliably increased following microdosing (Cavanna et al., 2022; Holze et al., 2021; Hutten et al., 2020; Murphy et al., 2023; Yanakieva et al., 2019). Ratings using standardised measures of consciousness alteration (the 5D-ASC, 11D-ASC or Ego Dissolution Inventory) were less clear. There were indications that microdosing both LSD and psilocybin impacted scores on ‘oceanic boundlessness’, ‘dread of ego dissolution’, ‘visionary restructuralisation’, ‘vigilance reduction’, ‘anxiety’, ‘experience of unity’, ‘blissful state’, ‘changed meanings of percepts’, ‘insightfulness’, ‘complex imagery’ and ‘impaired cognition and control’, but these findings were not consistent across all of the studies that used the ASC scales (Bershad et al., 2019, 2020; de Wit et al., 2022; Family et al., 2020; Hutten et al., 2020; Molla et al., 2023; Murray et al., 2024). Molla et al. (2023) reported that healthy participants who scored relatively high on a measure of depressive symptoms scored higher on ‘spiritual experience’, ‘blissful state’, ‘insightfulness’, ‘oceanic boundlessness’ and ‘disembodiment’ following an LSD microdose. None of the studies found evidence that microdosing increased altered state dimensions related to ‘ego dissolution’, ‘elemental imagery’ or ‘synaesthesia’. Similarly, studies using the Addiction Research Centre Inventory (ARCI; Haertzen et al., 1963) all showed that LSD microdosing scored higher than placebo but across different subscales in different studies (Bershad et al., 2020; Bershad et al., 2019; de Wit et al., 2022; Molla et al., 2023; Murray et al., 2022).
Affective: LSD microdosing was consistently shown to increase acute mood states related to feelings of vigour (Bershad et al., 2019; de Wit et al., 2022; Hutten et al., 2020; Molla et al., 2023; Murphy et al., 2023). There were also indications of increases in mood state scores related to ‘friendliness’, ‘anxiety’, ‘elation’, ‘depression’, ‘anger’, ‘fatigue’ and ‘confusion’, but these were not found across all studies (Hutten et al., 2020; Molla et al., 2023; Murphy et al., 2023; Murray et al., 2022). Murphy et al. (2023) found some evidence of acute increased positive mood states and decreased negative mood states only on the days that participants took an LSD microdose but little evidence of persisting mood changes after a period of 6 weeks of microdosing. By contrast, Molla et al. (2023) found positive mood impacts immediately after ingesting LSD and also 48 h post-dosing. These changes were particularly pronounced for individuals higher in depressive symptoms at baseline. Psilocybin microdosing did lead to increased perception of awe but did not lead to changes in aesthetic experience (van Elk et al., 2022). Neither LSD nor psilocybin microdosing led to changes on the Positive and Negative Affect Scale (Bershad et al., 2019; Cavanna et al., 2022), Emotional Images Task (Bershad et al., 2019; de Wit et al., 2022) nor an emotion-based go/no go task (Marschall et al., 2022).
Cognitive: There have been some intriguing indications that LSD microdosing may impact cognitive functioning, in particular leading to changes in time perception (Yanakieva et al., 2019) and reduced attentional lapses (Hutten et al., 2020). There were also mixed findings related to social cognition, with one study showing reduced negative social processing during a cyberball task (de Wit et al., 2022) and one showing no changes (Bershad et al., 2019). Psilocybin microdosing did lead to changes in language production, characterised by increased verbosity and sentiment scores (Sanz et al., 2022). However, these findings must be interpreted cautiously as several studies failed to find any evidence that microdosing impacts performance on standard cognitive batteries (Bershad et al., 2019; Cavanna et al., 2022; de Wit et al., 2022; Family et al., 2020; Murphy et al., 2023), creativity tasks (Bershad et al., 2019; Cavanna et al., 2022; Molla et al., 2023), suggestibility (Cavanna et al., 2022), or self-representation (van Elk et al., 2022).
Mental health: Only four studies investigated measures related to wellbeing or mental health. Molla et al. (2023) found that participants with relatively high rates of depressive symptoms showed reductions in depression following an LSD microdose (but not following placebo). Murphy et al. (2023) found no difference in depression anxiety stress scale (DASS) depression, anxiety or stress scores following LSD microdosing. Similarly, Marschall et al. (2022) found no difference in DASS scores following psilocybin microdosing. Finally, Cavanna et al. (2022) found no difference in wellbeing or any change in state or trait anxiety following psilocybin microdosing. It is worth noting, however, that all of these studies recruited healthy samples (see section ‘Studies have only looked at non-clinical populations’).
Risk of bias
A summary of risk of bias for the reviewed studies is shown in Figure 2. Overall, there was fairly low risk of bias across the reviewed literature, with studies on psilocybin appearing to be particularly rigorous. Across all studies, there was low risk of bias related to randomisation process, missing outcome data, and measurement of outcomes. The risk of bias analysis highlighted that, overall, blinding was poor across the reviewed studies. Specifically, 12/19 studies assessed blinding, and 11 of these reported that participants in at least one condition broke blind at rates greater than chance. However, there was no evidence that breaking blind led to any deviations in intended interventions, and so according to the Cochrane risk-of-bias algorithm, this was a low risk of bias. We believe that the relationship between blinding, expectations and outcomes is particularly nuanced and complex in the context of microdosing, and these issues are discussed in detail below. There were some concerns of bias in selection of reported results. This was due to a lack of pre-registered analyses for 14/19 studies (74%). Despite the majority of studies having clinical trial registrations, many of these did not include details on what statistical tests would be performed. A lack of open science practices has been identified as a particular problem for psychedelic science (Petranker et al., 2020) and was also evident in this review of microdosing.
Figure 2.
Risk of bias (Higgins et al., 2019).
Table is ordered alphabetically, based on the first published paper for each dataset.
Discussion
This review highlights a range of neurobiological, physiological, phenomenological, cognitive, and affective changes associated with microdosing psychedelics in placebo-controlled studies (see Table 1). On the one hand, this set of findings appears to indicate that microdosing is having some effects. The most compelling or reliable effects include neurobiological changes, changes in acute conscious state, increased feelings of vigour and increased pain tolerance.
Table 1 also shows that there are several variables that do not appear to differ between microdosing and placebo conditions. These results indicate that microdosing may not have beneficial effects on creativity or cognition despite these being the main benefits reported in anecdotes and media stories.
However, a methodological challenge for many microdosing studies is that the success of blinding methods is largely unknown, making any distinction between drug and expectancy effects difficult. Twelve studies assessed participants’ ability to guess their experimental condition, and these indicated only partial success of the blind in the drug condition (see Polito and Liknaitzky, 2022 for further discussion of blinding issues in microdosing research). No microdosing studies to date have used an active placebo. As the majority of studies reviewed here indicated significant subjective effects in the microdosing condition only, it is likely that a substantial proportion of participants in these studies were able to identify whether they had taken a microdose. Relatedly, Szigeti et al. (2021) reported that participants’ beliefs about what they had taken had a stronger influence on outcomes than their actual experimental condition.
Given the null findings reviewed above, difficulty blinding, and a small number of studies that suggest a larger role for expectancy than drug effects with microdosing, it is understandable that scepticism has dampened some of the early enthusiasm for the effects and potential usefulness of microdosing, at least within the scientific community. However, in our view, there is currently insufficient evidence to be confident that the effects attributed to microdosing are drug or placebo effects or some combination of both. Instead, the field is nascent, with good reasons for both scepticism and enthusiasm, with considerable need for more research. Below, we present eight reasons that one ought to be cautious about jumping to conclusions regarding the mechanisms driving current findings.
Only a small number of studies
First, there is a relatively small amount of empirical data to draw conclusions from. Although there have been 19 papers reporting dose-controlled microdosing studies, several of these papers have come from the same datasets. There have been just 10 independent dose-controlled microdosing experiments conducted by just six different labs (see Table 1). Only two of these experiments have investigated psilocybin (Cavanna et al., 2022; Marschall et al., 2022; Sanz et al., 2022; van Elk et al., 2022). Furthermore, the substance, doses, measures, and methods used have varied considerably across these studies, meaning that there have not been many directly replicated findings across this literature.
Studies have small sample sizes
Second, sample sizes in these controlled studies have been small. The average number of participants in microdosing conditions across all 10 experiments was 31. If there are true pharmacological effects of microdosing, these are likely to be relatively small (certainly smaller than the effect sizes found in high-dose psychedelic studies). To detect such effects, larger samples are likely to be needed.
Evidence of dose-dependent effects
Third, these studies have reported a range of outcomes that differ between microdosing and placebo conditions. For example, there is consistent evidence that both LSD and psilocybin microdosing lead to changes in neurophysiology and subjective effects. Of particular note, studies with LSD that included multiple doses within the microdosing range consistently showed dose-dependent effects. This was the case for both psychological (e.g., Hutten et al., 2020) and neurophysiological variables (e.g., Murray et al., 2022). This suggests pharmacology is impacting certain outcomes, distinct from any expectancy effects. So far, there have not been any psilocybin studies comparing multiple doses. Further, we note that although the findings summarised in this review are broadly compatible between LSD and psilocybin microdosing, psilocybin microdosing has been less well studied and there may turn out to be substance-specific effects that do not generalise between these substances.
Studies have only investigated a small number of doses
Fourth, most of these dose-controlled studies have investigated the acute effects of a single microdose. Only two psilocybin studies have investigated the effects of cumulative dosing: Marschall et al. (2022) and van Elk et al. (2022) reported on the cumulative effects of 5–7 doses of psilocybin taken over 3 weeks, and Cavanna et al. (2022) and Sanz et al. (2022) reported on the cumulative effects of 2 doses of psilocybin taken over a single week. Three LSD experiments have looked at cumulative dosing: de Wit et al. (2022) reported on the cumulative effects of four doses of LSD taken over 3 weeks, and Yanakieva et al. (2019) and Family et al. (2020) reported on the effects of 4 or 6 doses of LSD taken over 2 weeks. Only Murphy et al. (2023) have investigated the effects of microdosing for a period longer than a month. They reported on the cumulative effects of 14 doses of LSD taken over 6 weeks. Although it is scientifically interesting to explore the effects of a single dose or a small number of doses, findings from studies focused on short-term microdosing may have limited generalisability to the reported benefits of microdosing in naturalistic settings, which are generally associated with recurrent dosing for many weeks or months. Like pharmaceutical serotonergic medications, microdoses may have long-term cumulative effects. Studies to date have not investigated this possibility. As a comparison, if we were to assess changes to an individual’s mood after administration of a single dose of traditional antidepressant medication, we would be unlikely to find any effect, even though long-term use of that medication may lead to significant improvement. This may explain the apparent lack of mood and mental health benefits in these studies, despite common reports of such effects in ‘the wild’.
Doses investigated may be too small
Fifth, studies of psilocybin may have investigated doses that are too low for therapeutic or cognitive enhancement effects. Determining the appropriate doses for microdosing research is complex: the appropriate dose range is likely to be quite narrow, being high enough to produce meaningful changes but low enough to be sub-hallucinogenic and without functional impairment. However, people appear to show wide variability in dose response to psychedelics, implying that optimal microdoses and any associated benefits may depend on precise individual tailoring. Consequently, it is possible that many microdosing studies have used inadequately small doses to produce meaningful changes (see Polito and Liknaitzky, 2022 for related discussion on bidirectional effects). In particular, only two psilocybin experiments were included, one of which used psilocybin truffles with the equivalent of 0.8 mg synthetic psilocybin (Cavanna et al., 2022; Sanz et al., 2022), the other used truffles with the equivalent of 1.5 mg synthetic psilocybin (Marschall et al., 2022; van Elk et al., 2022). Madsen et al. (2019) reported pharmacokinetic analyses of low doses of psilocybin, showing that the peak plasma psilocin concentration following ingestion of 3 mg synthetic psilocybin was just 2 µg/L. Inferring from these results, it seems likely that the doses investigated in the psilocybin microdosing studies (0.8 and 1.5 mg psilocybin) would lead to psilocin concentration levels of approximately 1 µg/L or less. This may not be sufficient for meaningful psychopharmacological effects.
Studies have only looked at non-clinical populations
Sixth, all of the microdosing studies reviewed here investigated non-clinical volunteers. Findings across these samples were mostly not supportive of microdosing improving mental or physical health variables. However, Molla et al. (2023) compared healthy volunteers with high and low rates of depressive symptoms at baseline. They found improvements in depression and mood states immediately after taking an LSD microdose and 48 h later for the high depressive symptoms group only. This suggests that the general lack of mental health improvements across the reviewed studies may be explained by ceiling effects at the group level (e.g., the limited ability for any intervention to improve levels of depression in a non-depressed sample). Indeed, self-report data on microdosing indicates significant clinical benefits (Haijen et al., 2022; e.g., Hutten et al., 2019; Lea et al., 2020; Lyes et al., 2022). These claims can only be validly tested in controlled clinical samples, and this research has not yet been done.
Selection bias
Seventh, the studies reviewed involve considerable levels of selection bias. Specifically, all but one of these studies either recruited volunteers with prior experience of psychedelics or recruited from community events organised by psychedelic education organisations. This means that it is likely that participants across all of these studies had well-formed expectations and beliefs about the efficacy of psychedelics that may differ from psychedelic-naïve individuals. These expectations may have influenced results in several ways. For example, experienced psychedelic users may have been more able to distinguish when they were in a placebo condition and, therefore, more disappointed. Studies in more representative samples would provide a clearer test of potential pharmacological effects with less confounding effects related to beliefs and expectations.
Measured impact of expectancy is small
Eighth, although several papers have suggested that the effects of microdosing may be largely due to placebo and expectation effects (Cavanna et al., 2022; Kaertner et al., 2021; Szigeti et al., 2021; van Elk et al., 2022), when these effects are measured directly, findings are at best mixed. The strongest evidence for the claim that expectations drive the reported effects of microdosers comes from (a) Cavanna et al. (2022), who showed that participants who broke blind reported greater microdosing effects compared to those who remained blinded and (b) Szigeti et al. (2021), who showed that participants’ guess as to whether they had taken a microdose or placebo had a much greater impact on outcomes than whether or not they had actually consumed a microdose. These results are compelling; however, it is notable that Cavanna et al. may have used insufficient doses for pharmacological effects (0.8 mg psilocybin; see section ‘Doses investigated may be too small’) and Szigeti et al. (2021) was an observational study with unknown dosing. Additional evidence for expectancy effects comes from Kaertner et al. (2021), who found that baseline expectations predicted mood and wellbeing outcomes in an observational, prospective microdosing study. However, the proportion of variance explained by expectations was only 5%–8%, suggesting that this is not a primary mechanism for explaining the outcomes of microdosing. Similarly, the other studies reviewed here do not provide strong evidence for expectation effects. In the study of Leiden University (reported by Marschall et al., 2022; van Elk et al., 2022), the role of expectation was inconsistent: expectation did predict feelings of awe but did not predict mood or interoception. Finally, Hutten et al. (2020) reported a clear disconnect between expectations and outcomes on a cognitive vigilance task, with the majority of participants increasing accuracy in the microdosing condition but reporting expectations that their performance had deteriorated. Overall, based on the current data, it seems that although expectations likely have an influence on at least some microdosing outcomes, there is no compelling evidence to suggest that this is the primary mechanism for the majority of reported effects in these studies or in the wild.
Conclusion
So, is microdosing a placebo? This is a question that seems to evoke strong opinions among psychedelic researchers. A microdosing sceptic will look at the results in Table 1 and argue that all or most of the effects that have been reported are due to expectation and placebo effects. Ultimately, that may turn out to be correct. However, we argue that based on current data, there is no strong evidence for a placebo interpretation of the effects of microdosing. Specifically, there has only been a small number (section ‘Only a small number of studies’) of low-powered studies (section ‘Studies have small sample sizes’), with methodological concerns including selection bias (section ‘Selection bias’) and problematically small doses (section ‘Doses investigated may be too small’). Additionally, most research has looked only into the acute effects of microdosing in healthy populations – almost nothing is known about the sustained impacts of a course of microdoses in a controlled setting (section ‘Studies have only investigated a small number of doses’), and we have no data at all on potential clinical effects (section ‘Studies have only looked at non-clinical populations’). These issues mean that research to date may not have been sensitive enough to detect subtle pharmacological effects of low doses. Nevertheless, even within this restricted set of data there is considerable evidence of dose-dependent changes that do suggest microdosing drug effects (section ‘Evidence of dose-dependent effects’). Finally, studies that have directly investigated the role of expectation have not found consistent evidence that participants’ beliefs are the primary driver of outcomes (section ‘Measured impact of expectancy is small’), undermining the case for a placebo interpretation.
Overall, in light of consistent reports of benefits from self-report studies (e.g., Anderson et al., 2019; Cameron et al., 2020; Hutten et al., 2019; Lea et al., 2020; Polito and Stevenson, 2019; Rootman et al., 2021, 2022) and lack of clear evidence on the role placebo in controlled studies to date, further microdosing research is warranted. To definitively determine what is driving the positive effects reported by microdosers, we need well-powered, longitudinal studies across both healthy and clinical populations.
Acknowledgments
We thank Oscar Nagy and Matthew Downing for their assistance in collating the information in Table 1.
Footnotes
The author(s) declared the following potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: Vince Polito has received research funding from Mydecine Innovations Group and Woke Pharmaceuticals. Paul Liknaitzky has received research funding from Incannex Healthcare Ltd, the Multidisciplinary Association for Psychedelic Studies, and Beckley Psytech and is a member of the Medical Advisory Board of Incannex Healthcare Ltd and the Scientific Advisory Board of The MIND Foundation. These organisations were not involved in any aspect of this paper, including the decision to write it, drafting the paper, or its publication.
Funding: The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
ORCID iD: Vince Polito
https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3242-9074
References
- Anderson T, Petranker R, Christopher A, et al. (2019) Psychedelic microdosing benefits and challenges: An empirical codebook. Harm Reduct J 16: 43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bershad A, Preller K, Lee R, et al. (2020) Preliminary report on the effects of a low dose of LSD on resting-state amygdala functional connectivity. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 5: 461–467. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bershad A, Schepers ST, Bremmer MP, et al. (2019) Acute subjective and behavioral effects of microdoses of lysergic acid diethylamide in healthy human volunteers. Biol Psychiatry 86: 792–800. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cameron L, Nazarian A, Olson D. (2020) Psychedelic microdosing: Prevalence and subjective effects. J Psychoact Drugs 52: 113–122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cavanna F, Muller S, de la Fuente LA, et al. (2022) Microdosing with psilocybin mushrooms: A double-blind placebo-controlled study. Transl Psychiatry 12: 307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Wit H, Molla HM, Bershad A, et al. (2022) Repeated low doses of LSD in healthy adults: A placebo-controlled, dose–response study. Addict Biol 27: e13143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dolan EW. (2022, February 8) Psilocybin microdosing does not reduce symptoms of depression or anxiety, according to placebo-controlled study. PsyPost. Available at: https://www.psypost.org/2022/02/psilocybin-microdosing-does-not-reduce-symptoms-of-depression-or-anxiety-according-to-placebo-controlled-study-62495
- Family N, Maillet E, Williams L, et al. (2020) Safety, tolerability, pharmacokinetics, and pharmacodynamics of low dose lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) in healthy older volunteers. Psychopharmacology 237: 841–853. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gattuso JJ, Perkins D, Ruffell S, et al. (2022) Default mode network modulation by psychedelics: A systematic review. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 26: 155–188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glazer J, Murray CH, Nusslock R, et al. (2022) Low doses of lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD) increase reward-related brain activity. Neuropsychopharmacology 48: 418–426. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths R, Johnson M, Richards W, et al. (2018) Psilocybin-occasioned mystical-type experience in combination with meditation and other spiritual practices produces enduring positive changes in psychological functioning and in trait measures of prosocial attitudes and behaviors. J Psychopharmacol 32: 49–69. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haertzen CA, Hill HE, Belleville RE. (1963) Development of the Addiction Research Center Inventory (ARCI): Selection of items that are sensitive to the effects of various drugs. Psychopharmacologia 4: 155–166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haijen ECHM, Hurks PPM, Kuypers KPC. (2022) Microdosing with psychedelics to self-medicate for ADHD symptoms in adults: A prospective naturalistic study. Neurosci Appl 1: 101012. [Google Scholar]
- Haridy R. (2022, January 25) No difference between psilocybin microdosing and placebo, trial finds. New Atlas. Available at: https://newatlas.com/science/psilocybin-microdosing-equals-placebo-clinical-trial/
- Higgins JPT, Savović J, Page MJ, et al. (2019) Assessing risk of bias in a randomized trial. In: JPT Higgins, J Thomas, J Chandler, et al. (eds.) Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. 1st edn. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley, pp. 205–228. [Google Scholar]
- Holze F, Liechti M, Hutten N, et al. (2021) Pharmacokinetics and pharmacodynamics of lysergic acid diethylamide microdoses in healthy participants. Clin Pharmacol Thera 109: 658–666. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hutten NR, Mason N, Dolder P, et al. (2019) Self-rated effectiveness of microdosing with psychedelics for mental and physical health problems amongst microdosers. Front Psychiatry 10: 672. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hutten NR, Mason N, Dolder P, et al. (2020) Mood and cognition after administration of low LSD doses in healthy volunteers: A placebo controlled dose-effect finding study. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 41: 81–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hutten NR, Mason N, Dolder P, et al. (2021) Low doses of LSD acutely increase BDNF blood plasma levels in healthy volunteers. ACS Pharmacol Transl Sci 4: 461–466. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnstad PG. (2018) Powerful substances in tiny amounts: An interview study of psychedelic microdosing. Nordic Stud Alcohol Drugs 35: 39–51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaertner LS, Steinborn MB, Kettner H, et al. (2021) Positive expectations predict improved mental-health outcomes linked to psychedelic microdosing. Scient Rep 11: 1941. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lea T, Amada N, Jungaberle H, et al. (2020) Perceived outcomes of psychedelic microdosing as self-managed therapies for mental and substance use disorders. Psychopharmacology 237: 1521–1532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leonard A. (2015, November 20) How LSD microdosing became the hot new business trip. Rolling Stone. Avaiable at: https://www.rollingstone.com/culture/features/how-lsd-microdosing-became-the-hot-new-business-trip-20151120
- Lyes M, Yang KH, Castellanos J, et al. (2022) Microdosing psilocybin for chronic pain: A case series. Pain 164: 698–702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Madsen MK, Fisher PM, Burmester D, et al. (2019) Psychedelic effects of psilocybin correlate with serotonin 2A receptor occupancy and plasma psilocin levels. Neuropsychopharmacology 44: 1328–1334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marschall J, Fejer G, Lempe P, et al. (2022) Psilocybin microdosing does not affect emotion-related symptoms and processing: A preregistered field and lab-based study. J Psychopharmacol 36: 97–133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Molla H, Lee R, Tare I, et al. (2023) Greater subjective effects of a low dose of LSD in participants with depressed mood. Neuropsychopharmacology 49: 774–781. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy RJ, Godfrey K, Shaw AD, et al. (2024) Modulation of long-term potentiation following microdoses of LSD captured by thalamo-cortical modelling in a randomised, controlled trial. BMC Neurosci 25: 7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy RJ, Sumner R, Evans W, et al. (2023) Acute mood-elevating properties of microdosed LSD in healthy volunteers: A home-administered randomised controlled trial. Biol Psychiatry 94: 511–521. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray CH, Frohlich J, Haggarty CJ, et al. (2024) Neural complexity is increased after low doses of LSD, but not moderate to high doses of oral THC or methamphetamine. Neuropsychopharmacology 49: 1–9. DOI: 10.1038/s41386-024-01809-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray CH, Tare I, Perry CM, et al. (2022) Low doses of LSD reduce broadband oscillatory power and modulate event-related potentials in healthy adults. Psychopharmacology 239: 1735–1747. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petranker R, Anderson T, Farb N. (2020) Psychedelic research and the need for transparency: Polishing alice’s looking glass. Front Psychol 11: 1681. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polito V, Liknaitzky P. (2022) The emerging science of microdosing: A systematic review of research on low dose psychedelics (1955–2021) and recommendations for the field. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 139: 104706. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polito V, Stevenson RJ. (2019) A systematic study of microdosing psychedelics. PLoS One 14: e0211023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prochazkova L, Lippelt DP, Colzato LS, et al. (2018) Exploring the effect of microdosing psychedelics on creativity in an open-label natural setting. Psychopharmacology 235: 3401–3413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramaekers J, Hutten N, Mason N, et al. (2021) A low dose of lysergic acid diethylamide decreases pain perception in healthy volunteers. J Psychopharmacol 35: 4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rootman JM, Kiraga M, Kryskow P, et al. (2022) Psilocybin microdosers demonstrate greater observed improvements in mood and mental health at one month relative to non-microdosing controls. Scient Rep 12: 11091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rootman JM, Kryskow P, Harvey K, et al. (2021) Adults who microdose psychedelics report health related motivations and lower levels of anxiety and depression compared to non-microdosers. Scient Rep 11: 22479. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rouaud A, Calder AE, Hasler G. (2024) Microdosing psychedelics and the risk of cardiac fibrosis and valvulopathy: Comparison to known cardiotoxins. J Psychopharmacol 38: 217–224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanz C, Cavanna F, Muller S, et al. (2022) Natural language signatures of psilocybin microdosing. Psychopharmacology 239: 2841–2852. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siebert A. (2021) Largest study of psychedelics shows benefits of microdosing could be placebo effect. Forbes. Available at: https://www.forbes.com/sites/amandasiebert/2021/03/03/largest-study-of-psychedelics-shows-benefits-of-microdosing-could-be-placebo-effect/
- Smith DG. (2022, February 28) More people are microdosing for mental health. But does it work? The New York Times. Available at: https://www.nytimes.com/2022/02/28/well/mind/microdosing-psychelics.html
- Szigeti B, Kartner L, Blemings A, et al. (2021) Self-blinding citizen science to explore psychedelic microdosing. eLife 10: e62878. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tagen M, Mantuani D, van Heerden L, et al. (2023) The risk of chronic psychedelic and MDMA microdosing for valvular heart disease. J Psychopharmacol 37: 876–890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Elk M, Fejer G, Lempe P, et al. (2022) Effects of psilocybin microdosing on awe and aesthetic experiences: A preregistered field and lab-based study. Psychopharmacology 239: 1705–1720. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Winstock AR, Timmerman C, Davies E, et al. (2020) Global Drug Survey (GDS) 2020 psychedelics key findings report. Available at: https://www.globaldrugsurvey.com/wp-content/uploads/2021/03/GDS2020-Psychedelics-report.pdf
- Yanakieva S, Polychroni N, Family N, et al. (2019) The effects of microdose LSD on time perception: A randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Psychopharmacology 236: 1159–1170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]