Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2024 Aug 30;19(8):e0309287. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0309287

Effect of matcha green tea on cognitive functions and sleep quality in older adults with cognitive decline: A randomized controlled study over 12 months

Kazuhiko Uchida 1,2,*,#, Kohji Meno 2,#, Tatsumi Korenaga 2, Shan Liu 2, Hideaki Suzuki 2, Yoshitake Baba 3, Chika Tagata 3, Yoshiharu Araki 3, Shuto Tsunemi 3, Kenta Aso 3, Shun Inagaki 3, Sae Nakagawa 3, Makoto Kobayashi 3, Tatsuyuki Kakuma 4, Takashi Asada 5,6, Miho Ota 7, Takanobu Takihara 3, Tetsuaki Arai 7
Editor: Madepalli K Lakshmana8
PMCID: PMC11364242  PMID: 39213264

Abstract

Objective

Lifestyle habits after middle age significantly impact the maintenance of cognitive function in older adults. Nutritional intake is closely related to lifestyle habits; therefore, nutrition is a pivotal factor in the prevention of dementia in the preclinical stages. Matcha green tea powder (matcha), which contains epigallocatechin gallate, theanine, and caffeine, has beneficial effects on cognitive function and mood. We conducted a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled clinical study over 12 months to examine the effect of matcha on cognitive function and sleep quality.

Methods

Ninety-nine participants, including 64 with subjective cognitive decline and 35 with mild cognitive impairment were randomized, with 49 receiving 2 g of matcha and 50 receiving a placebo daily. Participants were stratified based on two factors: age at baseline and APOE genotype. Changes in cognitive function and sleep quality were analyzed using a mixed-effects model.

Results

Matcha consumption led to significant improvements in social acuity score (difference; -1.39, 95% confidence interval; -2.78, 0.002) (P = 0.028) as evaluated by the perception of facial emotions in cognitive function. The primary outcomes, that is, Montreal Cognitive Assessment and Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study Activity of Daily Living scores, showed no significant changes with matcha intervention. Meanwhile, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index scores indicated a trend toward improvement with a difference of 0.86 (95% confidence interval; -0.002, 1.71) (P = 0.088) between the groups in changes from baseline to 12 months.

Conclusions

The present study suggests regular consumption of matcha could improve emotional perception and sleep quality in older adults with mild cognitive decline. Given the widespread availability and cultural acceptance of matcha green tea, incorporating it into the daily routine may offer a simple yet effective strategy for cognitive enhancement and dementia prevention.

Introduction

Dementia, for which aging is the greatest risk factor, is a social burden in an aging society. According to the World Alzheimer Report 2022, the number of dementia patients will increase from 57.4 million in 2019 to 152.8 million in 2050 [1]. Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and related disorders have multifactorial and complex etiologies [2]. Despite advancements in the development of therapeutic agents, no curative therapies are currently available.

Accumulating evidence suggests that lifestyle habits after middle age (approximately 45–54 years-old) significantly impact the maintenance of cognitive function in older adults [35]. Tea intake is closely related to lifestyle habits. Matcha green tea powder (matcha), which contains epigallocatechin gallate (EGCG), theanine, and caffeine, has the beneficial effects of each constituent on cognitive functions and mood [68]. Catechins, key components of green tea, are polyphenols with antioxidant and anti-inflammatory properties. The oral administration of EGCG has been reported to reduce Aβ deposition in the olfactory cortex and hippocampus of AD mice [9]. Theanine has been shown to work synergistically with catechins to alleviate stress and improve sleep in both clinical and animal studies [10]. A cross-sectional observational study revealed that the risk of cognitive decline, indicated by a score of < 26 on the Mini Mental State Examination-Japanese version (MMSE-J), was significantly lower in the group that consumed ≥ 200 ml of green tea per day, indicating a positive correlation between regular green tea consumption and maintained cognitive function [11]. With a cutoff point of 26 in MMSE-J score, the odds ratios for the different frequencies of green tea consumption were 0.42 for 2–3 cups per day, compared with 1.00 (reference) for 3 cups per week. Two to three cups of green tea contain 135–200 mg of catechins, with green tea providing 67.5 mg of catechins per 100 ml [11, 12]. In comparison, in a traditional tea ceremony, Tea Otemae, 2 g of matcha contain 170 mg of catechins.

Randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trials have reported beneficial effects of matcha on cognitive function [6, 7, 13]. However, the intervention periods in these studies were relatively short, lasting only 12 weeks. Longer intervention periods, extending several months or more, are required to comprehensively evaluate the effects of matcha consumption as a lifestyle habit.

Aging causes sleep quality to deteriorate. Approximately 20% of adults in Japan experience sleep disturbance [14, 15], and 50–70 million people have sleep disorders in the United States [16]. Sleep disturbance is a risk factor for AD, and sleep deprivation leads to increased inflammation and amyloid beta (Aβ) burden in the brain [17, 18]. A sleep duration of ≤ 6 h at ages 50 and 60 years, compared with a 7-h sleep duration, was associated with a 30% increased dementia risk in the Whitehall II study [19]. Thus, sleep quality during midlife may contribute to the pathophysiology of cognitive impairment in old age, and improving sleep quality may prevent dementia.

Preventing disease progression and disease onset is crucial for mitigating the prevalence of dementia and other lifestyle-related diseases. Controlling blood pressure and blood sugar levels in midlife is vital for the prevention of cardiovascular disease and diabetes mellitus, respectively. Several cohort studies have identified blood-based biomarkers for dementia risk assessment, including phosphorylated tau-217 [20], α2-macroglobulin [21], apolipoproteins [22], and complement proteins [23]. Indicators of protein nutrition are likely associated with the disease processes in AD [2426]. Llewellyn et al. reported an association between low serum albumin levels and risk of cognitive impairment in older individuals [26]. In our previous studies, a composite marker of apoA1, transthyretin, and complement protein C3 in the serum, which are involved in lipid metabolism, protein nutrition, and inflammation, could differentiate between older adults with cognitive impairment and those without dementia at 74–84% accuracy [27, 28]. Thus, lifestyle habits and related biomarkers are pivotal factors in the prevention of dementia in the preclinical stages.

Here, we conducted a 12-month randomized, double-blind, and placebo-controlled clinical trial to assess the potential of matcha in improving sleep quality and delaying cognitive decline in older adults.

Materials and methods

Study design

A 12-month intervention was executed employing a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel-group design. Before the initiation of the study, the clinical trial was registered with the UMIN Clinical Trials Registry (UMIN-CTR), which is recognized by the International Committee of Medical Journal Editors (ICMJE) as an approved registry. This study was conducted and reported as a randomized controlled trial, adhering strictly to the CONSORT guidelines.

A meta-analysis was conducted to ascertain whether the effect size and statistical power were commensurate with the determined sample size, referencing three intervention studies that employed the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) as the evaluative criterion [2931]. Although the efficacy indices in this study vary slightly from those in previous research, we concluded that observing the anticipated effects outlined in the meta-analysis would render the data from all 66 patients a significant evidence base for preparing the subsequent validation study.

The required sample size was calculated using G*Power 3 software (Heinrich Heine University, Düsseldorf, Germany). We chose to assess the difference between the medians of two independent groups using the Wilcoxon or Mann–Whitney test. With the parameters set as two-tailed, logistic distribution, an effect size of 0.7, a significance level of 0.05, and a power of 0.8, it was determined that each group required 31 cases. Anticipating a dropout rate of 7 of 40 recruited cases per group, we established a target sample size of at least 33 cases per group to validate the robustness of our findings (details are described in S1 File).

Randomization

Sequence generation

Eligible participants with SCD or MCI were randomized using a computer-generated sequence. Participants were stratified based on two stratification factors, age at baseline (≥ 74 years / < 74 years) and APOE genotype (ε4 positive / negative). Participants were stratified based on an age cut-off of 74-years, because the 10-year AD risk is higher in people ≥ 74 years old than in those < 74 years old [32]. Allocation to either the control or intervention group was intended to be randomized in a 1:1 ratio using computerized minimization, which aims to minimize differences between groups with respect to the stratification factors [33].

Allocation concealment mechanism

Participants underwent randomization using a dynamic program implemented via a Microsoft Excel macro, employing simple randomization rather than permuted block randomization. Upon running the program, the participants were assigned to either Group A or Group B. However, the program did not reveal which was the intervention or control group. The randomization table created by the program was retained until all baseline stratification factors were fully aligned and participant inclusion was complete, ensuring concealment of the allocation.

All eligible participants who provided informed consent and met the predefined inclusion criteria underwent randomization, a process administered at the discretion of the study site personnel overseeing recruitment and medical interviews. Subsequently, the study site transmitted a stratification factor response form to an independent allocation manager who was responsible for the allocation process, ensuring separation from the outcome evaluation analysis. The allocation manager utilized a dynamic randomization program incorporating provided stratification factors to assign participants to their respective groups based on the program’s output. Physicians and nurses responsible for participant inclusion and symptom assessment remained blinded to group assignments.

The allocation sequence utilized a stratified approach, considering study site and medication compliance, employing a dynamic allocation method with the minimization technique. Concealment of randomization details from data management personnel and statistical analysts was upheld throughout the study while ensuring the database remained publicly accessible. The randomization list was securely maintained by designated allocation personnel throughout the study, mitigating any potential influence from principal investigators involved in the research.

Blinding

Throughout the study duration, participants were unaware of whether the capsules they consumed contained matcha or placebo until the study’s conclusion was obtained. Physicians, nurses, and principal investigators at the study site were informed that the participants had been assigned to provisional groups (Groups A and B) yet remained uninformed about the specific allocation to either the matcha or placebo group until the randomization list was released after the study period. No member of the research team knew the allocated sequence until the end of the statistical analysis, after the database was locked.

Participants

Community-dwelling older adults aged 60–85 years were recruited and assessment for eligibility was performed at the University of Tsukuba Hospital (Tsukuba, Ibaraki, Japan) and Memory Clinic Toride (Toride, Ibaraki, Japan) (Fig 1). Participants were enrolled based on predefined inclusion and exclusion criteria as follows: (1) Japanese-speaking men and women aged 60–85 years living with a study partner responsible for checking capsule intake and escorting visitors to the hospital, (2) presence of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) or subjective cognitive decline (SCD) assessed as described in the study design and participants, (3) no history of treatment for any serious disease (cancer, myocardial infarction, stroke) within 5 years; (4) no dementia treatment (donepezil, galantamine, rivastigmine, or memantine); (5) no brain atrophy seen in dementia on magnetic resonance imaging (MRI); (6) no participation in daycare interventions such as exercise intervention or cognitive training; and (7) no history of interventions such as exercise intervention or cognitive training. The exclusion criteria were as follows: (1) diagnosed with dementia or major depressive disorder at baseline; (2) MMSE-J score < 24; (3) history of therapeutic inpatient surgery due to a stroke, subarachnoid hemorrhage, or other head injury; (4) using medications that may affect this study (e.g., antipsychotics, anxiolytics, antidepressants); and (5) regular consumption of functional foods or more than seven cups of matcha per week that may affect the maintenance of cognitive function.

Fig 1. Study design and flowchart of participants throughout the study.

Fig 1

MCI, mild cognitive impairment; SCD, subjective cognitive decline.

In this study, we included patients with SCD and those with MCI to evaluate the effects of matcha consumption in the pre-stage of cognitive impairment. Although SCD is not classified as a disease, an increasing number of studies have suggested an association between SCD and an elevated risk of developing cognitive impairment [34, 35].

Participants with SCD and MCI were randomly divided into matcha and placebo groups (see Materials and Methods in S1 File). No member of the research team knew the allocated sequence until the end of the statistical analysis, after the database was locked. Clinical information, including neuropsychological test data, was collected, and managed using the Alliance Clinical Research Supporting System (ACReSS) V01L51 provided by the University Hospital Clinical Trial Alliance (UHCT) in Japan.

Neurocognitive tests were performed before and after the intervention (baseline and 12 months) and 6 months after the intervention. Amyloid PET was performed at the baseline and 12 months after the intervention in 12 participants each in the matcha group and placebo group. Written informed consent was obtained from all the eligible participants. The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and all protocols used in this study were approved by the Institutional Ethics Committee of the University of Tsukuba, Memory Clinic Toride, and MCBI. Prior to inclusion in the study, written informed consent was obtained from each participant, and all samples were rendered anonymous before being sent to the laboratory for analysis. We actively embraced the principles of diversity, equity, and inclusion (DEI) throughout the entirety of our research process. Our commitment to DEI was reflected not only in our team composition but also in our study design, execution, and interpretation of results. Each participant was treated with respect, dignity, and fairness, and every effort was made to ensure that the research process was culturally competent and responsive to the needs of the diverse participants.

Procedures for diagnosis

The MCI and SCD criteria were applied to individuals without dementia after the conference. The team reviewed functional, medical, neurological, psychiatric, and neuropsychological data to reach a diagnostic consensus on dementia and MCI according to the DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) criteria. SCD was defined according to recent criteria [36].

Consistent with the standard criteria, for all subtypes of MCI described below, the participants considered to have MCI were required to have (1) objective impairment in one cognitive domain based on the average of scores on neuropsychological measures within that domain, and 1 standard deviation (SD) and 1.5 SD cutoffs derived from normative corrections for age, years of education, and sex; (2) essentially preserved activities of daily living; (3) presence of memory complaints; and (4) no diagnosis of dementia by group consensus.

The participants labeled as having SCD were required to have (1) no diagnosis of MCI or dementia; (2) subjective complaints of forgetfulness; (3) decline in cognitive functioning unrelated to an event explaining the cognitive deficits; (4) Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) global score 0 and (5) MMSE-J score between 27 and 30, mild hippocampal atrophy on MRI, or mild regional cerebellar blood flow (rCBF) reduction on single-photon emission computed tomography (SPECT).

Intervention and outcome

The participants were required to consume nine capsules of matcha (equivalent to 2 g of matcha) or placebo daily at any time. As the amount of matcha in the traditional tea ceremony, Tea Otemae, is 2 g, the daily intake quantity of matcha was set at 2 g in this study. The matcha used, “Hojin no shiro” (provided by ITO EN, LTD., Tokyo, Japan) contained 170.8 mg of catechin, 48.1 mg of theanine, and 66.2 mg of caffeine per daily serving. The catechins consisted of 105.3 mg of EGCG, 1.5 mg of gallocatechin gallate, 20.3 mg of epicatechin gallate, 0.1 mg of catechin gallate, 33.1 mg of epigallocatechin, 1.6 mg of gallocatechin, 8.1 mg of epicatechin, and 0.8 mg of catechin. The placebo capsules, which were identical in appearance, color, and odor to the matcha capsules, were composed of cornstarch. Both capsules consisted of white porcine gelatin. A daily supply (nine capsules) was sealed in a sachet, and a large bag containing 1 month’s supply of capsules was provided to the participants every 3 months. During the 3-, 6-, 9-, and 12-month visits throughout the intervention periods, intake compliance was assessed by counting the number of capsules that remained in the bag.

Primary outcomes measures were Montreal Cognitive Assessment-Japanese version (MoCA-J) and Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study Activity of Daily Living (ADCS-MCI-ADL) scores, and secondary outcome measures were MMSE-J, Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status-Japanese version (RBANS), Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale-Cognitive Subscale-Japanese version (ADAS-Jcog), CNS vital signs computerized neurocognitive battery, Cognitrax [37, 38], and the Japanese version of the Pittsburg Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) scores.

Cognitive function and sleep quality measures

Cognitive function was assessed using MMSE-J, MoCA-J, ADAS-Jcog, RBANS, and CNS vital signs. CNS vital signs contains 6 neurocognitive domain scores assessed by 10 tests. In this study, we used the stroop test and tests for shift attention, continuous performance, and perception of emotion to evaluate cognitive functioning, based on neurocognitive domain scores of social acuity, reaction time, complex attention, cognitive flexibility, executive function, and simple attention (S1 Table). ADCS-ADL-MCI, with 24 items, was used to describe the participants’ ADL [39, 40]. Sleep quality was measured by PSQI [41].

Neuroimaging

Amyloid PET was performed at the AIC Imaging and Inspection Center in Tsukuba, Japan. Scans were performed using a TruePoint Biograph-PET-CT system (Siemens AG, Munich, Germany). 18F-Florbetapir (Fujifilm, Tokyo, Japan) were used as imaging agents. Transmission imaging (CT imaging for attenuation correction) was performed for approximately 30 s, and emission imaging (PET imaging) was performed in ListMode for 20 min. The standard uptake ratio (SUVR) was calculated from acquired PET images.

The voxel-based specific regional analysis system for atrophy detection (VSRAD) approach in MRI allows the evaluation of atrophy in specific regions of interest (ROIs) within medial temporal structures, particularly the entorhinal cortex, hippocampus, and amygdala. Each processed segmented image was compared with the mean and standard deviation of gray matter or white matter images derived from 80 healthy (40 male and 40 female, aged 54 and 86 years) controls. The severity of atrophic changes was calculated using the Z-score, defined as follows: Z-score = ([control mean]–[individual value]) / (control standard deviation). Each voxel had dimensions of 2 × 2 × 2 mm. The severity of ROI atrophy was determined by using the following equation: sum of positive Z-scores in the target ROI/total number of voxels in the target ROI. ROI atrophy was categorized into four states: 0–1 representing normal, 1–2 mild, 2–3 moderate, and > 3 severe, based on the reference literature [42].

The rCBF was evaluated using SPECT. The Z-score also allowed comparisons of the SPECT scans of participants and age-matched normal controls. Statistical analysis of the perfusion reduction was conducted. Severity perfusion reduction in (posterior cingulate gyrus, precuneus, and parietal cortex) was calculated using the following equation: severity = sum of positive Z-scores in the target ROI / total voxels in the target ROI [43].

Serum and plasma sampling

Serum and EDTA plasma stored at −80°C were transferred to the laboratory for analysis and thawed on ice; 40-μl aliquots were generated and stored at −80°C for immunoassay. Almost all serum samples were frozen and thawed twice, whereas some were frozen and thawed three times. Freezing and thawing up to three times did not affect the results of the immunoassay.

Immunoassay

Plasma Aβ1–40 and Aβ1–42 levels were determined by immunoassay (Euroimmun, Lübeck, Germany). Plasma levels of amyloid precursor protein (APP) and beta-site amyloid precursor protein cleaving enzyme 1 (BACE1), which are involved in Aβ production, were measured using enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay (ELISA). The human sAPPα assay kit (IBL 27734) and human sAPPβ-w assay kit (IBL 27732) were purchased from Immuno-Biological Laboratories (Gumma, Japan), and BACE-1 ELISA (Euroimmun) was used. The immunoassay kit for brain-derived neurotrophic factor was purchased from R&D Systems (Minneapolis, MN). The blood levels of malondialdehyde (MDA)-modified low-density lipoprotein (MDA-LDL), IL-6, and IL-10 were measured at the Clinical Chemistry Laboratory (BIKEN, Osaka, Japan). Plasma levels of apoA1, transthyretin, and complement protein C3 were determined as previously described [28].

Statistical analyses

The analysis population consisted of all participants, excluding those who met the discontinuation criteria assessed by a physician, discontinued participation in the study, and conformed to the study protocol. We evaluated the effects of the matcha intervention on cognitive function and sleep quality measures using a mixed-effects model that flexibly modeled the variance structure of the effect measures, including intra-individual correlations generated by repeated measurements as follows. Specifically, we employed an unstructured variance that did not presuppose a specific correlation structure and included group effects, timing effects, and group-by-time interaction terms.

Yijt represents the measured value of the evaluation item at time point t (= 1,2,3) for participant I who was randomly assigned to group j (= 1,2). We assume the following mixed-effects model:

Yijt=α+βj+γt+θjt+εijt

α is the intercept, βj is the group effect, γt is the timing effect, θjt is the group and timing interaction, εijt is the random error mean following a normal distribution with zero variance structure V(εijt)=[σ12σ12σ13σ12σ22σ23σ13σ23σ32]

The primary hypothesis to be examined is a two-group comparison of the mean change between baseline and time point 3 (and time point 2). We denote this hypothesis as H:δt(13)=0vsK:δt(13)0. Here, δt(1–3) is the group difference in the mean change between baseline and time 3, defined as δt(13)=(θ11θ13)(θ21θ23). The two-group difference in mean change between baseline and time point 2 is defined as δt(1−2).

We also estimated the change (mean difference) by time period, Δ(BL, 6M) = 6M –baseline or Δ(BL, 12M) = 12M-baseline. The difference between groups was defined by the following equation to estimate the difference in means (Estimate) between groups. Estimate ((difference of means A6M –A0M)–(difference of means B6M –B0M)), or Estimate ((difference in means A12M –A0M)–(difference in means B12M –B0M)). A t-test was conducted with the null (H) and alternative (K) hypotheses. H: δ = 0, K: δ≠0. The null hypothesis is “no variation, no intervention effect” and the null hypothesis is rejected, which is “there is an intervention effect”. In a mixed-effects model, if an intervention effect is found, there is at least a timing effect and a group effect, and an interaction between timing and group is also found. If δ is negative, there is an interaction between time and group, which indicates is an intervention effect of matcha intake, meaning higher values in the matcha group.

For biomarker analysis, differences between the intervention and non-intervention groups were tested using the Mann–Whitney U test. The associations between the cognitive tests were analyzed using Spearman’s correlation coefficients. P-value of ≤ 0.05 was deemed significant.

Results

Participants

As shown in Fig 1, we recruited 939 community-dwelling older adults aged 60–85 years and 124 participants were enrolled (from May 30, 2019 to August 31, 2020) according to the inclusion and exclusion criteria at the University of Tsukuba Hospital (Tsukuba, Ibaraki, Japan) and Memory Clinic Toride (Toride, Ibaraki, Japan). A total of 99 participants were randomized and allocated to the matcha group (n = 49) and matched placebo group (n = 50). Age, sex, and APOE genotype were adjusted for. The intervention was initiated from July 2019 to September 2020 and ended from August 2020 to October 2021.

Eight participants discontinued the study due to withdrawal of consent and disease onset, which could have affected continuation of the intervention. Discontinuations due to illnesses included one patient who developed lung cancer, one patient who developed pancreatic cancer, one patient with increased intraocular pressure, one patient with increased transaminases, and two patients with poor physical condition. Two participants withdrew consent.

There were no cases of a causal relationship between harmful events and matcha consumption. The total number of completed questionnaires was 46 in the matcha group and 45 in the placebo group. During the intervention, each participant with MCI in the matcha and placebo groups was diagnosed with dementia at the 12-month evaluation. After excluding these two participants, the per-protocol analysis of the intervention effects included 45 and 44 participants in the matcha and placebo groups, respectively. According to the intention-to-treat principle, we analyzed the changes in outcome variables from baseline to 12 months were statistically analyzed using a mixed-effects model, with 49 and 50 participants in the matcha and placebo groups, respectively.

Baseline characteristics of participants

All the baseline characteristics of biochemical blood tests and neuroimaging data of the participants, including APOE genotype, plasma Aβ, MRI, SPECT, amyloid PET, and Aβ sequester protein [27, 28] levels, are shown in Tables 13 and S2 Table.

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of participants in the matcha and placebo groups at baseline.

Characteristics Matcha group Placebo group
(n = 49) (n = 50)
N (M/F) 49 (23/26) 50 (20/30)
Age 72.1 ± 6.0* 71.9 ± 6.1
APOE Genotype, ε4, % 28.6 30.0
Aβ1–40, pg/ml 140.4 (41.1) 143.5 (35.3)
Aβ1–42, pg/ml 22.6 (5.6) 22.9 (6.0)
Aβ (42/40) 0.163 (0.035) 0.157 (0.037)
BACE1, pg/ml 461.17 (188.69) 491.93 (211.83)
sAPPα, pg/ml 6.31 (6.94) 7.75 (6.47)
sAPPβ, pg/ml 1.76 (2.23) 1.94 (2.92)
BDNF, ng/ml 28.1 (9) 28.4 (7.5)
ApoA-1, mg/dl 145.4 (44.5) 151.7 (42.8)
TTR, mg/dl 25.2 (5.2) 26.1 (5.9)
C3(p), μg/ml 10.8 (3.5) 9.7 (4)
MDA-LDL, U/l 106 (31) 102 (31)
IL-6, pg/ml 1.18 (1.91) 1.17 (0.88)
IL-10, pg/ml 2.08 (1.35) 1.85 (0.87)
PSQI 5.0 (3.0) 4.0 (3.0)
MoCA-J 26.0 (4.0) 26.0 (3.0)
ADCS-MCI-ADL 47.0 (5.5) 47.0 (6.0)
MMSE-J 28.0 (1.0) 29.0 (1.0)
ADAS-Jcog 3.7 (3.5) 3.7 (2.3)
RBANS 110.0 (17.0) 110.0 (11.0)
CNS vital signs (Neurocognitive domain scores)
Social acuity 5 (3) 5 (3)
Reaction time 868 (207) 870 (239)
Complex attention 12 (15) 12 (15)
Cognitive flexibility 24 (38) 19 (29)
Executive function 25 (37) 21.5 (28)
Simple attention 40 (1) 40 (1)

* mean ± SD

median (IQR)

Table 3. Amyloid PET of participants in matcha and placebo groups at baseline.

Amyloid PET Matcha group Placebo group
(n = 11) (n = 13)
Baseline SUVR 0.98 (0.06)* 1.13 (0.24)
12-month SUVR 1.00 (0.10) 1.06 (0.22)

* median (IQR, Interquartile range)

Table 2. Brain atrophy and rCBF levels of participants in matcha and placebo groups at baseline.

Neuroimaging Matcha group Placebo group
(n = 49) (n = 50)
MRI
    Severity of VOI atrophy 0.59 (0.48)* 0.79 (0.54)
    Extent of VOI atrophy, % 0.06 (2.14) 0.74 (4.41)
    Ratio of VOI/GM atrophy 0.02 (1.25) 0.25 (1.46)
SPECT
    Severity 0.93 (0.37) 0.91 (0.62)
    Extent, % 3.53 (6.61) 5.11 (10.39)
    Ratio 1.06 (1.75) 1.37 (2.93)

*median (IQR, Interquartile range)

In amyloid PET, participants > 75 years of age and participants with APOE ε4 were included regardless of age. MMSE-J, GDS, WMS-R, CDR, MRI, SPECT, and biochemical blood test were performed at baseline. APOE genotyping was performed only at baseline, as it was necessary for allocation.

There was no difference in the average age (P = 0.907) and percentage of APOE4 carriers (P = 0.876) between the groups, indicating random assignment. At baseline, all outcomes, including PSQI, cognitive functioning test scores, and ADCS-MCI-ADL scores, showed no difference between the matcha and placebo groups. The average PSQI scores in the matcha and placebo groups were 4.0 and 5.0, respectively, with no significant differences between the groups. Vitamin B12 and folic acid levels at baseline were similar between the groups (S2 Table). There were no significant differences in brain atrophy or rCBF at baseline. Amyloid PET was performed on 24 (9 male and 15 female) participants, harboring APOE4 genotype at baseline and 12 months, with no significant differences between the groups at baseline (Table 3).

Effect of 12-month matcha consumption on sleep quality and cognitive function

Intake compliance was assessed by counting the number of capsules that remained in the bag at the 3-, 6-, 9-, and 12-month visits. The intake rates were 98–99% throughout the intervention periods. We also verified matcha intake during the intervention period by measuring theanine levels in red blood cells. The matcha group demonstrated a significant increase in theanine levels (P = 1.9E-04), which was not observed in the placebo group. This means the participants consumed matcha and placebo capsules correctly and consistently throughout the intervention period.

For the intention-to-treat analysis of each outcome, we tested the intervention effects using mixed-effects models, with 46 and 45 participants in the matcha group and placebo groups, respectively. The model equation consisted of a time effect (baseline, 6 months, and 12 months), group effect (matcha and placebo), and interaction between time and group (see Methods and S1 File). As shown in Table 4, sleep quality was measured using the PSQI, and cognitive function was assessed using the MMSE-J, MoCA-J, ADAS-Jcog, RBANS, and CNS vital signs.

Table 4. Effect of 12-month matcha intervention on sleep quality and cognitive functioning.

Outcome Mixed effect model
Baseline to 12 months
Estimate* SE t-value P-value
PSQI 0.86 0.50 1.73 0.088§
ADCS-MCI-ADL 0.05 0.82 0.06 0.949
MoCA-J 0.70 0.51 1.37 0.175
MMSE-J -0.38 0.33 -1.18 0.242
ADAS-Jcog -0.33 0.44 -0.75 0.458
RBANS -0.09 2.20 -0.04 0.967
CNS vital signs (Neurocognitive domain scores)
    Social acuity -1.39 0.62 -2.23 0.028
    Reaction time 23.98 18.86 1.27 0.207
    Complex attention 0.27 1.70 0.16 0.875
    Cognitive flexibility 2.88 3.26 0.88 0.379
    Executive function 2.49 3.20 0.78 0.438
    Simple attention -1.17 0.87 -1.35 0.181

* Estimate: The two-group difference in mean change from baseline to 12-momths calculated as described in the Methods. Positive value means higher value and negative value means lower value in matcha group comparing to placebo group, respectively.

SE: Standard error

P < 0.05

§ P < 0.1

The effect of match consumption on the outcomes was evaluated at 12 months. Social acuity in the neurocognitive domain scores of CNS vital signs showed significant improvement (difference; -1.39, 95%CI; -2.78, 0.002) (P = 0.028) and PSQI scores differed by 0.86 between the groups, indicating a trend of an improvement in sleep quality (95% CI; -0.002, 1.71) (P = 0.088) by 12-month matcha consumption (Table 4). Other cognitive functions including the primary outcomes (MoCA-J and ADCS-MCI-ADL) showed no significant changes in the matcha group compared with the placebo control group. We obtained results from both intent-to-treat and per-protocol analyses. The per-protocol analysis showed that differences in social acuity and PSQI scores between the groups were -1.44 (95% CI; -2.88, 0.001; P = 0.077) and 0.89 (95% CI; -0.001, 1.74; P = 1.08), respectively (S3 and S4 Tables).

Since the score for social acuity was assessed by the perception of emotion, we analyzed the outcomes of each cognitive test performed in CNS vital signs (Fig 2). Commission errors were reduced in the matcha group compared with the placebo group (difference; 1.16, 95% CI; 2.5E-03, 2.31) (P = 0.035) in the perception of emotions. In the shifting attention test, the simple reaction time of the matcha group seemed to be shortened at 12 months compared with that at the baseline (difference; -48.93, 95% CI; -97.91, 0.06) (P = 0.068) in the placebo group. In the Stroop test, the matcha group demonstrated a reduction in reaction time at 12 months compared with that in the baseline, whereas the placebo group exhibited virtually no change.

Fig 2. Effect of 12-month matcha consumption on neurocognitive domain score in CNS vital signs.

Fig 2

The intervention effects on neurocognitive domain score in CNS vital signs are shown. Differences in the mean change from baseline to 12 months between the two groups were calculated using the mixed-effects model. Black circles represent the matcha group, and open circles represent the placebo group. The tables provided below represent the estimated differences between the groups; a positive value implies a higher value in the matcha group than in the placebo group, whereas a negative value suggests a lower value in the matcha group than in the placebo group.

Although there was no significant improvement in cognitive function as evaluated by conventional cognitive functioning tests used in clinical diagnosis (Fig 3), 12-month matcha consumption improved social acuity, as assessed by the neurocognitive domain scores of CNS vital signs (Fig 2). Accordingly, we analyzed the association between social acuity and conventional cognitive functioning test scores in all participants and the stratified groups. At both baseline and 12 months, significant positive correlations were observed between the social acuity score and both MoCA-J and MMSE-J scores, whereas negative correlations were observed between the social acuity score and ADAS-Jcog score. No correlation was observed between social acuity and ADCS-MCI-ADL scores. Stronger correlations between the social acuity score and MoCA-J were observed in participants with SCD than in those with MCI and in the APOE4 noncarriers than in the APOE4 carriers. The social acuity scores of all participants were significantly correlated with the MoCA-J, MMSE-J, ADCS-MCI-ADL, ADAS-Jcog, and RBANS scores. However, in the MCI group, there was no significant correlation between social acuity and cognitive test scores. Conversely, in the SCD group, social acuity scores were significantly correlated with the MoCA-J, ADCS-MCI-ADL, and ADAS-Jcog scores.

Fig 3. Effect of 12-month matcha consumption on sleep quality and cognitive function.

Fig 3

The intervention effects on PSQI, ADCS-MCI-ADL, cognitive function assessed by MMSE-J, MoCA-J, ADAS-Jcog, and RBANS are shown. Differences in the mean change from baseline to 12 months between the two groups were calculated using the mixed-effects model. Black circles represent the matcha group, and open circles represent the placebo group. The tables provided below represent the estimated differences between the groups; a positive value implies a higher value in the matcha group than in the placebo group, whereas a negative value suggests a lower value in the matcha group than in the placebo group.

Neuroimaging biomarkers

The amyloid PET SUVR displayed no difference from the baseline and at 12 months in either group (Table 5). No significant changes were observed in hippocampal atrophy or rCBF levels after 12 months of matcha consumption.

Table 5. Effect of 12-month matcha intervention on neuroimaging.

Outcome Mixed effect model
Baseline to 12 months
Estimate* SE t-value P-value
Amyloid PET SUVR 0.014 0.02 0.76 0.456
MRI
    Severity of VOI atrophy 0.108 0.14 0.78 0.435
    Extent of VOI atrophy, % -0.898 0.97 -0.92 0.359
    Ratio of VOI/GM atrophy 0.078 0.20 0.39 0.700
SPECT
    Severity -0.001 0.04 -0.02 0.982
    Extent, % 0.177 0.97 0.18 0.855
    Ratio -0.042 0.26 -0.16 0.872

* Estimate: The two-group difference in mean change from baseline to 12-momths calculated as described in the Methods. Positive value means higher value and negative value means lower value in matcha group comparing to placebo group, respectively.

SE: Standard error

Discussion

In this study, unbiased randomization was conducted, and the trial was implemented as a triple-blind study with blinding for the intervention and placebo groups as well as the statistical analyst, thus enhancing the reliability of the study. In the current study, amyloid PET measurements using 18F-Florbetapir were performed in 24 (9 male and 15 female) participants who were APOE4 carriers. There were no biases in age, sex, APOE genotype, and amyloid burden between the intervention and non-intervention groups.

Present findings suggest that the consumption of matcha enhances certain cognitive functions, such as facial expression recognition and attention, and improves sleep quality, which are beneficial for maintaining cognitive function in older adult. Matcha consumption may be considered as a lifestyle improvement strategy for dementia prevention.

The PSQI total score indicated a trend toward improved sleep with matcha consumption. Despite the presence of caffeine, which disrupts sleep, matcha demonstrated a sleep-enhancing effect. This beneficial effect is attributed to theanine, a constituent of matcha. A crossover study by Hidese et al. on healthy participants with an average age of 48 years revealed that theanine intake (200 mg/day) for 4 weeks significantly reduced the total PSQI score [44]. As the change in the total score in the matcha group compared with that in the placebo group showed a decreasing trend (P = 0.073), theanine was suggested to improve sleep quality. Furthermore, Ota et al. reported that in patients with schizophrenia with an average age of 42 years, theanine (250 mg/day) intake for 8 weeks alongside standard treatment led to a significant decrease in total PSQI scores [45]. Thus, while theanine intake has been demonstrated to enhance sleep, this study provides the first report of the sleep-improving effects of matcha.

Substantial evidence suggests a close relationship between sleep and cognitive function [4648]. Brachem et al. reported that in a longitudinal study of local residents aged 50–80 years, tracked for an average of 5.2 years, the risk of developing MCI was significantly increased (relative risk (RR) = 1.43, 95% CI; 1.12, 1.82) when the total PSQI score exceeded 5 points, as compared with those scoring 5 or less. Notably, in patients with MCI suffering from sleep disturbances (PSQI ≥ 5), significant improvement has been reported in the total PSQI and MoCA-J scores after 6 months of continuous positive airway pressure therapy in conjunction with donepezil administration [49]. These results suggest that enhancement of sleep quality could improve cognitive function.

In this study, a significant enhancement in social cognition was observed alongside an improvement in the total PSQI scores. It has been postulated that matcha may improve sleep quality and prevent or ameliorate cognitive decline. Thus, improvements in sleep quality associated with matcha intake may lead to cognitive function enhancement, even without the implementation of full-fledged sleep therapy. In the placebo group, a certain level of theanine was detected in the blood cells. This intervention study did not impose any restrictions on green tea intake before or during the study period. This may account for the presence of theanine, which may have been ingested during habitual green tea consumption.

Facial emotion recognition is impaired in patients with SCD and MCI [50, 51]. A cross-sectional analysis of 4,039 participants revealed that recognition of the emotion of facial expressions is affected at an early stage of cognitive impairment [52]. In the present study, a significant enhancement in social cognition was observed following matcha consumption. Social cognition is related to the perception of emotions, one of the tests of CNS vital signs, and the incidence of false responses was reduced by matcha consumption. False responses in the perception of emotions test pertained to inaccurate responses to facial expressions and descriptions (word meanings) administered during the test. A decrease in false response scores suggests that participants were more proficient in recognizing facial expressions and descriptions following 12-month matcha consumption. Social acuity, as evaluated by emotion recognition, was significantly associated with the MoCA-J score in patients with SCD and in APOE4 noncarriers, suggesting that this measure of social acuity may be a suitable test for the early stages of cognitive decline. However, the matcha intervention had no effect on the primary outcomes, MoCA-J and ADCS-MCI-ADL, possibly due to their sensitivity to slight declines in cognitive function. These tests are typically used during clinical diagnoses of cognitive impairment rather than for evaluating early-stage cognitive decline in the disease.

The matcha used in this study was powdered tea leaves. Its cultivation method differs from that of conventional green tea, as the tea leaves designated for matcha production are protected from sunlight prior to harvesting. This alternative cultivation approach increases the content of catechins and theanine, making it potentially superior to green tea in terms of the health benefits attributable to its ingredients. Participants drinking seven cups of matcha were excluded since it is equivalent to consuming 2 g of matcha contain 170 mg of catechins and 48 mg of theanine every day, which is the same amount of matcha as in this intervention study. In terms of catechin content, a single cup of matcha in a traditional tea ceremony, Tea Otemae, is equivalent to 250 mL (2–3 cups) of green tea; however, it should be noted that various types of green tea exist, and not all are directly comparable to matcha. Given the established health benefits of green tea and its constituents, we hypothesized that the increased catechin and theanine contents in matcha could help suppress cognitive decline and improve sleep quality.

The beneficial effects of matcha consumption revealed in this study may contribute to dementia prevention strategies, as these are relatively easy lifestyle changes to implement in daily life. Matcha consumption improved facial emotion recognition, as assessed by a computerized neurocognitive battery, since conventional psychological tests may not be suitable for evaluating cognitive decline in very mild cases. Thus, more sensitive neuropsychological testing, along with intervention strategies that are easy for elderly people to maintain, is necessary to assess the efficacy of these interventions in dementia prevention.

A limitation of the current study is the small number of participants. The observed PSQI score improvement within the intervention group suggests a potential influence of matcha consumption on sleep quality. However, future studies should employ more objective measures beyond the PSQI to comprehensively assess sleep architecture, including quantification of REM and non-REM sleep stages. A comprehensive clinical trial is required for a more robust evaluation of its effects on facial expression recognition. Furthermore, it is crucial to analyze the association between intervention outcomes and biomarkers to enhance our understanding of the mechanisms underlying the effects of matcha. Such investigations would make a significant contribution to the establishment of an effective ecosystem for dementia prevention.

Conclusions

The findings of this long-term intervention study suggest that regular matcha consumption can enhance emotional perception and sleep quality in older adults experiencing cognitive decline.

Supporting information

S1 File. Supporting materials and methods.

(PDF)

pone.0309287.s001.pdf (444KB, pdf)
S1 Table. Subsets of neurocognitive test corresponding to neurocognitive domain score used in the matcha intervention study.

(PDF)

pone.0309287.s002.pdf (107.7KB, pdf)
S2 Table. Baseline clinical chemistry parameters of participants in the matcha and placebo groups.

(PDF)

pone.0309287.s003.pdf (114.1KB, pdf)
S3 Table. Effect of 12-month matcha intervention on sleep quality and cognitive functioning in per-protocol analysis.

(PDF)

pone.0309287.s004.pdf (89.2KB, pdf)
S4 Table. Effect of 12-month matcha intervention on neuroimaging in per-protocol analysis.

(PDF)

pone.0309287.s005.pdf (86.7KB, pdf)

Acknowledgments

We thank all the participants in this study for their commitment and help in advancing this research. We thank Hitomi Ito and Makoto Inoue for helping to recruit the participants and Akiko Kaito for sample preparation. We also thank Tosie Shinagawa for critical reading of the manuscript. Registration number of present clinical research are UMIN000035658 (UMIN-CTR). The full trial protocol can be accessed in this clinical research registry. The data in this study can be accessed in Dryad Digital Repository (https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.2280gb61r).

Data Availability

The data can be shared publicly without permission from a public repository after acceptance. The data files are available from the Dryad Digital Repository. https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.2280gb61r.

Funding Statement

The author(s) received no specific funding for this work.

References

  • 1.Gauthier S, Webster C, Servaes S, Morais J, Rosa-Neto P. World Alzheimer Report 2022: Life after diagnosis: Navigating treatment, care and support. Alzheimer’s Disease International; 2022. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Kivipelto M, Mangialasche F, Snyder HM, Allegri R, Andrieu S, Arai H, et al. World-Wide FINGERS Network: A global approach to risk reduction and prevention of dementia. Alzheimer Dement. 2020;16: 1078–1094. doi: 10.1002/alz.12123 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Ngandu T, Lehtisalo J, Solomon A, Levälahti E, Ahtiluoto S, Antikainen R, et al. A 2 year multidomain intervention of diet, exercise, cognitive training, and vascular risk monitoring versus control to prevent cognitive decline in at-risk elderly people (FINGER): a randomised controlled trial. Lancet. 2015;385: 2255–2263. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)60461-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Chen H, Dhana K, Huang Y, Huang L, Tao Y, Liu X, et al. Association of the Mediterranean Dietary Approaches to Stop Hypertension Intervention for Neurodegenerative Delay (MIND) diet with the risk of dementia. JAMA Psychiatry. 2023. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2023.0800 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Spartano NL, Demissie S, Himali JJ, Dukes KA, Murabito JM, Vasan RS, et al. Accelerometer-determined physical activity and cognitive function in middle-aged and older adults from two generations of the Framingham Heart Study. Alzheimers Dement. 2019;5: 618–626. doi: 10.1016/j.trci.2019.08.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Baba Y, Inagaki S, Nakagawa S, Kobayashi M, Kaneko T, Takihara T. Effects of daily matcha and caffeine intake on mild acute psychological stress-related cognitive function in middle-aged and older adults: A Randomized Placebo-Controlled Study. Nutrients. 2021;13. doi: 10.3390/nu13051700 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Baba Y, Kaneko T, Takihara T. Matcha consumption maintains attentional function following a mild acute psychological stress without affecting a feeling of fatigue: A randomized placebo-controlled study in young adults. Nutr Res. 2021;88: 44–52. doi: 10.1016/j.nutres.2020.12.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Lyon MR, Kapoor MP, Juneja LR. The effects of L-theanine (Suntheanine®) on objective sleep quality in boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled clinical trial. Altern Med Rev. 2011;16: 348. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Rezai-Zadeh K, Arendash GW, Hou H, Fernandez F, Jensen M, Runfeldt M, et al. Green tea epigallocatechin-3-gallate (EGCG) reduces beta-amyloid mediated cognitive impairment and modulates tau pathology in Alzheimer transgenic mice. Brain Res. 2008;1214: 177–187. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.02.107 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Unno K, Hara A, Nakagawa A, Iguchi K, Ohshio M, Morita A, et al. Anti-stress effects of drinking green tea with lowered caffeine and enriched theanine, epigallocatechin and arginine on psychosocial stress induced adrenal hypertrophy in mice. Phytomedicine. 2016;23: 1365–1374. doi: 10.1016/j.phymed.2016.07.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Kuriyama S, Hozawa A, Ohmori K, Shimazu T, Matsui T, Ebihara S, et al. Green tea consumption and cognitive function: a cross-sectional study from the Tsurugaya Project. Am J Clin Nutr. 2006;83: 355–361. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Khokhar S, Magnusdottir SGM. Total phenol, catechin, and caffeine contents of teas commonly consumed in the United kingdom. J Agric Food Chem. 2002;50: 565–570. doi: 10.1021/jf010153l [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Sakurai K, Shen C, Ezaki Y, Inamura N, Fukushima Y, Masuoka N, et al. Effects of matcha green tea powder on cognitive functions of community-dwelling elderly individuals. Nutrients. 2020;12. doi: 10.3390/nu12123639 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Doi Y, Minowa M, Okawa M, Uchiyama M. Prevalence of sleep disturbance and hypnotic medication use in relation to sociodemographic factors in the general Japanese adult population. J Epidemiol. 2000;10: 79–86. doi: 10.2188/jea.10.79 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Kim K, Uchiyama M, Okawa M, Liu X, Ogihara R. An epidemiological study of insomnia among the Japanese general population. Sleep. 2000;23: 41–47. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Institute of Medicine (US) Committee on Sleep Medicine and Research. Sleep disorders and sleep deprivation: An unmet public health problem. Colten HR, Altevogt BM editors. Washington (DC): National Academies Press (US); 2006. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Shokri-Kojori E, Wang G-J, Wiers CE, Demiral SB, Guo M, Kim SW, et al. β-Amyloid accumulation in the human brain after one night of sleep deprivation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2018;115: 4483–4488. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Irwin MR, Vitiello MV. Implications of sleep disturbance and inflammation for Alzheimer’s disease dementia. Lancet Neurol. 2019;18: 296–306. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(18)30450-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Sabia S, Fayosse A, Dumurgier J, van Hees VT, Paquet C, Sommerlad A, et al. Association of sleep duration in middle and old age with incidence of dementia. Nat Commun. 2021;12: 2289. doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-22354-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Mattsson-Carlgren N, Salvadó G, Ashton NJ, Tideman P, Stomrud E, Zetterberg H, et al. Prediction of longitudinal cognitive decline in preclinical Alzheimer Disease using plasma biomarkers. JAMA Neurol. 2023. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2022.5272 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Varma VR, Varma S, An Y, Hohman TJ, Seddighi S, Casanova R, et al. Alpha-2 macroglobulin in Alzheimer’s disease: a marker of neuronal injury through the RCAN1 pathway. Mol Psychiatry. 2017;22: 13–23. doi: 10.1038/mp.2016.206 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Zuin M, Cervellati C, Trentini A, Passaro A, Rosta V, Zimetti F, et al. Association between serum concentrations of apolipoprotein A-I (apoA-I) and Alzheimer’s disease: Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Diagnostics. 2021;11: 984. doi: 10.3390/diagnostics11060984 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Rasmussen KL, Nordestgaard BG, Frikke-Schmidt R, Nielsen SF. An updated Alzheimer hypothesis: Complement C3 and risk of Alzheimer’s disease—A cohort study of 95,442 individuals. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14: 1589–1601. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.07.223 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Velayudhan L, Killick R, Hye A, Kinsey A, Güntert A, Lynham S, et al. Plasma transthyretin as a candidate marker for Alzheimer’s disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2012;28: 369–375. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2011-110611 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Tien Y-T, Lee W-J, Liao Y-C, Wang W-F, Jhang K-M, Wang S-J, et al. Plasma transthyretin as a predictor of amnestic mild cognitive impairment conversion to dementia. Sci Rep. 2019;9: 18691. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-55318-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Llewellyn DJ, Langa KM, Friedland RP, Lang IA. Serum albumin concentration and cognitive impairment. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2010;7: 91–96. doi: 10.2174/156720510790274392 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Uchida K, Shan L, Suzuki H, Tabuse Y, Nishimura Y, Hirokawa Y, et al. Amyloid-β sequester proteins as blood-based biomarkers of cognitive decline. Alzheimers Dement (Amst). 2015;1: 270–280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Liu S, Suzuki H, Ito H, Korenaga T, Akatsu H, Meno K, et al. Serum levels of proteins involved in amyloid-β clearance are related to cognitive decline and neuroimaging changes in mild cognitive impairment. Alzheimers Dement (Amst). 2019;11: 85–97. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Tsolaki M, Kounti F, Agogiatou C, Poptsi E, Bakoglidou E, Zafeiropoulou M, et al. Effectiveness of nonpharmacological approaches in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Neurodegener Dis. 2011;8: 138–145. doi: 10.1159/000320575 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Marzolini S, Oh P, McIlroy W, Brooks D. The effects of an aerobic and resistance exercise training program on cognition following stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2013;27: 392–402. doi: 10.1177/1545968312465192 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Sukontapol C, Kemsen S, Chansirikarn S, Nakawiro D, Kuha O, Taemeeyapradit U. The effectiveness of a cognitive training program in people with mild cognitive impairment: A study in urban community. Asian J Psychiatr. 2018;35: 18–23. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2018.04.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Livingston G, Huntley J, Sommerlad A, Ames D, Ballard C, Banerjee S, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet. 2020;396: 413–446. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Pocock SJ, Simon R. Sequential treatment assignment with balancing for prognostic factors in the controlled clinical trial. Biometrics. 1975;31: 103–115. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Mitchell AJ, Beaumont H, Ferguson D, Yadegarfar M, Stubbs B. Risk of dementia and mild cognitive impairment in older people with subjective memory complaints: meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2014;130: 439–451. doi: 10.1111/acps.12336 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Pike KE, Cavuoto MG, Li L, Wright BJ, Kinsella GJ. Subjective cognitive decline: Level of risk for future dementia and mild cognitive impairment, a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Neuropsychol Rev. 2022;32: 703–735. doi: 10.1007/s11065-021-09522-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Molinuevo JL, Rabin LA, Amariglio R, Buckley R, Dubois B, Ellis KA, et al. Implementation of subjective cognitive decline criteria in research studies. Alzheimers Dement. 2017;13: 296–311. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2016.09.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Iverson GL, Brooks BL, Weiss MD, Gualtieri CT, Johnson LG. Clinical usefulness of CNS vital signs for assessing neurocognition in ADHD. Clin Neuropsychol. 2007;21: 686. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Gualtieri CT, Johnson LG. Reliability and validity of a computerized neurocognitive test battery, CNS Vital Signs. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2006;21: 623–643. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2006.05.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Galasko D, Bennett D, Sano M, Ernesto C, Thomas R, Grundman M, et al. An inventory to assess activities of daily living for clinical trials in Alzheimer’s disease. The Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 1997;11 Suppl 2: S33-9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Galasko D, Bennett DA, Sano M, Marson D, Kaye J, Edland SD, et al. ADCS prevention instrument project: Assessment of instrumental activities of daily living for community-dwelling elderly individuals in dementia prevention clinical trials. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2006;20: S152. doi: 10.1097/01.wad.0000213873.25053.2b [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Doi Y, Minowa M, Uchiyama M, Okawa M, Kim K, Shibui K, et al. Psychometric assessment of subjective sleep quality using the Japanese version of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI-J) in psychiatric disordered and control subjects. Psychiatry Res. 2000;97: 165–172. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(00)00232-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Matsuda H, Mizumura S, Nemoto K, Yamashita F, Imabayashi E, Sato N, et al. Automatic voxel-based morphometry of structural MRI by SPM8 plus diffeomorphic anatomic registration through exponentiated lie algebra improves the diagnosis of probable Alzheimer Disease. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2012;33: 1109–1114. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A2935 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Matsuda H, Mizumura S, Nagao T, Ota T, Iizuka T, Nemoto K, et al. Automated discrimination between very early Alzheimer disease and controls using an easy Z-score imaging system for multicenter brain perfusion single-photon emission tomography. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2007;28: 731–736. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Hidese S, Ogawa S, Ota M, Ishida I, Yasukawa Z, Ozeki M, et al. Effects of L-theanine administration on stress-related symptoms and cognitive functions in healthy adults: A randomized controlled trial. Nutrients. 2019;11. doi: 10.3390/nu11102362 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Ota M, Wakabayashi C, Sato N, Hori H, Hattori K, Teraishi T, et al. Effect of L-theanine on glutamatergic function in patients with schizophrenia. Acta Neuropsychiatr. 2015;27: 291–296. doi: 10.1017/neu.2015.22 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Yaffe K, Falvey CM, Hoang T. Connections between sleep and cognition in older adults. Lancet Neurol. 2014;13: 1017–1028. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70172-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Sun J, Kang J, Wang P, Zeng H. Self-relaxation training can improve sleep quality and cognitive functions in the older: a one-year randomised controlled trial. J Clin Nurs. 2013;22: 1270–1280. doi: 10.1111/jocn.12096 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Xu L, Jiang CQ, Lam TH, Liu B, Jin YL, Zhu T, et al. Short or long sleep duration is associated with memory impairment in older Chinese: the Guangzhou Biobank Cohort Study. Sleep. 2011;34: 575–580. doi: 10.1093/sleep/34.5.575 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Huang H, Li M, Zhang M, Qiu J, Cheng H, Mou X, et al. Sleep quality improvement enhances neuropsychological recovery and reduces blood Aβ42/40 ratio in patients with mild–moderate cognitive impairment. Medicina. 2021;57: 1366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Pietschnig J, Aigner-Wöber R, Reischenböck N, Kryspin-Exner I, Moser D, Klug S, et al. Facial emotion recognition in patients with subjective cognitive decline and mild cognitive impairment. Int Psychogeriatr. 2016;28: 477–485. doi: 10.1017/S1041610215001520 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Weiss EM, Kohler CG, Vonbank J, Stadelmann E, Kemmler G, Hinterhuber H, et al. Impairment in emotion recognition abilities in patients with mild cognitive impairment, early and moderate Alzheimer disease compared with healthy comparison subjects. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2008;16: 974–980. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e318186bd53 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Virtanen M, Singh-Manoux A, Batty GD, Ebmeier KP, Jokela M, Harmer CJ, et al. The level of cognitive function and recognition of emotions in older adults. PLoS One. 2017;12: e0185513. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0185513 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Tamlyn Julie Watermeyer

10 Apr 2024

PONE-D-24-02571Effect of matcha green tea on cognitive functions and sleep quality in older adults with cognitive decline: A randomized controlled study over 12 monthsPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Uchida,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

The most pressing points to address are from Reviewer 2 who focused on the statistical aspects of the study and who raises several discrepancies from the CONSORT guidelines. Please ensure that you familiarise yourself with these criteria and address Reviewer 2's points in turn. Reviewer 1 and 3 raise some pertinent issues relating to definitions, language-use and areas that require expansion in the Introduction and Discussion sections.  Please submit your revised manuscript by May 25 2024 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Tamlyn Julie Watermeyer

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

1. When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Thank you for stating the following in the Competing Interests section: 

Authors with competing interests

Enter competing interest details beginning with this statement:

I have read the journal's policy and the authors of this manuscript have the following competing interests: [Kazuhiko Uchida serves as a board member of MCBI Inc. Kohji Meno, Tatsumi Korenaga, Liu Shan, and Hideaki Suzuki are employees of MCBI Inc. Yoshitake Baba, Chika Tagata, Yoshiharu Araki, Shuto Tsunemi, Kenta Aso, Shun Inagaki, Sae Nakagawa, Makoto Kobayashi, and Takanobu Takihara are employees of ITO EN , LTD. This research received no external funding. ]

   

We note that one or more of the authors are employed by a commercial company. 

a. Please provide an amended Funding Statement declaring this commercial affiliation, as well as a statement regarding the Role of Funders in your study. If the funding organization did not play a role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript and only provided financial support in the form of authors' salaries and/or research materials, please review your statements relating to the author contributions, and ensure you have specifically and accurately indicated the role(s) that these authors had in your study. You can update author roles in the Author Contributions section of the online submission form.

Please also include the following statement within your amended Funding Statement. 

“The funder provided support in the form of salaries for authors, but did not have any additional role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. The specific roles of these authors are articulated in the ‘author contributions’ section.”

If your commercial affiliation did play a role in your study, please state and explain this role within your updated Funding Statement. 

b. Please also provide an updated Competing Interests Statement declaring this commercial affiliation along with any other relevant declarations relating to employment, consultancy, patents, products in development, or marketed products, etc.  

Within your Competing Interests Statement, please confirm that this commercial affiliation does not alter your adherence to all PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials by including the following statement: "This does not alter our adherence to  PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials.” (as detailed online in our guide for authors http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/competing-interests) . If this adherence statement is not accurate and  there are restrictions on sharing of data and/or materials, please state these. Please note that we cannot proceed with consideration of your article until this information has been declared.

Please include both an updated Funding Statement and Competing Interests Statement in your cover letter. We will change the online submission form on your behalf.

3. In the online submission form, you indicated that The data cannot be shared publicly without permission from all authors. Access to the data is available through the Institutional Data Access/Ethics Committee (contactable via the corresponding author) for researchers who meet the criteria for accessing confidential data.

All PLOS journals now require all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript to be freely available to other researchers, either a. In a public repository, b. Within the manuscript itself, or c. Uploaded as supplementary information.

This policy applies to all data except where public deposition would breach compliance with the protocol approved by your research ethics board. If your data cannot be made publicly available for ethical or legal reasons (e.g., public availability would compromise patient privacy), please explain your reasons on resubmission and your exemption request will be escalated for approval. 

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Partly

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: I Don't Know

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This was a very interesting article. I have included some minor amendment below:

-Data statement – no explanation as to why data can’t be shared. States without permission from all authors but no explanation as to why all authors wouldn’t give permission. Is data owned by a third party?

- Typo on page 6, line 3, double comma

- Line 8 – could some context be given as to what is being classified as ‘middle age’ here.

- Within intervention section it would be useful to provide some justification as to why this dosage was decided upon.

- For statistical analysis is there justification as to why per protocol participants were analysed rather than Intent To Treat. CONSORT recommends reporting both in parallel, so some acknowledgement of ITT would be beneficial here, were the results the same?

- The results are interesting and placed well within previous literature. However there were no effects on primary outcomes and this should be acknowledged somewhere in the discussion.

Reviewer #2: Important note: This review pertains only to ‘statistical aspects’ of the study and so ‘clinical aspects’ [like medical importance, relevance of the study, ‘clinical significance and implication(s)’ of the whole study, etc.] are to be evaluated [should be assessed] separately/independently. Further please note that any ‘statistical review’ is generally done under the assumption that (such) study specific methodological [as well as execution] issues are perfectly taken care of by the investigator(s). This review is not an exception to that and so does not cover clinical aspects {however, seldom comments are made only if those issues are intimately / scientifically related & intermingle with ‘statistical aspects’ of the study}. Agreed that ‘statistical methods’ are used as just tools here, however, they are vital part of methodology [and so should be given due importance]. I look at the manuscript in/with statistical view point, other reviewer(s) look(s) at it with different angle so that in totality the review is very comprehensive. However, there should be efforts from authors side to improve (may be by taking clues from reviewer’s comments). Therefore, please do not limit the revision only (with respect) to comments made here.

COMMENTS: Kindly note that your ABSTRACT is well drafted (in my opinion), but is ‘assay type’. It is preferable [refer to item 1b of CONSORT checklist 2010: Structured summary of trial design, methods, results, and conclusions] to divide the ABSTRACT with small sections like ‘Objective(s)’, ‘Methods’, ‘Results’, ‘Conclusions’, etc. which is an accepted practice of most of the good/standard journals [including this one, though ‘The PLoS One Guidelines to Authors’ did not specify an Abstract format, it is desirable]. It will definitely be more informative then, I guess, whatever the article type may be. Since your ‘Article Type: Clinical Trial’, it very much desirable.

In addition, there are few issues/observations about which I have different opinion. Such observations/concerns are given below [if acceptable/agreed, try to incorporate them (consider positively wherever possible)]:

First is regarding choice of ‘Sample size’. One of the very important items in CONSORT guidelines is ’How sample size was determined (Item 7a)’ and expected/desired to described in ample details including all assumptions and software used. In fact, it is very surprising to note the ‘absence’ of the important term “CONSORT’’ despite that the manuscript is on ‘A randomized controlled clinical study/trial’. It is well-known that while reporting [findings from and even planning that is in ‘protocol’ of] ‘Clinical Trial’ one should follow/cover CONSORT guidelines.

In ‘Discussion’ it is just said/mentioned that “In this study, unbiased randomization was conducted, and the trial was implemented as a triple-blind study”. Regarding these important issues more discussion/detailed account is expected. Few important items {and all ‘Clinical Trials’ must follow} in CONSORT guidelines are ‘How sample size was determined (Item 7a)’,’ Random Sequence generation (Item 8a)’, ‘Allocation concealment (Item 9)’, Blinding (Item 11a). Since your article type is ‘Clinical Trial’, you are supposed to cover these items in the report or even in ‘Protocol’.

You may know that ‘Permuted Block Randomization’ ensures same group sizes (not simple randomization) and that randomization is a process [not only sequence generation] which includes ‘Allocation Concealment’}. Which ‘Randomization’ procedure is used in this study is not specified but almost equal group sizes are obtained. It must be ‘Permuted Block Randomization’ but such clear mention is desired.

In ‘Results-Participants’ section (as well as in Figure-1), you may use the word ‘screened’ in place of ‘recruited’ [As shown in Fig 1, we recruited 939 community-dwelling older adults aged 60–85 years and 124 participants were enrolled], in my opinion. May please confirm as word ‘recruited’ implies that 939 participated in trial. It may be known to you that when random allocation/assignment is used/done, any statistical comparison of baseline characteristics is not required. In this context [refer to Table-1], I request authors to read following note pasted from one famous standard textbook on ‘Medical Research Methodology’:

To provide a description of baseline characteristics is entirely reasonable (since it is clearly important in assessing to whom the results of the trial can be applied), however, statistical comparison of baseline characteristics when random allocation/assignment is used/done [often for good/standard/leading journals these days] is not required, because even if P-value(s) turn(s) out to be significant (while comparing baseline characteristics despite random allocation), it is, by definition, a false positive as you then are supposed to be testing ‘randomization’ then, which in any single trial may not balance all baseline characteristics (particularly when sample sizes are small). Remember that ‘randomization’ is a sort of ‘insurance’ and not a guarantee scheme. Authors may please refer to following articles:

References:

1. Stuart J. Pocock, et al., ‘Subgroup analysis, covariate adjustment and baseline comparisons in clinical trial reporting: current practice and problems’, Statistics in medicine, 2002; 21:2917–2930 [Particularly page 2927]

2. Harrington D, et al., ‘New guidelines for statistical reporting in the journal’, N Engl J Med 2019;381:285-6

[Important message (indirectly/ultimately indicated) from these articles: Never do any comparison with respect to ‘baseline’ characteristics {by applying statistical significance test(s)}, when allocation is done randomly].

However, Statistical comparison [only with respect to important/indicated variables] of baseline characteristics may be performed, to find out if analysis adjustment (say stratified analyses or else) is required with respect to these variables.

What is the purpose of Table-3? Why Between group comparison is performed separately at ‘Baseline’ and ’12-months’? What is/are interpretation of insignificant (both) P-vale(s)? Why not the comparison using ‘change scores’? In all the tables, test name is indicated only for few ‘variables/parameters’ {by giving symbol as superscript on P-value (last column)}. Is not it required to indicate test name in case of (i.e. with respect to) other/remaining variables/parameters? Or is it left for readers to guess? Remember/mind you that this is a scientific/academic document and so all details should be clearly/correctly communicated (do not take readers’ for granted). Most of the findings are reported without properly interpreting them or given relevance to study (example: page 26).

Only limitation of the study [A limitation of the current study is the small number of participants] mentioned on page 32 is agreed. However, does that mean {according to authors} there are none others? As pointed out in ‘important note’ above “This review pertains only to ‘statistical aspects’ of the study and so ‘clinical aspects’ should be assessed separately/independently. In my opinion, to make this article acceptable some amount of re-vision (re-drafting) may be needed. However, please do not limit the revision only (with respect) to comments made here. ‘Major revision’ is recommended.

Reviewer #3: The manuscript was well written and an interesting trial. I suggest some minor points to improve it:

- Introduction is well written with relevant links to supporting green tea literature. It would be useful to include rationale for the intervention timeframe (12 months). There does seem to be some literature on matcha tea interventions that haven't been included, which is more relevant than the included green tea literature - for example a paper by Sakurai (2020) used matcha for a 12 week intervention in elderly Japanese individuals.

- Page 7 line 5 - missing refernece for frequency of green tea consumption. It would also be useful here to have a brief explanation of this finding and the relation to this trial

- Page 7 line 8 - missing refernece for sleep disturbance/disorder stats, it would be more useful here to have the stats for sleep disorder prevalence in Japan rather than the US

- I am interested to know why age was stratified as >74 years old/ <74 years old, when the age range was 60-85 years. Is there a reason that 74 was chosen rather than 72? A brief comment at Page 9 line 2 would be useful

- Page 9 Line 16 - points 3 and 4 of the eligibility criteria are the same, please check

- Within eligibility criteria you mention that participants could not consume more that 7 cups of matcha per week, there is a comment within the discussion that the study did not impose restrictions on green tea intake during the trial, why was this decided? Did you collect information on habitual intake of green tea/matcha during the trial? If so, were there any differences in consumption between treatment groups? High consumption in the placebo group could explain lack of findings.

- Intervention information - I'd be interested to know what 2g of matcha equates to in comparison with consuming as a tea and/or how the catechin/theanine/caffeine content compares with green tea. I think this is particularly interesting given the citation of various green tea literature and the decision to exclude participants consuming 7+ cups matcha each week.

- Page 16 Line 2 - Please provide comment on if the repeated freezing/thawing of samples may have impacted the immunoassay results. Was this per protocol? It would also be useful here to include details of the blood sampling process. It would also be useful to include some information about the biomarkers assessed (e.g. assessing vitamin status)

- Page 23 Line 7 - compliance - earlier a pill count was mentioned, do you have the information on treatment compliance based on this? Was there a compliance cut off used for including participants in analysis?

- Within sample size calculation information I don't think it is clear why a medium-large effect size (cohen's d = 0.7) was used. Was this based off previous green tea literature? Please expand.

- Whilst this is recognised several times, consider highlighting each time that the PSQI finding is a trend only to be careful when over interpreting this finding.

I also found a few minor typos:

Page 6 Line 3 – extra comma

Page 20 line 4 – matcha misspelled

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Reviewer #3: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Attachment

Submitted filename: renamed_7c415.docx

pone.0309287.s006.docx (19KB, docx)
PLoS One. 2024 Aug 30;19(8):e0309287. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0309287.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


26 May 2024

Reponse to the reviewers’ comments

Reviewer 1:

Thank you for your insightful suggestions, we have made several changes in the revised manuscript according to the Reviewer’s comments. These changes have enriched the context of our research, providing a clearer pathway for our readers to understand the implications of our findings.

1. Data statement – no explanation as to why data can’t be shared. States without permission from all authors but no explanation as to why all authors wouldn’t give permission. Is data owned by a third party?

Ans: After careful consideration and discussion among the authors, we decided to share the data in this study. The data has been uploaded to Dryad.

https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.2280gb61r

We have added the following sentence.

Page 36, lines 563-564

The data in this study can be accessed in Dryad Digital Repository (https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.2280gb61r).

2. Typo on page 6, line 3, double comma

Page 5, lines 53

Ans: We have corrected it in the revised manuscript.

3. Line 8 – could some context be given as to what is being classified as ‘middle age’ here.

Ans: According to reference (5), people aged 45-54 years were classified as “middle age.” We changed the sentences as follows in the revised manuscript.

Page 5, lines 58-60

Accumulating evidence suggests that lifestyle habits after middle age (approximately 45–54 years old) significantly impact the maintenance of cognitive function in older adults [3–5]

4. Within intervention section it would be useful to provide some justification as to why this dosage was decided upon.

Ans: As the amount of matcha in the traditional tea ceremony (Tea Otemae) is 2 g, a daily intake of matcha was set at 2 g.

We have added the following sentence.

Page 15, lines 233-235

As the amount of matcha in the traditional tea ceremony (Tea Otemae) is 2 g, the daily intake quantity of matcha was set at 2 g in this study.

5. For statistical analysis is there justification as to why per protocol participants were analysed rather than Intent To Treat (ITT). CONSORT recommends reporting both in parallel, so some acknowledgement of ITT would be beneficial here, were the results the same?

Ans: We have clarified the analytical methodologies concerning the intent-to-treat (ITT) and per-protocol (PP) in the revised manuscript. According to the ITT principle, we presented the data by ITT analysis in the original and revised manuscript. We analyzed changes in outcome variables from baseline to 12 months using a mixed-effects model using 46 participants in the matcha group and 45 in the placebo group. In addition to the ITT analysis, PP analysis was conducted, the results are shown in the S3 and S4 Tables of the revised manuscript, and corresponding modifications and textual additions have been made in the revised manuscript as follows:

Page 21, lines 350-352

A total of 99 participants were randomized and allocated to the matcha group (n = 49) and matched placebo group (n = 50).

Page 22, lines 366-373

The total number of completed questionnaires was 46 in the matcha group and 45 in the placebo group. During the intervention, each participant with MCI in the matcha and placebo groups was diagnosed with dementia at the 12-month evaluation. After excluding these two participants, the per-protocol analysis of the intervention effects included 45 and 44 participants in the matcha and placebo groups, respectively. According to the intention-to-treat principle, we analyzed the changes in outcome variables from baseline to 12 months were statistically analyzed using a mixed-effects model, with 49 and 50 participants in the matcha and placebo groups, respectively.

Page 28, lines 422-425

We obtained results from both intent-to-treat and per-protocol analyses. The per-protocol analysis showed that differences in social acuity and PSQI scores between the groups were -1.44 (95% CI: -2.88, 0.001; P = 0.077) and 0.89 (95% CI: -0.001, 1.74; P = 1.08), respectively (S3 and S4 Tables).

6. The results are interesting and placed well within previous literature. However, there were no effects on primary outcome, and this should be acknowledged somewhere in the discussion.

Ans: We agree that the results of the primary outcome should be acknowledged. A corresponding statement has been added to the Discussion section. In the revised manuscript, we added the following sentences.

Page 28, lines 419-422

Other cognitive functions, including the primary outcomes (MoCA-J and ADCS-MCI-ADL), showed no significant changes in the matcha group compared with the placebo control group.

Page 34, lines 524-527

However, the matcha intervention had no effect on the primary outcomes (MoCA-J and ADCS-MCI-ADL) possibly due to their sensitivity to only slight declines in cognitive function. These tests are typically used during clinical diagnoses rather than for evaluating early-stage cognitive decline in the disease.

Reviewer 2:

Thank you for your crucial insights regarding the statistical elements of our study and the noted deviations from the CONSORT guidelines. These observations led us to meticulously revise the manuscript in accordance with the CONSORT standards. We believe that these adjustments have fortified the methods and results, and sharpened the conclusions, thereby enhancing the overall integrity of the manuscript.

1. Kindly note that your ABSTRACT is well drafted (in my opinion), but is ‘assay type’. It is preferable [refer to item 1b of CONSORT checklist 2010: Structured summary of trial design, methods, results, and conclusions] to divide the ABSTRACT with small sections like ‘Objective(s)’, ‘Methods’, ‘Results’, ‘Conclusions’, etc. which is an accepted practice of most of the good/standard journals [including this one, though ‘The PLoS One Guidelines to Authors’ did not specify an Abstract format, it is desirable]. It will definitely be more informative then, I guess, whatever the article type may be. Since your ‘Article Type: Clinical Trial’, it very much desirable.

Ans: Following the reviewer's comment, we have revised the abstract in accordance with the CONSORT guidelines

Pages 3-4, Abstract

Objective: Lifestyle habits after middle age significantly impact the maintenance of cognitive function in older adults. Nutritional intake is closely related to lifestyle habits; therefore, nutrition is a pivotal factor in the prevention of dementia in the preclinical stages. Matcha green tea powder (matcha), which contains epigallocatechin gallate, theanine, and caffeine, has beneficial effects on cognitive function and mood. We conducted a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled clinical study over 12 months to examine the effect of matcha on cognitive function and sleep quality.

Methods: Ninety-nine participants, including 64 with subjective cognitive decline and 35 with mild cognitive impairment, were randomized, with 49 receiving 2 g of matcha and 50 receiving a placebo daily. Participants were stratified based on two factors: age at baseline and APOE genotype. Changes in cognitive function and sleep quality were analyzed using a mixed-effects model.

Results: Matcha consumption led to significant improvements in social acuity score (difference: -1.39, 95% confidence interval; -2.78, 0.002) (P = 0.028) as evaluated by the perception of facial emotions in cognitive function. The primary outcomes, that is, Montreal Cognitive Assessment and Alzheimer’s Disease Cooperative Study Activity of Daily Living scores, showed no significant changes with matcha intervention. Meanwhile, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index scores indicated a trend toward improvement with a difference of 0.86 (95% confidence interval; -0.002, 1.71) (P = 0.088) between the groups in changes from baseline to 12 months.

Conclusions: The present study suggests regular consumption of matcha could improve emotional perception and sleep quality in older adults with mild cognitive decline. Given the widespread availability and cultural acceptance of matcha green tea, incorporating it into the daily routine may offer a simple yet effective strategy for cognitive enhancement and dementia prevention.

2. In addition, there are few issues/observations about which I have different opinion. Such observations/concerns are given below [if acceptable/agreed, try to incorporate them (consider positively wherever possible)]:

First is regarding choice of ‘Sample size’. One of the very important items in CONSORT guidelines is ’How sample size was determined (Item 7a)’ and expected/desired to described in ample details including all assumptions and software used.

In fact, it is very surprising to note the ‘absence’ of the important term “CONSORT’’ despite that the manuscript is on ‘A randomized controlled clinical study/trial’. It is well-known that while reporting [findings from and even planning that is in ‘protocol’ of] ‘Clinical Trial’ one should follow/cover CONSORT guidelines.

In ‘Discussion’ it is just said/mentioned that “In this study, unbiased randomization was conducted, and the trial was implemented as a triple-blind study”. Regarding these important issues more discussion/detailed account is expected. Few important items {and all ‘Clinical Trials’ must follow} in CONSORT guidelines are ‘How sample size was determined (Item 7a)’,’ Random Sequence generation (Item 8a)’, ‘Allocation concealment (Item 9)’, Blinding (Item 11a). Since your article type is ‘Clinical Trial’, you are supposed to cover these items in the report or even in ‘Protocol’.

You may know that ‘Permuted Block Randomization’ ensures same group sizes (not simple randomization) and that randomization is a process [not only sequence generation] which includes ‘Allocation Concealment’}. Which ‘Randomization’ procedure is used in this study is not specified but almost equal group sizes are obtained. It must be ‘Permuted Block Randomization’ but such clear mention is desired.

Ans: According to the reviewer’s comment, we added the following sentences and references in the Materials and Methods: “Study design,” “Randomization,” and “Participants” of the revised manuscript.

Page 8, line 110-page 9, line 123

Study design

A 12-month intervention was executed employing a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel-group design. Before the initiation of the study, the clinical trial was registered with the UMIN Clinical Trials Registry (UMIN-CTR), which is recognized by the International Committee of Medical Journal Editors (ICMJE) as an approved registry. This study was conducted as a randomized controlled trial, adhering strictly to the CONSORT guidelines. The required sample size was calculated using G*Power 3 software (Heinrich Heine University, Düsseldorf, Germany). We chose to assess the difference between the medians of two independent groups using the Wilcoxon or Mann–Whitney test. With the parameters set as two-tailed, logistic distribution, an effect size of 0.7, a significance level of 0.05, and a power of 0.8, it was determined that each group required 31 cases. Anticipating a dropout rate of 7 of 40 cases per group, we established a target sample size of at least 33 cases per group to validate the robustness of our findings (details are described in S1 File).

Page 9, lines 124-133

Randomization

Sequence generation

Eligible participants with SCD or MCI were randomized using a computer-generated sequence. Participants were stratified based on two stratification factors, age at baseline (≥ 74 years / < 74 years) and APOE genotype (ε4 positive/negative). Participants were stratified based on age at 74 years because a 10-year AD risk is higher in people ≥ 74 years old than in those < 74 years old [30]. Allocation to either the control or intervention group was intended to be randomized in a 1:1 ratio using computerized minimization, which aims to minimize differences between groups with respect to the stratification factors [31].

Page 9, line 134-page 11, line 166

Allocation concealment mechanism

Participants underwent randomization using a dynamic program implemented via a Microsoft Excel macro, employing simple randomization rather than permuted block randomization. Upon running the program, the participants were assigned to either Group A or Group B. However, the program did not reveal which was the intervention or control group. The randomization table created by the program was retained until all baseline stratification factors were fully aligned and participant inclusion was complete, ensuring concealment of the allocation.

All eligible participants who provided informed consent and met the predefined inclusion criteria underwent randomization, a process administered at the discretion of the study site personnel overseeing recruitment and medical interviews. Subsequently, the study site transmitted a stratification factor response form to an independent allocation manager who was responsible for the allocation process, ensuring separation from the outcome evaluation analysis. The allocation manager utilized a dynamic randomization program incorporating provided stratification factors to assign participants to their respective groups based on the program's output. Physicians and nurses responsible for participant inclusion and symptom assessment remained blinded to group assignments.

The allocation sequence utilized a stratified approach, considering study site and medication compliance, employing a dynamic allocation method with the minimization technique. Concealment of randomization details from data management personnel and statistical analysts was upheld throughout the study while ensuring the database remained publicly accessible. The randomization list was securely maintained by designated allocation personnel throughout the study, mitigating any potential influence from principal investigators involved in the research.

Blinding

Throughout the study duration, participants were unaware of whether the capsules they consumed contained matcha or placebo until the study's conclusion was obtained. Physicians, nurses, and principal investigators at the study site were informed that the participants had been assigned to provisional groups (Groups A and B), yet remained uninformed about the specific allocation to matcha or placebo groups until the randomization list was released after the study period. No member of the research team knew the allocated sequence until the end of the statistical analysis after the database was locked.

Page 11, lines 167-170

Participants

Community-dwelling older adults aged 60–85 years were recruited and assessment for eligibility was performed at the University of Tsukuba Hospital (Tsukuba, Ibaraki, Japan) and Memory Clinic (Toride, Ibaraki, Japan) (Fig 1).

3. In ‘Results-Participants’ section (as well as in Figure-1), you may use the word ‘screened’ in place of ‘recruited’ [As shown in Fig 1, we recruited 939 community-dwelling older adults aged 60–85 years and 124 participants were enrolled], in my opinion. May please confirm as word ‘recruited’ implies that 939 participated in trial. It may be known to you that when random allocation/assignment is used/done, any statistical comparison of baseline characteristics is not required. In this context [refer to Table-1], I request authors to read following note pasted from one famous standard textbook on ‘Medical Research Methodology’.

Ans: I would like to thank you very much for your valuable comments and recommendation of the textbook. I think that this is the book by Joseph Abramson.

I am sorry for the misuse of the term. We have corrected the word “screened” to “recruited.” We have also revised Fig 1 and Table 1.

4. To provide a description of baseline characteristics is entirely reasonable (since it is clearly important in assessing to whom the results of the trial can be applied), however, statistical comparison of baseline characteristics when random allocation/assignment is used/done [often for good/standard/leading journals these days] is not required, because even if P-value(s) turn(s) out to be significant (while comparing baseline characteristics despite random allocation), it is, by definition, a false positive as you then are supposed to be testing ‘randomization’ then, which in any single tri

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

pone.0309287.s007.docx (57.8KB, docx)

Decision Letter 1

Valerio Manippa

11 Jun 2024

PONE-D-24-02571R1Effect of matcha green tea on cognitive functions and sleep quality in older adults with cognitive decline: A randomized controlled study over 12 monthsPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Uchida,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Jul 26 2024 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Valerio Manippa

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

Reviewer #3: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #2: COMMENTS: Since all of the comments made on earlier draft are considered positively & mostly attended satisfactorily, I recommend the acceptance after minor revision. Although many modifications could be seen clearly, few details/modifications given while addressing comments were/are not very convincing [example: account regarding choice of ‘Sample size’]. ‘Minor revision’ is recommended so that authors can re-visit earlier comments and further modify by taking clues from earlier comments. However, please do not limit the revision only (with respect) to comments made there. More improvement is expected {note that ‘clinical implications’ are valuable}.

Reviewer #3: (No Response)

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #2: No

Reviewer #3: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2024 Aug 30;19(8):e0309287. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0309287.r004

Author response to Decision Letter 1


20 Jun 2024

Response to the reviewer’s comments

Reviewer 2:

COMMENTS:

Since all of the comments made on earlier draft are considered positively & mostly attended satisfactorily, I recommend the acceptance after minor revision. Although many modifications could be seen clearly, few details/modifications given while addressing comments were/are not very convincing [example: account regarding choice of ‘Sample size’].

‘Minor revision’ is recommended so that authors can re-visit earlier comments and further modify by taking clues from earlier comments. However, please do not limit the revision only (with respect) to comments made there. More improvement is expected {note that ‘clinical implications’ are valuable}.

ANSWER:

We are sincerely grateful for the opportunity to further improve our manuscript. In response to your valuable feedback, we have deepened the discussion regarding the clinical implications and limitations of our findings. We have incorporated a paragraph that explicitly outlines how our results concerning the effects of matcha consumption may contribute to dementia prevention strategies (page 35, line 554 – page 36, line 561). This paragraph also discusses the implications of our findings for the field of preventive medicine, thereby strengthening the connection between our research and its practical applications.

Regarding the chosen sample size, we acknowledge your feedback indicating that our rationale was not sufficiently articulated. To address this, we have anchored our determination of sample size in a robust statistical rationale, incorporating considerations such as power analysis and effect size, consistent with the guidelines recommended for studies of this nature. We have elaborated on this discussion in the revised manuscript, providing a more detailed justification and citing studies [30–32] employing analogous methodologies (page 8, line 120 – page 9, line 126).

We have diligently reviewed the manuscript to ensure consistency of content. Additionally, we have verified the references to ensure they are complete and correct, deleted duplicated references, and added three new references.

We added the following sentences and references in the Materials and Methods:

Page 8, line 120 – Page 9, line 126

A meta-analysis was conducted to ascertain whether the effect size and statistical power were commensurate with the determined sample size, referencing three intervention studies that employed the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) as the evaluative criterion [29–31]. Although the efficacy indices in this study vary slightly from those in previous research, we concluded that observing the anticipated effects outlined in the meta-analysis would render the data from all 66 patients a significant evidence base for preparing the subsequent validation study.

References

30. Tsolaki M, Kounti F, Agogiatou C, Poptsi E, Bakoglidou E, Zafeiropoulou M, et al. Effectiveness of nonpharmacological approaches in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Neurodegener Dis. 2011;8: 138–145.

31. Marzolini S, Oh P, McIlroy W, Brooks D. The effects of an aerobic and resistance exercise training program on cognition following stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2013;27: 392–402.

32. Sukontapol C, Kemsen S, Chansirikarn S, Nakawiro D, Kuha O, Taemeeyapradit U. The effectiveness of a cognitive training program in people with mild cognitive impairment: A study in urban community. Asian J Psychiatr. 2018;35: 18–23.

Page 9, lines 132 – 134

Anticipating a dropout rate of 7 of 40 recruited cases per group, we established a target sample size of at least 33 cases per group to validate the robustness of our findings (details are described in S1 File).

We added the following sentences in Discussion.

Page 35, line 554 – Page 36, line 561

The beneficial effects of matcha consumption revealed in this study may contribute to dementia prevention strategies, as these are relatively easy lifestyle changes to implement in daily life. Matcha consumption improved facial emotion recognition, as assessed by a computerized neurocognitive battery, since conventional psychological tests may not be suitable for evaluating cognitive decline in very mild cases. Thus, more sensitive neuropsychological testing, along with intervention strategies that are easy for elderly people to maintain, is necessary to assess the efficacy of these interventions in dementia prevention.

Attachment

Submitted filename: renamed_f977b.docx

pone.0309287.s008.docx (22.9KB, docx)

Decision Letter 2

Madepalli K Lakshmana

9 Aug 2024

Effect of matcha green tea on cognitive functions and sleep quality in older adults with cognitive decline: A randomized controlled study over 12 months

PONE-D-24-02571R2

Dear Dr. Uchida,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice will be generated when your article is formally accepted. Please note, if your institution has a publishing partnership with PLOS and your article meets the relevant criteria, all or part of your publication costs will be covered. Please make sure your user information is up-to-date by logging into Editorial Manager at Editorial Manager® and clicking the ‘Update My Information' link at the top of the page. If you have any questions relating to publication charges, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Madepalli K. Lakshmana, Ph.D

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #3: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #2: COMMENTS: Further to what I stated earlier that all of the comments made on original draft were attended satisfactorily, I add that comment made on revised draft also is attended and I recommend the acceptance without any hesitation now.

Reviewer #3: (No Response)

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #2: No

Reviewer #3: No

**********

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 File. Supporting materials and methods.

    (PDF)

    pone.0309287.s001.pdf (444KB, pdf)
    S1 Table. Subsets of neurocognitive test corresponding to neurocognitive domain score used in the matcha intervention study.

    (PDF)

    pone.0309287.s002.pdf (107.7KB, pdf)
    S2 Table. Baseline clinical chemistry parameters of participants in the matcha and placebo groups.

    (PDF)

    pone.0309287.s003.pdf (114.1KB, pdf)
    S3 Table. Effect of 12-month matcha intervention on sleep quality and cognitive functioning in per-protocol analysis.

    (PDF)

    pone.0309287.s004.pdf (89.2KB, pdf)
    S4 Table. Effect of 12-month matcha intervention on neuroimaging in per-protocol analysis.

    (PDF)

    pone.0309287.s005.pdf (86.7KB, pdf)
    Attachment

    Submitted filename: renamed_7c415.docx

    pone.0309287.s006.docx (19KB, docx)
    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    pone.0309287.s007.docx (57.8KB, docx)
    Attachment

    Submitted filename: renamed_f977b.docx

    pone.0309287.s008.docx (22.9KB, docx)

    Data Availability Statement

    The data can be shared publicly without permission from a public repository after acceptance. The data files are available from the Dryad Digital Repository. https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.2280gb61r.


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES