Keywords: association cortex, default network, fMRI, functional connectivity, parcellation
Abstract
The cerebral cortex is populated by specialized regions that are organized into networks. Here we estimated networks from functional MRI (fMRI) data in intensively sampled participants. The procedure was developed in two participants (scanned 31 times) and then prospectively applied to 15 participants (scanned 8–11 times). Analysis of the networks revealed a global organization. Locally organized first-order sensory and motor networks were surrounded by spatially adjacent second-order networks that linked to distant regions. Third-order networks possessed regions distributed widely throughout association cortex. Regions of distinct third-order networks displayed side-by-side juxtapositions with a pattern that repeated across multiple cortical zones. We refer to these as supra-areal association megaclusters (SAAMs). Within each SAAM, two candidate control regions were adjacent to three separate domain-specialized regions. Response properties were explored with task data. The somatomotor and visual networks responded to body movements and visual stimulation, respectively. Second-order networks responded to transients in an oddball detection task, consistent with a role in orienting to salient events. The third-order networks, including distinct regions within each SAAM, showed two levels of functional specialization. Regions linked to candidate control networks responded to working memory load across multiple stimulus domains. The remaining regions dissociated across language, social, and spatial/episodic processing domains. These results suggest that progressively higher-order networks nest outward from primary sensory and motor cortices. Within the apex zones of association cortex, there is specialization that repeatedly divides domain-flexible from domain-specialized regions. We discuss implications of these findings, including how repeating organizational motifs may emerge during development.
NEW & NOTEWORTHY The organization of cerebral networks was estimated within individuals with intensive, repeat sampling of fMRI data. A hierarchical organization emerged in each individual that delineated first-, second-, and third-order cortical networks. Regions of distinct third-order association networks consistently exhibited side-by-side juxtapositions that repeated across multiple cortical zones, with clear and robust functional specialization among the embedded regions.
INTRODUCTION
The primate cerebral cortex is populated by specialized networks that support sensory, motor, and higher-order cognitive and affective functions. Characterizing how the networks and their interconnected regions are organized on the cortical surface began more than a century ago with landmark studies of myelogenetic and architectonic patterns (e.g., Refs. 1–5) and continued with modern systems neuroscience integration of anatomical projection data and insights from study of brain lesions (e.g., Refs. 6–13). Over the past decades our laboratory, and the field more broadly, has undertaken data collection efforts and analyses of neuroimaging data with the goal of improving understanding of human network organization and providing noninvasive approaches to measure brain organization for clinical use.
It is beyond the present scope to cover the extensive literature that has evolved, but it is important to interpret the current effort with awareness that new details and revisions emerge incrementally as the methods and data quality progress. Our efforts presented here reflect another step in that progression. The specific challenge in examining the details of network organization in humans is that the methods are indirect and limited and often noisy within individuals. Despite limitations, advances in structural, diffusion, and functional MRI (fMRI) provide valuable information about human cortical organization, albeit with ambiguities consistent with the complexity of cortical architecture and the low resolution of the techniques.
Resting-state functional connectivity MRI (fcMRI), based on measuring spontaneous correlated fluctuations between brain regions, has been especially useful for estimating networks (Ref. 14; see also Refs. 15–20). Explorations in group-averaged fcMRI data, with sample sizes that range from 10 to thousands of participants, reveal network estimates that are consistent across analytical approaches and datasets (e.g., Refs. 21–27). Moreover, estimated networks show similarities to directly observed anatomical projection patterns from tracer injections in the monkey, providing support that they reflect, to a first approximation, anatomically connected networks (28–36). Correspondence is far from perfect, and there are unresolved aspects to how indirect human network estimates link to anatomy, a theme that we return to in discussion.
A recent advance in the field is to use within-individual estimates of networks without recourse to averaging across participants. Architectonic fields tile the cortical mantle with variability in their exact locations, sizes, and borders between individuals (37–45). Spatial blurring, inherent in group averaging, impedes the ability to estimate details of network organization. Precision neuroimaging, involving intensive sampling and analysis of data within the individual, preserves idiosyncratic anatomical features.
Within-individual approaches have been the mainstay in human neuroimaging studies of sensory and motor systems (e.g., Refs. 46–51) and emerged later as a viable method to estimate task-based responses in higher-order association cortex (e.g., Refs. 52–57). Within-individual precision mapping using fcMRI only became emphasized recently, even though the first report was within individuals (14). Following a landmark demonstration that intensive repeat scanning is possible (58, 59), multiple groups have pursued within-individual characterization of network organization (e.g., Refs. 60–68; for further discussion see Refs. 69, 70).
Here we continue the investigation of the detailed organization of the cerebral cortex, using within-individual approaches. There are multiple goals and methodological innovations that steer this work. First, we employ deep, intensive imaging to boost the signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) within individual participants. Each new participant was scanned on at least eight separate occasions and often more. Second, we applied a novel multisession hierarchical Bayesian model (MS-HBM; see Ref. 71) to automatically estimate networks in the intensively sampled participants. Specifically, the number of networks estimated was set at 15 to capture established networks sometimes missed in simpler network parcellations, as detailed within methods. Third, to enable clinical translational research, we developed an empirical method and projected all network estimates from the surface back into the native-space volume of individual participants, as is needed for presurgical planning and neuromodulation. Fourth, inspired by the possibility to chart global spatial relations between networks (e.g., Ref. 72), we also plotted the resulting network estimates on the fully flattened cortical surface (73, 74). As the results reveal, there are repeating patterns of spatial juxtapositions among networks that provide insight into their evolutionary and developmental origins. Finally, we collected and examined task data within the same intensively sampled participants to test whether within-individual network estimates predict functional response patterns and also to explore between-network functional dissociations.
The raw data and our network parcellations generated through this research effort are provided to the community as an open resource.
METHODS
Overview
We sought to estimate networks within individuals with high precision. The analyses proceeded in three stages: 1) a refinement stage established the methods for estimating networks; 2) an implementation stage applied the methods prospectively to 15 new participants; and 3) a functional testing stage explored functional response properties and dissociations between networks.
In the refinement stage, previously reported datasets (N = 2; see Refs. 62, 75) were analyzed to establish novel MS-HBM network estimates that incorporated priors for 15 distinct networks (in contrast to 10 networks used in earlier work). Each of the participants performed 31 independent MRI sessions, allowing considerable data to test for within-individual reliability.
In the implementation stage, the 15-network MS-HBM was prospectively applied to 15 new participants who were each scanned over 8–11 sessions. The model was estimated for each participant in a fully automated fashion, and the networks were confirmed by model-free seed region-based functional connectivity. After network estimation, the overlap and variability of each network across individuals were examined and atlases constructed for open use.
In the final functional testing stage, an extensive battery of tasks was administered to the same individuals and analyzed to explore whether the estimated networks predicted functional responses.
Participants
Seventeen native English-speaking volunteers participated for payment. History of a neurological or psychiatric illness was an exclusion criterion. Participants provided written informed consent with protocols approved by the Institutional Review Board of Harvard University. For the refinement stage data, two right-handed adult women ages 22 and 23 yr participated [data previously reported in Braga et al. (62) and Xue et al. (75)]. The refinement stage data participants are labeled S1 and S2 to match Xue et al. (75). For the implementation stage data, 15 right-handed adults ages 18–34 yr participated (mean = 22.1 yr, SD = 3.9 yr; 9 women). Participants came from diverse racial and ethnic backgrounds (9 of the 17 individuals self-reported as non-White and/or Hispanic). A subset of the participants contributing implementation stage data also enrolled in a study of motor movement mapping (76). The implementation stage participants are labeled P1–P15.
MRI Data Acquisition
Data were acquired at the Harvard Center for Brain Science with a 3-T Prismafit MRI scanner (Siemens Healthineers, Erlangen, Germany). A 64-channel phased-array head-neck coil (Siemens Healthineers) was used in the refinement stage and for a subset of motor task sessions in the implementation stage. A 32-channel phased-array head coil (Siemens Healthineers) was used to acquire all other data in the implementation stage. For functional neuroimaging, the differences between these two coils are minimal, and the data were treated as comparable. Foam and inflated padding mitigated head motion. Participants were instructed to remain still and alert and to look at a rear-projected display through a mirror attached to the head coil. The display had a resolution of 1,280 × 1,024 pixels and a screen width of 43 cm, with an effective viewing distance of 104 cm (54 pixels per visual degree). Eyes were video recorded with an EyeLink 1000 Plus with Long-Range Mount (SR Research, Ottawa, ON, Canada), and alertness was scored during each functional run. MRI data quality was monitored during the scan with Framewise Integrated Real-time MRI Monitoring (FIRMM; see Ref. 77).
Refinement stage data.
Each participant (S1 and S2) was scanned across 31 MRI sessions over 28–40 wk, with no sessions on consecutive days. Each session involved multiple resting-state fixation runs that were reanalyzed here for functional connectivity analysis, for a total of 63 functional MRI (fMRI) runs obtained for each individual. fMRI data were acquired with blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) contrast (78, 79). A custom multiband gradient-echo echo-planar pulse sequence provided by the Center for Magnetic Resonance Research (CMRR) at the University of Minnesota was used (Refs. 80, 81; see also Ref. 82): voxel size = 2.4 mm, repetition time (TR) = 1,000 ms, echo time (TE) = 32.6 ms, flip angle = 64°, matrix 88 × 88 × 65, anterior-to-posterior (AP) phase encoding, multislice 5× acceleration, fully covering the cerebrum and cerebellum. Signal dropout was minimized by automatically (83) selecting a slice 25° from the anterior-posterior commissural plane toward the coronal plane (84, 85). Each run lasted 7 min 2 s (422 frames, with the first 12 frames removed for T1 equilibration). A dual-gradient-echo B0 field map was acquired to correct for spatial distortions: TE = 4.45 and 6.91 ms, with slice prescription/spatial resolution matched to the BOLD sequence. During BOLD scanning, participants fixated a centrally presented plus sign (black on a gray background). The scanner room was illuminated.
A rapid T1-weighted (T1w) structural scan was obtained with a multiecho magnetization prepared rapid acquisition gradient echo (ME-MPRAGE) three-dimensional sequence (86): voxel size = 1.2 mm, TR = 2,200 ms, TE = 1.57, 3.39, 5.21, 7.03 ms, TI = 1,100 ms, flip angle = 7°, matrix 192 × 192 × 176, in-plane generalized autocalibrating partial parallel acquisition (GRAPPA) acceleration = 4.
Implementation stage data.
Each participant (P1−P15) was scanned across 8–11 sessions, most often over 6–10 wk. A few participants had longer gaps between the first and last MRI sessions up to 1 yr. Each session involved multiple fMRI runs to be used for functional connectivity analysis, for a total of 17–24 resting-state fixation runs obtained for each individual. BOLD acquisition parameters were similar to the refinement stage data: voxel size = 2.4 mm, TR = 1,000 ms, TE = 33.0 ms, flip angle = 64°, matrix 92 × 92 × 65 [field of view (FOV) = 221 × 221], 65 slices covering the full cerebrum and cerebellum. Each resting-state fixation run again lasted 7 min 2 s (422 frames, with the first 12 frames removed for T1 equilibration). Dual-gradient-echo B0 field maps were also acquired, with parameters matched to the refinement stage. The first two sessions of P12 were acquired in a different FOV (211 × 211); therefore, the matrix for both BOLD runs and field maps was 88 × 88 × 65 and BOLD TE = 32.6 ms, matching S1 and S2. The change in FOV did not affect the quality of registration or impact the analyses in any way we could detect.
High-resolution T1w and T2-weighted (T2w) scans were acquired for the implementation stage data based on the Human Connectome Project (HCP; Ref. 87): T1w MPRAGE parameters: voxel size = 0.8 mm, TR = 2,500 ms, TE = 1.81, 3.60, 5.39, and 7.18 ms, TI = 1,000 ms, flip angle = 8°, matrix 320 × 320 × 208, 144, in-plane GRAPPA acceleration = 2; T2w sampling perfection with application-optimized contrasts using different flip angle evolution sequence (SPACE) parameters: voxel size = 0.8 mm, TR = 3,200 ms, TE = 564 ms, 208 slices, matrix = 320 × 300 × 208, in-plane GRAPPA acceleration = 2. Rapid T1w structural scans were also obtained as backup with the refinement stage sequence but with matrix 192 × 192 × 144.
Functional testing stage data.
To explore functional response properties, extensive task-based BOLD fMRI data were collected on participants P1–P15. Task runs used the same sequence as the resting-state fixation runs, ensuring that the estimated networks would be spatially aligned to the task-based data. Details of the task designs, stimuli, and run structure are described in Task Paradigms.
Exclusion Criteria and Quality Control
Each BOLD fMRI run was examined for quality. Exclusion criteria generally consisted of the parameters reported in Xue et al. (75), including 1) maximum absolute motion >1.8 mm and 2) slice-based SNR <130. Runs with SNR >100 but also SNR <130 were retained if motion and visual inspection indicated adequate quality. For the functional testing stage data, the maximum absolute motion for the Episodic Projection task was >2.5 mm given the long duration. One borderline motor run (P2) was included with motion of 1.9 mm as the motion was largely due to a linear drift. For the refinement stage, usable resting-state runs were 62 (S1) and 61 (S2) runs. For the implementation stage, usable resting-state runs ranged from 15 (P11) to 24 (P12) runs. For the functional testing stage, usable task runs ranged from 18 (P5) to 70 (P12) runs (see Table 1). All data exclusions were finalized before functional connectivity and task response analyses.
Table 1.
Functional data analyzed for each participant
Participant | Fixation | Motor | Visual | Oddball | N-Back | Theory-of-Mind | Sentence Processing | Episodic Projection |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
S1 | 62(63) | |||||||
S2 | 61(63) | |||||||
P1 | 17(17) | 0(0) | 5(5) | 5(5) | 8(8) | 8(8) | 6(6) | 10(10) |
P2 | 16(17) | 11(12) | 5(5) | 5(5) | 8(8) | 8(8) | 6(6) | 10(10) |
P3 | 19(22) | 10(12) | 5(5) | 4(5) | 8(8) | 7(8) | 6(6) | 8(10) |
P4 | 20(22) | 10(12) | 5(5) | 5(5) | 8(8) | 8(8) | 5(6) | 9(10) |
P5 | 22(22) | 8(12) | 0(5) | 4(5) | 6(7) | 8(8) | 3(6) | 7(10) |
P6 | 21(22) | 12(12) | 5(5) | 5(5) | 8(8) | 8(8) | 6(6) | 10(10) |
P7 | 22(22) | 12(12) | 5(5) | 5(5) | 8(8) | 8(8) | 6(6) | 10(10) |
P8 | 21(22) | 12(12) | 5(5) | 5(5) | 8(8) | 8(8) | 6(6) | 10(10) |
P9 | 20(22) | 12(12) | 5(5) | 5(5) | 8(8) | 7(8) | 6(6) | 8(10) |
P10 | 23(23) | 24(24) | 0(5) | 2(5) | 7(8) | 8(8) | 12(12) | 10(10) |
P11 | 15(20) | 3(12) | 3(5) | 3(5) | 8(8) | 8(8) | 6(6) | 7(10) |
P12 | 24(24) | 24(24) | 5(5) | 4(5) | 8(8) | 8(8) | 11(12) | 10(10) |
P13 | 22(22) | 12(12) | 5(5) | 5(5) | 8(8) | 8(8) | 6(6) | 10(10) |
P14 | 19(19) | 9(11) | 5(5) | 5(5) | 8(8) | 8(8) | 6(6) | 10(10) |
P15 | 20(22) | 12(12) | 5(5) | 3(5) | 8(8) | 8(8) | 6(6) | 10(10) |
Numbers show functional MRI (fMRI) runs available for analysis after exclusions; numbers in parentheses are the total scanned runs. Bold indicates that data were included in final analyses; italics indicates that the task was excluded for that participant. The Theory-of-Mind numbers combine the Pain and False Belief task runs. Participants P10 and P12 had 24 Motor runs and up to 12 Sentence Processing runs because of their participation in Saadon-Grosman et al. (76).
Data Processing and Registration That Minimizes Spatial Blurring
Data were processed with an in-house preprocessing pipeline (“iProc”) that preserves spatial details by minimizing blurring and multiple interpolations [described in detail in Braga et al. (62)]. For the refinement stage data (S1 and S2), the processed data were taken directly from Xue et al. (75), with relevant method description repeated here. For the implementation stage data (P1–P15), the changes in processing included the use of the high-resolution T1w and T2w structural images. For one participant (P12), the registration failed with the 0.8-mm T1w structural image and their 1.2-mm image was used as a backup. For another participant (P1), only the 0.8-mm T1w structural image was used, without a paired T2w image.
Data were interpolated to a 1-mm isotropic T1w native-space atlas (with all processing steps composed into a single interpolation) that was then projected with FreeSurfer v6.0.0 to the fsaverage6 cortical surface (40,962 vertices per hemisphere; see Ref. 74). Four transformation matrices were calculated: 1) a motion correction matrix for each volume to the run’s middle volume [linear registration, 6 degrees of freedom (DOF); MCFLIRT, FSL], 2) a matrix for field map-unwarping the run’s middle volume, correcting for field inhomogeneities caused by susceptibility gradients (FUGUE, FSL), 3) a matrix for registering the field map-unwarped middle BOLD volume to the within-individual mean BOLD template (12 DOF; FLIRT, FSL), and 4) a matrix for registering the mean BOLD template to the participant’s T1w native-space image, which was resampled to 1.0-mm isotropic resolution (6 DOF; using boundary-based registration, FreeSurfer). The individual-specific mean BOLD template was created by averaging all field map-unwarped middle volumes after being registered to an upsampled 1.2 mm and unwarped midvolume template (an interim target, selected from a low-motion run, typically acquired close to a field map).
For resting-state fixation runs, confounding variables including six head motion parameters, whole brain signal, ventricular signal, deep cerebral white matter signal, and their temporal derivatives were calculated from the BOLD data in the T1w native-space volume. The signals were regressed out from the BOLD data with 3dTproject (AFNI; Refs. 88, 89). The residual BOLD data were then band-pass filtered at 0.01–0.1-Hz using 3dBandpass (AFNI; Refs. 88, 89). For task data runs, only whole brain signal was regressed out (see Ref. 90). Data registered to the T1w native-space atlas were resampled to the fsaverage6 standardized cortical surface mesh with trilinear interpolation (featuring 40,962 vertices per hemisphere; Ref. 74) and then surface-smoothed with a 2-mm full-width-at-half-maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel. The iProc pipeline thus allowed for high-resolution and robustly aligned BOLD data, with minimal interpolation and signal loss, output to two relevant spaces: the native-space volume and the fsaverage6 cortical surface. Analyses were performed on the fsaverage6 cortical surface, but the network estimates (parcellations) were projected back into the individual participant’s native-space volume, allowing both surface-based and volume visualization.
Individualized Network Estimates of the Cerebral Cortex Using a Multisession Hierarchical Bayesian Model
The MS-HBM was implemented to estimate cortical networks (71). The MS-HBM was independently implemented for the refinement stage data (S1 and S2) and then subsequently for the implementation stage data (in 3 separate groups: P1–P5, P6–P10, and P11–P15). Estimating the model separately for multiple small groups allowed for prospective replication. As the results reveal, the procedure was robust.
First, the connectivity profile of each vertex on the fsaverage6 cortical surface was estimated as its functional connectivity to 1,175 regions of interest (ROIs) uniformly distributed across the fsaverage surface (23). For each run of data, the Pearson’s correlation coefficients between the fMRI time series at each vertex (40,962 vertices/hemisphere) and the 1,175 ROIs were computed. The resulting 40,962 × 1,175 correlation matrix was then binarized by keeping the top 10% of the correlations to obtain the functional connectivity profiles (23).
Next, the expectation-maximization (EM) algorithm for estimating parameters in the MS-HBM was initialized with a group-level parcellation from a subset of the HCP S900 data release (that itself used the clustering algorithm from our previous study; Ref. 23). It is important to note that the goal of applying the model in this study was to obtain the best estimate of networks within each individual participant’s dataset, not to train parameters and apply them to unseen data from new participants (see Ref. 71). In this analysis, as with our other studies using this approach (68, 75), we did not include the validation step described in Kong et al. (71), so no spatial smoothness prior was applied. Only the training step described in Kong et al. (71) was conducted here. A network label assignment for each vertex was obtained for each participant within the training step.
Refinement stage data.
Data from the two participants (S1 and S2) were analyzed together with the same MS-HBM. The data were used to estimate and compare the 15-network MS-HBM and the 10-network MS-HBM, as well as to explore the reliability of model estimates when subsets of data were analyzed. The results from these initial two participants guided the subsequent processing of the implementation stage data.
The specific impetus for exploring a 15-network model was that networks at or near to the insula did not distinguish multiple networks that had been reported in the literature, variably labeled the cingulo-opercular network and the salience network (Ref. 91; see also Ref. 92), as well as established distinctions at or around primary visual and somatomotor cortex.1 The 15 candidate networks explored here are labeled2 Somatomotor-A (SMOT-A), Somatomotor-B (SMOT-B), Premotor-Posterior Parietal Rostral (PM-PPr), Cingulo-Opercular (CG-OP), Salience/Parietal Memory Network (SAL/PMN), Dorsal Attention-A (dATN-A), Dorsal Attention-B (dATN-B), Frontoparietal Network-A (FPN-A),3 Frontoparietal Network-B (FPN-B), Default Network-A (DN-A), Default Network-B (DN-B), Language (LANG), Visual Central (VIS-C), Visual Peripheral (VIS-P), and Auditory (AUD).
Implementation stage data: discovery, replication, and triplication datasets.
A key aspect of our methods is generalization and replication. The 15 participants in the implementation stage data were divided into discovery, replication, and triplication datasets of 5 participants each.4 The MS-HBM, initialized with a 15-network group-level parcellation obtained from a subset of the HCP S900 data (see Ref. 75), was applied independently to the three separate datasets.
Model-Free Seed Region-Based Confirmation of the Networks
When employing the MS-HBM, there are assumptions about the organization of the brain from the group prior, how many networks should be estimated, and assignment of vertices to only a single network. The idiosyncratic patterns of estimated networks thus could be distorted or fail to capture features of the underlying correlation matrix. To confirm that the individual network estimates were not obligated by the assumptions, a model-free seed region-based analysis was conducted using the same data as the MS-HBM, mirroring the procedures outlined by Braga and Buckner (60). The results were expected to converge if the model did not overly bias network assignments and to diverge if the assignments mismatched the underlying data patterns. Model-free seed region-based confirmation thus served as a check to ensure that network estimates properly captured individual correlation patterns.
For this control check, the pairwise Pearson correlation coefficients between the fMRI time courses at each surface vertex were calculated for each resting-state fixation run, yielding an 81,924 × 81,924 matrix (40,962 vertices/hemisphere). The matrix was then Fisher r-to-z transformed and averaged across all runs to yield a single best estimate of the within-individual correlation matrix. This averaged matrix was used to explore network organization. The mean correlation maps were assigned to a cortical template combining left and right hemispheres of the fsaverage6 surface into the CIFTI format to interactively explore correlation maps with the Connectome Workbench’s wb_view software (96, 97). Seed regions with robust functional connectivity correlation maps were manually selected within MS-HBM network boundaries. Anterior and posterior seed regions were recorded and visualized for each network in all the participants. Thresholds were set at z(r) > 0.2 for all seed regions. The color scales of correlation maps were set to range between 0.2 and 0.6, with the Jet lookup table (color bar) for visualization.
Visualization within the Individual Native-Space Volume
Networks were first estimated and analyzed for each individual on the normalized fsaverage6 surface of FreeSurfer. Surface-based analyses allowed comparisons across individuals and utilization of the group-based priors for initialization of the MS-HBM. However, many applications require network assignments to be utilized within the native-space anatomy of the individual’s own volume (e.g., for presurgical planning and neuromodulation targeting). Given these needs, we devised a robust empirical procedure to project the network estimates back into each individual’s native-space T1w anatomical volume, as well as a procedure to verify that the transformation was valid.
We constructed three separate images within the native-space volume that each varied from 0 to 255 in one of the three Cartesian x-, y-, and z-coordinate axes (e.g., the x-coordinate image possessed a volume that linearly varied in the x-dimension, going from 0 to 255 with no other variation across the image volume). Each separate axis volume was then projected to the fsaverage6 surface with mri_vol2surf and mri_surf2surf (FreeSurfer v6.0.0) with the same spatial transformation used for the projection of the participant’s BOLD fMRI data onto the fsaverage6 surface. Nearest-neighbor interpolation was used. The matrices for this projection were taken from each participant’s processing pipeline (iProc).
In this manner, x, y, and z volume coordinates were obtained on the surface using the exact same spatial transformation matrix as originally applied to the BOLD data. We assigned each surface network label to its corresponding x, y, and z coordinates in the native-space volume. This resulted in a sparse 256 × 256 × 256 matrix in the volume, which was filled with nearest-neighbor interpolation (MATLAB knnsearch). We then masked this with the individual’s FSL-reoriented and binarized cortical ribbon generated by FreeSurfer during preprocessing. As a validity check, the final native-space network estimates were projected back to the surface and compared to the original MS-HBM surface estimates for each participant to ensure no spatial distortions.
The parcellation results from MS-HBM were overlaid onto each individual’s T1w structural image. Sagittal, axial, and coronal slices were chosen to show common landmarks in each individual (midline, left and right insula, anterior commissure, primary sensory and motor cortices).
Signal-to-Noise Ratio Maps
Data acquired with BOLD-contrast (T2* images) and echo-planar imaging possess variable distortion and signal dropout due to magnetic susceptibility artifacts, especially near the sinus and ear canals (e.g., Ref. 98). Vertex-based SNR maps were computed by taking the preprocessed time series from each resting-state fixation run (before regressing out confounding variables) and dividing the mean signal at each vertex by its standard deviation over time. The SNR maps were then averaged across the runs, resulting in an aggregate within-individual SNR map on the fsaverage6 surface. To visualize these effects in the native anatomy, surface maps were projected to the native-space volume with the procedure described above. The only difference is that linear interpolation (MATLAB scatteredInterpolant) was used to fill in the sparsely filled 256 × 256 × 256 matrix.
Variability in Network Estimates between Individuals
To measure spatial variability across individuals, overlap maps of network assignments were computed. For each individual, the spatial extent of their estimated network was plotted simultaneously with all other participants and the percentage of overlap computed. In addition, the individual networks were plotted next to one another to appreciate the commonalities across individuals as well as the idiosyncratic features of each individual’s estimate (available in the Supplemental Materials).
Overlap maps were also computed for the model-free seed region correlation maps. These maps make no assumption of a winner-take-all network assignment, so they provide a different view of network consistency or inconsistency across participants. For this final analysis, each individual’s seed region correlation map for each network was thresholded at z(r) > 0.2 and the overlap across participants plotted. The analysis was performed separately for both the anterior and posterior seed regions for each network.
Finally, the estimates were used to construct consensus and agreement atlases intended to provide the community resources to integrate the present observations into their own processing pipelines.
Visualization on the Flattened Cortical Surface
The human cerebral cortex is a complex structure with numerous sulci and gyri that can make it difficult to appreciate topographic patterns, including patterns that evolve over medial to lateral views and through complex structures like the insula. To appreciate global topographic relations, a flattened surface was created by editing the inflated surface file with the “TKSurfer” tool of FreeSurfer v6.0.0. The flattened surface was constructed to especially focus on the anatomy at and around the central sulcus extending into the insula. Five linear cuts were made on the midline of the inflated cortical surface, including one along the calcarine sulcus and four roughly equally spaced cuts radiating out from the medial wall. Next, a circular cut was made on the midline to allow the surface to unravel. Finally, the “mris_flatten” tool of FreeSurfer v6.0.0 was employed to create the flattened surface. This procedure was performed separately for the left and right hemispheres.
Task Paradigms
After estimation of within-individual networks, functional response properties were explored in independent task-based data collected on the same individuals. The task paradigms were chosen based on literature review and our prior studies because of their ability to differentially activate distinct networks, and to do so robustly. A second feature of the selected task paradigms is that they were amenable to repeat testing either because extensive novel stimuli could be constructed (e.g., sentences, question probes) or because, by their nature, they were resilient to habituation even after many repetitions (e.g., flickering visual stimuli). Task details are described below.
Somatomotor topography.
The motor task was extended from Buckner et al. (99) to examine the organization of the foot, glute, hand, and tongue representations. Novel targeting of the glute representation allowed an intermediate body position to be mapped between the hand and foot [as reported earlier in Saadon-Grosman et al. (76)]. The goal of this task paradigm was to activate somatotopic portions of SMOT-A and SMOT-B while minimizing nonspecific shared responses.
After extensive prescan training, participants performed six types of active movements in the scanner: 1 and 2) left and right finger taps (thumb to index and thumb to middle); 3 and 4) left and right toes plantar flexion and dorsiflexion; 5) tongue movements from right to left (touching the premolar upper teeth); and 6) contraction and relaxation of their gluteal muscles. Each movement type was performed repeatedly across 10-s movement blocks. Before each movement block, a 2-s visual cue of a drawn body part with a text label informed the participant to initiate one of the six movement types. The fixation crosshair then changed to a slow-flickering black circle to pace the movements. The onset of the black circle cued movement of thumb to index finger, toe plantarflexion, tongue to the right, and glute contraction. The offset of the black circle cued movement of thumb to middle finger, toe dorsiflexion, tongue to the left, and glute relaxation. After five cycles, the word “END” instructed movement cessation. Twenty-four movement blocks (4 per movement type) occurred within each run, with 16-s blocks of passive fixation following each set of six movement blocks. Runs began and ended with fixation, yielding five fixation blocks per run.
Each run lasted 7 min 8 s (428 frames, with the first 12 frames removed for T1 equilibration). Six motor runs were collected with full counterbalanced orders of movement conditions on each day. Runs were excluded from analysis if participants missed or failed to respond to cues, as confirmed by operators observing their alertness and movements from the control room.
Visual topography.
A visual retinotopic stimulation task was used to map visual cortex (similar to Refs. 49, 100). Our design had three levels of eccentricity stimuli (to map eccentricity gradients that span the V1, V2, V3 cluster) and separate vertical versus horizontal meridian stimuli (to map polar angle reversals that separate the borders of V1, V2, and V3; Ref. 101, see also Ref. 102). The goal of this task was to activate retinotopic portions of VIS-C and VIS-P.
The basic stimulus consisted of a circular checkerboard that expanded outward from the central fixation point to approximate cortical expansion in visual cortex. Moving from center, the radius ring of the checkerboard became larger by a log step of 0.29. The resulting checkerboard was rendered out to 36 even rings cropped to a resolution of 1,024 × 1,024 pixels. To localize the meridians, two wedges masked the checkerboard, each covering 0.5° to 16.2° of eccentricity and 11.2° of polar angle. Horizontal wedges were centered at polar angles 360° and 180° and vertical wedges at 0° and 90°. To localize polar angle, the checkerboard was masked with a circular ring, which increased in size with increasing eccentricity. The center ring covered 0.5° to 1.6°, the middle ring 1.6° to 5.1°, and the peripheral ring 5.1° to 16.2°.
Each run consisted of ten 10-s blocks of visual stimulation (2 blocks of each of the 5 conditions). The beginning, middle, and end of each run included a 20-s block of extended fixation. During stimulation, the checkerboard changed 6 times/s in the order white/black, color, black/white, color, white/black, black/white. The black center fixation dot unpredictably changed to gray (every 1–5 s). To ensure continuous fixation, participants pressed a button every time the fixation dot changed to gray. The primary contrasts of interest were horizontal versus vertical meridian blocks and, separately, the three eccentricity blocks versus each other.
Each run lasted 4 min 30 s (270 frames, with the first 6 frames removed for T1 equilibration). Five runs were collected for each participant. Runs were excluded from analysis if participants missed trials and the eye video recordings indicated drowsiness. Lights within the scanner room were off during visual topography mapping, and a black occluding board was inserted into the scanner to prevent any light reflections.
Oddball task.
The oddball task explored detection of transient responses to salient, visual oddball targets that were uncommon relative to irrelevant nontargets and distracting nontargets [similar to Wynn et al. (103)]. The goal of the task was to activate the SAL/PMN and CG-OP networks. Both networks have regions at or near the anterior insula and have been variably associated with response to task-relevant transients (see Refs. 91, 92, 104 for discussion).
Participants viewed a sequence of uppercase letters O and K in either black or red. Participants pressed a button with their right index finger when a red K appeared and withheld their responses to all the other letter-color combinations. The random trial ordering was set with Optseq (105). In each run, 10% of the trials were target red Ks, 10% were lure red Os, 40% were distractor black Ks, and 40% were distractor black Os. The contrast of interest was the target red Ks versus all other trials coded as the implicit baseline.
Each run lasted 5 min 50 s (350 frames, with the first 6 frames removed for T1 equilibration). After 6 s of fixation overlapping the initial stabilization frames, an initial 20-s block of fixation was followed by a continuous extended block of 300 1-s trials (0.15-s presentation of the letter followed by 0.85 s of fixation) and then a final 20-s block of extended fixation. Before the first trial a 2-s start cue (1-s “Begin,” 1-s fixation) was presented, as well as a similar “End” cue after the final trial. Thus, the design was a rapid, event-related paradigm sandwiched between blocks of extended fixation. Five runs were collected for each participant. Runs were excluded from the analysis if participants missed more than six targets within a task run, which accounted for 20% of the total targets.
Working memory (N-Back) task.
The working memory (N-Back) task was extended from Cohen et al. (106) and Braver et al. (107) to explore demands on cognitive control under varied levels of memory load. Specifically, the N-Back task utilized a 2-back versus 0-back comparison to target FPN-A and FPN-B. In addition, following the design of the HCP N-Back task paradigm, multiple stimulus types/matching rules were included to explore whether the load effect was domain flexible or domain specialized (68, 108).
Stimuli were presented sequentially in the center of the computer screen. Participants decided whether the current stimulus matched a consistent template target (the 0-Back or low-load condition) or whether the current stimulus matched the stimulus presented two stimuli back in the sequence (the 2-Back or high-load condition). Participants maintained fixation on a central crosshair throughout the run.
The stimuli varied across four conditions (Face, Word, Scene, and Letter) that were each presented in separate blocks. Faces and scenes were color images, with scenes showing both indoor and outdoor spaces and chosen not to feature people (faces from HCP, Ref. 108; scenes generously provided by the Konkle laboratory, Refs. 109, 110). Letters included subsets of consonants, and words featured one-syllable words from 10-word sets matched for length and frequency with the Corpus of Contemporary English (Ref. 111; December 2015 version). In all but the Word condition, participants matched the stimuli to an exact stimulus referent, or the exact stimulus presented two trials before. For the Word condition, the participants decided whether the current word rhymed with the target (e.g., “dream” would be a positive match with “steam”).
Each N-Back run featured eight blocks (a 0-Back and a 2-Back for each of the 4 stimulus categories). Each block included a cue and nine trials. During the first cue stimulus, participants also saw the block type, either 2-Back or 0-Back. During 2-Back blocks participants looked for matches (identical images or rhyming words) with the stimulus two trials back, and during 0-Back blocks participants looked for matches to the cue. The background was black (matching the HCP format). All blocks included two target and two lure (repeated nontarget) trials. Targets and lures were equally likely to appear in each viable trial position within and across runs. Participants pressed a button for every trial, indicating match (right index finger) or no match (left index finger).
Each run lasted 4 min 44 s (284 frames, with the first 12 frames removed for T1 equilibration). After 12 s of fixation overlapping the initial stabilization frames, an additional block of 12 s of fixation was followed by blocks of the N-Back task interspersed with 15-s fixation blocks (the fixation blocks came after two 25-s N-Back task blocks). Across runs, 0-Back and 2-Back blocks, categories, and their interactions were counterbalanced. Each trial was 2.5 s in duration (2 s of stimulus presentation followed by 0.5 s of fixation). The fixation crosshair was white for the extended fixation blocks and green during the N-Back task blocks. Within a run, all categories were seen before a category repeated. Eight runs were collected for each participant. Runs where participants missed responses in more than two trials were excluded from analysis.
Sentence processing task.
The Sentence Processing task was adapted from Fedorenko et al. (52, 53) to examine domain-specialized processing related to accessing word meaning and phrase-level meaning. The target task involved sentences presented one word at a time. The reference control task was presentation of nonword strings that were matched in length and visually similar. The goal of this task was to activate the LANG network [see Braga et al. (63)].
Participants passively read real sentences (“IN THE MORNING THE TAILOR WAS SHOWING DIFFERENT FABRICS TO THE CUSTOMER”) or pronounceable nonword strings (“SMOLE MUFRISONA VEDER SMOP FO BON FE PAME OMOSTREME GURY U FO”). The centered stimuli were presented one word (or nonword) at a time (0.45 s per word). After each word or nonword string, a cue appeared for 0.50 s, prompting the participants to make a right index finger button press. Stimuli, generously provided by the Fedorenko laboratory, never repeated. Word or nonword strings (6 s each) were presented in 18-s blocks of three strings. Extended fixation blocks (18 s each) appeared at the start of each run and after every fourth string block. The primary comparison of interest was the contrast between sentence and nonword blocks.
Each run lasted 5 min 0 s (300 frames, with the first 12 frames removed for T1 equilibration). Six runs were collected for each participant. Runs were excluded if the participant did not read the stimuli (observed through eye monitoring) or missed responses.
Theory-of-Mind task.
The Theory-of-Mind tasks were adopted from Saxe and colleagues (112–115) to explore domain-specialized processing associated with representation of another person’s mental states. In the False Belief paradigm, participants viewed a brief story and then, on a separate screen, a question about that story. In the False Belief condition, the target stories described events surrounding a person’s perspective, followed by a question about the thoughts and beliefs of that person. In the control False Photo condition, stories described similar situations involving objects (e.g., in photos, on maps, and in descriptions). In the Emotional/Physical Pain Stories paradigm (subsequently abbreviated “Pain”), the target stories described a situation that evoked personal emotional pain (Emo Pain condition) and were contrasted with control stories of similar length and complexity involving physical pain (Phys Pain condition). Participants rated the level of pain from “None” to “A lot” during the question period. These two paradigms yield similar task activation maps (115). Here the task contrasts of False Belief versus False Photo and Emo Pain versus Phys Pain were combined, with the goal of activating DN-B (extending from Ref. 90).
Each run consisted of a series of stories and questions (15 s per individual story/question pairing). For both paradigms, each run included five target trials (False Belief or Emo Pain) and five control trials (False Photo or Phys Pain). Fifteen-second fixation periods occurred between trials. Stimuli never repeated.
Each run lasted 5 min 18 s (318 frames, with the first 12 frames removed for T1 equilibration). Eight runs were collected for each participant: four of the False Belief paradigm and four of the Pain paradigm. We implemented an exclusion criterion to exclude any run with more than one missed trial. No runs met this criterion.
Episodic projection task.
The Episodic Projection task was adapted from Andrews-Hanna et al. (116) and DiNicola et al. (90) to encourage processes related to remembering the past and imagining the future (prospection). In the target task conditions, participants viewed a brief scenario that oriented to a situation in the past (Past Self) or future (Future Self) simultaneously with a question about the event that encouraged participants to imagine the specific scenario described. The similarly structured control condition asked the participants about a present situation (Present Self). The task contrasts of Past Self versus Present Self and Future Self versus Present Self were combined, with the goal of activating DN-A [extending from DiNicola et al. (90)]. Of relevance, detailed behavioral analyses of these contrasts have suggested that the main component process tracking increased response in DN-A is the process of mentally constructing scenes (Refs. 68, 95; see also Ref. 117). Thus, the task contrast used here taps into domain-specialized processing related to spatial/scene processing (see Ref. 118 for discussion).
Each run contained a series of scenarios with questions (10 s of scenario/question presentation followed by 10 s of fixation). Thirty questions appeared per run, with three for each condition of relevance (Past Self, Future Self, Present Self). Additional conditions were included toward goals distinct from those targeted here. For our analyses, we focus on the condition contrasts that have previously dissociated DN-A from DN-B in DiNicola et al. (90). All scenarios were unique.
Each run lasted 10 min 17 s (617 frames, with the first 12 frames removed for T1 equilibration). Ten runs were collected for each participant that included 90 relevant trials across runs (30 of each of the 3 conditions). Runs with more than two missed trials were excluded.
Within-Individual Task Activation Analysis
Functional task data were analyzed with the general linear model (GLM) as implemented by FSL’s first-level FEAT (FSL version 5.0.4; Ref. 119). All conditions were included in each model design, even those not relevant to the contrasts of interest, except for the Oddball Effect task contrast, which coded the targets against the implicit baseline. The data were high-pass filtered with a cutoff of 100 s (0.01 Hz) to remove low-frequency noise within each run. GLM outputs included, for each contrast, β values for each vertex that were converted, within FEAT, to z values. Within each participant, z-value maps from all runs were averaged together by using fslmaths (120) to create a single cross-session map for each contrast of interest. For the N-Back task, we ran both block-level and condition-level GLMs. Block-level GLM outputs included z-value maps for each trial block, which were averaged by condition across runs. Condition-level GLM outputs included z-value maps for 2-Back and 0-Back conditions, which were averaged across runs. In both cases, a single cross-session contrast map was then created by taking the difference between condition mean maps.
Task contrasts were designed to functionally target specific networks and dissociate response properties between networks. Two convergent methods were used for visualization and quantification. First, z-value maps were compared visually by overlaying the borders of networks onto the task contrast maps on the same cortical surface (fsaverage6 cortical surface). This form of visualization allowed comprehensive assessment of task response patterns. Contrast z-value maps were manually thresholded to best demonstrate the task activation patterns for each participant. The PSYCH-FIXED lookup table within Connectome Workbench was used for the color scale.
Second, a priori networks within individuals were used to formally quantify differences in response levels between networks, including direct tests for significant differences between networks and between task contrasts. For each task contrast, the average z value was calculated for all vertices within each selected network, combining across hemispheres. Mean z values were computed for each task run, and the cross-run mean z values for each network were then plotted in a bar graph, along with the standard error of the mean across participants. This analysis has the advantage of quantifying the magnitude and variance of the response in each a priori-defined network for each participant, without any subjective decisions.
For both approaches to task response analysis, the networks were defined within the individuals before examination of the task maps, to avoid the possibility of bias.
Software and Statistical Analysis
Functional connectivity between brain regions was calculated in MATLAB (version 2019a; http://www.mathworks.com; MathWorks, Natick, MA) using Pearson’s product moment correlations. FreeSurfer v6.0.0, FSL, and AFNI were used during data processing. The estimates of networks in volume space were visualized in FreeView v6.0.0. The estimates of networks on the cortical space were visualized in Connectome Workbench v1.3.2. Statistical analyses were performed with R v3.6.2. Model-free seed region confirmations were performed in Connectome Workbench v1.3.2. Network parcellation was performed using code from Kong et al. (71) on GitHub (https://github.com/ThomasYeoLab/CBIG/tree/master/stable_projects/brain_parcellation/Kong2019_MSHBM).
RESULTS
Networks Can Be Estimated Robustly within Individuals
Networks were estimated for the refinement stage data with a 15-network MS-HBM model. Figure 1 and Figs. A1–A5 in appendix display the main results for S1 and S2 on the surface, and the Supplemental Materials display the comprehensive results and quality control visualizations on the surface and in the native-space volume.
Figure 1.
Temporal signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) map for participant S1. The mean estimate of temporal SNR for the functional MRI (fMRI) data is illustrated for multiple views of the left hemisphere on the inflated cortical surface (from 62 runs collected over 31 days). Note the low SNR within the orbitofrontal cortex and the temporal pole. This pattern is typical of the data across all participants in the present work and should be considered when evaluating network organization. A, anterior; D, dorsal; P, posterior; V, ventral. SNR maps for all participants are provided in the Supplemental Materials.
The first results pertain to data quality. The SNR maps are displayed on the cortical surface (Fig. 1 and Fig. A3). Most of the cortical mantle possessed high SNR. As expected, given signal dropout near the sinuses and the inner ear (98), there was variability in SNR across the cortical surface with orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and adjacent ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC), rostral inferior temporal cortex, and the temporal pole showing low SNR (see Supplemental Materials for additional visualizations). Network assignments in low-SNR regions should be interpreted cautiously.
The primary result of our procedures was an estimated parcellation into distinct candidate networks. Figs. A1 and A4 display the 15-network estimates for S1 and S2. All networks, including local sensory and motor networks as well as distributed association networks, were identified in both participants. Although the general organization was shared between the two participants, the spatial boundaries were idiosyncratic. These patterns are elaborated upon in detail in the results of the novel 15 participants. For these first two individuals, we focused on validating the methods.
Model-Free Seed Region-Based Correlation Confirms the 15-Network Parcellation
The network estimates were based on a 15-network MS-HBM. In addition to assuming a specific number of networks, the method also employed group priors to constrain the estimates (see Supplemental Fig. S1). As such, it is possible that the resultant networks do not accurately reflect the underlying within-individual correlation patterns as one might expect. To explore this possibility and intuitively visualize the degree to which the model captures underlying correlation patterns, a model-free seed region-based correlation analysis was performed. A seed region was placed in an anterior position and separately a posterior position within each network within each individual. The resulting correlation maps are displayed in Figs. A2 and A5 in relation to the MS-HBM network boundaries.
The estimated networks captured features of the correlation patterns remarkably well, including across small, distributed regions that might otherwise be overlooked. The alignments were not perfect. Specifically, the correlation patterns included most of the distributed regions in the MS-HBM solutions, and the patterns were largely selective to the estimated networks. Small deviations, in the form of extensions of the patterns beyond the network boundaries, were common, likely in part because the network estimates forced a winner-take-all assignment but also possibly because additional network details may be missed.5 The consistency between the general correlational structure and the network estimates in one sense is unsurprising because the underlying correlation matrix was employed by the network model. However, it is not obligated, and deviations could be seen if the model forced assignments or the model failed to capture the structure of the data.
The 15-Network Parcellation Captures Features That Are Not Captured by a 10-Network Parcellation
We next sought to explore what is gained by adopting the 15-network parcellation rather than the simpler 10-network parcellation. Figures A6 and A9 display the MS-HBM parcellation estimate for the 10-network and 15-network solutions for each participant. The first notable result is that, for most networks, there was little difference between the two models’ estimates. For example, the separation of DN-A and DN-B was well captured by both model solutions, with the distributed spatial patterns and idiosyncratic features quite similar between models. That is, if the goal were to study DN-A and DN-B, there is little gained by utilizing the more complex 15-network model. In both S1 and S2, many of the other major networks were also similar between the two parcellations, including FPN-A, FPN-B, SMOT-A, and SMOT-B. Thus, for networks well captured by the 10-network model, they appear to be roughly unchanged in the 15-network model. For other networks though, there were substantive differences.
One motivation for investigating a 15-network model was that certain networks did not differentiate established distinctions at or around somatomotor cortex and visual cortex, as well as between multiple networks within or adjacent to the insula including separation of a cingulo-opercular network from a salience network (Ref. 91; see also Ref. 92 for discussion). These features were captured in the 15-network MS-HBM. Specifically, the single visual network in the 10-network estimate was differentiated among dATN-B, VIS-C, and VIS-P in the 15-network solution (Fig. A7). The Salience (SAL) network in the 10-network estimate was differentiated into two separate networks, here labeled SAL/PMN and CG-OP (Fig. A11). The dATN in the 10-network estimate was differentiated into dATN-A and PM-PPr in the 15-network solution (Fig. A8), and a distinct AUD network emerged near LANG and SMOT-B (Fig. A10). Critically, seed region-based correlation patterns suggested that this expansion of networks from 10 to 15 captured clear features of the underlying correlation patterns (Figs. A7, A8, A10, and A11).
One unexpected result was that our 15-network parcellation included a single network that has been variably described in the literature. What has been called the “parietal memory network” (94), with focus on the posterior midline, has often been discussed separately from the network referred to by Seeley and colleagues as the “salience network” (92). Here a single distributed network was identified that possessed the canonical features of both networks. The seed region-based correlation maps supported that the two networks discussed historically as distinct are likely a single network (Figs. A2, A5, and A11), a result that is further examined in the prospectively acquired and analyzed data.
Network Estimates Are Reliable within Individuals
We next sought to address two related questions. First, are the network estimates described above reliable within individuals? Second, can they be obtained with a lesser amount of data? The resting-state fixation runs of S1 and S2 were divided into three datasets with roughly equal amounts of runs contributing to each data subset (20/20/22 runs of data for S1 and 20/20/21 runs of data for S2). The 15-network MS-HBM was estimated independently for each data subset. Results are displayed in Fig. 2.
Figure 2.
Cerebral cortical network estimates are reliable across independent datasets within individuals. Network estimates from the multisession hierarchical Bayesian model (MS-HBM) are displayed across 2 views for multiple independent datasets. Each color represents a distinct network estimated by the model. For each panel, the left lateral surface is at top and the medial surface at bottom. Some networks possess primarily local organization (e.g., somatomotor, visual), whereas other networks possess widely distributed organization (e.g., those involving prefrontal, temporal, and parietal association zones). Independently analyzed subsets of data from participants S1 (top) and S2 (bottom) illustrate the reliability of the network estimates. The resting-state fixation data of S1 and S2 were split into 3 datasets to estimate networks with the MS-HBM applied independently to each dataset. The individual-specific cortical parcellations are replicable within participants, critically for models based on ∼20 runs of resting-state fixation data as employed for the 15 new participants analyzed throughout the remainder of this article. The network labels are shown at bottom and used similarly throughout the figures. SMOT-A, Somatomotor-A; SMOT-B, Somatomotor-B; PM-PPr, Premotor-Posterior Parietal Rostral; CG-OP, Cingulo-Opercular; SAL/PMN, Salience/Parietal Memory Network; dATN-A, Dorsal Attention-A; dATN-B, Dorsal Attention-B; FPN-A, Frontoparietal Network-A; FPN-B, Frontoparietal Network-B; DN-A, Default Network-A; DN-B, Default Network-B; LANG, Language; VIS-C, Visual Central; VIS-P, Visual Peripheral; AUD, Auditory.
In S1, 82.7% on average of cortical vertices were assigned to the same networks across the independent datasets from within the individual. In S2, 86.9% of cortical vertices were assigned to the same networks. By contrast, overlap between the separate parcellations of S1 and S2 were 54.4%, 55.1%, and 55.4%, indicating that between-individual variability was substantially larger than within-individual variability (59, 71).
The reliability of network estimates derived from varying the amount of data, ranging from 2 to 20 runs of resting-state fixation data, is visually illustrated in Fig. 3 and Fig. 4. In each dataset, we independently applied the 15-network MS-HBM model. As shown in the figures, a few runs of data can capture the overall correlational patterns but fall short of revealing the idiosyncratic details of networks within the individual. As the quantity of data integrated into the model increased, both S1 and S2 demonstrated network estimates that displayed a noticeable trend toward greater consistency between independent datasets. This improvement is quantified in the Supplemental Materials. In general, robust networks emerged when we leveraged ∼20 runs of data for both S1 and S2. These findings suggest that cortical parcellations of the resolution and within-individual detail targeted here are replicable for models based on ∼20 runs of data. Notably, this is the amount of data collected for the 15 new participants in the implementation stage dataset analyzed throughout the rest of this article.
Figure 3.
Network estimates obtained using various amounts of resting-state fixation data from participant S1. Networks estimated using varying amounts of resting-state fixation data from S1 illustrate increasing reliability of the network estimates as the number of runs increases. The 15-network multisession hierarchical Bayesian model (MS-HBM) was applied independently to each dataset. Each color represents a distinct network, with the network labels shown at bottom in Fig. 2. As can be visualized, the network estimates are reliable in their broad details with as few as 2 runs of data, but the spatial details change and stabilize with increasing amounts of contributing data. See the Supplemental Materials for quantification.
Figure 4.
Network estimates obtained using various amounts of resting-state fixation data from participant S2. Networks estimated using varying amounts of resting-state fixation data from S2 illustrate increasing reliability of the network estimates as the number of runs increases. The 15-network multisession hierarchical Bayesian model (MS-HBM) was applied independently to each dataset. Each color represents a distinct network, with the network labels shown at bottom in Fig. 2. As can be visualized, the network estimates are reliable in their broad details with as few as 2 runs of data, but the spatial details change and stabilize with increasing amounts of contributing data. See the Supplemental Materials for quantification.
Network Estimates in 15 New Participants Reveal Organizational Features
Discovery, replication, and triplication in the implementation stage data.
Fifteen cerebral networks were estimated for all new participants. The 15 individuals were analyzed within subsamples (each n = 5) intended to replicate the MS-HBM’s network estimates in prospective participants, including novel discovery (P1–P5), replication (P6–P10), and triplication (P11–P15) datasets. Results were similar across all three subsamples, and the full parcellation for each individual is available in the Supplemental Materials on the surface and within the individual’s own native-space volume. Despite idiosyncratic spatial details of network organization, the broad properties were largely consistent. Three representative participants, one from each subsample, are displayed in Fig. 5, Fig. 6, and Fig. 7.
Figure 5.
Network estimates for participant P1. Network estimates from the multisession hierarchical Bayesian model (MS-HBM) are comprehensively displayed across 4 views for a representative participant (P1) from the novel discovery dataset. The left hemisphere is at top and right hemisphere at bottom. Each color represents a distinct network, with the network labels shown at bottom. Some networks possess primarily local organization (e.g., somatomotor, visual), whereas other networks possess widely distributed organization (e.g., those involving prefrontal, temporal, and parietal association zones). Similar maps for all available participants are provided in the Supplemental Materials. The network labels are used similarly throughout the figures. SMOT-A, Somatomotor-A; SMOT-B, Somatomotor-B; PM-PPr, Premotor-Posterior Parietal Rostral; CG-OP, Cingulo-Opercular; SAL/PMN, Salience/Parietal Memory Network; dATN-A, Dorsal Attention-A; dATN-B, Dorsal Attention-B; FPN-A, Frontoparietal Network-A; FPN-B, Frontoparietal Network-B; DN-A, Default Network-A; DN-B, Default Network-B; LANG, Language; VIS-C, Visual Central; VIS-P, Visual Peripheral; AUD, Auditory.
Figure 6.
Network estimates for participant P6. Networks estimated for another representative participant (P6) from the novel replication dataset are comprehensively displayed. The network estimates are from the 15-network multisession hierarchical Bayesian model (MS-HBM). Four views for each hemisphere show details of cortical network organization, with lateral and medial views as well as rotated frontal and posterior views. The left hemisphere is at top and right hemisphere at bottom. Each color represents a distinct network, with the network labels shown at bottom. SMOT-A, Somatomotor-A; SMOT-B, Somatomotor-B; PM-PPr, Premotor-Posterior Parietal Rostral; CG-OP, Cingulo-Opercular; SAL/PMN, Salience/Parietal Memory Network; dATN-A, Dorsal Attention-A; dATN-B, Dorsal Attention-B; FPN-A, Frontoparietal Network-A; FPN-B, Frontoparietal Network-B; DN-A, Default Network-A; DN-B, Default Network-B; LANG, Language; VIS-C, Visual Central; VIS-P, Visual Peripheral; AUD, Auditory.
Figure 7.
Network estimates for participant P11. Networks estimated for a final representative participant from the novel triplication (P11) dataset are comprehensively displayed. The network estimates are from the 15-network multisession hierarchical Bayesian model (MS-HBM). Four views for each hemisphere show details of cortical network organization, with lateral and medial views as well as rotated frontal and posterior views. The left hemisphere is at top and right hemisphere at bottom. Each color represents a distinct network, with the network labels shown at bottom. SMOT-A, Somatomotor-A; SMOT-B, Somatomotor-B; PM-PPr, Premotor-Posterior Parietal Rostral; CG-OP, Cingulo-Opercular; SAL/PMN, Salience/Parietal Memory Network; dATN-A, Dorsal Attention-A; dATN-B, Dorsal Attention-B; FPN-A, Frontoparietal Network-A; FPN-B, Frontoparietal Network-B; DN-A, Default Network-A; DN-B, Default Network-B; LANG, Language; VIS-C, Visual Central; VIS-P, Visual Peripheral; AUD, Auditory.
Network estimates reveal predominantly local sensory and motor networks.
VIS-C and VIS-P were identified in each participant, extending from the calcarine sulcus on the midline to the lateral surface. The extent of the two networks across the occipital lobe did not align them with individual visual areas but rather the expanded regions of the V1/V2/V3 retinotopic cluster, and likely adjacent retinotopic clusters (121, 122). The multiple networks appeared to divide along the eccentricity gradient (123). The VIS-C network overlapped regions likely aligned to the central portions of the V1/V2/V3 retinotopic representations, whereas VIS-P overlapped the peripheral retinotopic representations (see Ref. 23). The relation of VIS-C and VIS-P to task-elicited responses is directly explored in Somatomotor and Visual Networks Respond to Body Movements and Visual Stimulation in a Topographic Manner.
Although the VIS-C and VIS-P networks contained vertices that were mostly contiguous, there were exceptions. Discontinuous islands were sometimes found in occipital-temporal cortex, possibly a reflection of separate extrastriate retinotopic clusters (e.g., at or near the MT/V5 hemifield representation). VIS-P also occasionally contained small, punctate representations near dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC). These were the exceptions: the majority of the VIS-C and VIS-P network-included vertices were continuous and adjacent to one another, overlapping the expected location of early retinotopic visual cortex.
Similarly, SMOT-A and SMOT-B were identified reliably as spatially continuous networks along the central sulcus, extending onto the midline and into the posterior insula. These two somatomotor networks also do not likely align to individual architectonic areas but rather extend across the pre- and postcentral gyrus, including primary motor as well as somatosensory areas. The extent along the midline and into the posterior insula further suggests that the networks span multiple body maps, not simply the dominant inverted body map along the central sulcus.
A final predominantly local sensory network, AUD, was consistently identified near the superior temporal sulcus. This network extended across the full supratemporal plane including Heschl’s gyrus, and into adjacent regions.
Multiple distributed networks lie adjacent to the local sensory and motor networks.
Multiple distributed networks were identified in each participant that were immediately adjacent to the local sensory and motor networks, with each network containing distributed regions that spanned multiple zones of cortex. dATN-A and dATN-B were adjacent to VIS-C and VIS-P but also with distant regions in the frontal cortex, likely at or near the frontal eye field (FEF; Refs. 32, 124). Similarly, CG-OP and PM-PPr radiated outward from the early somatomotor networks SMOT-A and SMOT-B. CG-OP and PM-PPr sometimes contained small islands indenting or even within the SMOT network boundaries that may relate to interspersed intereffector regions along the central sulcus (67). CG-OP and PM-PPr also included regions abutting and within the Sylvian fissure. The relations among the networks became even clearer in the flat map visualizations.
A final network, SAL/PMN, displayed a spatial pattern that was adjacent to CG-OP in many locations but also with differences. Although SAL/PMN contained a prominent region in the anterior insula, the network’s positioning did not juxtapose the somatomotor networks. Rather, SAL/PMN was adjacent to a posterior midline cluster of association networks near regions of the canonical “default network” (e.g., Refs. 23, 24, 29, 125). SAL/PMN consistently included a region within ACC anterior to CG-OP and a prominent set of regions along the midcingulate and the posterior midline. As noted for S1 and S2, the SAL/PMN network’s spatial pattern combined features described in prior work on the salience network [Ref. 91; see Seeley (92) for discussion and Dosenbach et al. (104) for related work] and the parietal memory network (94).
Much of association cortex is populated by multiple parallel juxtaposed networks.
The remaining regions of association cortex, which contain the majority of PFC, a large region of PPC extending into the temporoparietal junction (TPJ), and lateral temporal cortex (LTC), were populated by five distinct networks. With some exceptions, each of these five networks tended to possess regions in each of the distributed zones. The five networks were interwoven with local patterns of adjacencies that repeated across the cortex.
Specifically, FPN-A and FPN-B were adjacent to one another throughout the cortical mantle. FPN-A and FPN-B displayed a distributed pattern consistent with the well-studied group-estimated frontoparietal control network, also referred to as the multiple-demand system (23, 24, 126). These two juxtaposed networks (FPN-A and FPN-B) consistently neighbored an additional clustered set of three networks: LANG, DN-B, and DN-A. These three additional networks were tightly juxtaposed among themselves on the lateral cortical surface including zones within PPC, LTC, and both DLPFC and VLPFC. DN-A and DN-B were interdigitated as well along the anterior and posterior midline, consistent with previous studies (60–62).
Despite being adjacent in many locations, clear features distinguished the three networks. The LANG network surrounded the Sylvian fissure and included regions near the AUD network and in VLPFC at or near historically defined “Broca’s area.” The LANG network was generally larger in the left compared with the right hemisphere [see Braga et al. (63)]. DN-A showed a strong correlation with the posterior parahippocampal cortex (PHC) [see also Reznik et al. (127) for further details]. Additionally, DN-A occupied regions at or adjacent to the retrosplenial cortex (RSC) and ventral posterior cingulate cortex (PCC). DN-B prominently included anterior regions of the inferior parietal lobule extending into the TPJ [a region of particular focus, e.g., Saxe and Kanwisher (112), Jacoby et al. (115)]. The posterior midline region of DN-B fell between regions of DN-A and specifically did not extend into RSC. DN-B also included a larger region of the LTC than DN-A; DN-A tended to include a small region or a few discontinuous regions in anterior LTC.
Of importance, the spatial arrangements of the five networks (FPN-A, FPN-B, LANG, DN-B, and DN-A) repeated multiple times across the cortical mantle, a discovery that is expanded upon in the analyses of spatial juxtapositions on the flattened cortical surface.
Network Estimates Projected to the Native-Space Volumes Enable Clinical Applications
To enable clinical applications, particularly for presurgical planning and neuromodulation, an empirical method was developed to project the networks estimated on the surface into the native-space volume of individual participants. Figure 8, Figure 9, and Figure 10 display examples for three representative participants from the discovery (P1), replication (P6), and triplication (P11) datasets. The network estimates in the native-space volume were overlaid onto each individual’s T1w structural image and displayed in sagittal, coronal, and axial views. Similar maps for all available participants can be found in the Supplemental Materials.
Figure 8.
Network estimates in native-space volume of participant P1. Network estimates were projected from the surface back into the native-space volume of individual participants, as is needed for presurgical planning and neuromodulation. A representative participant (P1) from the novel discovery dataset is displayed. The network estimates are from the 15-network multisession hierarchical Bayesian model (MS-HBM). The 3 sections display sagittal (left), coronal (center), and axial (right) views. Each color represents a distinct network, with the network labels shown at bottom in Fig. 2. Similar maps for all available participants are provided in the Supplemental Materials. Ant, anterior; Dor, dorsal; LH, left hemisphere; Pos, posterior; RH, right hemisphere; Ven, ventral.
Figure 9.
Network estimates in native-space volume of participant P6. Network estimates were projected from the surface back into the native-space volume of individual participants. A representative participant (P6) from the novel replication dataset is displayed. The network estimates are from the 15-network multisession hierarchical Bayesian model (MS-HBM). The 3 sections display sagittal (left), coronal (center), and axial (right) views. Each color represents a distinct network, with the network labels shown at bottom in Fig. 2. Ant, anterior; Dor, dorsal; LH, left hemisphere; Pos, posterior; RH, right hemisphere; Ven, ventral.
Figure 10.
Network estimates in native-space volume of participant P11. Network estimates were projected from the surface back into the native-space volume of individual participants. A representative participant (P11) from the novel triplication dataset is displayed. The network estimates are from the 15-network multisession hierarchical Bayesian model (MS-HBM). The 3 sections display sagittal (left), coronal (center), and axial (right) views. Each color represents a distinct network, with the network labels shown at bottom in Fig. 2. Ant, anterior; Dor, dorsal; LH, left hemisphere; Pos, posterior; RH, right hemisphere; Ven, ventral.
Several details become apparent that were lost in the surface visualizations, including that, within the native-space volume, regions that appear distant on the surface are often quite close in the volume. Furthermore, network assignments in one cortical gyrus were often similar to those in an adjacent gyrus, even those in separate lobes (and thus presumably quite distant from one another on the surface). For example, temporal pole regions, visualized particularly well in the sagittal plane, were often assigned to networks also present in the ventral prefrontal regions abutting the Sylvian fissure (e.g., regions linked to the DN-B and LANG networks). It remains an open question whether these between-gyri juxtapositions are a residual artifact of spatial blurring or an interesting feature of anatomy that minimizes absolute distances between functionally similar cortical regions.
A Cautionary Note about Potential Artifacts
Certain aspects of the network estimates were impacted by signal loss. Low-SNR regions were observed in the OFC, ventral regions of VLPFC, and anterior regions of the temporal lobe (see Fig. 1, Fig. A3, and Supplemental Materials). When interpreting the network assignments, it is important to keep these spatially variable effects in mind. For example, a localized AUD network was observed across the supratemporal plane including Heschl’s gyrus. Inconsistent, discontinuous vertices were also labeled as part of the AUD network in the inferior temporal cortex and OFC, in the regions of the greatest signal dropout due to magnetic susceptibility differences. The network assignments in low-SNR regions should not be trusted.
Model-Free Seed Region-Based Correlations Again Confirm Network Estimates
To demonstrate that the correlation properties of the within-individual data were captured in the network assignments, seed region-based correlation maps were visualized. Although there were minor differences between the MS-HBM network estimates and the seed region-based correlation maps,5 networks could be identified in all participants with both methods. Furthermore, the maps defined by anterior and posterior seed regions were similar, indicating that the seed region-based method was not dependent on a single vertex or one general region of cortex. Seed region-based confirmation for DN-A, DN-B, LANG, FPN-A, FPN-B, CG-OP, and SAL/PMN is displayed for three representative participants in Fig. 11, Fig. 12, and Fig. 13 and for all participants and additional networks in the Supplemental Materials.
Figure 11.
Model-free confirmation of networks using seed region-based correlation for participant P1. The correlation patterns from individual seed regions placed within networks are displayed for a representative participant (P1) from the novel discovery dataset. Left: correlation maps using an anterior seed region for each network. Right: correlation maps using a posterior seed region. Lateral and medial views are displayed for each seed region. Black outlines indicate the boundaries of corresponding individual-specific parcellation-defined networks estimated from the multisession hierarchical Bayesian model (MS-HBM) as shown in Fig. 5. The correlation maps are plotted as z(r), with the color scale at bottom. Strong agreement is evident between the seed region-based correlation maps and the estimated network boundaries. Similar maps for all available participants are provided in the Supplemental Materials. CG-OP, Cingulo-Opercular; DN-A, Default Network-A; DN-B, Default Network-B; FPN-A, Frontoparietal Network-A; FPN-B, Frontoparietal Network-B; LANG, Language; SAL/PMN, Salience/Parietal Memory Network.
Figure 12.
Model-free confirmation of networks using seed region-based correlation for participant P6. The correlation patterns from individual seed regions placed within networks are displayed for a representative participant (P6) from the novel replication dataset. Left: correlation maps using an anterior seed region for each network. Right: correlation maps using a posterior seed region. Lateral and medial views are displayed for each seed region. Black outlines indicate the boundaries of corresponding individual-specific parcellation-defined networks estimated from the multisession hierarchical Bayesian model (MS-HBM) as shown in Fig. 6. The correlation maps are plotted as z(r), with the color scale at bottom. Strong agreement is evident between the seed region-based correlation maps and the estimated network boundaries. CG-OP, Cingulo-Opercular; DN-A, Default Network-A; DN-B, Default Network-B; FPN-A, Frontoparietal Network-A; FPN-B, Frontoparietal Network-B; LANG, Language; SAL/PMN, Salience/Parietal Memory Network.
Figure 13.
Model-free confirmation of networks using seed region-based correlation for participant P11. The correlation patterns from individual seed regions placed within networks are displayed for a representative participant (P11) from the novel triplication dataset. Left: correlation maps using an anterior seed region for each network. Right: correlation maps using a posterior seed region. Lateral and medial views are displayed for each seed region. Black outlines indicate the boundaries of corresponding individual-specific parcellation-defined networks estimated from the multisession hierarchical Bayesian model (MS-HBM) as shown in Fig. 7. The correlation maps are plotted as z(r), with the color scale at the bottom. Strong agreement is evident between the seed region-based correlation maps and the estimated network boundaries. CG-OP, Cingulo-Opercular; DN-A, Default Network-A; DN-B, Default Network-B; FPN-A, Frontoparietal Network-A; FPN-B, Frontoparietal Network-B; LANG, Language; SAL/PMN, Salience/Parietal Memory Network.
Variability in Network Estimates across Individuals
An overlap map of assignments for each network from the MS-HBM for the 15 participants is displayed in Fig. 14. Results revealed that the general organization of the networks was highly conserved across individuals, but with differences in the idiosyncratic spatial positioning and extents of the networks.
Figure 14.
Overlap of network estimates derived from the multisession hierarchical Bayesian model (MS-HBM). Each row displays the overlap map from 1 target network for the full set of 15 novel participants using the estimates from the 15-network MS-HBM. The network targets are labeled on left. Default Network-A (DN-A), Default Network-B (DN-B), Language (LANG), Frontoparietal Network-A (FPN-A), Frontoparietal Network-B (FPN-B), Cingulo-Opercular (CG-OP), and Salience/Parietal Memory Network (SAL/PMN) networks are examined separately. The purpose of these maps is to illustrate the overlap of network organization across participants as well as illustrate how the separate networks are distinct from one another.
A challenge in examining spatial overlap is that there is circularity in network definition since the process initiates with the same 15-network group prior, which could bias the networks to show more overlap than is truly in the data. To mitigate this concern, we also examined overlap using the network estimates derived from the seed region correlation maps. These maps are not constrained by the group prior and do not enforce a winner-take-all assumption, allowing deviations to emerge. Overlap maps of correlation patterns were obtained using anterior and posterior seed regions within each network for all 15 participants (Fig. A12).
As another exploration of variability, the individual networks were plotted next to one another for all 15 participants, allowing another means to identify shared and idiosyncratic features of each participant’s estimate. The results are available in the Supplemental Materials.
Consensus and Thresholded Agreement Atlases
To provide atlases as open resources to the community for future use, the overlap of network estimates across the 15 participants was used to create 1) a network consensus atlas and 2) a series of thresholded network agreement atlases. These atlases can be used prospectively by researchers to construct regions that have high likelihood of being in one network or another without prior individual-level data.
In the network “consensus” atlas, each vertex was assigned to the network that was most probable. For example, if a vertex was labeled DN-A in seven participants, DN-B in five participants, and LANG in three participants, it would be assigned to DN-A in the consensus atlas. Every vertex was assigned the label of its most likely network, including uncertain vertices. Supplemental Fig. S18 displays the consensus atlas.
In the network “agreement” atlases, each vertex was assigned to its most common network but only if a certain number of participants showed agreement. For example, in the n ≥ 8 (53%) agreement atlas, displayed in Fig. 15, only vertices where eight or more participants agreed on the same network assignment were labeled. Higher-order networks exhibit a lower level of agreement among participants in the prefrontal, parietal, and temporal association cortices. Supplemental Fig. S12 displays agreement atlases for various count thresholds ranging from n ≥ 6 (40%) to n ≥ 12 (80%). These agreement atlases highlight cortical locations where many or most individuals would likely be assigned to the same network (128).
Figure 15.
Agreement map of network assignments across 15 participants. The agreement map represents brain regions where network agreement is observed in a majority of the participants, specifically illustrated here for agreement n ≥ 8 of the 15 participants. For a more comprehensive overview, agreement maps for various participant counts ranging from n ≥ 6 to n ≥ 12 can be found in Supplemental Fig. S12. Note that higher-order networks in the prefrontal, parietal and, temporal cortex exhibit less consensus among participants. The network labels are shown at bottom. SMOT-A, Somatomotor-A; SMOT-B, Somatomotor-B; PM-PPr, Premotor-Posterior Parietal Rostral; CG-OP, Cingulo-Opercular; SAL/PMN, Salience/Parietal Memory Network; dATN-A, Dorsal Attention-A; dATN-B, Dorsal Attention-B; FPN-A, Frontoparietal Network-A; FPN-B, Frontoparietal Network-B; DN-A, Default Network-A; DN-B, Default Network-B; LANG, Language; VIS-C, Visual Central; VIS-P, Visual Peripheral; AUD, Auditory.
Higher-Order Networks Nest Outward from Primary Cortices
To better reveal spatial relations among networks, a flattened cortical surface was constructed (Fig. 16). The 15 networks are displayed in representative participants in Fig. 17, Fig. 18, and Fig. 19 and for all participants in the Supplemental Materials. A broad observation was that higher-order networks nest outward from sensory and motor cortices.
Figure 16.
Visualization on the flattened cortical surface. A fully flattened cortical surface was constructed to better reveal topographic relations among networks. By application of 5 cuts along the colorful lines on the midline, the inflated cortical surface (A) was flattened (B). The 5 cuts included 1 cut along the calcarine sulcus (blue dotted line) and 4 additional cuts radiating outward from the medial wall. The surface enclosed by the circular cut was removed. Reference lines illustrate the inner and outer boundaries of the insula (Ins) as well as along the central sulcus (CS). Additional landmarks are dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), posterior parietal cortex (PPC), rostral lateral temporal cortex (rLTC), posteromedial cortex (PMC), parahippocampal cortex (PHC), and medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC). The procedure was applied separately to the 2 hemispheres.
Figure 17.
Higher-order networks nest outward from sensory and motor cortices in participant P2. Networks displayed on the flattened cortical surface reveal orderly spatial relations in a representative participant (P2) from the novel discovery dataset. The map at top displays all networks estimated using the multisession hierarchical Bayesian model (MS-HBM). The maps in A–D show subsets of networks to highlight spatial relations. A: Somatomotor-A (SMOT-A) and Somatomotor-B (SMOT-B) networks, in dark gray, are surrounded by spatially adjacent second-order Cingulo-Opercular (CG-OP) and Premotor-Posterior Parietal Rostral (PM-PPr) networks. The second-order networks are more distributed than the first-order SMOT-A and SMOT-B networks, which are primarily locally organized. B: Visual Central (VIS-C) and Visual Peripheral (VIS-P) networks, in dark gray, are surrounded by spatially adjacent second-order Dorsal Attention-A (dATN-A) and Dorsal Attention-B (dATN-B) networks, which possess distributed organization. C: the Salience/Parietal Memory Network (SAL/PMN) network has a widely distributed organization, which includes adjacency to Default Network-A (DN-A), shown in gray, especially along the posterior midline. D: the distributed association zones that fall outside of the first- and second-order networks are illustrated. These zones are populated by 5 distinct networks [DN-A, Default Network-B (DN-B), Language (LANG), Frontoparietal Network-A (FPN-A), and Frontoparietal Network-B (FPN-B)] that possess repeating spatial adjacencies across the cortex, most clearly visible in posterior parietal association cortex and temporal association cortex. FPN-A and FPN-B are adjacent to one another and together adjacent to the 3 other juxtaposed networks, LANG, DN-B, and DN-A. We call these repeating clusters of networks supra-areal association megaclusters (SAAMs) and explore them further in analyses below. The network labels in D are positioned around the SAAM in posterior parietal cortex. AUD, Auditory.
Figure 18.
Higher-order networks nest outward from sensory and motor cortices in participant P6. Networks displayed on the flattened cortical surface reveal orderly spatial relations in another representative participant (P6) from the novel replication dataset. The map at top displays all networks estimated using the multisession hierarchical Bayesian model (MS-HBM). The maps in A–D show subsets of networks to highlight spatial relations. A: Somatomotor-A (SMOT-A) and Somatomotor-B (SMOT-B) networks, in dark gray, are surrounded by spatially adjacent second-order Cingulo-Opercular (CG-OP) and Premotor-Posterior Parietal Rostral (PM-PPr) networks. The second-order networks are more distributed than the first-order SMOT-A and SMOT-B networks, which are primarily locally organized. B: Visual Central (VIS-C) and Visual Peripheral (VIS-P) networks, in dark gray, are surrounded by spatially adjacent Dorsal Attention-A (dATN-A) and Dorsal Attention-B (dATN-B) networks, which possess distributed organization. C: the Salience/Parietal Memory Network (SAL/PMN) network has a widely distributed organization, which includes adjacency to Default Network-A (DN-A), shown in gray, especially along the posterior midline. D: the distributed association zones that fall outside of the first- and second-order networks are illustrated. These zones are populated by 5 distinct networks [DN-A, Default Network-B (DN-B), Language (LANG), Frontoparietal Network-A (FPN-A), and Frontoparietal Network-B (FPN-B)] that possess repeating spatial adjacencies across the cortex, most clearly visible in posterior parietal association cortex and temporal association cortex. FPN-A and FPN-B are adjacent to one another and together adjacent to the 3 other juxtaposed networks, LANG, DN-B, and DN-A. We call these repeating clusters of networks supra-areal association megaclusters (SAAMs) and explore them further in analyses below. The network labels in D are positioned around the SAAM in posterior parietal cortex. AUD, Auditory.
Figure 19.
Higher-order networks nest outward from sensory and motor cortices in participant P12. Networks displayed on the flattened cortical surface reveal orderly spatial relations in a final representative participant (P12) from the novel triplication dataset. The map at top displays all networks estimated using the multisession hierarchical Bayesian model (MS-HBM). The maps in A–D show subsets of networks to highlight spatial relations. A: Somatomotor-A (SMOT-A) and Somatomotor-B (SMOT-B) networks, in dark gray, are surrounded by spatially adjacent second-order Cingulo-Opercular (CG-OP) and Premotor-Posterior Parietal Rostral (PM-PPr) networks. The second-order networks are more distributed than the first-order SMOT-A and SMOT-B networks, which are primarily locally organized. B: Visual Central (VIS-C) and Visual Peripheral (VIS-P) networks, in dark gray, are surrounded by spatially adjacent second-order Dorsal Attention-A (dATN-A) and Dorsal Attention-B (dATN-B) networks, which possess distributed organization. C: the Salience/Parietal Memory Network (SAL/PMN) network has a widely distributed organization, which includes adjacency to Default Network-A (DN-A), shown in gray, especially along the posterior midline. D: the distributed association zones that fall outside of the first- and second-order networks are illustrated. These zones are populated by 5 distinct networks [DN-A, Default Network-B (DN-B), Language (LANG), Frontoparietal Network-A (FPN-A), and Frontoparietal Network-B (FPN-B)] that possess repeating spatial adjacencies across the cortex, most clearly visible in posterior parietal association cortex and temporal association cortex. FPN-A and FPN-B are adjacent to one another and together adjacent to the 3 other juxtaposed networks, LANG, DN-B, and DN-A. We call these repeating clusters of networks supra-areal association megaclusters (SAAMs) and explore them further in analyses below. The network labels in D are positioned around the SAAM in posterior parietal cortex. AUD, Auditory.
Specifically, the networks could be heuristically grouped into three levels beginning with first-order sensory and motor networks.6 The first-order networks were primarily locally organized, spatially arranged along the central sulcus (for SMOT-A and SMOT-B) and near the calcarine sulcus (for VIS-C and VIS-P). Surrounding these first-order networks were adjacent networks that radiated outward. We refer to these as second-order networks. CG-OP and PM-PPr surrounded SMOT-A and SMOT-B, and dATN-A and dATN-B were adjacent to VIS-C and VIS-P. In between these second-order networks were third-order networks (FPN-A, FPN-B, LANG, DN-B, and DN-A) that populated the large, expanded zones of higher-order association cortex. The flattened representation allowed further features to be appreciated.
CG-OP and PM-PPr nearly fully surrounded both the anterior and posterior extents of the somatomotor networks, including the insular regions that are buried within the Sylvian fissure. Although CG-OP and PM-PPr were generally interdigitated around the first-order somatomotor networks, in several locations CG-OP fell distal to PM-PPr (meaning that PM-PPr directly juxtaposed SMOT-A and SMOT-B and CG-OP juxtaposed PM-PPr). Furthermore, whereas the PM-PPr network was adjacent to the somatomotor networks across its extent, CG-OP also involved distant regions in PFC and posterior association zones that were not adjacent to somatomotor networks. Thus, CG-OP possessed a partially distributed motif. Additional details of dATN-A and dATN-B were also evident. Of the two networks, dATN-B fell more proximal to the early visual networks and dATN-A more distal. dATN-A included distributed regions in frontal cortex at or near FEF, whereas dATN-B was more locally organized but not exclusively so.
Comparing dATN-A and dATN-B with CG-OP and PM-PPr, as highlighted in panels A and B of Fig. 17, Fig. 18, and Fig. 19, revealed parallel features. The second-order networks were all spatially anchored near the early (first order) sensory and motor networks, appearing as if they grew out or formed from the earlier networks. CG-OP in several individuals extended into the precentral gyrus.5 And, despite being far apart in their major extents, both sets of networks had distributed components throughout association cortex, including adjacencies in frontal cortex. Thus, from the standpoint of a potential hierarchy among networks, these second-order networks possess a motif that anchors them to the early sensory and motor networks and simultaneously connects them to distributed zones of association cortex.
We provisionally label SAL/PMN as a second-order network, but it has juxtapositions that differentiate it from the other second-order networks.6 Across much of its extent, SAL/PMN paralleled CG-OP with multiple juxtapositions. SAL/PMN differed in that it was not adjacent to early sensory and motor networks. Rather, SAL/PMN contained regions that were near the network labeled DN-A, especially along the posterior midline, where its regions could easily be confused with the large DN-A and DN-B regions that occupied much of the posterior midline.
The zones farthest away from the sensory and motor regions were populated by five third-order association networks (FPN-A, FPN-B, LANG, DN-B, and DN-A). Each third-order network possessed regions distributed widely throughout association cortex. Moreover, regions of distinct third-order networks displayed side-by-side juxtapositions with a pattern that repeated similarly across multiple zones of cortex. We focus on these repeating clusters of five networks extensively below.
Somatomotor and Visual Networks Respond to Body Movements and Visual Stimulation in a Topographic Manner
The spatial extent of task-elicited responses to body movements and visual stimulation was mapped in direct relation to the network boundaries. The goal of these analyses was to explore whether within-individual network estimates predict task responses. We start here with descriptions of sensory and motor responses and explore responses to varied cognitive task demands below. In all cases, the network boundaries were established before examination of the task responses. Fig. A13 illustrates the body movement and visual stimulation mapping strategy, and Fig. 20 displays the detailed maps for one representative participant on the inflated and flattened surfaces. Additional participants are displayed in Fig. 21, and all participants with available task data are shown in the Supplemental Materials.
Figure 20.
First-order somatomotor and visual networks respond to task stimulation in a topographically specific manner. Detailed views of the inflated (A) and flattened (B) surfaces display the somatomotor body axis and visual eccentricity maps for participant P6. The visualization combines panels D and F of Fig. A13, including binarized contrast maps targeting foot (red), hand (blue), tongue (yellow), and glute (green) movements, as well as central (red), middle (green), and peripheral (blue) visual stimulation. The black labeled outlines highlight Somatomotor-A (SMOT-A), Somatomotor-B (SMOT-B), Visual Central (VIS-C), and Visual Peripheral (VIS-P) networks. Thin colored outlines mark the boundaries of all other networks. At least 3 representations of body topography can be observed within the somatomotor networks SMOT-A and SMOT-B (labeled I, II, and III). The orientation of the main body map (I) along the central sulcus is shown by a stick figure. The second body map (II) is partially buried in the Sylvian fissure, and the third map (III) falls along the frontal midline. The visual gradient from central to peripheral eccentricity is mapped expanding from VIS-C to VIS-P subsuming the V1/V2/V3 cluster (as verified from the task contrast of meridian visual stimulation; see Fig. A13E). One exception is that the eccentricity map spares portions of VIS-P (marked by asterisks), likely because of the limited extent of peripheral stimulation (see methods). A second exception is the gap in the body topography (marked by a diamond) that may be an intereffector region.
Figure 21.
Somatomotor and visual topographic maps are aligned to first-order networks across multiple participants. Flattened surfaces display the somatomotor body axis and visual eccentricity maps in representative participants from the discovery (P2, P3), replication (P6, P7), and triplication (P12, P13) datasets. A body axis topography is evident in each individual by the ordering of tongue-hand-glute-foot along the central sulcus. A visual eccentricity gradient is evident along the calcarine sulcus. Although the idiosyncratic spatial details vary between individuals, the somatomotor and visual maps show substantial overlap in each instance with the first-order Somatomotor-A (SMOT-A), Somatomotor-B (SMOT-B), Visual Central (VIS-C), and Visual Peripheral (VIS-P) networks. Similar maps from all available participants are included in the Supplemental Materials. White-filled circles indicate the seed region locations, which are further explored in Fig. 22 to reveal intereffector regions.
Task activation maps revealed that body movement and visual stimulation task contrasts elicited responses that were aligned to, and generally filled in, the first-order network estimates (SMOT-A, SMOT-B, VIS-C, and VIS-P). That is, the idiosyncratic network estimates in each individual predicted the localization of the movement and visual stimulation responses. The visual responses often extended beyond the anterior boundaries of VIS-C and VIS-P, including portions of dATN-B, but generally followed the network boundaries. Several additional results are notable.
First, the main body map along the central sulcus extended across networks (SMOT-A and SMOT-B), as did the retinotopic eccentricity gradient (VIS-C and VIS-P). Within the visual system, there was a clear correspondence between the two visual networks and eccentricity. VIS-C, as anticipated given its anatomical position, tracked the central representation. VIS-P covered the peripheral representation. A gap emerged for the most peripheral regions of the dorsal and ventral eccentricity portions of VIS-P, possibly because the visual stimulation did not extend fully to the periphery [see Park et al. (131)]. Within the somatosensory and motor systems, there was a distinct gap between the representations of the hand and glutes, which may be an intereffector region (67), as discussed more below.
Second, the response patterns did not align to expected boundaries of individual brain areas (i.e., V1, S1). The body movements activated regions of pre- and postcentral gyrus, spanning multiple motor and somatosensory areas. Examined in detail, the body movement responses suggest at least three distinct maps of the main body axis, labeled I, II, and III in Fig. 20. The largest distinct body map was found aligned to primary somatomotor cortex, exhibiting a medial-to-lateral progression from foot to hand to tongue (Fig. 20, labeled I). In the posterior insula, the body map was buried with a posterior to anterior orientation (Fig. 20, labeled II). On the medial wall, the body map progressed from anterior to posterior (Fig. 20, labeled III). Similarly, the visual responses spanned the extent of at least the V1/V2/V3 retinotopic cluster, with networks cutting across areas (verified through polar mapping as illustrated in Fig. A13E). Thus, the response patterns confirm that early somatomotor and visual networks group multiple areas together and split areas along topographic gradients (e.g., VIS-C vs. VIS-P; see Ref. 123).
Third, the gap in the body map between the representations of the hand and glutes was associated with a distinct distributed correlation pattern that created discontinuities within the primary motor body map (including breaks between effector regions along the precentral gyrus). Replicating Gordon et al. (67), Fig. 22 illustrates the “intereffector” pattern. In almost every individual, placing a seed region within the gap revealed a robust distributed correlation pattern that typically contained at least two additional zones along the precentral gyrus: a region ventral to the face representation and a dorsomedial region near the foot representation. The pattern substantially overlapped the CG-OP network including regions surrounding the primary somatomotor networks and extending into the CG-OP regions distributed throughout the cortex. The intereffector seed region pattern also included prominent regions at and around the primary visual retinotopic cluster, most typically at the edges of the peripheral representation, consistent with the integration of the visual domain in addition to the motor domains noted by Gordon et al. (67).
Figure 22.
Intereffector regions situate in between the regions responsible for specific body movements along the central sulcus. Flattened surfaces display the intereffector regions in representative participants from the discovery (P2, P3), replication (P6, P7), and triplication (P12, P13) datasets. By placing a seed region within the gap separating hand and foot movement representations, we observe 3 intereffector regions demonstrating robust functional connectivity to the seed region. Furthermore, multiple distant regions of the second-order Cingulo-Opercular (CG-OP) network exhibit strong functional connectivity to the seed region located between the effector regions in the precentral gyrus in nearly all participants. Additionally, the intereffector regions are interconnected not only with the CG-OP network but also consistently with regions in the visual cortex (see the location marked by the black arrowheads for P6).
CG-OP and Sal/PMN Respond to Salient Transients
The oddball task was designed to measure the transient response to uncommon visually salient targets that require participant response. The mapping strategy is illustrated in Fig. A14. On the flattened cortical surface, the within-individual a priori-defined networks CG-OP and SAL/PMN are displayed in relation to the Oddball Effect task contrast. The details of the Oddball Effect task contrast are shown for one representative participant in Fig. 23. Figure 24 illustrates that the features can be observed in additional participants, and in all participants as shown in the Supplemental Materials.
Figure 23.
Second-order Cingulo-Opercular (CG-OP) and Salience/Parietal Memory Network (SAL/PMN) networks respond to transients associated with oddball detection. A detailed view of the inflated (A) and flattened (B) surfaces display the Oddball Effect task contrast map for participant P2. The black labeled outlines highlight networks CG-OP and SAL/PMN. Thin colored outlines mark the boundaries of all other networks. The oddball effect is a distributed with prominent response in the frontal insula, as well as along the posterior and anterior midline. The full response pattern involves many distributed regions of the CG-OP and SAL/PMN networks including posterior midline zones. The effect is not selective to these 2 networks, with a robust response in the hand region of left somatomotor cortex along the central sulcus (marked by asterisk) and the foveal region of visual cortex along the calcarine sulcus (marked by a double asterisk), presumably due to the oddball target response demanding a key press and enhanced attention to the visual cue. The response in the motor region is strongly lateralized (not shown) as expected given the right-handed response.
Figure 24.
The oddball effect is aligned to Cingulo-Opercular (CG-OP) and Salience/Parietal Memory Network (SAL/PMN) networks across multiple participants. Flattened surfaces display maps of the binarized oddball effect in representative participants from the discovery (P2, P3), replication (P6, P7) and triplication (P12, P13) datasets. Although the spatial details vary between individuals, the oddball effect is broadly localized to the CG-OP and SAL/PMN networks and less so in regions of adjacent association networks, a qualitative impression that is formally quantified in Fig. 25. Similar maps from all available participants are included in the Supplemental Materials.
The Oddball Effect task contrast response was widely distributed across the cortex. The response prominently involved the distributed regions of the CG-OP and SAL/PMN networks, including regions in the anterior insula as well as along the posterior midline. These collective regions have been the emphasis of prior studies separately focused on the salience network and the parietal memory network. Thus, as predicted by the hypothesis that SAL/PMN is a single network, the response pattern observed here extended across the full distributed extent of the network.
In addition to the consistent responses across the distributed regions of CG-OP and SAL/PMN, additional responses were reliably observed: a response along the central sulcus in the left hemisphere near the estimated location of the hand representation and along the calcarine sulcus near the central representation of the visual field (contrast Fig. 23 with Fig. 20). The response in the hand region of the central sulcus was exclusively in the left hemisphere, consistent with the right-handed response.
To quantify the selectivity of the task response, the mean z values for the Oddball Effect task contrast were calculated separately for each association network. The estimates were obtained within the bounds of each individual’s a priori-defined networks and then averaged across participants (N = 14). Results plotted in Fig. 25 revealed a strong, significant positive response to oddball targets in both the CG-OP [t(13) = 7.97, P < 0.001] and SAL/PMN [t(13) = 6.21, P < 0.001] networks. By contrast, for many of the third-order association networks the response was significantly negative [DN-A: t(13) = −11.76, P < 0.001, DN-B: t(13) = −8.81, P < 0.001, LANG: t(13) = −3.82, P < 0.01, FPN-B: t(13) = −3.02, P < 0.01], with FPN-A being the exception. FPN-A showed a weak, nonsignificant positive response [t(13) = 1.82, P = 0.09]. These observations suggest that the CG-OP and SAL/PMN networks are recruited during the Oddball Effect task contrast.
Figure 25.
Cingulo-Opercular (CG-OP) and Salience/Parietal Memory Network (SAL/PMN) networks respond preferentially to transients associated with oddball detection. Bar graphs quantify the oddball effect as mean z values (N = 14) across the multiple a priori-defined networks. A strong positive response was observed in the CG-OP and SAL/PMN networks, whereas adjacent networks displayed lesser (and most often significantly negative) response. Values significantly different from zero: *P < 0.05, **P < 0.001. Error bars show SE. Note that the CG-OP and SAL/PMN networks are each more active than the other 5 networks (10 of 10 tests significant, P < 0.05). DN-A, Default Network-A; DN-B, Default Network-B; FPN-A, Frontoparietal Network-A; FPN-B, Frontoparietal Network-B; LANG, Language.
Given the historical focus on the salience network and the parietal memory network as separate networks, and their proximity along the posterior midline to the historically defined default network, we replotted the Oddball Effect task contrast on the inflated surface (Fig. 26). For this visualization, the task map threshold was reduced to zero. Much of the full extent of the CG-OP and SAL/PMN networks was strongly activated. The positive response included the posterior midline regions that have been the focus of the parietal memory network (94) as well as the anterior insula region that has been a focus of the salience network (91, 92). An interesting feature is that islands of the CG-OP network that fell within the frontal midline showed positive responses in the within-individual maps (Fig. 26). These small responses, which were adjacent to large regions with an opposite response pattern, were absent in the group-averaged response (Fig. 26, bottom). The positive response was not selective to these two specific networks, with motor and visual responses as noted above. The positive response also extended into the region of the visual second-order networks (e.g., dATN-B).
Figure 26.
The oddball effect robustly dissociates Cingulo-Opercular (CG-OP) and Salience/Parietal Memory Network (SAL/PMN) networks from regions traditionally associated with the default network. Inflated surfaces display maps of the increases (red/yellow) and decreases (blue) in response for the Oddball Effect task contrast. No threshold is applied, to allow full visualization of the effect in both directions. Top: images from representative participants from the discovery (P2), replication (P7), and triplication (P12) datasets. Bottom: the group average (N = 14). The white outlines for the individual participants are the outline for the a priori-defined CG-OP and SAL/PMN networks. Note that the Oddball Effect task contrast increases response broadly across the CG-OP and SAL/PMN networks, whereas there are simultaneously distributed decreases that span multiple networks including Default Network-A (DN-A) and Default Network-B (DN-B). In the top and bottom images, arrowheads highlight the increases in response along the posterior midline (black arrowheads) that surround the canonical default network regional decreases (noted by a white asterisk) as well as increases in the anterior insula (white arrowhead). Similar maps from all available participants are included in the Supplemental Materials.
Critically, the networks at or near the historical default network, here estimated within individuals as encompassing at least DN-A and DN-B, were all strongly “deactivated,” meaning more active during the implicitly coded baseline reference than during the salient targets. That is, the contrast replicated the task deactivation pattern that originally generated interest in the default network (125, 132, 133) in the presence of a robust positive response across the distributed extent of the SAL/PMN network. Thus, the separation of the effects along the posterior midline revealed a spatial dissociation between the second-order network SAL/PMN and the third-order networks DN-A and DN-B.
Higher-Order Zones of Association Cortex Possess a Repeating Motif
Distributed throughout association cortex, in the zones roughly7 between the second-order networks, were the five association networks FPN-A, FPN-B, LANG, DN-B, and DN-A (Fig. 27). Among these networks, side-by-side juxtapositions repeated across multiple cortical zones (see I, II, III, and IV in Fig. 27). FPN-A and FPN-B were reliably positioned adjacent to one another and, as a pair, were adjacent to a repeating group of the three other networks, LANG, DN-B, and DN-A. We call these repeating clusters of five networks supra-areal association megaclusters or SAAMs. The reproducibility of the SAAMs across participants was striking and is illustrated for the posterior association zones in all 15 participants in Fig. 28. Although the idiosyncratic spatial details varied, multiple SAAMs were consistently observed. The remaining task analyses explored functional response properties of the association networks embedded within the SAAMs.
Figure 27.
Supra-areal association megaclusters (SAAMs). A detailed view of the inflated (A) and flattened (B) surfaces displays the full set of networks for participant P4 to visualize an interesting topographic feature of association cortex: a cluster of networks repeats across multiple zones, including within posterior parietal cortex (PPC, I), lateral temporal cortex (LTC, II), and multiple times throughout prefrontal cortex (PFC, III, IV). We refer to these repeating clusters as supra-areal association megaclusters or SAAMs. Within each SAAM, Frontoparietal Network-A (FPN-A) and Frontoparietal Network-A (FPN-B) are adjacent to one another and together are adjacent to Default Network-A (DN-A), Default Network-B (DN-B), and Language (LANG). Thick red outlines mark 4 SAAMs. The repeating motif is most clear for PPC (I), where the cluster has a “north-to-south” orientation, and LTC (II), where a similar set of juxtapositions display an “east-to-west” orientation. Within PFC, the pattern is present but more ambiguous. Two candidate SAAMs in ventrolateral PFC (VLPFC, III) and dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC, IV) are highlighted. Reference landmarks include the insula (Ins), central sulcus (CS), posteromedial cortex (PMC), parahippocampal cortex (PHC), and medial PFC (MPFC). Regions of poor signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) that do not allow for confident network assignment are noted by a double asterisk. The rectangle in B indicates the portion of the surface that is extracted and displayed for all participants in Fig. 28. SMOT-A, Somatomotor-A; SMOT-B, Somatomotor-B; PM-PPr, Premotor-Posterior Parietal Rostral; CG-OP, Cingulo-Opercular; SAL/PMN, Salience/Parietal Memory Network; dATN-A, Dorsal Attention-A; dATN-B, Dorsal Attention-B; VIS-C, Visual Central; VIS-P, Visual Peripheral; AUD, Auditory.
Figure 28.
Supra-areal association megaclusters (SAAMs) are reliably observed across all participants. Panels display a rotated portion of the flattened surface for 15 individuals (P1–P15). The displayed portion includes the 2 SAAMs within posterior parietal cortex (PPC, I) and lateral temporal cortex (LTC, II) as illustrated in Fig. 27B. Black outlines illustrate the boundaries of the 5 networks in each SAAM, including Frontoparietal Network-A (FPN-A), Frontoparietal Network-A (FPN-B), Default Network-A (DN-A), Default Network-B (DN-B), and Language (LANG). Although the idiosyncratic spatial details vary, in most individuals the separate SAAMs are clear and distinct. Within each SAAM, FPN-A falls at one end juxtaposed with FPN-B. The 3 side-by-side networks DN-A, DN-B, and LANG fall at the other end of the SAAM, with the LANG network most closely juxtaposed to DN-B. SMOT-A, Somatomotor-A; SMOT-B, Somatomotor-B; PM-PPr, Premotor-Posterior Parietal Rostral; CG-OP, Cingulo-Opercular; SAL/PMN, Salience/Parietal Memory Network; dATN-A, Dorsal Attention-A; dATN-B, Dorsal Attention-B; VIS-C, Visual Central; VIS-P, Visual Peripheral; AUD, Auditory.
FPN-A Responds to Domain-Flexible Working Memory Demands
The functional properties of the association networks comprising the SAAMs (FPN-A, FPN-B, LANG, DN-B, DN-A) were explored first in relation to domain-flexible demands on working memory and (in LANG, DN-B, and DN-A Respond Differentially to Distinct Cognitive Domains) in relation to domain-specialized processing functions. The hypothesis was that FPN-A and possibly FPN-B would modulate their response in relation to increasing working memory load across multiple verbal and nonverbal stimulus conditions (see Ref. 68). The mapping strategy is illustrated in Fig. A15. On the flattened cortical surface, the within-individual a priori-defined networks FPN-A and FPN-B are displayed in relation to the N-Back Load Effect task contrast (collapsed across stimulus conditions). The N-Back Load Effect task contrast is shown in detail in Fig. 29 for one representative participant. Figure 30 illustrates that the features can be observed in additional participants, and in all participants with available task data in the Supplemental Materials.
Figure 29.
Frontoparietal Network-A (FPN-A) network responds to high working memory load. A detailed view of the inflated (A) and flattened (B) surfaces displays the N-Back Load Effect task contrast map for participant P14. The black labeled outlines highlight the FPN-A network, and the light gray labeled outlines highlight the Frontoparietal Network-B (FPN-B) network. Thin colored outlines mark the boundaries of all other networks. The N-Back load effect shows prominent response across the multiple, distributed association zones preferentially within the FPN-A network and to a lesser degree FPN-B, including the relevant portions of the supra-areal association megaclusters (SAAMs). The zones are labeled I–IV to orient to the corresponding labels of the SAAMs as displayed in Fig. 27. The response also consistently includes a small subregion of the anterior insula that is associated with FPN-A (labeled with an asterisk).
Figure 30.
The N-Back load effect is aligned to Frontoparietal Network-A (FPN-A) network across multiple participants. Flattened surfaces display the binarized N-Back load effect maps for multiple participants from the discovery (P2, P3), replication (P6, P7) and triplication (P12, P13) datasets. Although individuals vary in anatomical details, the N-Back load effect is generally localized to the FPN-A network. Similar maps from all available participants are included in the Supplemental Materials. FPN-B, Frontoparietal Network-B.
As hypothesized, the N-Back Load Effect task contrast increased activation within and near the boundaries of the FPN-A network and, to a lesser extent, the FPN-B network (Fig. 29 and Fig. A15). The widely distributed response included extensive regions of PFC as well as regions of PPC and the dorsal ACC, all canonical regions associated with domain-flexible cognitive control (e.g., Refs. 53, 134–136). As predicted by the network estimates, there was also a response in LTC and a small subregion of the anterior insula that is spatially distinct from that of other networks. Of equal importance was the consistent absence of response in the distributed association regions linked to the LANG, DN-B, and DN-A networks, including within the PPC and LTC. In essence, the N-Back Load Effect task contrast split the SAAMs and activated the portions linked to the FPN-A network, with minimal or no response in the juxtaposed portions associated with the LANG, DN-B, and DN-A networks.
To quantify the selectivity of the task response, the mean z values output from the condition-level GLM for the N-Back Load Effect task contrast were calculated separately for each association network. The estimates were obtained within the bounds of each individual’s independent a priori-defined networks and then averaged (N = 15). Results plotted in Fig. 31 reveal a positive N-Back Load Effect response that was strongest in FPN-A [t(14) = 21.67, P < 0.001] and also present in FPN-B [t(14) = 6.45, P < 0.001]. SAL/PMN unexpectedly showed a significant positive response [t(14) = 7.91, P < 0.001] that was significantly weaker than either FPN-A [t(14) = −13.43, P < 0.001] or FPN-B [t(14) = −3.58, P < 0.01]. Thus, although SAL/PMN showed a response, the functional response was less relative to FPN-A and FPN-B, opposite to the pattern found above (contrast Fig. 31 with Fig. 25). The remaining networks, including the three additional networks that were adjacent within the SAAMs, showed a negative N-Back load effect. The effect was significantly negative for DN-A [t(14) = −4.85, P < 0.001] and DN-B [t(14) = −7.14, P < 0.001] but not LANG [t(14) = −0.81, P = 0.43]. These results provide evidence that the FPN-A network and possibly the FPN-B network are involved in processes enhanced by increasing working memory demands, whereas other juxtaposed networks, LANG, DN-B and DN-A, are functionally dissociated, consistent with the qualitative patterns visualized in the activation maps.
Figure 31.
Frontoparietal Network-A (FPN-A) network responds to high working memory load in a domain-flexible manner. Bar graphs quantify the N-Back load effect as mean z values (N = 15) across the multiple a priori-defined networks. Top: a strong positive response was observed in the FPN-A and a modest positive response in the Frontoparietal Network-B (FPN-B) network. Other association networks displayed minimal or no response, with the exception of the Salience/Parietal Memory Network (SAL/PMN) network, which also displayed a significant, positive response. Error bars show SE. Note that FPN-A and FPN-B are each more active than all 5 of the other networks (10 of 10 tests were significant, P < 0.05). Bottom left: the N-Back load effect is quantified separately for each stimulus domain (Face, Letter, Word, and Scene) within FPN-A. Note that the effect is robust and significant across domains. Bottom right: the N-Back load effect is quantified separately for each stimulus domain within FPN-B. Note again that the effect is positive and significant across domains. Values significantly different from zero: **P < 0.001. CG-OP, Cingulo-Opercular; DN-A, Default Network-A; DN-B, Default Network-B; LANG, Language.
To further investigate the domain flexibility of FPN-A and FPN-B, the mean z values output from the block-level GLM for each of the four stimulus conditions of the N-Back Load Effect task (Face, Letter, Word, and Scene) were separately plotted (Fig. 31). FPN-A [Face: t(14) = 11.74, Letter: t(14) = 16.03, Word: t(14) = 11.30, Scene: t(14) = 12.05; all P < 0.001] and to a lesser degree FPN-B [Face: t(14) = 5.13, Letter: t(14) = 5.60, Word: t(14) = 5.15, Scene: t(14) = 4.54; all P < 0.001] exhibited a significant response across all conditions of the N-Back Load Effect task contrast, supporting that their processing role generalizes across both verbal and nonverbal domains. That is, FPN-A and FPN-B responded to working memory demands more so than adjacent networks, and did so in a domain-flexible manner.
LANG, DN-B, and DN-A Respond Differentially to Distinct Cognitive Domains
Among the networks that populate the distributed zones of higher-order association cortex, FPN-A and possibly FPN-B responded in a domain-flexible manner to increasing working memory load. The adjacent trio of networks, LANG, DN-B, and DN-A, did not. In our final planned analyses, we explored the functional specialization of these additional three networks by examining Episodic Projection, Theory-of-Mind, and Sentence Processing task contrasts designed to emphasize distinct specialized domains of higher-order cognitive processing. This analysis framework and set of tests were envisioned prospectively [see Fig. 2 in DiNicola and Buckner (137)].
The mapping strategy is illustrated in Fig. A16. On the flattened cortical surface, the within-individual a priori-defined networks LANG, DN-B, and DN-A are displayed in relation to the three separate task contrasts simultaneously, to illustrate the adjacency of the responses in relation to each other and to the network boundaries. The details of one composite task contrast map are displayed for a representative participant in Fig. 32. Figure 33 illustrates additional participants, and all participants with available data are shown in the Supplemental Materials. Several results are notable.
Figure 32.
Default Network-A (DN-A), Default Network-B (DN-B), and Language (LANG) networks respond in a domain-selective manner. A detailed view of the inflated (A) and flattened (B) surfaces displays the Episodic Projection (dark red), Theory-of-Mind (light red), and Sentence Processing (blue) task contrast maps for participant P6. The black labeled outlines highlight the DN-A, DN-B, and LANG networks. Thin colored outlines mark the boundaries of all other networks. The task contrasts reveal clear spatial separation across the multiple, distributed association zones preferentially within the DN-A, DN-B, and LANG networks, including the relevant portions of the supra-areal association megaclusters (SAAMs). The zones are labeled I–IV to orient to the corresponding labels of the SAAMs as displayed in Fig. 27 and Fig. 29. The parahippocampal cortex (labeled with an asterisk) responds preferentially to the Episodic Projection task contrast without juxtaposed responses from other domains, unlike the SAAMs, which each have representation of all 3 domains, separate from (but adjacent to) zones responding in a domain-flexible manner to working memory load (see Fig. 30).
Figure 33.
Domain-selective responses are aligned to Default Network-A (DN-A), Default Network-B (DN-B), and Language (LANG) networks across multiple participants. Flattened surfaces display maps of the binarized Episodic Projection, Theory-of-Mind, and Sentence Processing task contrast maps for multiple participants from the discovery (P2, P3), replication (P6, P7) and triplication (P12, P13) datasets. The domain-preferential effects are generally localized to corresponding DN-A, DN-B, and LANG networks and separate from the adjacent zones that respond to working memory load (contrast the present maps with those of Fig. 30). Similar maps from all available participants are included in the Supplemental Materials.
First, the composite activation patterns across the three task contrasts filled in the remaining zones of association cortex. Strikingly, the domain-specialized task responses are situated adjacent to, but separate from, the regions activated by domain-flexible working memory demands (contrast Fig. 32 with Fig. 29). This separation can be seen in many locations, with a particularly clear example visualized within the PPC, where the N-Back Load Effect task contrast showed a posterodorsal response relative to the three current task contrasts. The side-by-side juxtaposition of domain-specialized and domain-flexible regions was also observed within LTC and multiple locations throughout PFC.
Second, within each juxtaposed cluster of domain-specialized regions, the region preferentially responding to the Sentence Processing task contrast abutted the region preferentially responding to the Theory-of-Mind task contrast, and these abutted the region preferentially responding to the Episodic Project task contrast. Although overlap and exceptions were found, the differential response patterns generally tracked the network separations between LANG, DN-B, and DN-A. The idiosyncratic positions and boundaries of the three networks in any given individual, LANG, DN-B, and DN-A, predicted the positions of the domain-specialized activation responses (Fig. 33).
Thus, within each local zone the regions associated with the separate networks responded to their distinct specialized cognitive domains. Moreover, the spatially differentiated response patterns repeated across the multiple SAAMs (see I, II, III, and IV in Fig. 32). There were exceptions. For example, regions of task activation in VLPFC did not overlap well with the estimated networks in P12. The discrepancies tended to fall within anterior temporal regions and PFC regions where SNR is low, raising the possibility that technical variance played a role. To reveal the details of the task maps more fully, the Supplemental Materials include task maps for each task contrast separately in addition to the composite maps for all available participants.
The response was quantified for each of the three task contrasts for each network to formally test for the hypothesized interaction. For each domain-specialized task contrast, the z values within the bounds of each individual’s three independent a priori-defined networks (LANG, DN-B, and DN-A) were obtained and then averaged (N = 13). The resulting mean z values are plotted in Fig. 34. A repeated-measures ANOVA on network-level task response revealed a significant 3 × 3 interaction between the effect of task contrast and network [F(4,48) = 77.82, P < 0.001]. Paired t tests then tested the individual contrasts, with the hypothesis that each network’s within-domain response would be significantly greater than either of the other two networks. All six of these planned comparisons were significant. The Episodic Projection task contrast recruited DN-A regions over those of DN-B [t(12) = 16.38, P < 0.001] and LANG [t(12) = 14.49, P < 0.001]. The Theory-of-Mind task recruited DN-B regions over those of DN-A [t(12) = 5.27, P < 0.001] and LANG [t(12) = 10.09, P < 0.001], and the Sentence Processing task contrast recruited the LANG regions over those of DN-A [t(12) = 6.55, P < 0.001] and DN-B [t(12) = 5.42, P < 0.001].
Figure 34.
Default Network-A (DN-A), Default Network-B (DN-B), and Language (LANG) networks respond in a domain-selective manner. Bar graphs quantify the Episodic Projection, Theory-of-Mind, and Sentence Processing task contrasts as mean z values (N = 13) across the multiple a priori-defined networks. Each plot displays data from a distinct task contrast; each bar represents a distinct network. The full 3 × 3 interaction (network by task contrast) is significant (P < 0.001). DN-A is robustly and preferentially activated for the Episodic Projection task contrast; DN-B is robustly and preferentially activated for the Theory-of-Mind task contrast; and LANG is robustly and preferentially activated for the Sentence Processing task contrast. All planned pairwise comparisons are significant, confirming the full triple dissociation. **P < 0.001.
Thus, in addition to the qualitative impressions (Fig. 32, Fig. 33, and Fig. A16), statistical tests revealed that the full interaction was significant, with all pairwise tests also significant in support of a triple functional dissociation across the three networks. These observations suggest that the parallel networks LANG, DN-B, and DN-A, with adjacent regions across multiple cortical zones, are specialized to support distinct higher-order cognitive domains.
The FPN-B Response Is Elusive
Our final post hoc analysis, as a reminder that our results and data patterns leave gaps, explored the possibility that we have not yet isolated the task demands that elicit an FPN-B network response. As depicted in Fig. 31, the response observed in FPN-B is notably weaker in comparison to that in FPN-A. Considering that FPN-B is adjacent to FPN-A, this raises the question of whether the observed response is located within the bounds of the FPN-B network or is due to spatial blur from the adjacent FPN-A network response. To weigh in on this question, we replotted the N-Back Load Effect, Episodic Projection, Theory-of-Mind, and Sentence Processing task contrasts simultaneously on a common surface in a representative participant, P6. The FPN-B network is outlined with a black border. As illustrated in Fig. 35, the regions of the FPN-B network do not display a distinct response in multiple zones including a clear gap in the large parietal association region despite adjacent responses in all of the other network regions. The response to the N-Back Load Effect within the FPN-B network might originate from signal blur from the adjacent FPN-A network. Our final result is thus the unmasking of an ambiguity. The present work does not disambiguate FPN-B from adjacent networks or provide clear evidence that we have found task demands that elicit a response.
Figure 35.
Frontoparietal Network-B (FPN-B) network displays minimal response to high working memory load. A detailed view of the inflated (A) and flattened (B) surfaces displays the N-Back load effect (yellow) and Episodic Projection (dark red), Theory-of-Mind (light red), and Sentence Processing (blue) task contrast maps for participant P6. The black labeled outlines highlight the FPN-B network. Thin colored outlines mark the boundaries of all other networks. The task contrast maps collectively reveal a pattern: the regions within the FPN-B network do not display a preferential response and tend to fill the gaps between the borders of adjacent networks, which is particularly evident in the parietal cortex, as shown by the FPN-B label in B. Given this response pattern, we suspect that our task contrasts have not isolated processing demands that are supported by the FPN-B network in contrast to the Frontoparietal Network-A (FPN-A) network, which is clearly activated across its distributed regions by the N-Back Load Effect task contrast.
DISCUSSION
Detailed network estimates reveal a global organization that can be conceptualized as three levels of cortical hierarchy6: locally organized first-order sensory and motor networks, spatially adjacent second-order networks that link to distant regions, and third-order networks that populate and connect widely distributed zones of higher-order association cortex. Repeating side-by-side spatial juxtapositions among the third-order association networks form organized motifs that we call supra-areal association megaclusters or SAAMs. Within each SAAM, the regions linked to distinct association networks demonstrate differential task response properties. Certain networks contribute to domain-flexible cognitive control and others to domain-specialized processes involved in language, social, and spatial/episodic functions. We discuss the practical and conceptual implications of these findings, including how repeating organizational motifs might arise during development.
Within-Individual Network Estimates
In the present work, we explored the utility of a 15-network MS-HBM estimate of cerebral cortical organization that allowed the idiosyncratic details of each individual’s own anatomy to guide the solutions (e.g., Fig. A1 and Fig. A4). The method yielded robust, stable network estimates that were confirmed by analyses of seed region-based correlation (e.g., Fig. A2 and Fig. A5). All quantitative analyses and visual inspections of the data reinforced that the present 15-network estimate captured a great deal of the structured correlations present in the underlying data. From a methodological standpoint, the present results indicate that a MS-HBM can be used to estimate networks automatically and robustly within individuals (71, 75). Several features of our network estimates revise or expand earlier ideas.
First, the present network parcellation falls into a class of within-individual network estimates that refine group-based estimates. In group-based estimates, including multiple estimates from our laboratory, large monolithic networks have been identified that encompass extensive regions of association cortex (e.g., Refs. 18, 22–25). The present network estimates are broadly similar but separate the large group-based networks into multiple distinct parallel networks. For example, the network historically known as the default network overlaps four separate networks in the present parcellation including networks LANG, DN-B, DN-A, and SAL/PMN. Each of these four distinct networks can be identified in every individual in the present study. The multiple networks are not estimated to be “subnetworks” with shared regions or anatomical convergence but rather are distinct networks that are near one another and often blurred in group-averaged data (see also Refs. 52, 53, 56, 59–61, 64, 93). Thus, an advance of within-individual network estimates, including the present contribution, is to fully resolve adjacent networks that are difficult to separate through approaches that average over people.
Second, among within-individual parcellation estimates, we settled on a 15-network solution because of our goal to separate nearby networks within the anterior insula (see Ref. 92), as well as to better separate early sensory and adjacent networks. Our analyses confirmed that the newly proposed 15-network parcellation could capture correlational features absent in simpler network solutions, including our own 10-network solution previously estimated in Xue et al. (75); see Figs. A7, A8, A10, and A11. In addition to detecting distinctions among networks that have close juxtapositions in the anterior insula, the present 15-network parcellation also revealed clear separation of the estimated AUD network from the nearby LANG network. Multiple networks identified in the simpler network solutions remained in the 15-network estimates, indicating that the refinements did not come at the expense of the established networks.
Third, the present parcellation identified a single distributed network, labeled SAL/PMN, that includes regions that have historically been studied separately as components of the salience network (91) and the parietal memory network (94). Note that we do not say “joins” two previously described networks, as we suspect there have never been two separate networks. Rather, different research lineages may have focused on distinct regional components of what is ultimately the same network. This hypothesis will require further testing, but several lines of evidence lead to the present proposal that SAL/PMN is a single, coherent network. In every individual, the estimated SAL/PMN network included regions along the posterior midline and within the anterior insula (Fig. 14). Seed region-based correlation patterns recapitulated the automated network estimates: seed regions placed in PFC and posterior cortex revealed clear regional correlation in the anterior insula as well as multiple distinct posterior midline regions (Fig. A12).
A recent study by Kwon et al. (138) is consistent with the hypothesis of SAL/PMN as a single distributed network. Using both seed-based and clustering approaches on within-individual data, their study revealed a network consistent with the SAL/PMN as delineated in our investigation including anterior insula, the inferior parietal lobule, and the posteromedial regions canonical of the parietal memory network. Additionally, in another study by Lynch et al. (139), a network exhibiting similar correlational structure as the SAL/PMN in our study was identified [see Extended Data Fig. 5 of Lynch et al. (139)]. Furthermore, our independent task data focused on salience processing, via the oddball task, elicited robust responses in the distributed regions of the SAL/PMN network including the posterior midline and anterior insula (Fig. 23, Fig. 24, and Fig. 26). Meta-analyses of task-relevant oddball effects have previously noted responses in the anterior insula and portions of posterior midline (140). Thus, multiple studies and analyses raise the possibility that the SAL/PMN is a single coherent network.
Despite these findings, it is important to acknowledge the results that have caused a distinction to be drawn between the parietal memory network and the salience network, including evidence from both anatomical and functional perspectives. The salience network has been emphasized to show strong connectivity to the ventral striatum (141) and the parietal memory network to the posterior hippocampus (142). Functionally, the salience network has been implicated in detecting important environmental stimuli and orchestrating task switching (92, 143), whereas the parietal memory network has been emphasized in paradigms involving stimulus repetitions (94, 144–147). Given these complexities, further in-depth investigations are required. For example, given Zheng et al.’s findings (142), the present hypothesis that SAL/PMN is a single coherent network predicts that the posterior hippocampus might respond to low-level oddball events and task transitions. We are presently exploring this possibility (see Ref. 148).
Finally, it is important to note that the present estimates assume (and are optimized to detect) large-scale distributed networks. For this reason, our resultant parcellation is different from parcellations that are optimized to detect local gradients of change and/or directly estimate “area” boundaries [e.g., Refs. 27, 149, 150; for discussion see Refs. 123, 151]. Although there is some convergence between approaches, and it is possible to apply mutual constraints (152), our present parcellation is weighted to estimate networks based on long-range correlational properties, without weighting local gradients.
Supra-Areal Association Megaclusters
A striking observation that is apparent in the flat map visualizations is the recurrent spatial grouping of the same five higher-order networks throughout association cortex (FPN-A, FPN-B, LANG, DN-B, DN-A). The clearest examples are found in PPC and LTC (Fig. 28), but the adjacencies are also present in multiple PFC zones (Fig. 27), as if a shared organizing force plays out repeatedly across different cortical territories. Each grouping of regions possesses similar spatial relations among the five networks: networks FPN-A and FPN-B are next to one another, and that pair of networks is adjacent to the trio of networks LANG, DN-B, and DN-A. These juxtaposed networks form SAAMs not only within the cerebral cortex but also within the cerebellum (75, 153) and the caudate (154).
SAAMs possess several additional features. Although their global patterning, meaning spatial adjacencies between networks, is identifiable for multiple SAAMs within and across individuals, the orientations shift and the exact spatial positions vary. For example, within the PPC the axis that begins with the FPN-A/FPN-B pairing and ends with the LANG/DN-B/DN-A triad is oriented dorsal to ventral. Within the LTC, the axis is oriented ventral to dorsal (Fig. 27). Moreover, although the SAAMs are readily identified in every person in the PPC and LTC, usually with a discontinuity between the two SAAMs, the idiosyncratic spatial details vary from one person to the next. In some individuals the two zones appear fused (Fig. 28). It is thus unsurprising that group-averaged data, while revealing certain spatial features apparent in the SAAMs, blur over the fine spatial details that are robust and consistently apparent in the within-individual maps.
The spatial juxtapositions that define the SAAMs in PPC and LTC are also present in multiple zones of the PFC. However, there is not always spatial separation. The boundaries of individual SAAMs in PFC are thus ambiguous. In Fig. 27 we note candidate SAAMs in VLPFC (labeled III) and DLPFC (labeled IV), recognizing that these are hypotheses. A future endeavor might explore how a repeating pattern could parsimoniously explain the juxtapositions in PFC with the assumption that multiple SAAMs are present like those observed in PPC and LTC, but with the additional complication that there are multiple adjacent SAAMs that collide into one another, perhaps as a consequence of their formation during development.
A final detail regarding the SAAMs is subtle but potentially informative. Although the presence of five regions linked to the distinct networks is a consistent feature of PPC, LTC, VLPFC, and DLPFC, there are also partial sets of the network juxtapositions in other cortical zones. For example, along the midline there is clear representation of networks DN-A and DN-B in PMC and medial PFC (MPFC) but not consistently the other networks (Fig. 27). The partial SAAMs may provide an insight into the origins of the patterning. DN-A is a putative hippocampal-cortical network that has been extensively studied in humans (e.g., Refs. 28, 60, 62, 127, 142, 155) and monkeys (e.g., Refs. 29–31, 35, 93). The hippocampal formation, via polysynaptic projections through entorhinal cortex and PHC, projects heavily to RSC and ventral PCC along the posterior midline, and also to MPFC (156–158). The exclusive assignment of PHC to DN-A and the predominance of DN-A along the midline may thus reflect connectivity to the hippocampal formation. The interdigitation of DN-A with other higher-order networks might emerge as the hippocampal-predominant projections intermix with other anatomical projection gradients in the apex association zones where the fully formed SAAMs are present.
The Relation of the Present Network Estimates with the Historical Default Network
The default network, or default mode network, has received considerable attention among investigations of cerebral networks (29, 159, 160). In relation to estimating networks using resting-state functional connectivity, after the seminal description of the method (14), the default network was the first distributed association network to be characterized in humans (155, 161) and in monkeys (28). All group-based network estimates, even low-dimensional solutions that identify as few as seven networks, find a large, distributed network that has the spatial pattern of the default network (e.g., Refs. 21–25). Thus, a critical issue to address, given the historical emphasis on the default network, is how the present network estimates relate to these earlier descriptions.
Our present hypothesis is that the large monolithic (or core subnetwork) descriptions of the default network based on group-averaged data, including our own contributions (e.g., Refs. 29, 116), are inaccurate and reflect an artifact of spatial blurring. As noted above, the canonical group-averaged default network overlaps fully or partially four distinct networks: LANG, DN-B, DN-A, and SAL/PMN. The separation of these networks is anticipated in some prior group-based analyses. For example, Andrews-Hanna and colleagues (162) noted that regions within PPC responding to social inference (theory of mind) tasks tended to activate an anterior region relative to tasks targeting remembering. This distinction likely captures the separation of DN-B and DN-A in PPC. Similarly, in a thorough analysis of functional connectivity in group data, the network identified here as LANG was separated from the canonical default network (Ref. 163; for discussion see Ref. 63). However, the blurring induced by between-subject averaging, to date, has negated the ability to resolve the spatial details that fully distinguish the four nearby networks that are described here.
A further observation emerges from our task-based results. In addition to the challenge of identifying the multiple, juxtaposed networks due to spatial blurring, there is a separate functional property that has anchored study of the default network. The default network was originally described based on task-induced deactivations, referring to the observation that the distributed association regions that comprise the default network are more active in passive tasks and fixation than active, externally-orientated tasks [Refs. 125, 132, 159; see Buckner and DiNicola (93) for review]. When a contrast is made between active and passive tasks, a distributed pattern of “deactivations” emerges that is robust and overlaps with group-based estimates of the default network (29, 125, 160). What is surprising and interesting is that, even with the present high-resolution within-individual estimates, the task-induced pattern of deactivation remains broad and spans multiple networks.
Specifically, the Oddball Effect task contrast reveals a broad task-induced deactivation pattern within individual participants (Fig. 26). That is, the regions deactivated by attending and responding to external stimuli span multiple association networks even when group averaging is not a factor. Figure 25 quantifies this effect: DN-A, DN-B, and LANG all show significant “deactivation,” with DN-A and DN-B being almost indistinguishable from one another, despite clear functional double dissociation during domain-relevant active tasks [e.g., Fig. 34; see also DiNicola et al. (90)]. One possibility is that, although DN-A and DN-B are anatomically and functionally distinct networks, they may collectively be suppressed during certain externally oriented task events, perhaps as a result of a broad antagonistic process between externally versus internally oriented processing modes (Ref. 93; see also Refs. 164–168). Thus, the phenomenon of task-induced deactivation, which is not selective to specific networks, may have reinforced an impression that there is a coherent monolithic function across large swaths of association cortex, a possibility refuted by a growing number of robust functional dissociations (e.g., Refs. 55, 90, 95, 169, 170).
Another relevant observation surrounds the relation between the default network and the present estimate of network SAL/PMN. The SAL/PMN network possesses regions distributed across the cortex, including multiple distinct regions along the posterior midline side by side with DN-A and DN-B network regions. The adjacencies make the regions easy to confuse. Despite their spatial proximity, Zheng et al. (Ref. 142; see their Fig. 6) noted that “deactivations” are restricted to the default network and separate from their estimate of SAL/PMN (labeled as the parietal memory network in their paper). The transient positive response in SAL/PMN observed here to salient oddballs is robust, including the regions along the posterior midline, separate from the juxtaposed DN-A and DN-B regions showing deactivation (Fig. 26). Moreover, SAL/PMN has small, focal regions of response in MPFC, which are also surrounded by DN-A and DN-B network regions. In the group-averaged map displayed in Fig. 26, bottom, there is no detectable positive response in MPFC. Each individual shows a response but in slightly different spatial positions from one person to the next. The positive task response in MPFC is likely lost in the process of spatial averaging.
Our results thus converge with Zheng et al. (142) to suggest that SAL/PMN is spatially and functionally distinct from the network historically described as the default network. The SAL/PMN network does not exhibit task-induced deactivation; rather, it displays an opposite functional response pattern: transiently activating to salient external task events, including in both posterior and anterior regions along the midline.
Hierarchical Organization of the Cerebral Cortex
Paul Flechsig (1, 129, 130) contributed the powerful but simple idea that the cerebral cortex develops sequentially, radiating outward from motor and sensory cortex (see Refs. 171–174 for translations and discussion; see Ref. 175 for further context). The basis of Flechsig’s hierarchy was the developmental timing of myelination of the fibers reaching the cortex. By his account “in the cerebral convolutions, as in all other parts of the central nervous system, the nerve-fibers do not develop everywhere simultaneously, but step by step in a definitive succession” [translated in Clarke and O’Malley (173), p. 548]. The cortical motor and sensory (and certain limbic) zones myelinate first. Next are the intermediate zones that surround the motor and sensory zones. The terminal zones myelinate in the final stage, beginning ∼4 mo after birth, and encompass prefrontal, temporal, and parietal regions thought of today as higher-order association cortex. The prescient lens of hierarchical cortical organization provides a framework to understand our findings.
Specifically, the candidate assignments of first-, second-, and third-order networks6 are motivated by (and agree well with) Flechsig’s reference maps of sequential myelination (Fig. 36). In particular, the distributed regions late to myelinate (the terminal zones) are positionally similar to our estimated association zones containing the five higher-order networks that make up the SAAMs. These same general zones were emphasized more than a century ago as the regions distinguishing human and ape brains from those of smaller monkeys (174) and have been supported, based on modern comparative anatomical approaches, to be disproportionately expanded in humans relative to monkeys (137, 176–178). Taken together, the global spatial relations among networks (Fig. 17, Fig. 18, and Fig. 19) and the repeating fractionation of the higher-order association zones into five networks (Fig. 27 and Fig. 28) are consistent with processes that organize the cortex through distinct developmental stages.
Figure 36.
Hierarchical development might give rise to network patterning. Top: an illustration of Paul Flechsig’s maps of sequential myelination. This map is based on Refs. 1, 170 and also the composite in Bailey and von Bonin (171). The numbers reflect Flechsig’s estimates of the temporal ordering of myelination. Blue stippled areas receive projections that myelinate first (before birth), green striped areas next (during the first months after birth), and the reddish-brown areas last (starting several months after birth). Bottom: the present network estimates from a representative participant (P1) are recolored and grouped into first-, second-, and third-order networks to align to Flechsig’s maps. Note the similarity between the global spatial patterns and the locations of the distributed association third-order network zones and Flechsig’s zones of late-myelinating, terminal fibers.
In a hypothesized first stage, cortical networks might progressively organize from the early sensory and motor areas that themselves are patterned through structured inputs. For example, retinotopic organization is imparted on early visual cortex via spontaneous retinal activity waves that are present before birth and carried to the cortex through the thalamic nuclei (179, 180). These early organizing events may anchor the formation of the retinotopic clusters (122, 181) captured by our estimates of the VIS-C and VIS-P networks. The second-order networks may then organize tethered to these first-order networks, but with progressively more distributed regions, corresponding to Flechsig’s intermediate (or border) zones. Averbeck and colleagues have also proposed a similar nesting of networks outward from the primary motor and somatosensory areas (S1-M1) based on extensive analyses on anatomical connectivity patterns (Refs. 13, 182; see also Refs. 36, 183). The zones that generally fall between the regions of the second-order networks include large swaths of prefrontal, temporal, and posterior parietal association cortex that correspond to Flechsig’s terminal (or central) zones and are hypothesized to be the last to develop, forming our hypothesized third-order networks (Fig. 36). Thus, much of the cortical mantle may be patterned by a series of networks that nest outward from the primary cortical areas (72, 160, 184).
In a second developmental stage, we hypothesize that, as the networks sequentially form, they may undergo a second process of fractionation and specialization (137). Our proposal of a distinct second process is specifically put forth to explain how juxtapositions might arise similarly across widely distributed (noncontiguous) zones of cortex, such as observed for the distinct SAAMs in LTC and PPC (Fig. 28). A specific prediction of this hypothesis is that, as development progresses, activity-dependent processes may eliminate and/or stabilize synapses that support specialization evident in the adult (for discussion see Refs. 185–188). In the cortical mantle of humans, the expanded association zones may fractionate and specialize into the multiple juxtaposed networks that support higher-order cognition.
Functional Specialization of Higher-Order Association Networks
By combining network estimation and task-based fMRI within the same individuals, the present results provide insight into the functional specialization of the networks. A broad observation was that the second-order distributed networks SAL/PMN and CG-OP were dissociated from the third-order association networks via their robust, transient response to oddballs (Fig. 25) consistent with prior studies (91, 92, 104). None of the third-order networks that populate the SAAMs displayed a robust transient positive response. In fact, four of the five networks within the SAAMs (FPN-B, LANG, DN-B, and DN-A) showed a significant negative response (network FPN-A was equivocal). By contrast, all of the third-order association networks responded robustly to ongoing task demands with distinct forms of functional specialization as described below.
A first robust dissociation among the third-order networks came in their differential response to working memory demands. FPN-A in particular responded to high memory load in the N-Back Load Effect task contrast and did so similarly across verbal and nonverbal materials (Fig. 31). FPN-B’s response was quantitatively lower and may reflect spatial blur from FPN-A (Fig. 35).8 Furthermore, FPN-A displays the same general spatial pattern as the previously described multiple-demand network (126, 191–195). Our data are thus convergent with the existing literature to suggest there is a distributed frontal-parietal network (or networks) that responds when tasks become more effortful, perhaps related to processing functions associated with cognitive control (e.g., Refs. 194, 196). The within-individual precision mapping allowed spatially precise network estimates to be made of FPN-A that predicted the idiosyncratic response patterns across participants.
A few further details are of interest. First, across most individuals, FPN-A included a small region in the anterior insula (labeled in Fig. 29). This small region showed a N-Back load effect response surrounded by spatially distinct components of the CG-OP and SAL/PMN networks. Although separations are shown in some group-averaging parcellations (24, 59, 71), it can be easy to blur over or miss this buried insular region in group analyses. Our present estimates suggest that the anterior insula is a particularly challenging region of the cortex to study because multiple, distinct networks are spatially juxtaposed near where the cortex folds onto itself in the volume. Examinations of group data, especially data that average across participants within the volume, may be particularly vulnerable to distorting functional properties of this region.
Second, the spatially circumscribed regions of each SAAM that aligned to FPN-A, with some exceptions in low-SNR regions, tended to show a robust N-Back load effect (Fig. 29 and Fig. 30). The adjacent regions of LANG, DN-B, and DN-A did not. Thus, the N-Back load effect functionally dissociated FPN-A from the LANG, DN-B, DN-A cluster multiple times across the distributed association zones including PPC, LTC, VLPFC, and DLPFC.
Fedorenko and colleagues (52, 53) have previously noted that regions of the multiple-demand network lie side by side with functionally distinct domain-selective regions (specifically in the language domain). Our present results are consistent with their observations and reinforce that the functional dissociation is a general property of the association cortex including close spatial juxtapositions in temporal and parietal cortical association zones, not only within PFC. We interpret the repeating pattern across the cortical mantle to reflect that functional specialization is a property of the networks, including all their distributed regions (see also Ref. 54). Furthermore, robust functional dissociations were present for higher-order cognitive domains beyond language (see also Ref. 68). That is, although networks LANG, DN-B, and DN-A did not modulate in a domain-flexible manner to working memory demands, each network responded robustly and selectively to a distinct specialized domain of higher-order cognition.
The most striking functional observation of the present study was the robust triple dissociation across networks LANG, DN-B, and DN-A, as predicted by DiNicola and Buckner (137). The LANG network responded when participants processed meaningful sentences; the DN-B network when participants engaged Theory-of-Mind tasks; and the DN-A network when participants remembered from their past or contemplated a personal future scenario. The triple dissociation was carried by a formal statistical interaction (Fig. 34) and could be visualized qualitatively on the flat maps of individual participants (Fig. 32 and Fig. 33; see also Refs. 63, 90). Considering that until recently we and others conceptualized these zones of association cortex as being deployed flexibly across a range of higher-order cognitive domains (e.g., Refs. 29, 197, 198), this is a major revision to our understanding.
Our composite results suggest that higher-order association cortex possesses at least three domain-specialized parallel networks supporting language, social behaviors, and remembering the past and imagining the future. These domain-specialized networks are themselves separate from domain-flexible networks that participate in cognitive control. We do not know how these networks interact or whether they remain functionally separate across multiple task classes, but the robust dissociations among juxtaposed regions demonstrated here suggest that there is more modularity in association cortex, including PFC, than has typically been considered.
Limitations and Future Directions
A key limitation of the present work is the reliance on correlational, indirect methods to infer network organization. The caveats surrounding interpreting such network estimates, and the empirical tests of their utility despite known limitations, are discussed elsewhere (15–20, 75). Specific to the present work, it is notable that the boundaries in networks generally predicted task response patterns, bolstering confidence that valid organizational features are being described. However, there were exceptions and regions of mismatch, consistent with poor signal quality around the sinuses and inner ear (see Fig. 1 and Fig. A3). Network estimates in these poorly sampled regions of cortex may be distorted.
There are also limitations to our modeling approach. In choosing the present parameters of the MS-HBM used to estimate networks, decisions were made that influence the estimates. Specifically, we choose to model 15 networks and initiated the model with a prior that arose from a group-averaged dataset. As the seed region analyses verified, the model captured within-individual correlational properties well, but not perfectly. Thus, a limitation in our present model is knowing whether one could do better and whether our specific decisions imparted bias. We assume the answer is yes to both questions. As our own work has evolved from a relatively crude 7-network estimate in average participant groups (23) to a 10-network estimate within individuals (75), we expect the that the present network estimates will be refined further and eventually replaced. As a specific example, it is unclear that the present model fully captures the details of the recently described intereffector connectivity pattern (67). The structured correlations they observed, and we also find, are partially incorporated in our estimate of the CG-OP network (Fig. 22) but not entirely (e.g., see diamond in Fig. 20). Placing seed regions within the body map gap in the SMOT-B network reproduces the intereffector regions as well as the distributed pattern of the CG-OP network (Fig. 22), indicating that the spatial prior of the CG-OP network5 might benefit from revision in a future iteration. With the expectation of further improvements in mind, we are struck by how the present network parcellation captures multiple functional dissociations prospectively in task data, including idiosyncratic and small regions of response.
Another limitation is that, although the task contrasts allowed for robust functional dissociations, the tasks were designed and implemented to differentiate networks, which is a different goal from interrogating in detail a hypothesized cognitive operation. That is, the limited task data we collected fall far short of systematically manipulating variables to clarify the component computations performed by each of the networks. As an example of a task domain needing further exploration, the Oddball Effect task contrast maps were fairly broad, spanning both the CG-OP and SAL/PMN networks and extending beyond them. One possibility is that we have not yet found the right task demands to dissociate the two networks. Another possibility is that the two networks do not respond differentially. The CG-OP and SAL/PMN networks may respond together to common task events linked to surprise and novelty. Within this possibility, rather than being distinguished by what they respond to, they may be distinguished by their anatomical interactions with other networks. CG-OP and SAL/PMN are both distributed networks that are near one another in many zones of cortex, but they are also adjacent to distinct networks in other portions of cortex. That is, CG-OP and SAL/PMN may both be networks that respond to transient orienting events but are ultimately functionally distinguished by interactions with their extended partner regions. The present work did not explore or test such possibilities.
Another future direction pertains to the need to better understand the relation of traditional area estimates with the present network estimates. By “area” we mean the demarcation of regions of cortex as separate, defined zones using functional, architectonic, connectivity, and topography constraints (199, 200). We previously noted discrepancies between functional connectivity patterns and areal boundaries (e.g., Refs. 23, 93, 123), as have others (e.g., Ref. 201). There are two topics to be considered.
First, for regions of cortex that have well-recognized areas, our network borders do not align with the areal borders (e.g., Fig. A13E). For example, networks VIS-C and VIS-P group together V1, V2, and V3 and split them roughly along the 5° eccentricity line. The estimated networks likely reflect the dominant anatomical connectivity gradient that, within early visual areas, progresses along eccentricity (202–204). The V1/V2/V3 areal boundaries are distinguished by a local reversal in polar angle along the horizontal meridian. Thus, connectivity transitions between early visual areas are relatively subtle [for further discussion of this issue see Buckner and Yeo (123)]. The somatomotor networks similarly group M1/S1 and multiple body maps that span architectonically distinct areas (205). One future direction is to understand the relation of the networks estimated here and the finer-scale anatomical differences that demarcate adjacent areas.
The second related topic is the relation between the present network estimates and architectonic features in higher-order association cortex. This is a trickier topic. Varied perspectives have been put forth on whether association cortex possesses sharp areal boundaries that parallel those found in sensory systems (for discussion see Refs. 93, 171, 181, 206). There is also an open question of whether, in practice, there are known stable features that can define areal borders in association cortex, especially when architectonics are considered in isolation (e.g., Refs. 171, 207). We will not resolve the debate here, but some of our observations are relevant to the discussion.
Most critically, the extent and complexity of the network juxtapositions encompassed within the SAAMs are of such a spatial scale that they seem unlikely to align to traditional architectonic borders, at least those reflected in any of the commonly used atlases. In the spirit of supra-areal clusters reported in the visual system (see Refs. 123, 208; see also Refs. 122, 181), we refer to the repeated groupings of multiple association networks as supra-areal association megaclusters specifically to reinforce the possibility that they might span and split traditional architectonic patterns. One possibility is that future advances will find architectonic features that align to the transitions between SAAMs as well as between the multiple network regions within the SAAMs (perhaps via spatial transcriptomics; Ref. 209). Alternatively, there may be broad patterning forces during development, such as captured in Flechsig’s maps of sequential myelination, that reflect processes that guide where SAAMs develop but that do not specify the details of the borders and regional specializations within the SAAMs. The local spatial arrangements might be carried by extrinsic anatomical connectivity differences that refine relatively late in development through activity-dependent processes, without rigid alignment to architectonic features (93, 137).
Data to inform these and other possibilities will emerge as the field charts development of association networks in nonhuman primates with direct anatomical techniques and in human infants with noninvasive approaches.
Conclusions
The present study examined the organization of cerebral networks within intensively sampled individual participants. We provide the resulting network estimates and the raw data used to derive them as open resources for the community. Our initial explorations on the data uncovered a hierarchical organization that distinguishes three levels of cortical hierarchy: first-, second-, and third-order networks. Notably, regions of distinct third-order association networks consistently displayed side-by-side juxtapositions that repeated across multiple cortical zones, with clear and robust functional specialization among the embedded regions.
DATA AVAILABILITY
The DU15NET atlases are available (https://freesurfer.net/fswiki/CorticalParcellation_DU15NET). Additional parcellation, SNR, and task maps related to this manuscript are available on Balsa (https://balsa.wustl.edu/study/zK166). Task descriptions, contrast descriptions, and code are provided on Harvard Dataverse (https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/AVB4BW). Individual participant data are available in the NIH repository (https://nda.nih.gov).
SUPPLEMENTAL MATERIALS
Supplemental Figs. 1–18: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.24990657.v2.
Supplemental Figs. 19–227: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.24990663.v2.
GRANTS
This work was supported by NIH Grant MH124004, NIH Shared Instrumentation Grant S10OD020039, and National Science Foundation (NSF) Grant 2024462. L.M.D. was supported by NSF Graduate Research Fellowship Program Grant DGE1745303. W.S. was supported by the Paul and Daisy Soros Foundation.
DISCLAIMERS
Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Science Foundation.
DISCLOSURES
No conflicts of interest, financial or otherwise, are declared by the authors.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
J.D., L.M.D., P.A.A., N.S.-G., W.S., M.C.E., and R.L.B. conceived and designed research; J.D., L.M.D., P.A.A., N.S.-G., and R.L.B. analyzed data; J.D., N.S.-G., W.S., S.K., J.L., and R.L.B. prepared figures; J.D., L.M.D., P.A.A., N.S.-G., and R.L.B. interpreted results of experiments; J.D. and R.L.B. drafted manuscript; J.D., L.M.D., P.A.A., N.S.-G., W.S., S.K., J.L., A.X., B.T.T.Y., M.C.E., and R.L.B. edited and revised manuscript; J.D., L.M.D., P.A.A., N.S.-G., W.S., S.K., J.L., A.X., B.T.T.Y., M.C.E., and R.L.B. approved the final version of the manuscript.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
We thank the Harvard Center for Brain Science neuroimaging core and FAS Division of Research Computing. We thank T. O’Keefe for assisting in optimization of data processing and R. Mair for MRI physics support. H. Kosakowski and R. Braga provided valuable discussion. K. Ntoh and F. Davy-Falconi assisted with data acquisition. E. Fedorenko, T. Konkle, and R. Saxe generously provided task stimuli. The multiband EPI sequence was generously provided by the Center for Magnetic Resonance Research (CMRR) at the University of Minnesota.
APPENDIX
Figures A1–A16 appear below. These figures provide detailed views of the validation checks that establish the MS-HBM (Figs. A1–A12) as well as illustrating how the task maps were constructed (Figs. A13–A16).
Figure A1.
Fifteen-network cerebral cortical parcellation estimated for participant S1. Network estimates from the multisession hierarchical Bayesian model (MS-HBM) are displayed across 4 views for participant S1. The left hemisphere is at top and right hemisphere at bottom. Each color represents a distinct network estimated by the model. Some networks possess primarily local organization (e.g., somatomotor, visual), whereas other networks possess widely distributed organization (e.g., those involving prefrontal, temporal, and parietal association zones). The network labels are used similarly throughout the figures. SMOT-A, Somatomotor-A; SMOT-B, Somatomotor-B; PM-PPr, Premotor-Posterior Parietal Rostral; CG-OP, Cingulo-Opercular; SAL/PMN, Salience/Parietal Memory Network; dATN-A, Dorsal Attention-A; dATN-B, Dorsal Attention-B; FPN-A, Frontoparietal Network-A; FPN-B, Frontoparietal Network-B; DN-A, Default Network-A; DN-B, Default Network-B; LANG, Language; VIS-C, Visual Central; VIS-P, Visual Peripheral; AUD, Auditory.
Figure A2.
Model-free confirmation of networks using seed region correlation for participant S1. The correlation patterns from individual seed regions placed within networks are displayed. In each row, a distinct network is targeted, labeled on left. Left: correlation maps using an anterior seed region of each network. Right: correlation maps using a posterior seed region. Lateral and medial views are displayed. White-filled circles display the seed region locations. Black outlines show the boundaries of individual-specific networks estimated from the multisession hierarchical Bayesian model (MS-HBM) as shown in Fig. A1. The correlation maps are plotted as z(r), with the color scale at bottom. The correlation maps are not constrained to fall within the estimated network boundaries. Nonetheless, the network boundaries capture a great deal of the spatial correlational properties of the underlying data.
Figure A3.
Temporal signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) map for participant S2. Paralleling Fig. 1, the mean estimate of temporal SNR for the functional MRI (fMRI) data is illustrated for multiple views of the left hemisphere on the inflated cortical surface (from 61 runs collected over 31 days). A, anterior; D, dorsal; P, posterior; V, ventral.
Figure A4.
Fifteen-network cerebral cortical parcellation estimated for participant S2. Paralleling Fig. A1, network estimates from the multisession hierarchical Bayesian model (MS-HBM) are displayed across 4 views for S2. The left hemisphere is at top and right hemisphere at bottom. Each color represents a distinct network estimated by the model. The names of cortical networks are shown at bottom. SMOT-A, Somatomotor-A; SMOT-B, Somatomotor-B; PM-PPr, Premotor-Posterior Parietal Rostral; CG-OP, Cingulo-Opercular; SAL/PMN, Salience/Parietal Memory Network; dATN-A, Dorsal Attention-A; dATN-B, Dorsal Attention-B; FPN-A, Frontoparietal Network-A; FPN-B, Frontoparietal Network-B; DN-A, Default Network-A; DN-B, Default Network-B; LANG, Language; VIS-C, Visual Central; VIS-P, Visual Peripheral; AUD, Auditory.
Figure A5.
Model-free confirmation of networks using seed region correlation for participant S2. Paralleling Fig. A2, the correlation patterns from individual seed regions placed within networks are displayed for S2. Left: correlation maps using an anterior seed region for each network. Right: correlation maps using a posterior seed region. Lateral and medial views are displayed for each seed region. White-filled circles display the seed region locations. Black outlines indicate the boundaries of corresponding individual-specific parcellation-defined networks estimated from the multisession hierarchical Bayesian model (MS-HBM) as shown in Fig. A4. The correlation maps are plotted as z(r), with the color scale at the bottom.
Figure A6.
Direct comparison of 10-network and 15-network cerebral cortical parcellations for participant S1. Left: the 10-network estimate. Right: the 15-network estimate. Many of the major networks are similar between the two parcellations, including Language (LANG), Default Network-A (DN-A), Default Network-B (DN-B), Frontoparietal Network-A (FPN-A), Frontoparietal Network-B (FPN-B), Somatomotor-A (SMOT-A), and Somatomotor-B (SMOT-B). Visual (VIS) network in the 10-network estimate is differentiated into Dorsal Attention-B (dATN-B), Visual Central (VIS-C), and Visual Peripheral (VIS-P) networks in the 15-network estimate. A monolithic large network in the 10-network estimate is differentiated into Salience/Parietal Memory Network (SAL/PMN) and Cingulo-Opercular (CG-OP) networks in the 15-network estimate. Dorsal Attention (dATN) network in the 10-network estimate is differentiated into Dorsal Attention-A (dATN-A) and Premotor-Posterior Parietal Rostral (PM-PPr) networks in the 15-network estimate, and a distinct Auditory (AUD) network emerges near LANG and SMOT-B. The network labels are shown at bottom.
Figure A7.
Model-free estimates illustrate the utility of the 15-network cerebral parcellation for visual networks for participant S1. Seed region correlation maps illustrate features captured by the 15-network estimate in contrast to the 10-network estimate. Visual (VIS) network in the 10-network estimate (A) is differentiated into Dorsal Attention-B (dATN-B), Visual Central (VIS-C), and Visual Peripheral (VIS-P) networks in the 15-network estimate (E). White-filled circles display the seed region locations. Black outlines indicate the boundaries of the networks. The network labels are shown at bottom. Correlation maps for 3 distinct seed regions in and around the vicinity of visual cortex are illustrated within the boundaries of the 10-network estimate (B–D) and the 15-network estimate (F–H). Note that the correlation patterns are well captured by the 15-network estimate. Black and gray outlines illustrate the networks from each parcellation estimate. The correlation maps are plotted as z(r), with the color scale at bottom.
Figure A8.
Model-free estimates illustrate the utility of the 15-network cerebral parcellation for networks surrounding somatomotor cortex for participant S1. Paralleling Fig. A7, seed region correlation maps illustrate features captured by the 15-network estimate in contrast to the 10-network estimate. Dorsal Attention (dATN) in the 10-network estimate (A) is differentiated into Dorsal Attention-A (dATN-A) and Premotor-Posterior Parietal Rostral (PM-PPr) in the 15-network estimate (E). White-filled circles display the seed region locations. Black outlines indicate the boundaries of the networks. The network labels are shown at bottom. Correlation maps for 3 distinct seed regions surrounding somatomotor cortex are illustrated within the boundaries of the 10-network estimate (B–D) and the 15-network estimate (F–H). Black and gray outlines illustrate the networks from each parcellation estimate. The correlation maps are plotted as z(r), with the color scale at bottom.
Figure A9.
Direct comparison of 10-network and 15-network cerebral cortical parcellations for participant S2. Paralleling Fig. A6, the 10-network estimate (left) and the 15-network estimate (right) are shown. The network labels are shown at bottom. SMOT-A, Somatomotor-A; SMOT-B, Somatomotor-B; PM-PPr, Premotor-Posterior Parietal Rostral; CG-OP, Cingulo-Opercular; SAL/PMN, Salience/Parietal Memory Network; dATN-A, Dorsal Attention-A; dATN-B, Dorsal Attention-B; FPN-A, Frontoparietal Network-A; FPN-B, Frontoparietal Network-B; DN-A, Default Network-A; DN-B, Default Network-B; LANG, Language; VIS-C, Visual Central; VIS-P, Visual Peripheral; AUD, Auditory.
Figure A10.
Model-free estimates illustrate the utility of the 15-network cerebral parcellation for auditory and language networks for participant S2. Seed region correlation maps illustrate features captured by the 15-network estimate in contrast to the 10-network estimate. Language (LANG) in the 10-network estimate (A) is differentiated into Auditory (AUD) and Language (LANG) in the 15-network estimate (E). White-filled circles display the seed region locations. Black outlines indicate the boundaries of the networks. The network labels are shown at bottom. Correlation maps for 3 distinct seed regions in and around the vicinity of auditory cortex are illustrated within the boundaries of the 10-network estimate (B–D) and the 15-network estimate (F–H). Black and gray outlines illustrate the networks from each parcellation estimate. The correlation maps are plotted as z(r), with the color scale at bottom.
Figure A11.
Model-free estimates illustrate the utility of the 15-network cerebral parcellation for networks at and around cingulate cortex for participant S2. Paralleling Fig. A10, seed region maps illustrate features captured by the present 15-network estimate in contrast to the 10-network estimate. Salience (SAL) network in the 10-network estimate (A) is differentiated into the Salience/Parietal Memory Network (SAL/PMN) and Cingulo-Opercular (CG-OP) networks in the 15-network estimate (E). White-filled circles display the seed region locations. Black outlines indicate the boundaries of the networks. Correlation maps for 3 seed regions around the cingulate are illustrated within the boundaries of the 10-network estimate (B–D) and the 15-network estimate (F–H). Black and gray outlines illustrate the networks from each parcellation estimate. The correlation maps are plotted as z(r), with the color scale at bottom.
Figure A12.
Overlap of network estimates derived from model-free seed region correlation maps. Each row displays the overlap map from 1 target network for the full set of 15 novel participants using only seed region-based correlation estimates of the networks. Left: the overlap map of correlation patterns based on an anterior seed region. Right: the overlap map based on a posterior seed region. The network targets are labeled on left. Default Network-A (DN-A), Default Network-B (DN-B), Language (LANG), Frontoparietal Network-A (FPN-A), Frontoparietal Network-B (FPN-B), Cingulo-Opercular (CG-OP), and Salience/Parietal Memory Network (SAL/PMN) networks are examined separately. The purpose of these maps is to illustrate the overlap of network organization without strong model assumptions (priors) that might bias the degree of overlap.
Figure A13.
Strategy for exploring somatomotor and visual task responses in relation to networks. Steps employed to generate a combined motor movement and visual stimulation map for a representative participant (P6) are illustrated. A: the within-individual a priori-defined somatomotor networks SMOT-A and SMOT-B (blue colors) and visual networks VIS-C and VIS-P (purple colors) are displayed on the flattened cortical surface. Thin colored outlines mark the boundaries of all other networks. B: the borders of SMOT-A, SMOT-B, VIS-C, and VIS-P are isolated as black outlines. C: the task contrasts of right vs. left foot movements (red) and right vs. left hand movements (blue) are mapped in relation to the network boundaries. Presentation of the hand and foot representations in isolation allows visualization of 3 separate candidate body maps (labeled I–III). The thresholds are z > 2.13 in all cases. D: binarized motor task contrast maps combine the foot (red), hand (blue), tongue (yellow), and glute (green) movements. Note how adding body parts fills in much of the remaining cortical regions within the somatomotor networks. The thresholds are z > 2.13 in all cases. E: the task contrast of horizontal vs. vertical meridian visual stimulation is mapped in relation to the network boundaries to illustrate that multiple areas fall within the VIS-C and VIS-P networks. The thresholds are z < −2.86 and z > 3.16. F: binarized visual task contrast maps combine the center vs. the other apertures (red), the middle vs. other apertures (green), and the peripheral vs. other apertures (blue). The threshold is z > 4.15. For display purposes, the binarized maps from D and F were combined to yield a combined map of somatomotor topography along the body axis and visual topography along the eccentricity gradient.
Figure A14.
Strategy for exploring responses to oddball detection in relation to networks. Steps employed to generate a map of the oddball effect for a representative participant (P2) are illustrated. A: the within-individual a priori-defined Cingulo-Opercular (CG-OP), and Salience/Parietal Memory Network (SAL/PMN) networks are displayed on the flattened cortical surface. Thin colored outlines mark the boundaries of all other networks. B: the borders of CG-OP and SAL/PMN are isolated as black outlines. C: the task contrast of oddball event detection vs. nontargets, labeled the oddball effect, is mapped in relation to the network boundaries. D: the binarized Oddball Effect task contrast map is shown in pink. The threshold is z > 1.70.
Figure A15.
Strategy for exploring responses to high working memory load in relation to networks. Steps employed to generate a map of the N-Back load effect for a representative participant (P14) are illustrated. A: the within-individual a priori-defined Frontoparietal Network-A (FPN-A) and Frontoparietal Network-B (FPN-B) networks (orange and yellow colors) are displayed on the flattened cortical surface. Thin colored outlines mark the boundaries of all other networks. B: the borders of FPN-A and FPN-B are isolated as black outlines. C: the task contrast of 2-Back (high load) vs. 0-Back (0-Back), labeled the N-Back load effect (red/yellow), is mapped in relation to the network boundaries. D: the binarized N-Back Load Effect task contrast map is shown in yellow. The threshold is z > 3.00.
Figure A16.
Strategy for exploring domain-preferential higher-order responses in relation to networks. Steps employed to generate a combined map revealing domain-selective responses for a representative participant (P6) are illustrated. A: the within-individual a priori-defined Default Network-A (DN-A, dark red), Default Network-B (DN-B, light red), and Language (LANG, blue) networks are displayed on the flattened cortical surface. Thin colored outlines mark the boundaries of all other networks. B: the borders of DN-A, BN-B, and LANG are isolated as black outlines. C: the Episodic Projection task contrast (red/yellow) is mapped on its own in relation to the DN-A network boundary. D: the Theory-of-Mind task contrast (red/yellow) is mapped on its own in relation to the DN-B network boundary. E: the Sentence Processing task contrast (red/yellow) is mapped on its own in relation to the LANG network boundary. F: binarized task contrast maps are shown together (dark red, Episodic Projection; light red, Theory-of-Mind; blue, Sentence Processing). The threshold is z > 1.80. The combined, binarized map allows visualization of the multiple functional domains in the same view.
Footnotes
We term these networks somatomotor (SMOT) networks because each comprises both somatosensory cortex and motor cortex.
Network labels use conventions that often reflect historical origins and diverge from current understanding. For example, the canonical default network, originally identified in group‐based positron emission tomography (PET) data, is now postulated to comprise multiple, distinct networks [see Buckner and DiNicola (93) for review]. The names DN‐A and DN‐B reflect the historical naming convention modified to the current understanding of multiple networks. As another example, the network labeled here as SAL/PMN has two distinct origins. Seeley et al. (91) referred to the network as the salience network and Gilmore et al. (94) as the parietal memory network. Ideas about network organization and function are continuously evolving, whereas the labels often reflect historical (not contemporary) understanding.
The frontoparietal network (FPN) has been fractionated into distinct, parallel networks in multiple prior studies but has not been consistently named. Here we label FPN‐A and FPN‐B to be consistent with the order (A/B naming convention) of Kong et al. (71) and Xue et al. (75), who also applied an MS‐HBM to estimate networks. Other studies have used the reverse convention, which could lead to confusion [e.g., Braga et al. (63); DiNicola et al. (95)].
Pilot analyses were conducted to test whether an individual’s network estimate was influenced by the group in which the participant was analyzed. In our explorations, the individual’s parcellation was nearly identical whether the participant was grouped with one set of other individuals or another set. We do not assume this will always be the case, as our analyses were conducted for a group of healthy young adult participants with large amounts of data.
Gordon et al. (67) recently described a set of intereffector regions along the central sulcus, which was not explicitly incorporated into our group prior or model. Examining the details of Figs. A2 and A5 shows that the correlation patterns from the seed regions placed within the CG‐OP extend farther into the precentral gyrus than the MS‐HBM‐defined CG‐OP network regions themselves. As revealed in analyses below, placing seed regions where the CG‐OP extends into the precentral sulcus yields the intereffector correlation pattern, confirming discontinuities in the body map as discovered by Gordon et al. (67).
Any heuristic framework will necessarily emphasize certain features of organization and deemphasize others. A three‐level hierarchy, while not capturing local features of organization, is a useful framework to emphasize aspects of global organization that are the focus of this article and, as discussed below, converges with similar frameworks that have arisen from direct anatomical description (e.g., Refs. 129, 130). Alternative organizational schemes are possible, and the three‐level hierarchy proposed should be viewed as an orienting framework.
Although the higher‐order association networks were generally positioned between the second‐order networks, an exception to that pattern is that the LANG network juxtaposes the AUD network, without second‐order networks interdigitated. It is unclear whether this is a true exception or there are local organizational details that are not resolved by our current methods.
We do not yet interpret the differential response between FPN‐A and FPN‐B, as there was no condition where FPN‐B responded more than FPN‐A, and the maps suggest FPN‐B may not directly show a response (Fig. 35). Main effects in BOLD response magnitude between regions can come from any number of technical reasons including the regional vasculature sampled, the inclusion of voxels with susceptibility artifact, and spatial blur. We thus conservatively interpret differential responses when there is direct evidence for a double dissociation within or across task contrasts (following the logic of Refs. 189, 190).
REFERENCES
- 1. Flechsig P. Developmental (myelogenetic) localisation of the cerebral cortex in the human subject. Lancet 158: 1027–1030, 1901. [Google Scholar]
- 2. Campbell AW. Histological Studies on the Localisation of Cerebral Function. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 1905. [Google Scholar]
- 3. Brodmann K. Brodmann’s Localisation in the Cerebral Cortex, trans. by Garey LJ. New York: Springer, 1909/2006. [Google Scholar]
- 4. von Economo C, Koskinas GN. Atlas of Cytoarchitectonics of the Adult Human Cerebral Cortex, trans. by Triarhou LC. Basel: Karger, 1925/2008. [Google Scholar]
- 5. von Bonin G, Bailey P. The Neocortex of Macaca mulatta. (Illinois Monogr med Sci 5, No. 4). Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press, 1947. [Google Scholar]
- 6. Geschwind N. Disconnexion syndromes in animals and man. Part 1. Brain 88: 237–294, 1965. doi: 10.1093/brain/88.2.237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Ungerleider LG, Desimone R. Cortical connections of visual area MT in the macaque. J Comp Neurol 248: 190–222, 1986. doi: 10.1002/cne.902480204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Goldman-Rakic PS. Topography of cognition: parallel distributed networks in primate association cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci 11: 137–156, 1988. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.11.030188.001033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Mesulam MM. Large‐scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory. Ann Neurol 28: 597–613, 1990. doi: 10.1002/ana.410280502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Mesulam MM. From sensation to cognition. Brain 121: 1013–1052, 1998. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.6.1013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Van Essen DC, Anderson CH, Felleman DJ. Information processing in the primate visual system: an integrated systems perspective. Science 255: 419–423, 1992. doi: 10.1126/science.1734518. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Pandya D, Seltzer B, Petrides M, Cipolloni PB. Cerebral Cortex: Architecture, Connections, and the Dual Origin Concept. New York: Oxford University Press, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 13. Giarrocco F, Averbeck BB. Anatomical organization of forebrain circuits in the primate. Brain Struct Funct 228: 393–411, 2023. doi: 10.1007/s00429-022-02586-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Biswal B, Yetkin FZ, Haughton VM, Hyde JS. Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo‐planar MRI. Magn Reson Med 34: 537–541, 1995. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Fox MD, Raichle ME. Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat Rev Neurosci 8: 700–711, 2007. doi: 10.1038/nrn2201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Van Dijk KR, Hedden T, Venkataraman A, Evans KC, Lazar SW, Buckner RL. Intrinsic functional connectivity as a tool for human connectomics: theory, properties, and optimization. J Neurophysiol 103: 297–321, 2010. doi: 10.1152/jn.00783.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Murphy K, Birn RM, Bandettini PA. Resting-state fMRI confounds and cleanup. Neuroimage 80: 349–359, 2013. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Smith SM, Vidaurre D, Beckmann CF, Glasser MF, Jenkinson M, Miller KL, Nichols TE, Robinson EC, Salimi-Khorshidi G, Woolrich MW, Barch DM, Uğurbil K, Van Essen DC. Functional connectomics from resting-state fMRI. Trends Cogn Sci 17: 666–682, 2013. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Power JD, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Studying brain organization via spontaneous fMRI signal. Neuron 84: 681–696, 2014. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.09.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Buckner RL, Krienen FM, Yeo BTT. Opportunities and limitations of intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Nat Neurosci 16: 832–837, 2013. doi: 10.1038/nn.3423. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Beckmann CF, DeLuca M, Devlin JT, Smith SM. Investigations into resting-state connectivity using independent component analysis. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 360: 1001–1013, 2005. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Damoiseaux JS, Rombouts SARB, Barkhof F, Scheltens P, Stam CJ, Smith SM, Beckmann CF. Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103: 13848–13853, 2006. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601417103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Yeo BTT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Lashkari D, Hollinshead M, Roffman JL, Smoller JW, Zöllei L, Polimeni JR, Fischl B, Liu H, Buckner RL. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol 106: 1125–1165, 2011. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Power JD, Cohen AL, Nelson SM, Wig GS, Barnes KA, Church JA, Vogel AC, Laumann TO, Miezin FM, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron 72: 665–678, 2011. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Doucet G, Naveau M, Petit L, Delcroix N, Zago L, Crivello F, Jobard G, Tzourio-Mazoyer N, Mazoyer B, Mellet E, Joliot M. Brain activity at rest: a multiscale hierarchical functional organization. J Neurophysiol 105: 2753–2763, 2011. doi: 10.1152/jn.00895.2010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Miller KL, Alfaro-Almagro F, Bangerter NK, Thomas DL, Yacoub E, Xu J, Bartsch AJ, Jbabdi S, Sotiropoulos SN, Andersson JL, Griffanti L, Douaud G, Okell TW, Weale P, Dragonu I, Garratt S, Hudson S, Collins R, Jenkinson M, Matthews PM, Smith SM. Multimodal population brain imaging in the UK Biobank prospective epidemiological study. Nat Neurosci 19: 1523–1536, 2016. doi: 10.1038/nn.4393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Glasser MF, Coalson TS, Robinson EC, Hacker CD, Harwell J, Yacoub E, Ugurbil K, Andersson J, Beckmann CF, Jenkinson M, Smith SM, Van Essen DC. A multi-modal parcellation of human cerebral cortex. Nature 536: 171–178, 2016. doi: 10.1038/nature18933. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Vincent JL, Patel GH, Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Baker JT, Van Essen DC, Zempel JM, Snyder LH, Corbetta M, Raichle ME. Intrinsic functional architecture in the anaesthetized monkey brain. Nature 447: 83–86, 2007. doi: 10.1038/nature05758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Buckner RL, Andrews‐Hanna JR, Schacter DL. The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1124: 1–38, 2008. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Binder JR, Desai RH, Graves WW, Conant LL. Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. Cereb Cortex 19: 2767–2796, 2009. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Margulies DS, Vincent JL, Kelly C, Lohmann G, Uddin LQ, Biswal BB, Villringer A, Castellanos FX, Milham MP, Petrides M. Precuneus shares intrinsic functional architecture in humans and monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106: 20069–20074, 2009. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905314106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Hutchison RM, Gallivan JP, Culham JC, Gati JS, Menon RS, Everling S. Functional connectivity of the frontal eye fields in humans and macaque monkeys investigated with resting-state fMRI. J Neurophysiol 107: 2463–2474, 2012. doi: 10.1152/jn.00891.2011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Ghahremani M, Hutchison RM, Menon RS, Everling S. Frontoparietal functional connectivity in the common marmoset. Cereb Cortex 27: 3890–3905, 2017. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Liu C, Yen CC-C, Szczupak D, Frank QY, Leopold DA, Silva AC. Anatomical and functional investigation of the marmoset default mode network. Nat Commun 10: 1975–1978, 2019. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-09813-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Buckner RL, Margulies DS. Macroscale cortical organization and a default-like apex transmodal network in the marmoset monkey. Nat Commun 10: 1976, 2019. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-09812-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Du J, Buckner RL. Precision estimates of macroscale network organization in the human and their relation to anatomical connectivity in the marmoset monkey. Curr Opin Behav Sci 40: 144–152, 2021. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.04.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Amunts K, Schleicher A, Bürgel U, Mohlberg H, Uylings HB, Zilles K. Broca's region revisited: Cytoarchitecture and intersubject variability. J Comp Neurol 412: 319–341, 1999. doi:. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Amunts K, Malikovic A, Mohlberg H, Schormann T, Zilles K. Brodmann's areas 17 and 18 brought into stereotaxic space—where and how variable? Neuroimage 11: 66–84, 2000. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1999.0516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Amunts K, Mohlberg H, Bludau S, Zilles K. Julich-Brain: a 3D probabilistic atlas of the human brain’s cytoarchitecture. Science 369: 988–992, 2020. doi: 10.1126/science.abb4588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Rademacher J, Caviness VS Jr, Steinmetz H, Galaburda AM. Topographical variation of the human primary cortices: implications for neuroimaging, brain mapping, and neurobiology. Cereb Cortex 3: 313–329, 1993. doi: 10.1093/cercor/3.4.313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Rajkowska G, Goldman-Rakic PS. Cytoarchitectonic definition of prefrontal areas in the normal human cortex: II. Variability in locations of areas 9 and 46 and relationship to the Talairach coordinate system. Cereb Cortex 5: 323–337, 1995. doi: 10.1093/cercor/5.4.323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Caspers S, Geyer S, Schleicher A, Mohlberg H, Amunts K, Zilles K. The human inferior parietal cortex: Cytoarchitectonic parcellation and interindividual variability. Neuroimage 33: 430–448, 2006. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.06.054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Fischl B, Rajendran N, Busa E, Augustinack J, Hinds O, Yeo BTT, Mohlberg H, Amunts K, Zilles K. Cortical folding patterns and predicting cytoarchitecture. Cereb Cortex 18: 1973–1980, 2008. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Henssen A, Zilles K, Palomero-Gallagher N, Schleicher A, Mohlberg H, Gerboga F, Eickhoff SB, Bludau S, Amunts K. Cytoarchitecture and probability maps of the human medial orbitofrontal cortex. Cortex 75: 87–112, 2016. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Palomero-Gallagher N, Hoffstaedter F, Mohlberg H, Eickhoff SB, Amunts K, Zilles K. Human pregenual anterior cingulate cortex: Structural, functional, and connectional heterogeneity. Cereb Cortex 29: 2552–2574, 2019. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Sereno MI, Dale AM, Reppas JB, Kwong KK, Belliveau JW, Brady TJ, Rosen BR, Tootell RBH. Borders of multiple visual areas in humans revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Science 268: 889–893, 1995. doi: 10.1126/science.7754376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Rao SM, Binder JR, Hammeke TA, Bandettini PA, Bobholz JA, Frost JA, Myklebust BM, Jacobson RD, Hyde JS. Somatotopic mapping of the human primary motor cortex with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neurology 45: 919–924, 1995. doi: 10.1212/wnl.45.5.919. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48. DeYoe EA, Carman GJ, Bandettini P, Glickman S, Wieser J, Cox R, Miller D, Neitz J. Mapping striate and extrastriate visual areas in human cerebral cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 93: 2382–2386, 1996. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.6.2382. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Engel SA, Glover GH, Wandell BA. Retinotopic organization in human visual cortex and the spatial precision of functional MRI. Cereb Cortex 7: 181–192, 1997. doi: 10.1093/cercor/7.2.181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Kanwisher N, McDermott J, Chun MM. The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. J Neurosci 17: 4302–4311, 1997. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-11-04302.1997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Epstein R, Kanwisher N. A cortical representation of the local visual environment. Nature 392: 598–601, 1998. doi: 10.1038/33402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Fedorenko E, Hsieh PJ, Nieto-Castañón A, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Kanwisher N. New method for fMRI investigations of language: defining ROIs functionally in individual subjects. J Neurophysiol 104: 1177–1194, 2010. doi: 10.1152/jn.00032.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Fedorenko E, Duncan J, Kanwisher N. Language-selective and domain-general regions lie side by side within Broca’s area. Curr Biol 22: 2059–2062, 2012. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.09.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Blank I, Kanwisher N, Fedorenko E. A functional dissociation between language and multiple-demand systems revealed in patterns of BOLD signal fluctuations. J Neurophysiol 112: 1105–1118, 2014. doi: 10.1152/jn.00884.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Peer M, Salomon R, Goldberg I, Blanke O, Arzy S. Brain system for mental orientation in space, time, and person. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 112: 11072–11077, 2015. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1504242112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Michalka SW, Kong L, Rosen ML, Shinn-Cunningham BG, Somers DC. Short-term memory for space and time flexibly recruit complementary sensory-biased frontal lobe attention networks. Neuron 87: 882–892, 2015. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.07.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Huth AG, De Heer WA, Griffiths TL, Theunissen FE, Gallant JL. Natural speech reveals the semantic maps that tile human cerebral cortex. Nature 532: 453–458, 2016. doi: 10.1038/nature17637. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Poldrack RA, Laumann TO, Koyejo O, Gregory B, Hover A, Chen MY, , et al. Long-term neural and physiological phenotyping of a single human. Nat Commun 6: 8885, 2015. doi: 10.1038/ncomms9885. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Laumann TO, Gordon EM, Adeyemo B, Snyder AZ, Joo SJ, Chen MY, Gilmore AW, McDermott KB, Nelson SM, Dosenbach NUF, Schlaggar BL, Mumford JA, Poldrack RA, Petersen SE. Functional system and areal organization of a highly sampled individual human brain. Neuron 87: 657–670, 2015. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.06.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Braga RM, Buckner RL. Parallel interdigitated distributed networks within the individual estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. Neuron 95: 457–471.e5, 2017. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.06.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Gordon EM, Laumann TO, Gilmore AW, Newbold DJ, Greene DJ, Berg JJ, Ortega M, Hoyt-Drazen C, Gratton C, Sun H, Hampton JM, Coalson RS, Nguyen AL, McDermott KB, Shimony JS, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE, Nelson SM, Dosenbach NUF. Precision functional mapping of individual human brains. Neuron 95: 791–807.e7, 2017. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.07.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Braga RM, Van Dijk KR, Polimeni JR, Eldaief MC, Buckner RL. Parallel distributed networks resolved at high resolution reveal close juxtaposition of distinct regions. J Neurophysiol 121: 1513–1534, 2019. doi: 10.1152/jn.00808.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63. Braga RM, DiNicola LM, Becker HC, Buckner RL. Situating the left-lateralized language network in the broader organization of multiple specialized large-scale distributed networks. J Neurophysiol 124: 1415–1448, 2020. doi: 10.1152/jn.00753.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Smith DM, Perez DC, Porter A, Dworetsky A, Gratton C. Light through the fog: using precision fMRI data to disentangle the neural substrates of cognitive control. Curr Opin Behav Sci 40: 19–26, 2021. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Somers DC, Michalka SW, Tobyne SM, Noyce AL. Individual subject approaches to mapping sensory-biased and multiple-demand regions in human frontal cortex. Curr Opin Behav Sci 40: 169–177, 2021. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.05.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Noyce AL, Lefco RW, Brissenden JA, Tobyne SM, Shinn-Cunningham BG, Somers DC. Extended frontal networks for visual and auditory working memory. Cereb Cortex 32: 855–869, 2022. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab249. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Gordon EM, Chauvin RJ, Van AN, Rajesh A, Nielsen A, Newbold DJ, , et al. A somato-cognitive action network alternates with effector regions in motor cortex. Nature 617: 351–359, 2023. doi: 10.1038/s41586-023-05964-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. DiNicola LM, Sun W, Buckner RL. Side-by-side regions in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex estimated within the individual respond differentially to domain-specific and domain-flexible processes. J Neurophysiol 130: 1602–1615, 2023. doi: 10.1152/jn.00277.2023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Gratton C, Braga RM. Deep imaging of the individual brain: past, practice, and promise. Curr Opin Behav Sci 40: iii–vi, 2021. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.06.011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Laumann TO, Zorumski CF, Dosenbach NUF. Precision neuroimaging for localization-related psychiatry. JAMA Psychiatry 80: 763–764, 2023. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2023.1576. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Kong R, Li J, Orban C, Sabuncu MR, Liu H, Schaefer A, Sun N, Zuo XN, Holmes AJ, Eickhoff SB, Yeo BTT. Spatial topography of individual-specific cortical networks predicts human cognition, personality, and emotion. Cereb Cortex 29: 2533–2551, 2019. [Erratum in Cereb Cortex 31: 3974, 2021]. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Margulies DS, Ghosh SS, Goulas A, Falkiewicz M, Huntenburg JM, Langs G, Bezgin G, Eickhoff SB, Castellanos FX, Petrides M, Jefferies E, Smallwood J. Situating the default-mode network along a principal gradient of macroscale cortical organization. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 113: 12574–12579, 2016. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1608282113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Van Essen DC, Maunsell J. Two‐dimensional maps of the cerebral cortex. J Comp Neurol 191: 255–281, 1980. doi: 10.1002/cne.901910208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Fischl B, Sereno MI, Dale AM. Cortical surface-based analysis: II: Inflation, flattening, and a surface-based coordinate system. Neuroimage 9: 195–207, 1999. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75. Xue A, Kong R, Yang Q, Eldaief MC, Angeli PA, DiNicola LM, Braga RM, Buckner RL, Yeo BTT. The detailed organization of the human cerebellum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity within the individual. J Neurophysiol 125: 358–384, 2021. doi: 10.1152/jn.00561.2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Saadon-Grosman N, Angeli PA, DiNicola LM, Buckner RL. A third somatomotor representation in the human cerebellum. J Neurophysiol 128: 1051–1073, 2022. doi: 10.1152/jn.00165.2022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. Dosenbach NUF, Koller JM, Earl EA, Miranda-Dominguez O, Klein RL, Van AN, Snyder AZ, Nagel BJ, Nigg JT, Nguyen AL, Wesevich V, Greene DJ, Fair DA. Real-time motion analytics during brain MRI improve data quality and reduce costs. Neuroimage 161: 80–93, 2017. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.08.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78. Kwong KK, Belliveau JW, Chesler DA, Goldberg IE, Weisskoff RM, Poncelet BP, Kennedy DN, Hoppel BE, Cohen MS, Turner R. Dynamic magnetic resonance imaging of human brain activity during primary sensory stimulation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 89: 5675–5679, 1992. doi: 10.1073/pnas.89.12.5675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79. Ogawa S, Tank DW, Menon R, Ellermann JM, Kim SG, Merkle H, Ugurbil K. Intrinsic signal changes accompanying sensory stimulation: functional brain mapping with magnetic resonance imaging. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 89: 5951–5955, 1992. doi: 10.1073/pnas.89.13.5951. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80. Xu J, Moeller S, Strupp J, Auerbach E, Chen L, Feinberg DA, Ugurbil K, Yacoub E. Highly accelerated whole brain imaging using aligned-blipped-controlled-aliasing multiband EPI (Abstract). Proc Int Soc Magn Reson Med 20: 2306, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 81. Van Essen DC, Smith SM, Barch DM, Behrens TE, Yacoub E, Ugurbil K; WU-Minn HCP Consortium. The WU-Minn human connectome project: an overview. Neuroimage 80: 62–79, 2013. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82. Setsompop K, Gagoski BA, Polimeni JR, Witzel T, Wedeen VJ, Wald LL. Blipped‐controlled aliasing in parallel imaging for simultaneous multislice echo planar imaging with reduced g‐factor penalty. Magn Reson Med 67: 1210–1224, 2012. doi: 10.1002/mrm.23097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83. van der Kouwe AJ, Benner T, Fischl B, Schmitt F, Salat DH, Harder M, Sorensen AG, Dale AM. On-line automatic slice positioning for brain MR imaging. Neuroimage 27: 222–230, 2005. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.03.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84. Weiskopf N, Hutton C, Josephs O, Deichmann R. Optimal EPI parameters for reduction of susceptibility-induced BOLD sensitivity losses: a whole-brain analysis at 3 T and 1.5 T. Neuroimage 33: 493–504, 2006. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.07.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85. Mennes M, Jenkinson M, Valabregue R, Buitelaar JK, Beckmann C, Smith S. Optimizing full-brain coverage in human brain MRI through population distributions of brain size. Neuroimage 98: 513–520, 2014. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.04.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86. van der Kouwe AJ, Benner T, Salat DH, Fischl B. Brain morphometry with multiecho MPRAGE. Neuroimage 40: 559–569, 2008. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87. Harms MP, Somerville LH, Ances BM, Andersson J, Barch DM, Bastiani M, , et al. Extending the Human Connectome Project across ages: imaging protocols for the Lifespan Development and Aging projects. Neuroimage 183: 972–984, 2018. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88. Cox RW. AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput Biomed Res 29: 162–173, 1996. doi: 10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89. Cox RW. AFNI: what a long strange trip it’s been. Neuroimage 62: 743–747, 2012. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90. DiNicola LM, Braga RM, Buckner RL. Parallel distributed networks dissociate episodic and social functions within the individual. J Neurophysiol 123: 1144–1179, 2020. [Erratum in J Neurophysiol 124: 307, 2020]. doi: 10.1152/jn.00529.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91. Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, Kenna H, Reiss AL, Greicius MD. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J Neurosci 27: 2349–2356, 2007. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92. Seeley WW. The salience network: a neural system for perceiving and responding to homeostatic demands. J Neurosci 39: 9878–9882, 2019. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1138-17.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93. Buckner RL, DiNicola LM. The brain’s default network: updated anatomy, physiology and evolving insights. Nat Rev Neurosci 20: 593–608, 2019. doi: 10.1038/s41583-019-0212-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94. Gilmore AW, Nelson SM, McDermott KB. A parietal memory network revealed by multiple MRI methods. Trends Cogn Sci 19: 534–543, 2015. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95. DiNicola LM, Ariyo OI, Buckner RL. Functional specialization of parallel distributed networks revealed by analysis of trial-to-trial variation in processing demands. J Neurophysiol 129: 17–40, 2023. doi: 10.1152/jn.00211.2022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96. Marcus D, Harwell J, Olsen T, Hodge M, Glasser M, Prior F, Jenkinson M, Laumann T, Curtiss S, Van Essen DC. Informatics and data mining tools and strategies for the human connectome project. Front Neuroinform 5: 4, 2011. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2011.00004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97. Glasser MF, Sotiropoulos SN, Wilson JA, Coalson TS, Fischl B, Andersson JL, Xu J, Jbabdi S, Webster M, Polimeni JR, Van Essen DC, Jenkinson M; WU-Minn HCP Consortium. The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project. Neuroimage 80: 105–124, 2013. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98. Ojemann JG, Akbudak E, Snyder AZ, McKinstry RC, Raichle ME, Conturo TE. Anatomic localization and quantitative analysis of gradient refocused echo-planar fMRI susceptibility artifacts. Neuroimage 6: 156–167, 1997. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99. Buckner RL, Krienen FM, Castellanos A, Diaz JC, Yeo BTT. The organization of the human cerebellum estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol 106: 2322–2345, 2011. doi: 10.1152/jn.00339.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100. Fox PT, Miezin FM, Allman JM, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME. Retinotopic organization of human visual cortex mapped with positron-emission tomography. J Neurosci 7: 913–922, 1987. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.07-03-00913.1987. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101. Tootell RB, Reppas JB, Kwong KK, Malach R, Born RT, Brady TJ, Rosen BR, Belliveau JW. Functional analysis of human MT and related visual cortical areas using magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci 15: 3215–3230, 1995. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-04-03215.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102. Wandell BA, Winawer J. Imaging retinotopic maps in the human brain. Vision Res 51: 718–737, 2011. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.08.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103. Wynn JK, Jimenez AM, Roach BJ, Korb A, Lee J, Horan WP, Ford JM, Green MF. Impaired target detection in schizophrenia and the ventral attentional network: findings from a joint event-related potential–functional MRI analysis. Neuroimage Clin 9: 95–102, 2015. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.07.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104. Dosenbach NUF, Visscher KM, Palmer ED, Miezin FM, Wenger KK, Kang HC, Burgund ED, Grimes AL, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. A core system for the implementation of task sets. Neuron 50: 799–812, 2006. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.04.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105. Dale AM. Optimal experimental design for event‐related fMRI. Hum Brain Mapp 8: 109–114, 1999. doi:. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106. Cohen JD, Forman SD, Braver TS, Casey BJ, Servan‐Schreiber D, Noll DC. Activation of the prefrontal cortex in a nonspatial working memory task with functional MRI. Hum Brain Mapp 1: 293–304, 1994. doi: 10.1002/hbm.460010407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107. Braver TS, Cohen JD, Nystrom LE, Jonides J, Smith EE, Noll DC. A parametric study of prefrontal cortex involvement in human working memory. Neuroimage 5: 49–62, 1997. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1996.0247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108. Barch DM, Burgess GC, Harms MP, Petersen SE, Schlaggar BL, Corbetta M, Glasser MF, Curtiss S, Dixit S, Feldt C, Nolan D, Bryant E, Hartley T, Footer O, Bjork JM, Poldrack R, Smith S, Johansen-Berg H, Snyder AZ, Van Essen DC; WU-Minn HCP Consortium. Function in the human connectome: task-fMRI and individual differences in behavior. Neuroimage 80: 169–189, 2013. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109. Konkle T, Oliva A. A real-world size organization of object responses in occipitotemporal cortex. Neuron 74: 1114–1124, 2012. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110. Josephs EL, Konkle T. Large-scale dissociations between views of objects, scenes, and reachable-scale environments in visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 117: 29354–29362, 2020. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1912333117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111. Davies M. The Corpus of Contemporary American English as the first reliable monitor corpus of English. Lit Linguist Comput 25: 447–464, 2010. doi: 10.1093/llc/fqq018. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 112. Saxe R, Kanwisher N. People thinking about thinking people: the role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind”. Neuroimage 19: 1835–1842, 2003. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00230-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113. Dodell-Feder D, Koster-Hale J, Bedny M, Saxe R. fMRI item analysis in a theory of mind task. Neuroimage 55: 705–712, 2011. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114. Bruneau EG, Pluta A, Saxe R. Distinct roles of the ‘Shared Pain’ and ‘Theory of Mind’ networks in processing others’ emotional suffering. Neuropsychologia 50: 219–231, 2012. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115. Jacoby N, Bruneau E, Koster-Hale J, Saxe R. Localizing pain matrix and theory of mind networks with both verbal and non-verbal stimuli. Neuroimage 126: 39–48, 2016. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116. Andrews-Hanna JR, Reidler JS, Sepulcre J, Poulin R, Buckner RL. Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brain’s default network. Neuron 65: 550–562, 2010. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117. Hassabis D, Maguire EA. Deconstructing episodic memory with construction. Trends Cogn Sci 11: 299–306, 2007. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118. Hassabis D, Maguire EA. The construction system of the brain. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 364: 1263–1271, 2009. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0296. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119. Woolrich MW, Ripley BD, Brady M, Smith SM. Temporal autocorrelation in univariate linear modeling of FMRI data. Neuroimage 14: 1370–1386, 2001. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0931. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120. Smith SM, Jenkinson M, Woolrich MW, Beckmann CF, Behrens TEJ, Johansen-Berg H, Bannister PR, De Luca M, Drobnjak I, Flitney DE, Niazy RK, Saunders J, Vickers J, Zhang Y, De Stefano N, Brady JM, Matthews PM. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23, Suppl 1: S208–S219, 2004. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121. Wandell BA, Brewer AA, Dougherty RF. Visual field map clusters in human cortex. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 360: 693–707, 2005. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122. Wandell BA, Dumoulin SO, Brewer AA. Visual field maps in human cortex. Neuron 56: 366–383, 2007. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123. Buckner RL, Yeo BTT. Borders, map clusters, and supra-areal organization in visual cortex. Neuroimage 93: 292–297, 2014. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124. Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 3: 201–215, 2002. doi: 10.1038/nrn755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125. Shulman GL, Corbetta M, Buckner RL, Fiez JA, Miezin FM, Raichle ME, Petersen SE. Common blood flow changes across visual tasks: I. Increases in subcortical structures and cerebellum but not in nonvisual cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 9: 624–647, 1997. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126. Duncan J. The multiple-demand (MD) system of the primate brain: mental programs for intelligent behaviour. Trends Cogn Sci 14: 172–179, 2010. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127. Reznik D, Trampel R, Weiskopf N, Witter MP, Doeller CF. Dissociating distinct cortical networks associated with subregions of the human medial temporal lobe using precision neuroimaging. Neuron 111: 2756–2772.e7, 2023. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2023.05.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128. Dworetsky A, Seitzman BA, Adeyemo B, Neta M, Coalson RS, Petersen SE, Gratton C. Probabilistic mapping of human functional brain networks identifies regions of high group consensus. Neuroimage 237: 118164, 2021. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118164. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129. Flechsig P. Einige Bemerkungen über die Untersuchungsmethoden der Grosshirnrinde insbesondere des Menschen. Berlin: De Gruyter, 1904. [Google Scholar]
- 130. Flechsig P. Anatomie des Menschlichen Gehirns und Rückenmarks auf Myelogenetischer Grundlage. Leipzig: Verlag von Georg Thieme, 1920. [Google Scholar]
- 131. Park J, Soucy E, Segawa J, Mair R, Konkle T. Ultra-wide angle neuroimaging: insights into immersive scene representation (Preprint). bioRxiv 2023.05.14.540275, 2023. doi: 10.1101/2023.05.14.540275. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 132. Mazoyer B, Zago L, Mellet E, Bricogne S, Etard O, Houdé O, Crivello F, Joliot M, Petit L, Tzourio-Mazoyer N. Cortical networks for working memory and executive functions sustain the conscious resting state in man. Brain Res Bull 54: 287–298, 2001. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(00)00437-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133. Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98: 676–682, 2001. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134. Duncan J. An adaptive coding model of neural function in prefrontal cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci 2: 820–829, 2001. doi: 10.1038/35097575. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135. Cromer JA, Roy JE, Miller EK. Representation of multiple, independent categories in the primate prefrontal cortex. Neuron 66: 796–807, 2010. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.05.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136. Duncan J. The structure of cognition: attentional episodes in mind and brain. Neuron 80: 35–50, 2013. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.09.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137. DiNicola LM, Buckner RL. Precision estimates of parallel distributed association networks: evidence for domain specialization and implications for evolution and development. Curr Opin Behav Sci 40: 120–129, 2021. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.03.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138. Kwon Y, Salvo JJ, Anderson N, Holubecki AM, Lakshman M, Yoo K, Kay K, Gratton C, Braga RM. Situating the parietal memory network in the context of multiple parallel distributed networks using high-resolution functional connectivity (Preprint). bioRxiv 2023.08.16.553585, 2023. doi: 10.1101/2023.08.16.553585. [DOI]
- 139. Lynch CJ, Elbau I, Ng T, Ayaz A, Zhu S, Manfredi N, Johnson M, Wolk D, Power JD, Gordon EM, Kay K, Aloysi A, Moia S, Caballero-Gaudes C, Victoria LW, Solomonov N, Goldwaser E, Zebley B, Grosenick L, Downar J, Vila-Rodriguez F, Daskalakis ZJ, Blumberger DM, Williams N, Gunning FM, Liston C. Expansion of a frontostriatal salience network in individuals with depression (Preprint). bioRxiv 2023.08.09.551651, 2023. doi: 10.1101/2023.08.09.551651. [DOI]
- 140. Kim H. Involvement of the dorsal and ventral attention networks in oddball stimulus processing: a meta‐analysis. Hum Brain Mapp 35: 2265–2284, 2014. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22326. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141. Gordon EM, Laumann TO, Marek S, Newbold DJ, Hampton JM, Seider NA, Montez DF, Nielsen AM, Van AN, Zheng A, Miller R, Siegel JS, Kay BP, Snyder AZ, Greene DJ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE, Nelson SM, Dosenbach NUF. Individualized functional subnetworks connect human striatum and frontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 32: 2868–2884, 2022. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab387. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142. Zheng A, Montez DF, Marek S, Gilmore AW, Newbold DJ, Laumann TO, Kay BP, Seider NA, Van AN, Hampton JM, Alexopoulos D, Schlaggar BL, Sylvester CM, Greene DJ, Shimony JS, Nelson SM, Wig GS, Gratton C, McDermott KB, Raichle ME, Gordon EM, Dosenbach NUF. Parallel hippocampal-parietal circuits for self-and goal-oriented processing. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 118: e2101743118, 2021. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2101743118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143. Menon V, Uddin LQ. Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct Funct 214: 655–667, 2010. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144. Nelson SM, Cohen AL, Power JD, Wig GS, Miezin FM, Wheeler ME, Velanova K, Donaldson DI, Phillips JS, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. A parcellation scheme for human left lateral parietal cortex. Neuron 67: 156–170, 2010. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.05.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145. Gilmore AW, Kalinowski SE, Milleville SC, Gotts SJ, Martin A. Identifying task-general effects of stimulus familiarity in the parietal memory network. Neuropsychologia 124: 31–43, 2019. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.12.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146. McDermott KB, Szpunar KK, Christ SE. Laboratory-based and autobiographical retrieval tasks differ substantially in their neural substrates. Neuropsychologia 47: 2290–2298, 2009. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.12.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147. McDermott KB, Gilmore AW, Nelson SM, Watson JM, Ojemann JG. The parietal memory network activates similarly for true and associative false recognition elicited via the DRM procedure. Cortex 87: 96–107, 2017. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148. Angeli PA, DiNicola LM, Saadon-Grosman N, Eldaief MC, Buckner RL. Specialization of the human hippocampal long axis revisited (Preprint). bioRxiv 2023.12.19.572264, 2023. doi: 10.1101/2023.12.19.572264. [DOI]
- 149. Cohen AL, Fair DA, Dosenbach NUF, Miezin FM, Dierker D, Van Essen DC, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Defining functional areas in individual human brains using resting functional connectivity MRI. Neuroimage 41: 45–57, 2008. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150. Gordon EM, Laumann TO, Adeyemo B, Huckins JF, Kelley WM, Petersen SE. Generation and evaluation of a cortical area parcellation from resting-state correlations. Cereb Cortex 26: 288–303, 2016. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151. Eickhoff SB, Yeo BTT, Genon S. Imaging-based parcellations of the human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 19: 672–686, 2018. doi: 10.1038/s41583-018-0071-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152. Schaefer A, Kong R, Gordon EM, Laumann TO, Zuo XN, Holmes AJ, Eickhoff SB, Yeo BTT. Local-global parcellation of the human cerebral cortex from intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Cereb Cortex 28: 3095–3114, 2018. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx179. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 153. Saadon-Grosman N, Du J, Kosakowski HL, Angeli PA, DiNicola LM, Eldaief MC, Buckner RL. Within-individual organization of the human cognitive cerebellum: evidence for closely juxtaposed, functionally specialized regions (Preprint). bioRxiv 2023.12.18.572062, 2023. doi: 10.1101/2023.12.18.572062. [DOI]
- 154. Kosakowski HL, Saadon-Grosman N, Du J, Eldaief MC, Buckner RL. Human striatal association megaclusters. J Neurophysiol 2024. doi: 10.1152/jn.00387.2023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 155. Greicius MD, Srivastava G, Reiss AL, Menon V. Default-mode network activity distinguishes Alzheimer’s disease from healthy aging: Evidence from functional MRI. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101: 4637–4642, 2004. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0308627101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 156. Suzuki WL, Amaral DG. Perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices of the macaque monkey: cortical afferents. J Comp Neurol 350: 497–533, 1994. doi: 10.1002/cne.903500402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 157. Lavenex P, Suzuki WA, Amaral DG. Perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices of the macaque monkey: projections to the neocortex. J Comp Neurol 447: 394–420, 2002. doi: 10.1002/cne.10243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 158. Blatt GJ, Pandya DN, Rosene DL. Parcellation of cortical afferents to three distinct sectors in the parahippocampal gyrus of the rhesus monkey: an anatomical and neurophysiological study. J Comp Neurol 466: 161–179, 2003. doi: 10.1002/cne.10866. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 159. Gusnard DA, Raichle ME. Searching for a baseline: functional imaging and the resting human brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 2: 685–694, 2001. doi: 10.1038/35094500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 160. Smallwood J, Bernhardt BC, Leech R, Bzdok D, Jefferies E, Margulies DS. The default mode network in cognition: a topographical perspective. Nat Rev Neurosci 22: 503–513, 2021. doi: 10.1038/s41583-021-00474-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 161. Greicius MD, Krasnow B, Reiss AL, Menon V. Functional connectivity in the resting brain: a network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100: 253–258, 2003. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0135058100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 162. Andrews-Hanna JR, Saxe R, Yarkoni T. Contributions of episodic retrieval and mentalizing to autobiographical thought: evidence from functional neuroimaging, resting-state connectivity, and fMRI meta-analyses. Neuroimage 91: 324–335, 2014. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 163. Lee MH, Hacker CD, Snyder AZ, Corbetta M, Zhang D, Leuthardt EC, Shimony JS. Clustering of resting state networks. PLoS One 7: e40370, 2012. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0040370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 164. Nyberg L, McIntosh AR, Cabeza R, Nilsson LG, Houle S, Habib R, Tulving E. Network analysis of positron emission tomography regional cerebral blood flow data: ensemble inhibition during episodic memory retrieval. J Neurosci 16: 3753–3759, 1996. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-11-03753.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 165. Fransson P. Spontaneous low‐frequency BOLD signal fluctuations: an fMRI investigation of the resting‐state default mode of brain function hypothesis. Hum Brain Mapp 26: 15–29, 2005. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 166. Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102: 9673–9678, 2005. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 167. Miller KJ, Weaver KE, Ojemann JG. Direct electrophysiological measurement of human default network areas. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106: 12174–12177, 2009. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0902071106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 168. Anticevic A, Cole MW, Murray JD, Corlett PR, Wang XJ, Krystal JH. The role of default network deactivation in cognition and disease. Trends Cogn Sci 16: 584–592, 2012. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 169. Silson EH, Steel A, Kidder A, Gilmore AW, Baker CI. Distinct subdivisions of human medial parietal cortex support recollection of people and places. eLife 8: e47391, 2019. doi: 10.7554/eLife.47391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 170. Deen B, Freiwald WA. Parallel systems for social and spatial reasoning within the cortical apex (Preprint). bioRxiv 2021.09.23.4615502022, 2021. doi: 10.1101/2021.09.23.461550. [DOI]
- 171. Bailey P, von Bonin G. The Isocortex of Man. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press, 1951. [Google Scholar]
- 172. Meyer A. Paul Flechsig’s system of myelogenetic cortical localization in the light of recent research in neuroanatomy and neurophysiology part I. Can J Neurol Sci 8: 1–6, 1981. doi: 10.1017/s031716710004275x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 173. Clarke E, O’Malley CD. The Human Brain and Spinal Cord: A Historical Study Illustrated by Writings from Antiquity to the Twentieth Century. San Francisco, CA: Norman Publishing, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- 174. Mesulam MM. Fifty years of disconnexion syndromes and the Geschwind legacy. Brain 138: 2791–2799, 2015. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 175. Zilles K. Brodmann: a pioneer of human brain mapping—his impact on concepts of cortical organization. Brain 141: 3262–3278, 2018. doi: 10.1093/brain/awy273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 176. Amlien IK, Fjell AM, Tamnes CK, Grydeland H, Krogsrud SK, Chaplin TA, Rosa MG, Walhovd KB. Organizing principles of human cortical development—thickness and area from 4 to 30 years: insights from comparative primate neuroanatomy. Cereb Cortex 26: 257–267, 2016. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 177. Hill J, Inder T, Neil J, Dierker D, Harwell J, Van Essen D. Similar patterns of cortical expansion during human development and evolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 107: 13135–13140, 2010. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1001229107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 178. Chaplin TA, Yu HH, Soares JG, Gattass R, Rosa MGP. A conserved pattern of differential expansion of cortical areas in simian primates. J Neurosci 33: 15120–15125, 2013. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2909-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 179. Katz LC, Shatz CJ. Synaptic activity and the construction of cortical circuits. Science 274: 1133–1138, 1996. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5290.1133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 180. Ackman JB, Burbridge TJ, Crair MC. Retinal waves coordinate patterned activity throughout the developing visual system. Nature 490: 219–225, 2012. doi: 10.1038/nature11529. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 181. Rosa MGP, Tweedale R. Brain maps, great and small: lessons from comparative studies of primate visual cortical organization. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 360: 665–691, 2005. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1626. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 182. Giarrocco F, Averbeck BB. Organization of parietoprefrontal and temporoprefrontal networks in the macaque. J Neurophysiol 126: 1289–1309, 2021. doi: 10.1152/jn.00092.2021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 183. Vijayakumar S, Sallet J, Verhagen L, Folloni D, Medendorp WP, Mars RB. Mapping multiple principles of parietal-frontal cortical organization using functional connectivity. Brain Struct Funct 224: 681–697, 2019. doi: 10.1007/s00429-018-1791-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 184. Huntenburg JM, Bazin PL, Margulies DS. Large-scale gradients in human cortical organization. Trends Cogn Sci 22: 21–31, 2018. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 185. Schwartz ML, Goldman-Rakic P. Development and plasticity of the primate cerebral cortex. Clin Perinatol 17: 83–102, 1990. doi: 10.1016/S0095-5108(18)30591-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 186. Bourgeois JP, Goldman-Rakic PS, Rakic P. Synaptogenesis in the prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkeys. Cereb Cortex 4: 78–96, 1994. doi: 10.1093/cercor/4.1.78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 187. Price DJ, Kennedy H, Dehay C, Zhou L, Mercier M, Jossin Y, Goffinet AM, Tissir F, Blakey D, Molnár Z. The development of cortical connections. Eur J Neurosci 23: 910–920, 2006. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04620.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 188. Cadwell CR, Bhaduri A, Mostajo-Radji MA, Keefe MG, Nowakowski TJ. Development and arealization of the cerebral cortex. Neuron 103: 980–1004, 2019. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.07.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 189. Shallice T. From Neuropsychology to Mental Structure. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 1988. [Google Scholar]
- 190. Teuber HL. Physiological psychology. Annu Rev Psychol 6: 267–296, 1955. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.06.020155.001411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 191. Duncan J, Owen AM. Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends Neurosci 23: 475–483, 2000. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(00)01633-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 192. Fedorenko E, Duncan J, Kanwisher N. Broad domain generality in focal regions of frontal and parietal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110: 16616–16621, 2013. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1315235110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 193. Assem M, Shashidhara S, Glasser MF, Duncan J. Precise topology of adjacent domain-general and sensory-biased regions in the human brain. Cereb Cortex 32: 2521–2537, 2022. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab362. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 194. Badre D, Nee DE. Frontal cortex and the hierarchical control of behavior. Trends Cogn Sci 22: 170–188, 2018. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.11.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 195. Friedman NP, Robbins TW. The role of prefrontal cortex in cognitive control and executive function. Neuropsychopharmacology 47: 72–89, 2022. doi: 10.1038/s41386-021-01132-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 196. Miller EK, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu Rev Neurosci 24: 167–202, 2001. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 197. Buckner RL, Carroll DC. Self-projection and the brain. Trends Cogn Sci 11: 49–57, 2007. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 198. Spreng RN, Mar RA, Kim ASN. The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: a quantitative meta-analysis. J Cogn Neurosci 21: 489–510, 2009. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 199. Kaas JH. The organization of neocortex in mammals: implications for theories of brain function. Annu Rev Psychol 38: 129–151, 1987. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.38.020187.001021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 200. Felleman DJ, Van Essen DC. Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cereb Cortex 1: 1–47, 1991. doi: 10.1093/cercor/1.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 201. Van Essen DC, Glasser MF. In vivo architectonics: a cortico-centric perspective. Neuroimage 93: 157–164, 2014. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 202. Maunsell J, Van Essen DC. The connections of the middle temporal visual area (MT) and their relationship to a cortical hierarchy in the macaque monkey. J Neurosci 3: 2563–2586, 1983. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.03-12-02563.1983. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 203. Ungerleider LG, Galkin TW, Desimone R, Gattass R. Subcortical projections of area V2 in the macaque. J Cogn Neurosci 26: 1220–1233, 2014. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00571. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 204. Falchier A, Clavagnier S, Barone P, Kennedy H. Anatomical evidence of multimodal integration in primate striate cortex. J Neurosci 22: 5749–5759, 2002. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-13-05749.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 205. Hatanaka N, Nambu A, Yamashita A, Takada M, Tokuno H. Somatotopic arrangement and corticocortical inputs of the hindlimb region of the primary motor cortex in the macaque monkey. Neurosci Res 40: 9–22, 2001. doi: 10.1016/s0168-0102(01)00210-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 206. Rosa MGP. Visual maps in the adult primate cerebral cortex: some implications for brain development and evolution. Braz J Med Biol Res 35: 1485–1498, 2002. doi: 10.1590/s0100-879x2002001200008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 207. Lashley KS, Clark G. The cytoarchitecture of the cerebral cortex of Ateles: a critical examination of architectonic studies. J Comp Neurol 85: 223–305, 1946. doi: 10.1002/cne.900850207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 208. Arcaro MJ, Pinsk MA, Kastner S. The anatomical and functional organization of the human visual pulvinar. J Neurosci 35: 9848–9871, 2015. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1575-14.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 209. Nano PR, Nguyen CV, Mil J, Bhaduri A. Cortical cartography: mapping arealization using single-cell omics technology. Front Neural Circuits 15: 788560, 2021. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2021.788560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Supplemental Figs. 1–18: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.24990657.v2.
Supplemental Figs. 19–227: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.24990663.v2.
Data Availability Statement
The DU15NET atlases are available (https://freesurfer.net/fswiki/CorticalParcellation_DU15NET). Additional parcellation, SNR, and task maps related to this manuscript are available on Balsa (https://balsa.wustl.edu/study/zK166). Task descriptions, contrast descriptions, and code are provided on Harvard Dataverse (https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/AVB4BW). Individual participant data are available in the NIH repository (https://nda.nih.gov).