Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2024 Aug 20;14:19224. doi: 10.1038/s41598-024-68328-4

A rank ordering and analysis of four cognitive-behavioral stress-management competencies suggests that proactive stress management is especially valuable

Robert Epstein 1,, Jessica Aceret 1, Ciara Giordani 1, Vanessa R Zankich 1, Lynette Zhang 1
PMCID: PMC11333707  PMID: 39160168

Abstract

The main objective of this study was to determine the relative value of four cognitive-behavioral competencies that have been shown in empirical studies to be associated with effective stress management. Based on a review of relevant psychological literature, we named the competencies as follows: Manages or Reduces Sources of Stress, Manages Thoughts, Plans and Prevents, and Practices Relaxation Techniques. We measured their relative value by examining data obtained from a diverse convenience sample of 18,895 English-speaking participants in 125 countries (65.0% from the U.S. and Canada) who completed a new inventory of stress-management competencies. We assessed their relative value by employing a concurrent study design, which also allowed us to assess the validity of the new instrument. Regression analyses were used to rank order the four competencies according to how well they predicted desirable outcomes. Both regression and factor analyses pointed to the importance of proactive stress-management practices over reactive methods, but we note that the correlational design of our study has no implications for the possible causal effects of these methods. Questionnaire scores were strongly associated with self-reported happiness and also significantly associated with personal success, professional success, and general level of stress. Data were collected between 2007 and 2022, but we found no effect for time. The study supports the value of stress-management training, and it also suggests that moderate levels of stress may not be as beneficial as previously thought.

Keywords: Stress management, Epstein stress management inventory, ESMI, Stress-management skills, Stress-management competencies, Online tests

Subject terms: Psychology, Human behaviour

Introduction

The main objectives of the present study were to (a) introduce and evaluate a new instrument—the Epstein Stress Management Inventory for individuals (ESMI-i)—for assessing four cognitive-behavioral competencies that have been shown in empirical studies to be associated with effective stress management, (b) analyze data from a large, international group of English-speaking people who completed the new questionnaire online, (c) compare the relative value of the four competencies, and (d) analyze the data from a demographic perspective. By using a competencies approach, we are providing both the public and practitioners with a practical tool for measuring and potentially improving practices associated with effective stress management.

The ESMI-i joins a large number of test instruments and tools that have been developed since the 1950s to help people deal with various kinds of mental health challenges. It is also fairly unique in some respects. For one thing, it was designed for the general population, rather than for a particular group, and it was designed to measure broad competency areas rather than skills that might be helpful mainly in specific contexts. Similar inventories have been designed, for example, to assist individuals who regularly face stress in their work environments13. Other inventories have been designed to measure people’s coping skills in response to specific stressors4,5.

The ESMI-i is also available online, non-commercial, and free of charge, maintained by a nonprofit organization. Validated instruments measuring the “level” of stress people feel, such as the Holmes and Rahe Stress Scale6 and the Perceived Stress Scale7, are currently available online, and so are numerous non-validated tests of this sort, accessible at websites such as OkCupid.com. Validated inventories that assess “coping styles” also exist, such as the Multidimensional Coping Inventory8. These instruments differ from the ESMI-i in that they are designed to classify people, while we are careful to avoid labeling those who complete the ESMI-i; we will explore this matter later in more detail.

Other validated inventories measure cognitive-behavioral skills and overlap to some extent with the ESMI-i. However, most of these instruments were developed with cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) in mind and thus may not be ideal measures of stress-management competencies per se. In addition, many of these inventories, such as the Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy Skills Questionnaire9, the Skills of Cognitive Therapy measure10, and the CBT Skills Checklist11, include items that sometimes conflate skills with reductions in symptoms of depression12.

Validated inventories that measure stress-management or coping skills exist, such as the Proactive Coping Inventory13,14 , Chronic Pain Coping Inventory1519, the Coping Inventory for Stressful Situations2022, the COPE Inventory2325 , the Coping with Stress Scale26 , the Coping Intelligence Questionnaire 27, the Dispositional Resilience Scale28,29, the Stress Mindset Scale3032, and the Performance of Cognitive Therapy Strategies measure33. However, they are either not available online or, in some cases, they can only be administered by licensed professionals or trained observers12. Because many people are now relying on the internet as a major resource for self-evaluation3437, we believe that it is important to make validated tests widely available online. Self-help books that teach similar stress-management techniques are available to members of the general public, such as Mind Over Mood38; however, such books are not free, and they take much longer to read than it takes to complete an online inventory.

By stress, we are referring to internal, usually unpleasant physiological and psychological states that are often induced by perceived environmental threats or environmental demands, which are sometimes called “stressors”3943. Actual environmental threats do not necessarily produce stress reactions, and the same stressor can cause different stress reactions in different people—or even no stress reaction at all40,41.

We are not concerned in the present paper with the definitional ambiguities in the terms “stress” and “stressor.” Rather, we are focusing on stress-management practices—thoughts and behaviors that reduce stress—and we are especially interested in practices of this sort that can be both measured and trained by coaches, therapists, or counselors. The instrument we developed focuses on four classes of such behaviors; we define each class of behaviors to be a stress-management competency. The term “coping skills” is sometimes used to describe practices of this sort44. For purposes of the present discussion, we will avoid using that term, as well as the related term “coping strategies,”45, because we view these terms as referring mainly to reactive practices. In the present study, we will be measuring both reactive and proactive competencies, and we will use our data to compare the relative value of each type.

We believe that it is important to identify and measure stress-management competencies—especially those that can be trained—because of their enormous practical value. Stress-management competencies not only reduce levels of reported stress but have also been associated with increased functioning and well-being, as well as with improvements in mental, emotional, and physical health4664. Unmanaged stress is costly, both in personal and economic terms60,6570. Fortunately, stress management can be trained, and benefits of such training have been demonstrated46,7177. Levels of stress can also be measured7883, and so can stress-management proficiency17,18,21,24,27,28,30,8487.

The nature and value of a competencies approach

Many, if not most, test instruments used by psychologists are based on theories, and those theories are often about hypothetical constructs such as intelligence or personality traits. The methodology for developing and evaluating such instruments is quite advanced. Factor and item analyses are often employed, for example, to remove items that do not improve the statistical validity of the construct measures.

The ESMI-i is not a theory-based questionnaire, and it does not introduce or attempt to measure constructs. The ESMI-i is a competency test, developed in the spirit of a testing approach strongly advocated by David McClelland, notably in a seminal paper published in The American Psychologist in 197388; other experts have also been strong advocates of this approach over the years89,90. A competencies approach to understanding and improving human performance is widely used in multiple fields and arenas—by the military91 , in healthcare92, in business93,94 , in education95, and in other areas in which human performance is important96101 .

As McClelland and others have pointed out, a competencies approach to understanding human functioning has some practical advantages over more traditional psychological approaches. Before he and his colleagues applied this approach to the study of leadership, for example, both businesses and armies had long been searching for “natural born leaders,” and such people exist, of course102 . But leadership, along with many other areas of human functioning—even intelligence88—can be broken down into a number of skill areas that not only can be observed and measured; they can often be trained. Those skill areas, such as the set of skills one needs to drive a car, are not hypothetical, and they are also not constructs. They are sets of behaviors, many of which are observable. Tests that measure traits or constructs often leave people with labels, such as the trait measures yielded by the ubiquitous Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory, and labels can be both demoralizing103,104 and self-fulfilling105108. In contrast, competency scores simply tell people where they stand at the moment; they are often used in combination with training programs that employ questionnaires to measure post-training improvements.

Development of the ESMI-i questionnaire

The ESMI-i measures “competencies,” a term that is typically defined as “the knowledge, skills, abilities, and behaviors that contribute to individual and organizational performance”109—specifically those competencies that are mainly cognitive or behavioral in nature and that might help people to reduce, eliminate, or avoid stress. Beginning in 2001, the first author of this paper, with the help of his students, set about searching the psychology literature to find peer-reviewed papers that identified skills, behavior, or knowledge that were associated with successful stress management, the goal being to use these papers to develop a test instrument that could measure the strength of such competencies. We were especially interested in competencies of the sort that therapists or counselors might be likely to teach—those, as opposed to stress-management techniques that might be taught by medical personnel, nutritionists, or other experts.

Over time, we developed a questionnaire that measured four relatively distinct cognitive-behavioral competencies: Manages or Reduces Sources of Stress, Manages Thoughts, Plans and Prevents, and Practices Relaxation Techniques. Table 1 shows the competencies, definitions, the scored items for each competency, and a list of relevant references. Below are examples of how studies published between 2001 and 2007 were employed to develop the four competency categories and to compose a total of 24 scored items. Table 1 also includes relevant references found after the questionnaire was posted online in May of 2007.

Table 1.

Four empirically-supported stress-management competencies.

Manages or Reduces Sources of Stress: You routinely manage and reduce sources of stress in life

Proactive or reactive: Either

Corresponding items:

 “I’m comfortable seeking help from other people.”

 “I have trouble keeping my work area organized.” (reverse scored)

 “I consistently put important tasks ahead of unimportant tasks.”

 “I have adequate shelf, file, and drawer space to serve my needs.”

 “I have trouble delegating.” (reverse scored)

 “I try to schedule appointments and meetings so that they won’t overlap.”

References: 113,115,177,180,181

Manages Thoughts: You practice thought-management techniques in order to reduce the likelihood of perceiving an event negatively

Proactive or reactive: Either

Corresponding items:

 “I often reinterpret events in order to lower my stress.”

 “I ignore hostile people.”

 “Negative events can always be reinterpreted so that they seem more positive.”

 “I regularly examine and try to correct any irrational beliefs I might have.”

 “My thinking is as clear and as rational as it can possibly be.” (reverse scored)

 “I often try to use humor to diffuse tension.”

References: 30,49,54,55,64,69,118121,123,124,129,130,182186

Plans and Prevents: You take a proactive approach to stress management, plan day and life and avoid destructive ways of dealing with stress

Proactive or reactive: Proactive

Corresponding items:

 “I keep an up-to-date list of things I’m supposed to do.”

 “I have a clear picture of how I’d like my life to proceed.”

 “I try to fight stress before it starts.”

 “I try to avoid destructive ways of dealing with stress.”

 “I spend a few moments each morning planning my day.”

 “I’m very vulnerable to stress when it hits.” (reverse scored)

References: 22,59,67,73,118,119,121,123,129,130,185,187

Practices Relaxation Techniques: You regularly practice a variety of relaxation techniques to prevent or relieve stress

Proactive or reactive: Either

Corresponding items:

 “I frequently use special breathing techniques to help me relax.”

 “I frequently visualize soothing scenes in order to relax.”

 “I regularly tense and relax my muscles as a way of fighting stress.”

 “I sit and stand in special ways to help me stay relaxed.”

 “Breathing is a very hard thing to control.” (reverse scored)

 “I schedule relaxation time every day.”

References: 51,75,132,134,135,188193

When possible, we tried to create items that “pinpoint” specific behaviors. Items that pinpoint behavior are good predictors of actual behavior94,110112. So instead of saying, “I’m great at making people laugh,” we say, “I often try to use humor to diffuse tension.” That wording tells us about behavior and also, to some extent, about the frequency of that behavior. We were not able to do this for every item, but we used pinpointing as a standard for item composition.

Competency 1: manages or reduces sources of stress

In a year-long study with 100 adult residents of the Alameda County area of California, Folkman and Lazarus113 interviewed participants once every four weeks to determine what strategies they employed to help them cope with the stress they experienced as a result of activities of daily living. The researchers were guided by Richard Lazarus’ cognitive-phenomenological approach to analyzing psychological stress114. One of two types of coping strategies analyzed in the 1980 study was labeled “problem-focused” (p. 223), and it suggested, along with other studies (see below), that one robust category of stress management was managing sources of stress. In defining this strategy, the authors spoke of “management or alteration of the person-environment relationship that is the source of stress” (p. 223), “cognitive problem-solving efforts and behavioral strategies for altering or managing the source of the problem” (p. 224), and other actions that reduced or eliminated sources of stress.

We also used language from the Folkman and Lazarus113 study to help us construct one of our questionnaire items. In elaborating on problem-focused coping strategies, the authors stated that these strategies “include seeking information, trying to get help, inhibiting action, and taking direct action” (p. 229). Item 2 on the ESMI-i is, “I’m comfortable seeking help from other people” (Table 1).

Problem-focused coping strategies were also analyzed in a 2006 study with 67 families of young children with disabilities. Stoneman and Gavidia-Payne115 found that marital harmony was higher when fathers in these families employed problem-focused strategies to overcome challenges, thus reducing sources of stress.

Competency 2: manages thoughts

Thought management—often taught by counselors and therapists as part of therapeutic treatment—can be a powerful means for reducing or eliminating stress. “Reframing,” a technique most often associated with Albert Ellis116 and a main component of rational emotive behavior therapy117, is just one example of a thought-management technique. Others include cognitive restructuring118121, cognitive reappraisal47,54,64, cognitive redefinition26, and cognitive defusion122124. Most mindfulness techniques, such as acceptance and commitment therapy, incorporate methods for managing thoughts125. Cognitive-behavioral therapy, developed by Aaron Beck in the 1960s126,127, also emphasizes techniques aimed at reducing automatic thoughts and cognitive distortions128. Generally speaking, people have little control over the undesirable things that happen to them, but, in theory, they have—or could be trained to have—complete control over how they interpret such events; hence, the logic of using thoughts to manage stress.

Murphy120 conducted a meta-analysis of 64 studies that examined ways in which people managed workplace stress. The most common methods used to manage stress in work settings were meditation, muscle relaxation, cognitive-behavioral skills, and biofeedback. Cognitive-behavioral techniques, which included “thought restructuring” and other methods, proved to be especially effective in reducing what the author called “psychologic” stress (as opposed to “physiologic” stress). The Murphy study proved to be especially helpful in generating possible items for inclusion in the ESMI-i. Items based in part or in full on content from the Murphy study include, “I often reinterpret events in order to lower my stress” (item 5), “Negative events can always be reinterpreted so that they seem more positive” (item 9), and “I regularly examine and try to correct any irrational beliefs I might have” (item 10).

A comprehensive literature review by Giga et al.129 also proved helpful in developing our Manages Thoughts competency category (as well as the competency that follows—see below). Focusing again on the work environment, the authors found that thought restructuring, reframing, and similar techniques were helpful in managing stress. Language from the Giga et al. 129 study was helpful in developing items 5 and 9 (see above paragraph).

An experimental study by Keogh et al.119 also helped us develop this competency category. 209 students in the UK were randomly assigned to either a cognitive-behaviorally oriented treatment group or a no-treatment control group. Stress reduction was significantly higher in the treatment group, with students being taught, among other things, to replace irrational beliefs (such as “I am bad at taking tests” [p. 342]) with more rational ones (such as “I can take tests, if I prepare appropriately” [p. 342]). This study helped us to develop items such as the reverse-scored item, “My thinking is as clear and as rational as it can possibly be” (item 11), as well as item 10 (noted above).

Competency 3: plans and prevents

Once again, the Giga et al.129 literature review was helpful in developing this category. It spoke specifically about the value of “plan[ning] to prevent and manage stress,” and it helped us develop two items: “I have a clear picture of how I’d like my life to proceed” (item 15) and “I try to fight stress before it starts” (item 17).

McWilliams et al.22 studied a group of 298 outpatients with major depressive disorder, having them complete multiple questionnaires, such as the Coping Inventory for Stressful Situations21. They concluded that planning and scheduling, among other strategies, were associated with lower levels of psychological stress. Content from this article helped us compose two ESMI-i items: “I keep an up-to-date list of things I’m supposed to do” (item 7), and “I spend a few moments each morning planning my day” (item 22). In addition, the Folkman et al. study130, mentioned earlier, helped us compose item 18: “I try to avoid destructive ways of dealing with stress.”

Competency 4: practices relaxation techniques

The value of practicing various relaxation techniques in managing stress began to be established even before the concept of “biological stress” was introduced in the early 1900s and before Hans Selye’s breakthrough research in the 1930s on the relationship between the stress response and disease131. Edmund Jacobson’s classic book, Progressive Relaxation132—based on techniques he had been developing and studying since 1915—asserted that progressive muscle relaxation exercises had multiple benefits, including improvements in memory, attention, thinking, and emotions133.

Later sources have repeatedly confirmed the value that various relaxation techniques have in stress management. For example, Pawlow and Jones134 conducted a controlled experiment on progressive muscle relaxation with 55 undergraduate students, concluding that the experimental group benefitted in multiple ways from the relaxation exercises. Among other benefits, the exercises “produced significantly reduced self ratings of perceived stress and state anxiety [and] significantly increased ratings of relaxation from immediately before to immediately after the training” (p. 381). In contrast, quiet sitting (practiced by the control group) produced no such benefits. The Pawlow and Jones134 study helped us to compose the questionnaire item, “I regularly tense and relax my muscles as a way of fighting stress” (item 13).

Smith et al.135 compared progressive muscle relaxation to yoga with 131 adults from South Australia in a randomized study, concluding that yoga (which included breathing exercises and postures) was as effective as muscle relaxation in producing positive outcomes, including reductions in stress and anxiety. This study helped us develop three ESMI-i items: “I frequently use special breathing techniques to help me relax” (item 1), “I regularly tense and relax my muscles as a way of fighting stress” (item 13), and the reverse-scored item, “Breathing is a very hard thing to control” (item 20).

Dummy items and internal consistency score (ICS)

As is common in competency questionnaires designed by the first author96,98,99,101, the ESMI-i includes one dummy item for each of the four competencies assessed. Each dummy item rephrases a corresponding scored item. The purpose of having these dummy pairs is to be able, at the end of each user session, to quickly compute how closely the answers within each pair match each other. The match is computed using a modified version of Cohen’s kappa coefficient, a standard measure of inter-observer agreement136 (see Supplementary Text S1 to compare the two formulas). We call this calculation our “internal consistency score” (ICS). In theory, if the ICS is low, we can ask a user to retake the questionnaire. In the present study, no users were asked to retake the questionnaire based on a low ICS. Instead, we elected to examine this issue as part of the data cleaning process (see below).

Methods

Participants

Before data cleaning, our dataset included 21,398 people who had completed the ESMI-i between May 3, 2007, and June 1, 2022. If someone completed the questionnaire more than once on the same day, we preserved only the first instance in which more than half the questionnaire items were answered. We also removed all cases in which self-reported English fluency was below 6 (on a scale from 1 to 10, where 10 indicated the highest level of fluency). After cleaning, 18,895 participants remained in the dataset.

The self-reported demographic characteristics of the participants were as follows (for details, see Table 2): Age ranged from 12 to 83 (M = 30.4 [SD = 14.1]). Because the ESMI-i has a Flesch–Kincaid reading level of 5.8, and because most 11-year-old children in the U.S. have completed the fifth grade, we received Institutional Review Board (IRB) approval for participants age 11 and over; however, our youngest participants (after cases were removed because of low self-reported fluency levels) were 12 (n = 19).

Table 2.

Demographic comparisons.

n (%) Mean manages sources of stress (SD) Mean manages thoughts score (SD) Mean plans and prevents score (SD) Mean practices relaxation score (SD) Mean total score (SD) Significance test P
Age
12–17 3,257 (17.2) 54.7 (18.2) 54.5 (15.0) 48.4 (18.6) 39.3 (18.8) 49.3 (12.9) U = 19,539,532  < 0.001
18–83 15,638 (82.8) 62.7 (18.2) 57.7 (14.5) 56.3 (19.7) 42.4 (20.8) 54.8 (13.4)
Gender
Female 12,242 (64.8) 61.4 (18.2) 57.1 (14.6) 55.1 (19.6) 41.3 (20.3) 53.7 (13.3) H = 42.7  < 0.001
Male 6,565 (34.7) 61.3 (18.7) 57.3 (14.8) 54.8 (20.0) 43.2 (20.8) 54.1 (13.7)
Other 88 (0.5) 45.8 (18.8) 57.7 (17.8) 38.0 (21.7) 34.8 (22.7) 44.1 (15.4)
Race/Ethnicity
American Indian 128 (0.7) 60.7 (18.5) 55.8 (15.9) 56.5 (21.2) 45.1 (21.7) 54.5 (14.8) H = 330.1  < 0.001
Asian 2,379 (12.7) 64.5 (18.1) 60.8 (13.6) 58.1 (19.1) 49.7 (20.5) 58.3 (13.4)
Black 1,165 (6.2) 61.3 (19.3) 57.2 (15.3) 57.5 (20.3) 42.4 (20.4) 54.6 (14.0)
Hispanic 1,107 (5.9) 59.6 (19.3) 57.6 (15.0) 52.8 (19.4) 41.9 (20.3) 53.0 (13.8)
Other 869 (4.6) 59.8 (18.5) 58.5 (15.2) 55.0 (20.3) 42.5 (21.5) 53.9 (14.1)
White 13,097 (69.3) 60.9 (18.2) 56.4 (14.6) 54.2 (19.7) 40.3 (20.0) 53.0 (13.2)
Level of education
None 2,115 (11.2) 54.3 (18.6) 54.6 (15.4) 47.6 (18.6) 38.6 (19.1) 48.8 (13.1) H = 604.3  < 0.001
High school 5,857 (31.0) 59.6 (18.4) 56.7 (14.6) 52.7 (19.4) 40.0 (19.4) 52.3 (12.9)
Associates 1,844 (9.8) 64.9 (17.8) 56.8 (14.3) 57.3 (20.0) 40.8 (19.9) 54.9 (13.1)
Bachelor’s 5,361 (28.4) 63.2 (18.1) 57.9 (14.6) 56.3 (19.7) 42.9 (21.0) 55.1 (13.6)
Master’s 2,952 (15.6) 64.0 (17.6) 58.9 (14.1) 59.6 (18.9) 45.9 (21.2) 57.1 (13.2)
Doctorate 644 (3.4) 62.1 (19.1) 57.5 (15.0) 59.8 (20.7) 45.9 (24.0) 56.3 (15.1)
Sexual orientation
Straight 16,713 (88.5) 62.1 (18.2) 57.2 (14.6) 55.9 (19.5) 42.1 (20.4) 54.3 (13.3) H = 203.0  < 0.001
Bisexual 1,211 (6.4) 52.6 (19.0) 57.3 (15.6) 46.2 (20.5) 39.9 (20.4) 49.0 (14.0)
Gay or lesbian 558 (3.0) 57.0 (19.6) 56.6 (15.3) 47.6 (20.0) 39.0 (20.8) 50.0 (14.2)
Other 25 (0.1) 50.8 (19.5) 60.7 (14.9) 51.8 (23.1) 45.5 (23.0) 52.2 (17.1)
Country of origin
U.S. & Canada 12,279 (65.0) 61.2 (18.2) 56.9 (14.7) 54.9 (19.3) 41.3 (20.0) 53.6 (13.2) U = 33,681,800  < 0.001
Other 5,712 (30.2) 61.7 (18.7) 57.9 (14.5) 55.1 (20.4) 43.4 (21.4) 54.5 (14.0)
Ever been married
Yes 7,124 (37.7) 63.6 (18.0) 56.6 (14.6) 57.7 (19.9) 42.3 (21.3) 55.0 (13.6) U = 37,980,528.5  < 0.001
No 11,634 (61.6) 59.8 (18.6) 57.5 (14.7) 53.2 (19.5) 41.6 (20.0) 53.0 (13.3)
Ever been divorced
Yes 2,719 (14.4) 63.4 (18.1) 57.3 (14.7) 57.5 (20.2) 43.5 (22.0) 55.4 (14.0) U = 20,128,336.5  < 0.001
No 15,992 (84.6) 60.9 (18.5) 57.2 (14.7) 54.5 (19.6) 41.6 (20.2) 53.6 (13.4)

Significance tests and p values compare mean total scores.

After cleaning by English fluency and duplicate cases, we had no need to remove cases because of low ICSs. We made this determination based on the value of Cronbach’s alpha for groups of people with differing ICSs. The group of people with ICSs between 0.4 and 0.5 (or, more precisely, 0.4 < ICS ≤ 0.5) had an alpha of 0.71, and with each successive group of people with higher ICSs (0.5–0.6, 0.6–0.7, 0.7–0.8, 0.8–0.9, 0.9–1.0), alpha increased (range 0.71 to 0.88). Because alphas greater than 0.7 are normally considered acceptable in test development137139 , using this criterion, we could not justify removing cases based on low ICSs. A small number of people (40 in total, 0.21% of the total N) had ICSs less than 0.4, but that was too few people for us to compute an alpha. Because we had no objective reason to eliminate these people from our study, we took the conservative course of action and let them remain (see Supplementary Table S1 and Supplementary Figure S1 for details).

Overall, 12,242 (64.8%) of our participants identified themselves as female, 6,565 (34.7%) as male, and 88 (0.5%) as other. Racial and ethnic background was as follows: 128 (0.7%) of our participants identified themselves as American Indian, 2,379 (12.6%) as Asian, 1,165 (6.2%) as Black, 1,107 (5.9%) as Hispanic, 13,097 (69.3%) as White, and 869 (4.6%) as Other; 150 individuals (0.8%) did not answer this question. Overall, 29.9% of the individuals in the sample identified themselves as non-White.

Regarding level of education completed: 2,115 (11.2%) reported not having a high school degree; 5,857 (31.0%) reported completing high school; 1,844 (9.8%) reported having an associates degree; 5,361 (28.4%) reported having completed college; 2,952 (15.6%) reported having a master’s degree; 644 (3.4%) reported having a doctoral degree; and 122 (0.6%) did not answer the question. Regarding sexual orientation: 16,713 (88.5%) identified themselves as straight; 558 (3.0%) as gay or lesbian; 1,211 (6.4%) as bisexual; 25 (0.1%) as other, and 388 (2.1%) did not answer this question. Regarding country of origin: 12,279 (65.0%) were from the United States and Canada; 5,712 (30.2%) were from 123 other countries; and 904 (4.8%) did not answer this question.

Study design

The present investigation utilized a “concurrent study design” that used criterion validity evidence, consistent with guidelines in the most recent edition of Standards for Educational and Psychological Testing140, prepared jointly by the American Educational Research Association, the American Psychological Association, and the National Council on Measurement in Education. Specifically, we sought to measure the strength of the relationships between our questionnaire scores and the scores on our self-reported criterion questions. This design is called “concurrent” because we obtained questionnaire scores and criterion measures at the same time, a strategy that avoids possible temporal confounds. Results from studies employing this design are considered especially robust when the pattern of relationships between questionnaire scores and criterion measures proves to be consistent across different demographic groups.

Measures

As noted above, the questionnaire employed in the study measured four cognitive-behavioral competencies. A total score was calculated, with higher scores indicating greater stress-management competence. Separate scores were also calculated for each of the competencies. We report all questionnaire scores as a percentage of possible maximum scores rather than as raw scores. We also calculated scores for each of the four criterion questions (see Procedure below).

Procedure

Participants were first presented with brief instructions informing them, for example, that there are no right or wrong answers to the items on the questionnaire. They were then asked some basic demographic questions, following which they were asked four criterion questions regarding desirable outcomes that are sometimes associated with successful stress management, namely: How happy and fulfilled are you? How much success have you had in your personal life? How much success have you had in your professional life? How stressed do you generally feel? Answers were given on a 10-point Likert scale from Low to High (see Supplementary Figures S2-S4 for the demographic questions and the questionnaire items). After completing the 28-item questionnaire (24 items were scored), participants were given their overall questionnaire score as well as scores on the four subscales and detailed explanations about the nature of each competency (see Supplementary Fig. S5 for a screenshot of the results page). Primary access to the questionnaire was at the URL https://MyStressManagementSkills.com. Over time, links to the questionnaire appeared elsewhere on the internet, a process over which we had no control. We also had no control over the demographic characteristics of the participants (see Discussion).

Ethics statement

The federally registered Institutional Review Board (IRB) of the sponsoring institution (American Institute for Behavioral Research and Technology) approved this study with exempt status and a waiver of the requirement for informed consent under U.S. Department of Health and Human Services regulations (45 CFR 46.116(d), 45 CFR 46.117(c)(2), and 45 CFR 46.111) because (a) the anonymity of participants was preserved and (b) the risk to participants was minimal. The IRB is registered with the Office for Human Research Protections under number IRB00009303, and the Federalwide Assurance number for the IRB is FWA00021545.

Results

Regressions and factor analysis

Linear regression was used to determine which competencies were most strongly associated with self-reported levels of happiness, personal success, professional success, and general level of stress. Notably, the Plans and Prevents competency proved to be the best predictor of all four criterion variables (Tables 3 and 4). Please note that by using the language of prediction, we do not mean to imply causation.

Table 3.

Regression results showing how well the most predictive competency predicted answers to the criterion questions.

Competency Happiness Level of stress Personal success Professional success
Standardized Standardized Standardized Standardized
β t r2 β t r2 β t r2 β t r2
Plans and Prevents 0.46*** 71.42 0.21 − 0.34*** − 49.07 0.11 0.38*** 56.30 0.14 0.35*** 50.38 0.12

Results are shown for one-component models in stepwise regressions, one for each of the four criterion questions.

***p < 0.001.

Table 4.

Regression results showing how well the four competencies predicted answers to the criterion questions.

Competency Happiness Level of stress Personal success Professional success
Standardized Standardized Standardized Standardized
β t r2 β t r2 β t r2 β t r2
Plans and Prevents 0.31*** 35.62 0.24 − 0.21*** − 22.47 0.13 0.25*** 27.58 0.17 0.22*** 24.56 0.14
Manages Sources of Stress 0.19*** 23.34 − 0.15*** − 17.67 0.20*** 23.55 0.18*** 21.59
Practices Relaxation Techniques 0.06*** 7.77 − 0.07*** − 8.20 0.03*** 3.46 0.02* 2.47
Manages Thoughts 0.04*** 5.63 − 0.02** − 2.71 N/A N/A N/A N/A

Results are shown for multi-component models in stepwise multiple regressions, one for each of the four criterion questions.

*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

An exploratory principal components factor analysis for the 24 scored items in the questionnaire yielded a Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin sampling adequacy of 0.89, which is well above the recommended cutoff of 0.6, as well as a highly significant Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity (p < 0.001). The factor analysis yielded four components that overlap our original four competencies and that can reasonably be described as: (1) Plans Ahead, (2) Practices Relaxation Techniques, (3) Regulates and Manages Stressors, and (4) Recognizes Weaknesses (Table 5). The results of the factor analysis were not used to revise the original competencies or items, because these competencies are practical skillsets, not hypothetical constructs. We did not conduct a confirmatory factor analysis for the same reason.

Table 5.

Exploratory factor analysis for the 24 scored items.a

Component
Item Plans ahead Practices relaxation techniques Regulates and manages stressors Recognizes one’s weaknesses
7 0.74
22 0.67
25 0.59
21 0.58
4 0.53 0.41
12 0.47 0.40
13 0.74
14 0.71
3 0.64
1 0.64
27 0.42
9 0.65
10 0.65
5 0.53
18 0.50
19 0.45
8 0.40
17 0.38
2
23 0.72
24 0.68
11 0.53
20 0.50
15 0.36

Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis.

Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization.

Values less than 0.35 are omitted.

aCorrected for reverse-scored items.

Reliability and validity evidence

The questionnaire had moderate but acceptable internal-consistency reliability (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.79; Guttman split-half = 0.70)137139 . Because the study was conducted over the internet and because we could not collect contact information for our participants (in order to preserve their anonymity), test–retest reliability could not be assessed. We also did not develop an alternate form of the questionnaire, so alternate-form reliability could not be estimated.

Internal consistency scores for the four competencies varied considerably: Manages or Reduces Sources of Stress: Cronbach’s alpha = 0.60; Guttman split-half = 0.64. Manages Thoughts: Cronbach’s alpha = 0.34; Guttman split-half = 0.37. Plans and Prevents: Cronbach’s alpha = 0.64; Guttman split-half = 0.68. Practices Relaxation Techniques: Cronbach’s alpha = 0.65; Guttman split-half = 0.61.

Regarding validity evidence, total scores were correlated with scores on our criterion questions, as we had predicted: Total scores were positively correlated with participants’ self-reported level of happiness (Spearman’s ρ = 0.45, p < 0.001), personal success (ρ = 0.35, p < 0.001), and professional success (ρ = 0.32, p < 0.001), and negatively correlated with participants’ general level of stress (ρ = -0.33, p < 0.001). Because this was an internet-based study in which the anonymity of participants was protected, we could not assess validity by comparing scores on our questionnaire to scores on comparable questionnaires (see Discussion). (Because scores on the ESMI-i lie on an ordinal scale, nonparametric statistical tests such as Spearman’s ρ, the Mann–Whitney U, and the Kruskal–Wallis H are used throughout this study, except in our regression analyses. Nonparametric regressions are generally used only when extreme values might distort the results141; outliers are unlikely with an instrument like the ESMI-i in which scores are constrained.).

Although not specifically predicted, the validity of the measuring instrument is also suggested by the fact that the mean total score for those who reported having had stress-management training was significantly higher than the mean total score for those who did not (MYes = 58.8 [13.2], MNo = 52.6 [13.3], U = 19,888,845, p < 0.001, r = 0.18). In addition, overall questionnaire scores were positively correlated with the number of hours of stress-management training participants reported having (ρ = 0.24, p < 0.001).

Gender, race, and other demographic effects

The overall mean total score was 53.8 [13.5] and subscale means were as follows: Manages or Reduces Sources of Stress (M = 61.3 [18.4]), Manages Thoughts (M = 57.2 [14.7]), Plans and Prevents (M = 54.9 [19.7]), and Practices Relaxation Techniques (M = 41.9 [20.5]).

We found a significant but not necessarily substantial effect for education level (MNone = 48.8 [13.1], MHighschool = 52.3 [12.9], MAssociates = 54.9 [13.1], MBachelors = 55.1 [13.6], MMasters = 57.1 [13.2], MDoctorate = 56.3 [15.1], H = 604.3, p < 0.001, E2R = 0.03), and scores were higher for participants who reported having been married (MYes = 55.0 [13.6], MNo = 53.0 [13.3], U = 37,980,528.5, p < 0.001, r = 0.07) and also, surprisingly, for participants who reported having been divorced (MYes = 55.4 [14.0], MNo = 53.6 [13.4], U = 20,128,336.5, p < 0.001, r = 0.05).

We found a significant but not necessarily substantial effect for gender (Mfemale = 53.7 [13.3], Mmale = 54.1 [13.7], Mother = 44.1 [15.4], H = 42.7, p < 0.001, E2R = 0.00) and no significant male/female difference (U = 39,631,749.5, p = 0.12, r = 0.01). Participants ages 18 and older scored significantly higher than minors (M12-17 = 49.3 [12.9], M18-83 = 54.8 [13.4], U = 19,539,532, p < 0.001, r = 0.15). (Note that because this is a large-n study, statistical significance is not necessarily a good indicator of the importance of mean differences. For this reason, we also have included two different measures of effect size: r, where we are comparing two means, and epsilon-squared, where we are comparing three or more means 142. We also found a significant but not necessarily substantial effect for ethnicity, with respondents of Asian descent outscoring all other ethnicities (MAmericanIndian = 54.5 [14.8], MAsian = 58.3 [13.4], MBlack = 54.6 [14.0], MHispanic = 53.0 [13.8], MWhite = 54.0 [14.1], MOther = 53.0 [13.2], H = 330.1, p < 0.001, E2R = 0.02; Asian vs. non-Asian: MAsian = 58.3 [13.4], MNonAsian = 53.2 [13.4], U = 15,133,970.5, p < 0.001, r = 0.13), a finding that is consistent with other research143145.

We also found a significant but not necessarily substantial effect for sexual orientation, with self-labeled straights outscoring self-labeled gays, lesbians, and bisexuals (MBisexual = 49.0 [14.0], MGay/Lesbian = 50.0 [14.2], MStraight = 54.3 [13.3], MOther = 52.2 [17.1], H = 203.0, p < 0.001, E2R = 0.01; straight vs. non-straight: MStraight = 54.3 [13.3], MNonStraight = 49.4 [14.1], U = 11,946,435, p < 0.001, r = 0.10). Participants outside the U.S. and Canada scored higher than participants from the U.S. and Canada (MUS/Canada = 53.6 [13.2], MOther = 54.5 [14.0], U = 33,681,800, p < 0.001, r = 0.03), possibly because of the higher scores of Asian participants in the study. We did not have enough participants in individual countries outside the U.S. and Canada for us to conduct, with sufficient statistical power, a country-by-country analysis; a larger sample might allow us to conduct such an analysis in future years. Age proved to be a small but significant predictor of questionnaire scores (ρ = 0.12, p < 0.001).

Apparent value of low stress

Our results suggest that high stress is associated with low levels of happiness, personal success, and professional success; that low stress is associated with high levels of these outcomes; and that the benefits sometimes associated with a moderate level of stress might not be as beneficial as previously thought (Figs. 1 and 2; see Discussion). Although the spikes toward the center of the curves in Fig. 1 could be interpreted as indicating possible benefits of moderate stress, when one looks closely at the happiness, personal success, and professional success ratings reported by people who experience different levels of stress in their lives, it seems evident that low stress is more consistently associated with desirable outcomes (Fig. 2).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Relationship between self-reported levels of stress and self-reported levels of happiness, personal success, and professional success. Although higher stress is generally associated with poorer outcomes (note the overall downward slopes of the curves), the upward spikes in the center of the graph are sometimes mistakenly interpreted to mean that moderate stress is beneficial. Vertical bars show 95% confidence intervals.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Histograms showing distributions of self-reported levels of happiness, personal success, and professional success, separated into three categories of level of stress. Although one can find high levels of happiness, personal success, and professional success in the bottom two rows of graphs, the patterns of scores are more nearly optimal in the top row, which shows data only for people reporting their overall level of stress as very low (1 on a scale from 1 to 10).

Year-by-year analysis

Because our data were collected over a period of more than 14 years, we asked whether any trends were evident in scores, as well as in demographic characteristics of the sample. Although statistically significant changes were evident in both scores and demographic characteristics over the course of the study (Table 6), we did not find a linear trend in the mean total scores (p = 0.813, r2 = 0.005, β = 0.07, t = 0.24).

Table 6.

Differences in mean scores and changes in key demographic groups by year.

2008 2009 2010 2011 2012 2013 2014 2015
n 1615 724 735 3495 1692 1193 1248 1596
Mean, total score (SD) 53.1 (13.8) 53.3 (13.4) 52.4 (13.6) 52.4 (13.0) 53.7 (13.0) 56.1 (13.0) 54.4 (12.6) 54.5 (13.6)
Mean, manages sources of stress (SD) 61.1 (18.1) 60.4 (18.0) 60.4 (17.9) 59.1 (17.8) 62.1 (17.9) 64.2 (17.5) 63.2 (18.2) 62.3 (18.7)
Mean, manages thoughts (SD) 56.2 (14.6) 58.1 (13.7) 57.0 (15.1) 56.1 (14.9) 56.5 (14.9) 58.0 (14.1) 56.8 (14.2) 57.8 (14.5)
Mean, plans and prevents (SD) 53.9 (19.9) 53.6 (19.7) 52.8 (20.0) 53.2 (18.9) 55.1 (19.0) 59.1 (18.9) 57.0 (18.8) 56.7 (19.8)
Mean, practices relaxation (SD) 41.1 (21.8) 41.1 (21.5) 39.5 (20.4) 41.3 (19.8) 41.2 (19.6) 43.4 (20.9) 40.5 (19.3) 41.3 (20.3)
Mean age (SD) 40.9 (16.7) 31.6 (12.6) 32.9 (13.0) 29.0 (15.6) 29.9 (14.0) 31.6 (13.3) 29.2 (11.9) 27.7 (12.5)
Gender (% Fem.) 61.2 67.7 68.0 57.7 64.1 69.7 72.4 65.7
Race (% White) 76.9 77.8 81.2 79.4 78.2 73.9 64.1 75.4
2016 2017 2018 2019 2020 2021 Significance testa
n 1147 786 795 697 771 845
Mean, total score (SD) 52.1 (14.3) 55.6 (13.7) 53.3 (12.3) 53.2 (13.6) 51.2 (13.7) 54.9 (13.3) 159.5***(H)
Mean, manages sources of stress (SD) 59.1 (19.8) 63.3 (19.1) 60.5 (18.1) 59.4 (19.9) 58.0 (18.9) 61.0 (18.1) 166.3***(H)
Mean, manages thoughts (SD) 56.8 (15.1) 57.0 (14.5) 57.9 (14.1) 58.4 (14.1) 56.5 (15.3) 58.0 (14.7) 46.0***(H)
Mean, plans and prevents (SD) 52.3 (21.1) 57.6 (19.8) 54.0 (19.1) 53.3 (20.8) 50.4 (21.0) 55.6 (20.0) 196.3***(H)
Mean, practices relaxation (SD) 40.1 (20.4) 44.5 (21.0) 40.6 (18.6) 41.6 (19.5) 40.0 (20.3) 44.8 (20.7) 71.1***(H)
Mean age (SD) 28.0 (12.6) 30.8 (12.6) 27.6 (11.5) 27.5 (12.1) 31.3 (13.9) 29.5 (12.5) 90.4***(F)
Gender (% Fem.) 67.0 67.6 69.3 72.0 72.5 70.9 378.7***(X2)
Race (% White) 64.7 66.4 64.8 58.2 60.1 50.8 1115.0***(X2)

Only years in which 12 months of data were collected are shown (n = 17,339). % Female is shown for gender because the majority of participants were female. % White is shown for Race/Ethnicity because the majority of participants were White.

aMean total and competency scores were compared using the Kruskal–Wallis H. Mean ages were compared using a one-way ANOVA. Percentages for gender and race were compared using Pearson chi-square.

***p < 0.001.

Discussion and limitations

The present study sheds light on various aspects of people’s ability to manage stress. One of its greatest limitations—that the data were collected over the internet—is also a strength. On the downside, internet sampling gives one no control over demographics, and all participants are self-selected. Our sample presumably consisted of people who were concerned about stress or how they managed it. This could mean, among other things, that our mean level of self-reported stress (6.5 out of 10) is higher than that of the general population and, perhaps, that the stress-management proficiency level we found (M = 53.8) is lower than normal. A 2013 report by the American Psychological Association65 states that the average stress level for Americans is 4.9 out of 10, 1.6 points below the mean we found. We might also be attracting people with abnormally low levels of happiness or success.

On the upside, the internet allows researchers to look at a large, diverse, international sample, which almost certainly yields more valid findings than the proverbial pool of second-year college students146148. There is also accumulating evidence that people are more honest when answering personal questions through anonymous internet surveys than perhaps through any other means149153; a recent study by Robertson et al.154 suggests that anonymous internet surveys yield more valid responses than sixteen other common survey techniques. Surveys yield especially valid responses when people are completing them voluntarily and they know that the results will not be used by supervisors or other authority Figures.155,156 . For these reasons, we conjecture that our participants were probably honest in their responding. We also have no a priori reason to believe that the relationships we have found among variables—for example, the negative correlation between total questionnaire scores and self-reported levels of stress (ρ = − 0.33, p < 0.001)—are invalid.

We also have no reason to doubt the validity of some of the more distinctive demographic findings in the study, particularly where such findings are consistent with those of other research. Especially notable in this study is the relatively high mean score of participants identifying as Asian. Other studies looking more directly at this issue have also found that various Asian groups are better at managing stress than non-Asians, perhaps because of the collectivist nature of many Asian cultures157. Tweed et al.145 found, for example, that East Asian Canadians reported using internal strategies to manage stressful situations more often than European Canadians did. In collectivist cultures, people tend to be more mutually supportive than in individualistic cultures158,159, and Asian cultures also tend to teach explicit techniques—yoga, meditation, tai chi, and so on—which have been shown to improve well-being and lower stress71,160,161. In many non-Asian cultures, well-being is often sought through self-destructive means (alcoholism, drug abuse, overeating) or, at best, left to chance. Similarly, our findings that self-reported straights outscored self-reported non-straights on the ESMI-i and that self-reported straights reported experiencing less stress than self-reported non-straights are consistent with the findings of other researchers162165.

Our study found no significant difference between scores for males and scores for females. Researchers disagree about gender differences in both stress-management proficiency and perceived stress levels. Some studies suggest that women are more likely to utilize emotion-focused coping in response to stressors while men more often use problem-focused coping166. The 2013 report on stress published by the American Psychological Association stated that women report higher stress levels than men65. However, and consistent with our results, some studies have found that gender differences in coping styles are not apparent when confounding factors such as socioeconomic status and race are controlled for167.

The two largest demographic effects we found should be studied in further detail in future studies. Self-labeled straights outscored non-straights by 4.9 points (r = 0.10), and adults outscored minors (ages 12–17) by 5.5 points (r = 0.15). Considerable research has examined the emotional problems often experienced by non-straights (brought about, most likely, by entrenched heteronormativity in most cultures168170), but why straights should score higher on a test of stress-management competencies is unclear. The age difference seems less mysterious. Competencies take time to learn, after all171, but it would be interesting to look at this learning process in more detail, especially over the teen years.

A second notable limitation of the present study is that it is correlational in design. In follow-ups to this study, one could, by employing either between-subjects or within-subjects experimental designs, assess the possible causal impact of each of the four competencies we have examined in this report.

Perhaps the clearest and, in some sense, the most surprising finding in this study is that proactive stress-management methods appear to be more helpful than reactive ones. All four of our criterion variables were best predicted by the Plans and Prevents competency, of which all questionnaire items describe proactive methods of fighting stress—in other words, ways of trying to ensure that stressful situations never arise (Tables 3 and 4). A planning competency also emerged in our factor analysis (Table 5). Unfortunately, our respondents scored relatively poorly on Plans and Prevents (M = 54.9), which ranked third on actual competency scores. We note that our findings about proactive methods do not necessarily show that such methods are more beneficial than reactive ones; it might simply be the case that people with lower stress levels rely less on reactive coping strategies than on proactive ones. Again, questions of cause and effect can only be answered with experimental research.

Our study also yielded intriguing findings regarding the supposed value of moderate stress. Ever since the formation of the Yerkes-Dodson law in the early 1900s, researchers have suggested that moderate levels of stress (at least for stressors of certain types) are beneficial172176. Our study suggests another possibility—namely, that the bulge that often appears in the center of performance or other curves where the stress level is moderate is a statistical anomaly. As we noted earlier, this seems evident when we examine the relationship between participants’ self-reported levels of stress and their self-reported levels of happiness, personal success, and professional success (Fig. 1), as well as when we look closely at the distributions of self-reported levels of happiness, personal success, and professional success when separated into low, medium, and high of levels of self-reported stress (Fig. 2).

The strong relationship (ρ = 0.45, p < 0.001, ρ2 = 0.20) we found between total questionnaire scores and self-reported happiness is also notable, suggesting the importance of stress management in having a happy life177 (although, once again, we remind the reader that this is a correlational study). Unfortunately, our results suggest that people are generally poor at stress management; the mean percentage score on the questionnaire was 53.8%, with Practices Relaxation Skills having a mean score of only 41.9%. Our study confirms the need to educate and train people in how to manage stress, although, as noted above, our questionnaire scores might be lower than the average scores one would find in the general population. Fortunately, although the present study employed a concurrent design140, not an experimental one, our results are consistent with the view that stress-management training has value; as noted earlier, study participants who had had such training scored significantly higher than participants who had not, and questionnaire scores were positively correlated with the number of training hours reported.

We have already mentioned several ways in which our analysis was constrained because we collected our data online. These limitations are not trivial, because collecting data on the internet—especially when one is required to protect the anonymity of the participants—means that some of the standard tools used to validate new test instruments cannot be employed. Ideally, one would like to compare test scores to those obtained on previously validated tests. Again, ideally, one would like to be able to measure the stability of test scores by readministering the test to the same cohort after different periods of time have passed. One would also like to use multiple measures to validate the test, such as ratings by peers or clinicians. None of these methods is possible given our current design.

As noted above, future versions of the ESMI-i might include additional competency categories. People can mitigate stress using many tools, such as exercise or changes in diet; the present study focuses on cognitive-behavioral methods that might be taught by therapists and counselors. More detailed demographic analyses might also be conducted, especially with larger samples. The current online version of the ESMI-i (as of July 8, 2024) already includes additional gender and sexual orientation categories. The present study also assumes, implicitly, that stress management is the same in all cultures around the world, which is clearly not the case. Stressors themselves are culturally based163,165,178,179, and so are effective techniques of stress management145.

Supplementary Information

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Marco Buenaventura, Philip Cheung, Matea Djokic, Shannon Fox, Allison He, Paul McKinney, Krystie Mei, and Rachel Smith for assistance in various aspects of this research. Funding was provided by the American Institute for Behavioral Research and Technology, which also provided IRB approval (IRB registered with OHRP under number IRB00009303, Federalwide Assurance number FWA00021545). This report is based in part on papers presented at the 91st (2011), 94th (2014), and 101st (2021) annual meetings of the Western Psychological Association. Each successive report analyzed larger datasets. Brief summaries of these talks were posted on the first author’s website.

Author contributions

R.E. conceptualized the study, supervised all aspects of the research, and wrote the original draft. J.A., C.G., V.Z., and L.Z. assisted with statistical analysis and the literature review. V.Z. prepared Figs. 1 and 2. All authors reviewed the manuscript.

Data availability

Anonymized raw data employed in the present study will be made available at Zenodo.com upon acceptance of the manuscript. Anonymized raw data can also be requested by writing to info@aibrt.org. Anonymization was required to comply with the requirement of the sponsoring institution’s Institutional Review Board that the identities of the participants be protected in accordance with HHS Federal Regulation 45 CFR 46.101.(b)(2).

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-024-68328-4.

References

  • 1.Toderi, S. & Balducci, C. Stress-preventive management competencies, psychosocial work environments, and affective well-being: A multilevel, multisource investigation. Int. J. Env. Res. Pub. H.15, 397. 10.3390/ijerph15030397 (2018). 10.3390/ijerph15030397 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Toderi, S., Gaggia, A., Balducci, C. & Sarchielli, G. Reducing psychosocial risks through supervisors’ development: A contribution for a brief version of the “Stress Management Competency Indicator Tool”. Sci. Tot. Env.518–519, 345–351. 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2015.02.082 (2015). 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2015.02.082 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Gallagher-Thompson, G., Gray, H. L., Dupart, T., Jimenez, D. & Thompson, L. W. Effectiveness of cognitive/behavioral small group intervention for reduction of depression and stress in non-Hispanic White and Hispanic/Latino women dementia family caregivers: Outcomes and mediators of change. J. Ration.-Emot. Cogn.-B.26(4), 286–303. 10.1007/s10942-008-0087-4 (2008). 10.1007/s10942-008-0087-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Barber, J. P. & DeRubeis, R. J. The ways of responding: A scale to assess compensatory skills taught in cognitive therapy. Behav. Assess.14(1), 93–115 (1992). [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Folkman, S. & Lazarus, R. S. Manual for the Ways of Coping Questionnaire (Consulting Psychologists Press, 1988). [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Holmes, T. H. & Rahe, R. H. The social readjustment rating scale. J. Psychol. Res.11, 213–218. 10.1016/0022-3999(67)90010-4 (1967). 10.1016/0022-3999(67)90010-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Cohen, S., Kamarck, T. & Mermelstein, R. A global measure of perceived stress. J. H. Soc. Behav.24, 385–396. 10.2307/2136404 (1983). 10.2307/2136404 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Endler, N. S. & Parker, J. D. Multidimensional assessment of coping: A critical evaluation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.58(5), 844–854. 10.1037/0022-3514.58.5.844 (1990). 10.1037/0022-3514.58.5.844 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Jacob, K. L., Christopher, M. S. & Neuhaus, E. C. Development and validation of the cognitive-behavioral therapy skills questionnaire. Behav. Modif.35(6), 595–618. 10.1177/0145445511419254 (2011). 10.1177/0145445511419254 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Jarrett, R. B., Clark, L. A., Vittengl, J. R. & Thase, M. E. Skills of cognitive Therapy (SoCT): A new measure of patients’ comprehension and use. Psychol. Assess.23(3), 578–586. 10.1037/a0022485 (2011). 10.1037/a0022485 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Neuhaus, E. C., Christopher, M., Jacob, K., Guillamot, J. & Burns, J. P. Short-term cognitive behavioral partial hospital treatment: A pilot study. J. Psychiatr. Pract.13(5), 298–307. 10.1097/01.pra.0000290668.10107.f3 (2007). 10.1097/01.pra.0000290668.10107.f3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Hundt, N. E., Mignogna, J., Underhill, C. & Cully, J. A. The relationship between use of CBT skills and depression treatment outcome: A theoretical and methodological review of the literature. Behav. Ther.44, 12–26. 10.1016/j.beth.2012.10.001 (2013). 10.1016/j.beth.2012.10.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Greenglass, E. R. Proactive coping in Beyond coping. In Meeting goals vision and challenges (ed. Frydenberg, E.) 37–62 (Oxford University Press, 2002). [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Greenglass, E., Schwarzer, R., Jakubiec, D., Fiksenbaum, L., & Taubert, S. The Proactive Coping Inventory (PCI): A multidimensional research instrument. https://estherg.info.yorku.ca/files/2014/09/pci.pdf (paper presented at the 20th International Conference of the Stress and Anxiety Research Society (STAR), Cracow, Poland, July 12–14, 1999).
  • 15.Ektor-Andersen, J., Ørbæk, P. & Isacsson, S.-O. Behavior-focused pain coping: Consistency and convergence to work capability of the Swedish version of The chronic pain coping inventory. J. Rehab. Med.34, 33–39. 10.1080/165019702317242686 (2002). 10.1080/165019702317242686 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Hadjistavropoulos, H. D., MacLeod, F. K. & Asmundson, G. J. Validation of the chronic pain coping inventory. Pain80, 471–481. 10.1016/s0304-3959(98)00224-3 (1999). 10.1016/s0304-3959(98)00224-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Jensen, M. P., Turner, J. A., Romano, J. M. & Strom, S. E. The chronic pain coping inventory: Development and preliminary validation. Pain60, 203–216. 10.1016/0304-3959(94)00118-X (1995). 10.1016/0304-3959(94)00118-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Jensen, M. P., Turner, J. A., Romano, J. M., Nielson, W. R., & PAR Staff. Chronic Pain Coping Inventory: CPCI Longitudinal Report. Psychol. Assess. Res. (1995).
  • 19.Rahe, R. H., Veach, T. L., Tolles, R. L. & Murakami, K. The Stress and Coping Inventory: An educational and research instrument. Stress Med.16, 199–208. 10.1002/1099-1700(200007)16:4%3c199::AID-SMI848%3e3.0.CO;2-D (2000). [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Cosway, R., Endler, N. S., Sadler, A. J. & Deary, I. J. The Stress and Coping Inventory: An educationaological health. J. App. BioBehav. Res.5, 121–143. 10.1111/j.1751-9861.2000.tb00069.x (2000). 10.1111/j.1751-9861.2000.tb00069.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Endler, N. S. & Parker, J. D. A. Coping Inventory for Stressful Situations (CISS): Manual 2nd edn. (Multi-Health Systems, 1990). [Google Scholar]
  • 22.McWilliams, L. A., Cox, B. J. & Enns, M. W. Use of the coping inventory for stressful situations in a clinically depressed sample: Factor structure, personality correlates, and prediction of distress. J. Clin. Psychol.59, 423–437. 10.1002/jclp.10080 (2003). 10.1002/jclp.10080 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Baumstarck, K. et al. Assessment of coping: A new French four-factor structure of the Brief COPE Inventory. Health Qual. Life Outcomes15, 8. 10.1186/s12955-016-0581-9 (2017). 10.1186/s12955-016-0581-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F. & Weintraub, J. K. Assessing coping strategies: A theoretically based approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.56, 267–283. 10.1037/0022-3514.56.2.267 (1989). 10.1037/0022-3514.56.2.267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Eklund, R. C., Grove, J. R. & Heard, N. P. The measurement of slump-related coping: Factorial validity of the COPE and modified-COPE inventories. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol.20, 157–175. 10.1123/jsep.20.2.157 (1998). 10.1123/jsep.20.2.157 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Brown, R. D. & Speth, C. Professorial responses to stress: A self-assessment scale. Rev. High. Educ.11, 285–296. 10.1353/rhe.1988.0021 (1988). 10.1353/rhe.1988.0021 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Libin, E. Coping intelligence: Efficient life stress management. Front. Psychol.10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00302 (2017). 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00302 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Bartone, P. T. Test-retest reliability of the dispositional resilience Scale-15, a brief hardiness scale. Psychol. Rep.101, 943–944. 10.2466/pr0.101.3.943-944 (2007). 10.2466/pr0.101.3.943-944 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Wong, J. Y. et al. Transcultural and psychometric validation of the dispositional resilience scale (DRS-15) in Chinese adult women. Qual. Life Res.23, 2489–2494. 10.1007/s11136-014-0713-9 (2014). 10.1007/s11136-014-0713-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Crum, A. J., Salovey, P. & Achor, S. Rethinking stress: The role of mindsets in determining the stress response. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.104, 716–733. 10.1037/a0031201 (2013). 10.1037/a0031201 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Horiuchi, S., Tsuda, A., Aoki, S., Yoneda, K. & Sawaguchi, Y. Coping as a mediator of the relationship between stress mindset and psychological stress response: A pilot study. Psychol. Res. Behav.11, 47–54. 10.2147/PRBM.S150400 (2018). 10.2147/PRBM.S150400 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Keech, J. J., Hagger, M. S. & Hamilton, K. Changing stress mindsets with a novel imagery intervention: A randomized controlled trial. Emotion21, 123–136. 10.1037/emo0000678 (2019). 10.1037/emo0000678 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Strunk, D. R., DeRubeis, R. J., Chiu, A. W. & Alvarez, J. Patients’ competence in and performance of cognitive therapy skills: Relation to the reduction of relapse risk following treatment for depression. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol.75(4), 523–530. 10.1037/0022-3514.58.5.844 (2007). 10.1037/0022-3514.58.5.844 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Alhusseini, N., Banta, J. E., Oh, J. & Montgomery, S. Understanding the use of electronic means to seek personal health information among adults in the United States. Cureus10.7759/cureus.11190 (2020). 10.7759/cureus.11190 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Fox, S., & Duggan, M. Health. Pew Research Center. https://www.pewresearch.org/internet/2013/01/15/health-online-2013/ (2013).
  • 36.Rutten, L. J. F. et al. Online health information seeking among US adults: Measuring progress toward a healthy people 2020 objective. Pub. H. Rep.10.1177/0033354919874074 (2019). 10.1177/0033354919874074 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Davis, J. E. The appeal, and the peril, of self-diagnosis. Psychology Today. https://www.psychologytoday.com/us/blog/our-new-discontents/202209/the-appeal-and-the-peril-self-diagnosis (2022).
  • 38.Greenberger, D. & Padesky, C. A. Mind over mood: Change how you feel by changing the way you think 2nd edn. (The Guilford Press, 2016). [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Jex, S. M., Beehr, T. A. & Roberts, C. K. The meaning of occupational stress items to survey respondents. J. App. Psychol.77, 623–628. 10.1037/0021-9010.77.5.623 (1992). 10.1037/0021-9010.77.5.623 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Lazarus, R. S. & Folkman, S. Stress, appraisal, and coping (Springer, 1984). [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Selye, H. Confusion and controversy in the stress field. J. Hum. Stress1, 37–44. 10.1080/0097840X.1975.9940406 (1975). 10.1080/0097840X.1975.9940406 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Stress. In American Psychological Association Dictionary of Psychology. https://dictionary.apa.org/stress (n.d.).
  • 43.Stressor. In American Psychological Association Dictionary of Psychology.https://dictionary.apa.org/stressor (n.d.).
  • 44.Coping skills. In National Cancer Institute’s Dictionary of Cancer Terms.https://www.cancer.gov/publications/dictionaries/cancer-terms/def/coping-skills (n.d.).
  • 45.Coping strategy. In American Psychological Association Dictionary of Psychology.https://dictionary.apa.org/coping-strategies (n.d.).
  • 46.Au, D. W. et al. Psychosomatic and physical responses to a multi-component stress management program among teaching professionals: A randomized study of cognitive behavioral intervention (CB) with complementary and alternative medicine (CAM) approach. Behav. Res. Ther.80, 10–16. 10.1016/j.brat.2016.02.004 (2016). 10.1016/j.brat.2016.02.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Bossi, F. et al. Mindfulness-based online intervention increases well-being and decreases stress after COVID-19 lockdown. Sci. Rep.12, 6483. 10.1038/s41598-022-10361-2 (2022). 10.1038/s41598-022-10361-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Connolly, D. et al. The impact of a primary care stress management and wellbeing programme (RENEW) on occupational participation: A pilot study. Brit. J. Occup. Ther.82, 112–121. 10.1177/0308022618793323 (2019). 10.1177/0308022618793323 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Dijkstra, M. T. M. & Homan, A. C. Engaging in rather than disengaging from stress: Effective coping and perceived control. Front. Psychol.10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01415 (2016). 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01415 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Freire, C., Ferradás, M. D. M., Valle, A., Núñez, J. C. & Vallejo, G. Profiles of psychological well-being and coping strategies among university students. Front. Psychol.10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01554 (2016). 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01554 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Marzorati, C., Pizzoli, S. F. M., Grasso, R. & Pravettoni, G. Remote relaxation and acceptance training for the management of stress in cancer patients: A study protocol. Front. Psychol.10.3389/fpsyg.2021.710861 (2021). 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.710861 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Millegan, J. et al. A mind-body program for pain and stress management in active duty service members and veterans. Psychol. Serv.18, 186–194. 10.1037/ser0000376 (2019). 10.1037/ser0000376 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Moeller, R. W. et al. The differential role of coping, physical activity, and mindfulness in college student adjustment. Front. Psychol.10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01858 (2020). 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01858 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Moriya, J. & Takahashi, Y. Depression and interpersonal stress: The mediating role of emotion regulation. Motiv. Emo.37, 600–608. 10.1007/s11031-012-9323-4 (2013). 10.1007/s11031-012-9323-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Neely, M. E., Schallert, D. L., Mohammed, S. S., Roberts, R. M. & Chen, Y.-J. Self-kindness when facing stress: The role of self-compassion, goal regulation, and support in college students’ well-being. Motiv. Emot.33, 88–97. 10.1007/s11031-008-9119-8 (2009). 10.1007/s11031-008-9119-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Nijjar, P. S. et al. Randomized trial of mindfulness-based stress reduction in cardiac patients eligible for cardiac rehabilitation. Sci. Rep.9, 18415. 10.1038/s41598-019-54932-2 (2019). 10.1038/s41598-019-54932-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Perilli, E. et al. Reduced anxiety associated to adaptive and mindful coping strategies in general practitioners compared with hospital nurses in response to COVID-19 pandemic primary care reorganization. Front. Psychol.10.3389/fpsyg.2022.891470 (2022). 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.891470 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Rentala, S., Thimmajja, S. G., Tilekar, S. D., Nayak, R. B. & Aladakatti, R. Impact of holistic stress management program on academic stress and well-being of Indian adolescent girls: A randomized controlled trial. J. Edu. Health Promot.8, 253. 10.4103/jehp.jehp_233_19 (2019). 10.4103/jehp.jehp_233_19 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Riley, K. E. et al. Improving physical and mental health in frontline mental health care providers: Yoga-based stress management versus cognitive behavioral stress management. J. Workplace Behav. Health32, 26–48. 10.1080/15555240.2016.1261254 (2017). 10.1080/15555240.2016.1261254 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Salimzadeh, R., Hall, N. C. & Saroyan, A. Examining academics’ strategies for coping with stress and emotions: A review of research. Front. Edu.10.3389/feduc.2021.660676 (2021). 10.3389/feduc.2021.660676 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Shannon, S. et al. Effects of a mental health intervention in athletes: Applying self-determination theory. Front. Psychol.10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01875 (2019). 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01875 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Stefanaki, C. et al. Impact of a mindfulness stress management program on stress, anxiety, depression and quality of life in women with polycystic ovary syndrome: A randomized controlled trial. Stress18, 57–66. 10.3109/10253890.2014.974030 (2015). 10.3109/10253890.2014.974030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Velana, M. & Rinkenauer, G. Individual-level interventions for decreasing job-related stress and enhancing coping strategies among nurses: A systematic review. Front. Psychol.10.3389/fpsyg.2021.708696 (2021). 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.708696 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Zahniser, E. & Conley, C. S. Interactons of emotion regulation and perceived stress in predicting emerging adults’ subsequent internalizing symptoms. Motiv. Emot.42, 763–773. 10.1007/s11031-018-9696-0 (2018). 10.1007/s11031-018-9696-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.American Psychological Association. Stress in America: Missing the health care connection. (2013).
  • 66.Eppelmann, L. et al. Stress, mental and physical health and the costs of health care in German high school students. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry.10.1007/s00787-019-01441-2 (2020). 10.1007/s00787-019-01441-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Hinshaw, K. J., Richter, L. T. & Kramer, G. A. Stress, burnout, and renewal activities of dental hygiene education administrators in six U.S. Midwestern states. J. Dent. Educ.74, 235–250 (2010). 10.1002/j.0022-0337.2010.74.3.tb04869.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Johnston, K., Westerfield, W., Momin, S., Phillippi, R. & Naidoo, A. The direct and indirect costs of employee depression, anxiety, and emotional disorders—An employer case study. J. Occup. Environ. Med.51, 564–577. 10.1097/JOM.0b013e3181a1f5c8 (2009). 10.1097/JOM.0b013e3181a1f5c8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Olusoga, P., Butt, J., Maynard, I. & Hays, K. Stress and coping: A study of world class coaches. J. Appl. Sport Psychol.22, 274–293. 10.1080/10413201003760968 (2010). 10.1080/10413201003760968 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Thoits, P. A. Stress and health: Major findings and policy implications. J. Health Soc. Behav.51, S41–S53. 10.1177/0022146510383499 (2010). 10.1177/0022146510383499 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Alexander, C. N. et al. Effects of the transcendental meditation program on stress reduction, health, and employee development: A prospective study in two occupational settings. Anxiety Stress Coping6, 245–262. 10.1080/10615809308248383 (1993). 10.1080/10615809308248383 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Frazier, P. et al. Randomized controlled trial evaluating the effectiveness of a web-based stress management program among community college students. Anxiety Stress Coping28, 576–586. 10.1080/10615806.2014.987666 (2015). 10.1080/10615806.2014.987666 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Heber, E. et al. Efficacy and cost-effectiveness of a web-based and mobile stress-management intervention for employees: Design of a randomized controlled trial. BMC Public Health13, 655. 10.1186/1471-2458-13-655 (2013). 10.1186/1471-2458-13-655 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Moen, P., Kelly, E. L., Tranby, E. & Huang, Q. Changing work, changing health: Can real work-time flexibility promote health behaviors and well-being?. J. Health Soc. Behav.52, 404–429. 10.1177/0022146511418979 (2011). 10.1177/0022146511418979 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Richardson, K. M. & Rothstein, H. R. Effects of occupational stress management intervention programs: A meta-analysis. J. Occup. Health Psychol.13, 69–93. 10.1037/1076-8998.13.1.69 (2008). 10.1037/1076-8998.13.1.69 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Sheppard, W. D. II., Staggers, F. J. & John, L. The effects of a stress management program in a high security governments agency. Anxiety Stress Coping10, 341–350. 10.1080/10615809708249308 (1997). 10.1080/10615809708249308 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Willert, M. V., Thulstrup, A. M., Hertz, J. & Bonde, J. P. Changes in stress and coping from a randomized controlled trial of a three-month stress management intervention. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health35, 145–152. 10.5271/sjweh.1313 (2009). 10.5271/sjweh.1313 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Crosswell, A. D. & Lockwood, K. G. Best practices for stress measurement: How to measure psychological stress in health research. Health Psychol. Open.10.1177/2055102920933072 (2020). 10.1177/2055102920933072 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Kanner, A. D., Coyne, J. C., Schaefer, C. & Lazarus, R. S. Comparison of two modes of stress measurement: Daily hassles and uplifts versus major life events. J. Behav. Med.4, 1–39. 10.1007/BF00844845 (1981). 10.1007/BF00844845 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K.-M. & Hellhammer, D. H. The ,trier social stress test,—A tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology28, 76–81. 10.1159/000119004 (1993). 10.1159/000119004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Littman, A. J., White, E., Satia, J. A., Bowen, D. J. & Kristal, A. R. Reliability and validity of 2 single-item measures of psychosocial stress. Epidemiology17, 398–403. 10.1097/01.ede.0000219721.89552.51 (2006). 10.1097/01.ede.0000219721.89552.51 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Milfont, T., Denny, S., Ameratunga, S., Robinson, E. & Merry, S. Burnout and wellbeing: Testing the Copenhagen Burnout Inventory in New Zealand teachers. Soc. Indic. Res.89, 169–177. 10.1007/s11205-007-9229-9 (2008). 10.1007/s11205-007-9229-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Shilton, A. L., Laycock, R. & Crewther, S. G. The maastricht acute stress test (MAST): Physiological and subjective responses in anticipation, and post-stress. Front. Psychol.8, 567. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00567 (2017). 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00567 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Ben-Zur, H., Gil, S. & Shamshins, Y. The relationship between exposure to terror through the media, coping strategies and resources, and distress and secondary traumatization. Int. J. Stress Manag.19, 132–150. 10.1037/a0027864 (2012). 10.1037/a0027864 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Littleton, H., Horsley, S., John, S. & Nelson, D. V. Trauma coping strategies and psychological distress: A meta-analysis. J. Trauma.20, 977–988. 10.1002/jts.20276 (2007). 10.1002/jts.20276 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Tomaka, J., Blascovich, J., Kelsey, R. M. & Leitten, C. L. Subjective, physiological, and behavioral effects of threat and challenge appraisal. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.65, 248–260. 10.1037/0022-3514.65.2.248 (1993). 10.1037/0022-3514.65.2.248 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Yagil, D., Ben-Zur, H. & Tamir, I. Do employees cope effectively with abusive supervision at work? An exploratory study. Int. J. Stress Manag.18, 5–23. 10.1037/a0020548 (2011). 10.1037/a0020548 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.McClelland, D. C. Testing for competence rather than for “Intelligence”. Amer. Psychol.28, 1–14. 10.1037/h0034092 (1973). 10.1037/h0034092 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Deming, W. E. Out of the Crisis. (Cambridge, Mass: MIT, Center for Advanced Educational Services, 1986).
  • 90.Quinn, R., Faerman, S., Thompson, M. & McGrath, M. Becoming a Master Manager: A Competency Framework (Wiley, 1990). [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Zook, Jr., A. M. Military Competency-Based Human Capital Management: A Step Toward the Future. U.S. Army War College. https://apps.dtic.mil/sti/pdfs/ADA448319.pdf (2006).
  • 92.Marrelli, A. F., Tondora, J. & Hoge, M. A. The fundamentals of workforce competency: Implications for behavioral health. Adm. Policy Ment Health32, 509–531. 10.1007/s104880053263-1 (2005). 10.1007/s104880053263-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Shippmann, J. S. et al. The practice of competency modeling. Pers. Psychol.53, 703–740. 10.1111/j.1744-6570.2000.tb00220.x (2000). 10.1111/j.1744-6570.2000.tb00220.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Spencer, L. M. & Spencer, S. M. Competence at work: Models for superior performance (Wiley, 1993). [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Warn, J. & Tranter, P. Measuring quality in higher education: A competency approach. Qual. High. Educ.7, 191–201. 10.1080/13538320120098078 (2001). 10.1080/13538320120098078 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Epstein, R. & Phan, V. Which competencies are most important for creative expression?. Creat. Res. J.24, 278–282. 10.1080/10400419.2012.726579 (2012). 10.1080/10400419.2012.726579 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Epstein, R., Warfel, R., Johnson, J., Smith, R. & McKinney, P. Which relationship skills count most?. J. Couple Relatsh. Ther.12, 297–313. 10.1080/15332691.2013.836047 (2013). 10.1080/15332691.2013.836047 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Epstein, R., Robertson, R. E., Smith, R., Vasconcellos, T. & Lao, M. Which relationship skills count most? A large-scale replication. J. Couple Relatsh. Ther.15, 341–356. 10.1080/15332691.2016.1141136 (2016). 10.1080/15332691.2016.1141136 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Epstein, R., Schmidt, S. M. & Warfel, R. Measuring and training creativity competencies: Validation of a new test. Creat. Res. J20, 7–12. 10.1080/10400410701839876 (2008). 10.1080/10400410701839876 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Epstein, R., Kaminaka, K., Phan, V. & Uda, R. How is creativity best managed? Some empirical and theoretical guidelines. Creativity Innov. Manag.22, 359–374. 10.1111/caim.12042 (2013). 10.1111/caim.12042 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Epstein, R., Ho, M. E., Scandalis, Z. & Ginther, A. The motivation competencies that count most: An online international study. Open Psychol. J.10.2174/18743501-v15-e2202030 (2022). 10.2174/18743501-v15-e2202030 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.De Neve, J.-E., Mikhaylov, S., Dawes, C. T., Christakis, N. A. & Fowler, J. H. Born to lead? A twin design and genetic association study of leadership role occupancy. Leadersh. Q.24, 45–60. 10.1016/j.leaqua.2012.08.001 (2013). 10.1016/j.leaqua.2012.08.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Heyman, G. D. Talking about success: Implications for achievement motivation. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol.29, 361–370. 10.1016/j.appdev.2008.06.003 (2008). 10.1016/j.appdev.2008.06.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Yeager, D. S. et al. The far-reaching effects of believing people can change: Implicit theories of personality shape stress, health, and achievement during adolescence. J. Pers. Soc Psychol.10.1037/a0036335 (2014). 10.1037/a0036335 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Aronson, J. & McGlone, M. S. Stereotype and Social Identity Threat. In Handbook of Prejudice, Stereotyping, and Discrimination (ed. Nelson, T. D.) 153–178 (Psychology Press, 2009). [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Miller, A. L. Stereotype threat as a psychological feature of work-life conflict. Perspect. Psychol. Sci.22(2), 302–320. 10.1177/1368430217735578 (2017). 10.1177/1368430217735578 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Steele, C. & Aronson, J. Stereotype threat and the intellectual test-performance of African-Americans. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.69, 797–811. 10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797 (1995). 10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Steele, C., Aronson, J. & Spencer, S. Contending with group image: The psychology of stereotype and social identity threat. Adv. Exp. Soc. Pyschol.34, 379–440. 10.1016/S0065-2601(02)80009-0 (2002). 10.1016/S0065-2601(02)80009-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.National Institutes of Health. What are competencies? https://hr.nih.gov/about/faq/working-nih/competencies/what-are-competencies (n.d.).
  • 110.Boyatzis, R. E. The competent manager: A model for effective performance (Wiley, 1982). [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Smith, E. A. Communities of competence: New resources in the workplace. J. Workplace Learn.17, 7–23. 10.1108/13665620510574423 (2005). 10.1108/13665620510574423 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Wood, R. & Payne, T. Competency-based recruitment and selection (Wiley, 1998). [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Folkman, S. & Lazarus, R. S. An analysis of coping in a middle-aged community sample. J. Health Soc. Beh.21, 219–239. 10.2307/2136617 (1980). 10.2307/2136617 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Lazarus, R. S., Averill, J. R. & Opton, E. M. Towards a cognitive theory of emotion. Feel. Emot.10.1016/B978-0-12-063550-4.50023-1 (1970). 10.1016/B978-0-12-063550-4.50023-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Stoneman, Z. & Gavidia-Payne, S. Marital adjustment in families of young children with disabilities: Associations with daily hassles and problem-focused coping. Am. J. Ment. Retard.111, 1. 10.1352/0895-8017(2006)111[1:MAIFOY]2.0.CO;2 (2006). 10.1352/0895-8017(2006)111[1:MAIFOY]2.0.CO;2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Ellis, A. Rational psychotherapy and individual psychology. J. Individ. Psychol.13, 38–44 (1957). [Google Scholar]
  • 117.David, D. Rational emotive behavior therapy in the context of modern psychological research. The Albert Ellis Institute.https://albertellis.org/rebt-in-the-context-of-modern-psychological-research/ (n.d.).
  • 118.Dubuc-Charbonneau, N. & Durand-Bush, N. Moving to action: The effects of a self-regulation intervention on the stress, burnout, well-being, and self-regulation capacity levels of university student-athletes. J. Clin. Sport Psychol.9, 173–192. 10.1123/jcsp.2014-0036 (2015). 10.1123/jcsp.2014-0036 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Keogh, E., Bond, F. W. & Flaxman, P. E. Improving academic performance and mental health through a stress management intervention: Outcomes and mediators of change. Behav. Res. Ther.44, 339 (2006). 10.1016/j.brat.2005.03.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Murphy, L. R. Stress management in work settings: A critical review of the health effects. Amer. J. H. Promo.11, 112–135 (1996). 10.4278/0890-1171-11.2.112 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J. & Skinner, E. A. Review: The development of coping across childhood and adolescence: An integrative review and critique of research. Int. J. Behav. Dev.35, 1–17. 10.1177/0165025410384923 (2011). 10.1177/0165025410384923 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D. & Wilson, K. G. Acceptance and Commitment Therapy: The Process and Practive of Mindful Change 2nd edn. (Guilford Press, 2011). [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Frögéli, E., Rudman, A. & Gustavsson, P. Preventing stress-related ill health among future nurses: Effects over 3 years. Int. J. Stress Manag.26, 272–286. 10.1037/str0000110 (2019). 10.1037/str0000110 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Querstret, D., Morison, L., Dickinson, S., Cropley, M. & John, M. Mindfulness-based stress reduction and mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for psychological health and well-being in nonclinical samples: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Stress Manag.27, 394–411. 10.1037/str0000165 (2020). 10.1037/str0000165 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Hayes, S. C. & Wilson, K. G. Acceptance and commitment therapy: Altering the verbal support for experiential avoidance. Behav. Anal.17, 289–303. 10.1007/bf03392677 (1994). 10.1007/bf03392677 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Beck, A. T. Thinking and depression: I. Idiosyncratic content and cognitive distortions. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry9, 324–333 (1963). 10.1001/archpsyc.1963.01720160014002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Beck, A. T. Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects (Harper & Row, 1967). [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Chand, S. P., Kuckel, D. P. & Huecker, M. R. Cognitive behavior therapy (StatPearls Publishing, 2023). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Giga, S. I., Cooper, C. L. & Faragher, B. The development of a framework for a comprehensive approach to stress management interventions at work. Int. J. Stress Manag.10, 280–296. 10.1037/1072-5245.10.4.280 (2003). 10.1037/1072-5245.10.4.280 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Folkman, S., Lazarus, R. S., Dunkel-Schetter, C., DeLongis, A. & Gruen, R. J. Dynamics of a stressful encounter: Cognitive appraisal, coping, and encounter outcomes. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.50, 992–1003. 10.1037/0022-3514.50.5.992 (1986). 10.1037/0022-3514.50.5.992 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Selye, H. A syndrome produced by diverse nocuous agents. Nature138, 32. 10.1038/138032a0 (1936). 10.1038/138032a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Jacobson, E. Progressive relaxation: A physiological and clinical investigation of muscular states and their significance in psychology and medical practice (University of Chicago Press, 1929). [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Jacobson, E. Progressive relaxation. Am. J. Psychol.136, 73–87. 10.2307/1413507 (1925). 10.2307/1413507 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Pawlow, L. A. & Jones, G. E. The impact of abbreviated progressive muscle relaxation on salivary cortisol and salivary immunoglobulin A (sIgA). Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback30, 375–387. 10.1007/s10484-005-8423-2 (2005). 10.1007/s10484-005-8423-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Smith, C., Hancock, H., Blake-Mortimer, J. & Eckert, K. A randomized comparative trial of yoga and relaxation to reduce stress and anxiety. Complement Ther. Med.15, 77–83. 10.1016/j.ctim.2006.05.001 (2007). 10.1016/j.ctim.2006.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Cohen, J. A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educ. Psychol. Meas.20, 37–46. 10.1177/001316446002000104 (1960). 10.1177/001316446002000104 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Schrepp, M. On the usage of Cronbach’s alpha to measure reliability of ux scales. J. Usability Stud.15, 247–258 (2020). [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Taber, K. S. The use of Cronbach’s alpha when developing and reporting research instruments in science education. Res. Sci. Educ.48, 1273–1296. 10.1007/s11165-016-9602-2 (2018). 10.1007/s11165-016-9602-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Tavakol, M. & Dennick, R. Making sense of Cronbach’s alpha. Int. J. Med. Educ.2, 53–55. 10.5116/ijme.4dfb.8dfd (2011). 10.5116/ijme.4dfb.8dfd [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.American Educational Research Association. Standards for educational and psychological testing. (2014).
  • 141.Conover, W. J. Practical nonparametric statistics 3rd edn. (Wiley, 1999). [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Tomczak, M. & Tomczak, E. The need to report effect size estimates revisited. An overview of some recommended measures of effect size. Trends Sport Sci.1, 19–25 (2014). [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Strauss, V. East Asians topped U.S. students again on international tests. But are their schools really better?. The Washington Post.https://www.washingtonpost.com/news/answer-sheet/wp/2016/11/30/east-asians-topped-u-s-students-again-on-international-tests-but-are-their-schools-really-better/ (2016).
  • 144.Asakawa, K. & Csikszentmihalyi, M. The quality of experience of Asian American adolescents in activities related to future goals. J. Youth Adolesc.27, 141–163. 10.1023/a:1021659612041 (1998). 10.1023/a:1021659612041 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Tweed, R. G., White, K. & Lehman, D. R. Culture, stress, and coping: Internally- and externally-targeted control strategies of European Canadians, East Asian Canadians, and Japanese. J. Cross Cult Psychol.35, 652–668. 10.1177/0022022104270109 (2004). 10.1177/0022022104270109 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Goodwin, K. A. & Goodwin, C. J. Research in psychology: Methods and design 8th edn. (Wiley, 2016). [Google Scholar]
  • 147.Reips, U. D. Using the Internet to collect data. In APA handbook of research methods in psychology (eds Cooper, H. et al.) (American Psychological Association, 2012). [Google Scholar]
  • 148.Kühberger, A., Fritz, A. & Scherndl, T. Publication bias in psychology: A diagnosis based on the correlation between effect size and sample size. PLoS One9, e105825. 10.1371/journal.pone.0105825 (2014). 10.1371/journal.pone.0105825 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 149.Dillman, D. A., Smyth, J. D. & Christian, L. M. 2014 Internet, phone, mail, and mixed-mode surveys: The tailored design method (Wiley, 2014). [Google Scholar]
  • 150.Durant, L. E., Carey, M. P. & Schroder, K. E. Effects of anonymity, gender, and erotophilia on the quality of data obtained from self-reports of socially sensitive behaviors. J. Behav. Med.25, 438–467. 10.1023/a:1020419023766 (2002). 10.1023/a:1020419023766 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 151.Joinson, A. Social desirability, anonymity, and internet-based questionnaires. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comp.31, 433–438. 10.3758/BF03200723 (1999). 10.3758/BF03200723 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 152.Krumpal, I. Determinants of social desirability bias in sensitive surveys: A literature review. Qual. Quant.47, 2025–2047. 10.1007/s11135-011-9640-9 (2013). 10.1007/s11135-011-9640-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 153.Villarroel, M. A. et al. Same-gender sex in the United States impact of T-ACASI on prevalence estimates. Public Opin. Quart.70, 166–196. 10.1093/poq/nfj023 (2006). 10.1093/poq/nfj023 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 154.Robertson, R. E., Tran, F. W., Lewark, L. N. & Epstein, R. Estimates of non-heterosexual prevalence: The roles of anonymity and privacy in survey methodology. Arch. Sex. Behav.47, 1069–1084. 10.1007/s10508-017-1044-z (2018). 10.1007/s10508-017-1044-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 155.Dwight, S. A. & Feigelson, M. E. A quantitative review of the effect of computerized testing on the measurement of social desirability. Educ. Psychol. Meas.60, 340–360. 10.1177/00131640021970583 (2000). 10.1177/00131640021970583 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 156.Ong, A. D. & Weiss, D. J. Impact of anonymity on responses to sensitive questions. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol.30, 1691–1708. 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2000.tb02462.x (2000). 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2000.tb02462.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 157.Chang, E. C., Tugade, M. M. & Asakawa, K. 2006 Stress and coping among Asian Americans: Lazarus and Folkman’s model and beyond. In Stress and coping: A multicultural perspective (eds Scott, C. & Wong, P. T.) (Kluwer Academic Publishers, 2006). [Google Scholar]
  • 158.Markus, H. R. & Kitayama, S. Cultures and selves: A cycle of mutual constitution. Perspect. Psychol. Sci.5, 420–430. 10.1177/1745691610375557 (2010). 10.1177/1745691610375557 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 159.Triandis, H. C. The psychological measurement of cultural syndromes. Am. Psychol.51, 407–415. 10.1037/0003-066X.51.4.407 (1996). 10.1037/0003-066X.51.4.407 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 160.Hagen, I. & Nayar, U. S. Yoga for children and young people’s mental health and well-being: Research review and reflections on the mental health potentials of yoga. Front. Psychiatry.10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00035 (2014). 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00035 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 161.Harkess, K. N., Delfabbro, P., Mortimer, J., Hannaford, Z. & Cohen-Woods, S. Brief report on the psychophysiological effects of a yoga intervention for chronic stress: Preliminary findings. J. Psychophysiol.31, 38–48. 10.1027/0269-8803/a000169 (2017). 10.1027/0269-8803/a000169 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 162.Krueger, E. A., Meyer, I. H. & Upchurch, D. M. Sexual orientation group differences in perceived stress and depressive symptoms among young adults in the United States. LGBT Health5, 242–249. 10.1089/lgbt.2017.0228 (2018). 10.1089/lgbt.2017.0228 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 163.Gonsiorek, J. C. Mental health issues of gay and lesbian adolescents. J. Adolesc. Health Care9, 114–122. 10.1016/0197-0070(88)90057-5 (1988). 10.1016/0197-0070(88)90057-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 164.Herek, G. M. & Garnets, L. D. Sexual orientation and mental health. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol.3, 353–375. 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091510 (2007). 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091510 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 165.Lewis, R. J., Derlega, V. J., Griffin, J. L. & Krowinski, A. C. Stressors for gay men and lesbians: Life stress, gay-related stress, stigma consciousness, and depressive symptoms. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol.22, 716–729. 10.1521/jscp.22.6.716.22932 (2003). 10.1521/jscp.22.6.716.22932 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 166.El-Shormilisy, N., Strong, J. & Meredith, P. J. Associations between gender, coping patterns and functioning for individuals with chronic pain: A systematic review. Pain Res. Manag.20, 48–55. 10.1155/2015/490610 (2015). 10.1155/2015/490610 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 167.Mejía, O. L. & McCarthy, C. J. Acculturative stress, depression, and anxiety in migrant farmwork college students of Mexican heritage. Int. J. Stress Manag.17, 1–20. 10.1037/a0018119 (2010). 10.1037/a0018119 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 168.Bailey, J. M. et al. Sexual orientation, controversy, and science. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest17, 45–101 (2016). 10.1177/1529100616637616 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 169.Just the Facts Coalition. Just the facts about sexual orientation and youth: A primer for principals, educators, and school personnel. (American Psychological Association, 2008).
  • 170.Mendos, L. R., et al. State-sponsored homophobia 2020: Global legislation overview updata. (ILGA, 2020).
  • 171.Green, D., & Levy, C. eCampusOntario open competency toolkit. https://ecampusontario.pressbooks.pub/competencytoolkit/front-matter/introduction/ (2021).
  • 172.Travis, J., Kaszycki, A., Geden, M. & Bunde, J. Some stress is good stress: The challenge-hindrance framework, academic self-efficacy, and academic outcomes. J. Educ. Psychol.112, 1632–1643. 10.1037/edu0000478 (2020). 10.1037/edu0000478 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 173.Kirby, E. D. et al. Acute stress enhances adult rat hippocampal neurogenesis and activation of newborn neurons via secreted astrocytic FGF2. eLife10.7554/eLife.00362 (2013). 10.7554/eLife.00362 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 174.McGonigal, K. 2016 The upside of stress Why stress is good for you and how to get good at it (Avery, 2016). [Google Scholar]
  • 175.Shields, G. S., Lam, J. C., Trainor, B. C. & Yonelinas, A. P. Exposure to acute stress enhances decision-making competence: Evidence for the role of DHEA. Psychoneuroendocr.67, 51–60. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.01.031 (2016). 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.01.031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 176.Lepine, J., Podsakoff, N. & Lepine, M. A meta-analytic test of the challenge stressor-hindrance stressor framework: An explanation for inconsistent relationships among stressors and performance. Acad. Manage. J.48, 764–775. 10.5465/AMJ.2005.18803921 (2005). 10.5465/AMJ.2005.18803921 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 177.Chang, E. C., D’Zurilla, T. J. & Sanna, L. J. Social problem solving as a mediator of the link between stress and psychological well-being in middle-adulthood. Cognit. Ther. Res.33, 33–49. 10.1007/s10608-007-9155-9 (2009). 10.1007/s10608-007-9155-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 178.Li, C., Li, H. & Niu, J. Intercultural stressors of Chinese immigrant students: Voices of Chinese-American mental health professionals. Asian Am. J. Psychol.7, 64–73. 10.1037/aap0000044 (2016). 10.1037/aap0000044 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 179.Peeters, M. C. W. & Oerlemans, W. G. M. The relationship between acculturation orientations and work-related well-being: Differences between ethnic minority and majority employees. Int. J. Stress Manag.16, 1–24. 10.1037/a0014832 (2009). 10.1037/a0014832 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 180.Ghannam, J., Afana, A., Ho, E. Y., Al-Khal, A. & Bylund, C. L. The impact of a stress management intervention on medical residents' stress and burnout. Int. J. Stress Manag.27, 65–73. 10.1037/str0000125 (2020). 10.1037/str0000125 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 181.Grover, K. E. et al. Problem solving moderates the effects of life event stress and chronic stress on suicidal behaviors in adolescence. J. Clin. Psychol.65, 1281–1290. 10.1002/jclp.20632 (2009). 10.1002/jclp.20632 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 182.Nezu, A. M., Nezu, C. M. & Blissett, S. E. Sense of humor as a moderator of the relation between stressful events and psychological distress: A prospective analysis. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.54, 520–525 (1988). 10.1037/0022-3514.54.3.520 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 183.Overholser, J. C. Sense of humor when coping with life stress. Pers. Individ. Differ.13, 799–804. 10.1016/0191-8869(92)90053-R (1992). 10.1016/0191-8869(92)90053-R [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 184.Staniford, A. K., Dollard, M. F. & Guerin, B. Stress and help-seeking for drought-stricken citrus growers in the riverland of South Australia. Aust. J. Rural Health17, 147–154. 10.1111/j.1440-1584.2009.01059.x (2009). 10.1111/j.1440-1584.2009.01059.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 185.Yarker, J., Lewis, R., Donaldson-Feilder, E. & Flaxman, P. Management competencies for preventing and reducing stress at work (University of London, 2008). [Google Scholar]
  • 186.Zhao, J., Li, X. & Shields, J. Managing job burnout: The effects of emotion-regulation ability, emotional labor, and positive and negative affect at work. Int. J. Stress Manag.26, 315–320. 10.1037/str0000101 (2019). 10.1037/str0000101 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 187.Häfner, A., Stock, A., Pinneker, L. & Ströhle, S. Stress prevention through a time management training intervention: An experimental study. Edu. Psychol.34, 403–416. 10.1080/01443410.2013.785065 (2013). 10.1080/01443410.2013.785065 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 188.Dolbier, C. L. & Rush, T. E. Efficacy of abbreviated progressive muscle relaxation in a high-stress college sample. Int. J. Stress Manag.19, 48–68. 10.1037/a0027326 (2012). 10.1037/a0027326 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 189.Hourani, L. et al. Effect of stress inoculation training with relaxation breathing on perceived stress and posttraumatic stress disorder in the military: A longitudinal study. Int. J. Stress Manag.25, 124–136. 10.1037/str0000082 (2018). 10.1037/str0000082 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 190.Kantziari, M. A. et al. Stress management during the second trimester of pregnancy. Int. J. Stress Manag.26, 102–105. 10.1037/str0000078 (2019). 10.1037/str0000078 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 191.Magnon, V., Dutheil, F. & Vallet, G. T. Benefits from one session of deep and slow breathing on vagal tone and anxiety in young and older adults. Sci. Rep.11, 19267. 10.1038/s41598-021-98736-9 (2021). 10.1038/s41598-021-98736-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 192.Tragea, C., Chrousos, G. P., Alexopoulos, E. C. & Darviri, C. A randomized controlled trial of the effects of a stress management program during pregnancy. Complement Ther. Med.22, 203–211. 10.1016/j.ctim.2014.01.006 (2014). 10.1016/j.ctim.2014.01.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 193.West, J., Liang, B. & Spinazzola, J. Trauma sensitive yoga as a complementary treatment for posttraumatic stress disorder: A qualitative descriptive analysis. Int. J. Stress Manag.24, 173–195. 10.1037/str0000040 (2017). 10.1037/str0000040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

Anonymized raw data employed in the present study will be made available at Zenodo.com upon acceptance of the manuscript. Anonymized raw data can also be requested by writing to info@aibrt.org. Anonymization was required to comply with the requirement of the sponsoring institution’s Institutional Review Board that the identities of the participants be protected in accordance with HHS Federal Regulation 45 CFR 46.101.(b)(2).


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES