Skip to main content
JMIR Research Protocols logoLink to JMIR Research Protocols
. 2024 Sep 10;13:e56977. doi: 10.2196/56977

Impact of Biological Sex on Emotional Perception Among Adults With Schizophrenia Spectrum Disorders: Protocol for a Systematic Review

Gabrielle Ritchie 1,2,, Harriet Nwachukwu 1, Stephen Parker 3, Frances Dark 1,2
Editor: Amaryllis Mavragani
Reviewed by: Rocco de Filippis, Julian K Christians
PMCID: PMC11422750  PMID: 39255474

Abstract

Background

It is well established that individuals with schizophrenia experience deficits in emotional perception that can impact long-term social and occupational functioning. Understanding the factors that impact these impairments is important for targeting interventions to improve recovery. In the general population, compared with males, females tend to show greater perception of emotions. Whether this sex difference persists in schizophrenia is less clear. In contrast to males, females diagnosed with schizophrenia tend to have a higher age of disease onset and better premorbid functioning but do not necessarily have better outcomes. Effective treatments for social cognitive impairments are highly relevant to long-term functional rehabilitation. A greater understanding of the cognitive deficits in emotional perception within females and males living with schizophrenia may assist interventions to be better tailored to individuals.

Objective

This systematic review aims to collate, synthesize, and critically appraise evidence considering the influence of biological sex (female and male) on the emotional perception of individuals with schizophrenia.

Methods

This is a systematic review protocol based on the PRISMA-P (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses Protocols) guidelines. The electronic databases MEDLINE, Embase, CENTRAL, CINAHL, and PsycINFO will be systematically searched. To be included in this review, studies must compare the emotional perceptions of male and female participants older than 18 years who have a primary diagnosis of a schizophrenia spectrum disorder. Qualitative studies, case reports, case series, unpublished manuscripts, and studies not reported in English will be excluded. Key search strategies will include combinations of the following terms: “men,” “male,” “man,” “female,” “women,” “woman,” “sex,” “gender,” “emotional perception,” “emotional processing,” “schizophrenia,” “schizophren,” “psychotic disorders,” “psychosis,” “psychoses,” “psychotic,” “schizoaffective,” “schizotypal personality disorder,” and “schizotyp.” Identified studies will be uploaded to the web-based Covidence systematic review management software. The risk of bias for individual studies will be assessed using the relevant Joanna Briggs Institute checklist tools. The GRADE (Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development, and Evaluation) system will also be used to evaluate the strength of the evidence base. Findings will be synthesized to provide a systematic summary of the existing literature. If sufficiently comparable data to permit meta-analysis emerges, a random-effects meta-analysis will be performed.

Results

This systematic review was registered with the PROSPERO (International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews) in October 2023. The search and screening of study titles and abstracts are currently underway. Data are expected to be extracted and analyzed in July 2024.

Conclusions

Results will contribute to an improved understanding of the social cognitive profiles of males and females with schizophrenia. This knowledge is expected to inform the adaptation of interventions to improve functional outcomes.

Trial Registration

PROSPERO CRD42023463561; https://tinyurl.com/34sr3rnf

International Registered Report Identifier (IRRID)

DERR1-10.2196/56977

Keywords: schizophrenia, emotional perception, systematic review, sex differences, psychosis, social cognition, emotional processing

Introduction

Schizophrenia spectrum disorders (SSDs) are estimated to affect 24 million (or 1 in 300) people globally [1]. Overall mortality rates for people with schizophrenia are 3 times greater than for the general population [2], with life expectancy reduced by an average of 14.5 years [3]. These disorders are characterized by both positive (ie, hallucinations, delusions, and disorganized thoughts and behavior) and negative (ie, reduced speech and emotional expression, social withdrawal, and anhedonia) symptoms [4], which can contribute to significant distress and functional impairment across multiple life domains. Deficits in cognition are also a key feature of SSD, and their presence is predictive of poor functional outcomes [5,6]. Current pharmacological treatments have relative success in reducing positive psychiatric symptoms; however, they have limited capacity to target cognitive impairments [7,8]. These cognitive deficits extend not only to neurocognitive domains (eg, memory and attention) but also to social cognitive processes (ie, those involved in social interactions) [9-12]. In particular, it is well established that individuals with SSD have problems with emotional perception [13], the mental operations involved in the ability to understand and recognize the emotions of others [9].

There is strong evidence that individuals with schizophrenia perform significantly worse on tasks of emotional perception when compared with healthy individuals [13,14]. These deficits have been found to persist across different stages of the illness, worsen with the number of psychotic episodes, and be present in those at risk of developing schizophrenia [15-17]. It is noteworthy that previous meta-analyses of emotional perception in schizophrenia have tended to focus on facial emotion perception (FEP) [14]. This is unsurprising given that emotional perception is often conceptualized as the information conveyed in facial expressions. However, it can include other nonverbal (eg, body posture, movement, and physical touch) and verbal communication cues (eg, speech rate, vocal pitch, and voice intensity) [18]. Meta-analyses that have looked at the recognition of emotional perception in speech [19,20] have demonstrated significant deficits in individuals with schizophrenia, suggesting that emotional perception impairments extend across both verbal and nonverbal channels of communication. These impairments in emotional perception mean that individuals with schizophrenia can find adjusting and responding to social situations more challenging than healthy individuals. This can negatively impact a range of functional outcomes, including the ability to work and live independently [21], as well as the ability to demonstrate effective social skills and social problem-solving [22], both of which are important for developing meaningful social connections and support.

Despite emotional perception deficits being well established in the literature, there is still a lack of consensus regarding the specific factors contributing to impairments in this social cognitive domain [9]. For instance, in nonclinical populations, there appears to be a relationship between emotional perception and sex, such that females tend to perceive emotions better than males [23-25]. However, it is unclear whether this female advantage exists in schizophrenia [26-29]. Females with schizophrenia typically present with disease onset later than males and thus have higher premorbid functioning, although not necessarily better long-term outcomes [30-32]. There are a range of sex-related differences in the prevalence, presentation, and management of schizophrenia; however, these differences are still poorly understood, and research in this field is still in its infancy [33]. We identified 1 systematic review and meta-analysis [34] on the sex differences in FEP in a schizophrenia population. The sample included adults with chronic schizophrenia, individuals with early or recent onset of schizophrenia, and individuals with a first-degree relative with schizophrenia. This review found limited studies that reported sex differences in FEP, although those that did tended to show that females performed better than males. This study’s authors emphasized the importance of future research into the potential sex differences in FEP to improve the long-term outcomes of individuals with schizophrenia. Indeed, biomedical research, more generally, has tended to demonstrate sex disparity in participant recruitment (ie, males favored over females) [35]; there is a need for more research into sex-related differences in SSD [33]. Indeed, the inclusion of sex-specific clinical practice guidelines to guide the treatment of schizophrenia has been considered [35] as a potential step in managing the disorder.

Social cognition is significantly associated with long-term function, irrespective of factors such as symptom severity and length of illness [36]. It can also act as a mediator between neurocognition and functional outcomes [5,37]. Effective treatments for social cognitive impairments are highly relevant to long-term functional rehabilitation. We argue that a greater understanding of the deficits in emotional perception, a key aspect of social cognition, is needed to assist such interventions to be better tailored to individuals with schizophrenia. We also argue that understanding the role individual variables, such as biological sex, play on emotional perception task performance is important in developing the social cognitive profile of schizophrenia, particularly for females with the disorder, that has typically been underresearched. Previous systematic reviews and meta-analyses have studied emotional perception impairments in schizophrenia; however, preliminary searches in the PROSPERO (International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews), CENTRAL, and the Joanna Briggs Institute Database of Systematic Reviews revealed no recent systematic reviews that have examined the role of biological sex on this social cognitive domain and no previous systematic reviews that have looked at the impact of biological sex on emotional perception outside of FEP tasks. This study aims to systematically review and critically appraise the quantitative literature considering the impact of biological sex on emotional perception in adults with SSDs. Our primary goal is to identify differences in emotional perception performance between males and females to contribute to the knowledge of the social cognitive profile of schizophrenia.

Methods

This protocol was developed in consultation with the PRISMA-P (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses Protocols) guidelines [38,39]. This systematic review is registered with PROSPERO (ID: PROSPERO 2023 CRD42023463561).

Eligibility Criteria

Participants

Participants must be older than 18 years with a primary diagnosis of SSD, as diagnosed by any recognized diagnostic criteria, such as the DSM-V (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders [Fifth Edition]) or the ICD-11 (International Classification of Diseases, 11th Revision). Studies that include animals, children, or adolescents will be excluded. Participants who are at high risk of schizophrenia or who have experienced a first episode of psychosis but do not yet meet the full criteria for an SSD diagnosis will be excluded from this review. Studies that group adolescents and young adults together will also be excluded unless it is possible to separate participants who are older than 18 years.

Intervention

Studies will not need to include an intervention to be included in this systematic review. However, studies will need to examine at least 1 measure of emotional perception (eg, Bell Lysaker Emotion Recognition Task [28]; Penn Emotion Recognition Test [29]). We will define emotional perception as the social cognitive processes involved in comprehending and recognizing emotions in others. We will include any aspect of emotional perception (ie, verbal, and visual). Studies that are attempting to validate new tasks will be excluded.

Comparator

There will be no restriction on the inclusion of comparator groups (eg, intervention, treatment-as-usual, and healthy controls); however, studies must include male and female participants. Studies that combine participants (both males and females) into 1 group can also be included if the available data allow us to separate these participant groups. Studies that do not report the biological sex of participants will be excluded.

Outcomes

This systematic review will include quantitative, empirical studies with no restrictions on the type of setting or timing of publication. The exclusion criteria include qualitative studies, case reports, case series, unpublished manuscripts or abstracts, and papers not available in English. Studies must provide performance outcomes on the emotional perception task. These may be primary or secondary outcomes of the included studies.

Search Strategy

Initial search terms will be developed and piloted by the research team. Key search terms will include “men,” “male,” “man,” “female,” “women,” “woman,” “sex,” “gender,” “emotional perception,” “emotional processing,” “schizophrenia,” “schizophren,” “psychotic disorders,” “psychosis,” “psychoses,” “psychotic,” “schizoaffective,” “schizotypal personality disorder,” and “schizotyp.” Boolean operators “OR” and “AND” will combine these keywords. We will build a search terms and limits table to consider the keywords, terminology, and search limits. This table will also capture these keywords’ synonyms, abbreviations, alternative spellings, and collective nouns. We will use the MeSH (Medical Subject Headings) Browser to search for our topics and review the MeSH tree structures for each keyword to see whether there are any narrower terms to include in our search. We will then use this table to develop a preliminary search strategy with MEDLINE (PubMed; Multimedia Appendix 1). Peer review will be sought from a health services librarian with expertise in systematic reviews and meta-analyses. We will use the Polyglot search translator to adjust and translate our search strategy syntax to different databases [40]. The following electronic databases will be searched: MEDLINE, Embase, CENTRAL, CINAHL, and PsycINFO. Reference lists of included studies will also be manually scanned. Databases will continue to be searched for new publications until the end of June 2024. Study authors may be contacted to request full-text publications and clarify questions about study results. We will also search, at minimum, the first 3 pages of Google and Google Scholar to test our search strategy.

Selection Process

The literature search results will be uploaded to the web-based Covidence systematic review management software (Veritas Health Innovation) [41]. Abstracts that meet the search terms will be imported into Covidence, and titles and abstracts will be screened to determine whether they meet the inclusion criteria for a full-text review. Any duplicates will be removed during the screening process. Full-text papers will be uploaded for the formal reviewing process. If full-text versions are unavailable, the researchers will attempt to contact the study authors to request a copy of their paper via a maximum of 3 email attempts. Researchers (FD and GR) will independently screen titles and abstracts based on the study selection criteria. Before this screening process, screening questions will be developed to assist reviewers. Covidence software will flag any conflicts between reviewers at each of these stages. Reviewers will meet to discuss and resolve these conflicts. A third reviewer (HN) will be sought if a consensus cannot be reached. Full-text versions of studies that are considered relevant will then be formally reviewed by at least 3 researchers. Again, any conflicts during this process will be flagged, and reviewers will meet to seek consensus. Another reviewer will be contacted if an agreement cannot be reached. Eligible full-text publications will be stored in Endnote referencing software using smart groups. These selected papers will be checked for retractions and errata. Before data extraction, a search will be rerun to identify any new studies to be retrieved for inclusion. A PRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses) flow diagram will record search results during the study selection process [42].

Methodological Quality Assessment

Two reviewers will independently critically appraise the quality of studies included in the systematic review for risk of bias using the relevant Joanna Briggs Institute checklist tools [43]. These tools provide checklists for a range of study designs, including randomized and nonrandomized controlled trials. Assessment will consider potential sources of bias for each study, such as in the selection of participants, reliability in the measurement of outcomes, appropriateness of the statistical analysis undertaken, and potential selective reporting of results. Reviewers will meet to resolve any discrepancies in these ratings. A third reviewer will be sought if required. No reviewers will be blind to journal titles, study authors, or institutions. We will report the results of this quality assessment in tabular format in the systematic review.

Data Extraction and Synthesis

One researcher will extract the data from the selected full-text publications using the web-based Covidence systematic review management software [41]. This process will be reviewed by a second researcher. A data extraction template will be developed, piloted, and reviewed by the researchers before formal data extraction. If any data are unclear, the researchers will attempt to contact study authors to clarify their findings via a maximum of 3 email attempts over 28 days. We expect to extract the following data from the selected full-text publications as available: author and institution details, publication information (including study title, journal title, publication date, and study location), participant demographic and clinical information (including age, biological sex, primary and secondary mental health diagnoses, mean duration of onset of symptoms, premorbid functioning, current stage of illness, any other cognitive or physical impairments, and current medications), study methodology (including trial design, trial size, and emotional perception task), and all quantitative data on dynamic perception task performance including any dropouts or missing data.

The characteristics of the included studies will be presented in tabular format and summarized in narrative synthesis as appropriate. If sufficiently comparable data to permit meta-analysis emerges, this will be completed using the random-effects model. Both continuous (eg, standardized mean difference) and categorical outcomes (eg, odds ratio and relative risk) in the extracted studies will be assessed and compared. The heterogeneity of effect sizes of pooled studies will be measured via forest plots using chi-square and I2 values. If appropriate, a funnel plot and Egger test will be used to report publication bias. No subgroup analyses are planned at this stage. The GRADE (Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development, and Evaluation) system will be used to consider the strength of the evidence available [44]. Two reviewers will rate their level of confidence (ie, very low, low, moderate, or high) in the quality of evidence for outcomes. A third reviewer will be sought if consensus cannot be reached in these ratings. We will report this evaluation of the evidence in tabular format in the systematic review.

Ethical Considerations

This study does not require ethics approval and consent to participate. No data used for analysis or reported in the systematic review will be identifiable.

Results

This systematic review was submitted to PROSPERO in September 2023 and registered in October 2023. As of January 2024, preliminary searches and piloting of the study selection process have been completed. Formal screening of study titles and abstracts against eligibility criteria has commenced, with 973 studies imported to Covidence management software for screening. Data are expected to be extracted and analyzed in July 2024, and we aim to submit results for peer-reviewed publication by December 2024. This study is self-funded.

Discussion

Principal Findings

This study will systematically review the impact of biological sex on emotional perception in adults with SSD. In comparison with the general population, deficits in emotional perception in schizophrenia are well established [13,14], but there is very little research into the potential impact sex has on performance. We did identify 1 systematic review and meta-analysis [34] that found only limited evidence of a female advantage for emotional perception. However, it is important to note that this construct was restricted to FEP tasks, and the study authors highlighted the need for future research into this area. We hypothesize that our review may provide support to these findings. In healthy adults, females tend to perform better at emotional perception tasks. Our review aims to understand whether this advantage extends to females with schizophrenia or whether the advantage is attenuated. The social cognitive profile of schizophrenia, more generally, is still largely heterogeneous and how it differs to other psychiatric disorders is still under investigation [9]. We hope to provide a greater understanding of how sex-related differences in emotional perception may explain some of this variation and, in turn, inform future treatment that targets functional outcomes in these individuals. The need for more research into sex-related differences in schizophrenia has been emphasized as a new but important field of research [33].

Limitations

The main limitation anticipated in this review relates to the lack of consensus within the literature on how to measure emotional perception [12], compounded by the limited availability of well-validated and reliable tools. Often, attempts have been made to adapt measures from other participant samples to a schizophrenia population but with limited research on the psychometric properties of these measures in SSD [9]. A similar critique relates to existing social cognitive tasks’ lack of cross-cultural validity [9]. These restraints may limit the ability to draw conclusions and pool outcomes from comparable studies. If this is the case, we plan to provide a narrative synthesis and group studies by the type of emotional perception task, reporting on the reliability and validity of these measures. The study researchers must also acknowledge the cultural biases we bring to this review and be cautious in generalizing any conclusions drawn to other cultures, especially given the limit to studies available in English included in our study criteria. We also plan to measure the quality of evidence to assess the confidence in the available data and report these findings in the systematic review.

Conclusions

This study will review the evidence for sex differences in emotional perception within SSD. The results will provide a greater understanding of the social cognitive profile of schizophrenia and, in turn, inform future interventions tailored to improve long-term rehabilitation.

Acknowledgments

FD is the guarantor. All authors read and approved the final manuscript. No professional writers or generative artificial intelligence was used in any portion of the manuscript writing.

Abbreviations

DSM-V

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (Fifth Edition)

FEP

facial emotion perception

GRADE

Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development, and Evaluation

ICD-11

International Classification of Diseases, 11th Revision

MeSH

Medical Subject Headings

PRISMA

Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses

PRISMA-P

Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses Protocol Statement

PROSPERO

International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews

SSD

schizophrenia spectrum disorder

Multimedia Appendix 1

Draft MEDLINE search—PubMed interface.

Data Availability

Access to research data is restricted and governed by the Queensland Health Government; however, extracted data may be available upon request. All researchers will have access to the final data set. Extracted data will be made publicly available as multimedia appendices.

Footnotes

Authors' Contributions: GR participated in the methodology, investigation, writing original draft, and writing review and editing; HN participated in the investigation, writing review, and editing; SP participated in the methodology, writing review, and editing; and FD participated in the conceptualization, methodology, investigation, writing review and editing, and supervision.

Conflicts of Interest: FD has received honorariums from Janssen and Lundbeck for delivering lectures at clinician education events. In the past 5 years, SP has received research funding from the Common Good Foundation, Johnson & Johnson, Metro North Foundation, PA Foundation, RANZCP, and Suicide Prevention Australia; and honoraria from Johnson & Johnson, RANZCP, Queensland Psychotherapy Training, CSL Seqirus, and Tasmania Health. The other authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  • 1.Schizophrenia. World Health Organization. 2022. [2022-01-10]. https://tinyurl.com/ys3h3sm2 .
  • 2.Gatov E, Rosella L, Chiu M, Kurdyak PA. Trends in standardized mortality among individuals with schizophrenia, 1993-2012: a population-based, repeated cross-sectional study. CMAJ. 2017;189(37):E1177–E1187. doi: 10.1503/cmaj.161351. http://www.cmaj.ca/cgi/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=28923795 .189/37/E1177 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Hjorthøj C, Stürup AE, McGrath JJ, Nordentoft M. Years of potential life lost and life expectancy in schizophrenia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry. 2017;4(4):295–301. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30078-0.S2215-0366(17)30078-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Publishing; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kharawala S, Hastedt C, Podhorna J, Shukla H, Kappelhoff B, Harvey PD. The relationship between cognition and functioning in schizophrenia: a semi-systematic review. Schizophr Res Cogn. 2022;27:100217. doi: 10.1016/j.scog.2021.100217. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S2215-0013(21)00024-X .S2215-0013(21)00024-X [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Poulton R, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Cannon M, Murray R, Harrington H. Children's self-reported psychotic symptoms and adult schizophreniform disorder: a 15-year longitudinal study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57(11):1053–1058. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.11.1053.yoa9496 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Bowie CR, Harvey PD. Cognitive deficits and functional outcome in schizophrenia. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2006;2(4):531–536. doi: 10.2147/nedt.2006.2.4.531. https://europepmc.org/abstract/MED/19412501 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Harvey PD, Keefe RS. Studies of cognitive change in patients with schizophrenia following novel antipsychotic treatment. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(2):176–184. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.2.176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Green MF, Horan WP, Lee J. Nonsocial and social cognition in schizophrenia: current evidence and future directions. World Psychiatry. 2019;18(2):146–161. doi: 10.1002/wps.20624. https://europepmc.org/abstract/MED/31059632 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Lee TY, Hong SB, Shin NY, Kwon JS. Social cognitive functioning in prodromal psychosis: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2015;164(1-3):28–34. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.02.008.S0920-9964(15)00083-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.van Donkersgoed RJM, Wunderink L, Nieboer R, Aleman A, Pijnenborg GHM. Social cognition in individuals at ultra-high risk for psychosis: a meta-analysis. PLoS One. 2015;10(10):e0141075. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0141075. https://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0141075 .PONE-D-15-10569 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Pinkham AE, Penn DL, Green MF, Buck B, Healey K, Harvey PD. The social cognition psychometric evaluation study: results of the expert survey and RAND panel. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40(4):813–823. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt081. https://europepmc.org/abstract/MED/23728248 .sbt081 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Savla GN, Vella L, Armstrong CC, Penn DL, Twamley EW. Deficits in domains of social cognition in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of the empirical evidence. Schizophr Bull. 2013;39(5):979–992. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs080. https://europepmc.org/abstract/MED/22949733 .sbs080 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Kohler CG, Walker JB, Martin EA, Healey KM, Moberg PJ. Facial emotion perception in schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Schizophr Bull. 2010;36(5):1009–1019. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn192. https://europepmc.org/abstract/MED/19329561 .sbn192 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Pena-Garijo J, Lacruz M, Masanet MJ, Palop-Grau A, Plaza R, Hernandez-Merino A, Edo-Villamon S, Valllina O. Specific facial emotion recognition deficits across the course of psychosis: a comparison of individuals with low-risk, high-risk, first-episode psychosis and multi-episode schizophrenia-spectrum disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2023;320:115029. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2022.115029.S0165-1781(22)00620-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Martin D, Croft J, Pitt A, Strelchuk D, Sullivan S, Zammit S. Systematic review and meta-analysis of the relationship between genetic risk for schizophrenia and facial emotion recognition. Schizophr Res. 2020;218:7–13. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2019.12.031.S0920-9964(19)30601-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Gao Z, Zhao W, Liu S, Liu Z, Yang C, Xu Y. Facial emotion recognition in schizophrenia. Front Psychiatry. 2021;12:633717. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.633717. https://europepmc.org/abstract/MED/34017272 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Green MF, Horan WP, Lee J. Social cognition in schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16(10):620–631. doi: 10.1038/nrn4005.nrn4005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hoekert M, Kahn RS, Pijnenborg M, Aleman A. Impaired recognition and expression of emotional prosody in schizophrenia: review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2007;96(1-3):135–145. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.07.023.S0920-9964(07)00322-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Edwards J, Jackson HJ, Pattison PE. Emotion recognition via facial expression and affective prosody in schizophrenia: a methodological review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2002;22(6):789–832. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(02)00130-7.S0272-7358(02)00130-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Kee KS, Green MF, Mintz J, Brekke JS. Is emotion processing a predictor of functional outcome in schizophrenia? Schizophr Bull. 2003;29(3):487–497. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Irani F, Seligman S, Kamath V, Kohler C, Gur RC. A meta-analysis of emotion perception and functional outcomes in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2012;137(1-3):203–211. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.01.023. https://europepmc.org/abstract/MED/22341200 .S0920-9964(12)00048-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Hall JA. Gender effects in decoding nonverbal cues. Psychol Bull. 1978;85(4):845–857. doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.85.4.845. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Hall J, Carter J, Horgan T, Fischer AH. Gender and Emotion: Social Psychological Perspectives. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Thompson AE, Voyer D. Sex differences in the ability to recognise non-verbal displays of emotion: a meta-analysis. Cogn Emot. 2014;28(7):1164–1195. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2013.875889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Danaher H, Allott K, Killackey E, Hester R, Cotton S. An examination of sex differences in neurocognition and social cognition in first-episode psychosis. Psychiatry Res. 2018;259:36–43. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.09.053.S0165-1781(17)30173-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Fernández-Sotos P, Torio I, Fernández-Caballero A, Navarro E, González P, Dompablo M, Rodriguez-Jimenez R. Social cognition remediation interventions: a systematic mapping review. PLoS One. 2019;14(6):e0218720. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0218720. https://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218720 .PONE-D-18-31063 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Bell M, Bryson G, Lysaker P. Positive and negative affect recognition in schizophrenia: a comparison with substance abuse and normal control subjects. Psychiatry Res. 1997 Nov 14;73(1-2):73–82. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(97)00111-x. https://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0165-1781(97)00111-X .S0165-1781(97)00111-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Gur Ruben C, Sara Radim, Hagendoorn Michiel, Marom Oren, Hughett Paul, Macy Larry, Turner Travis, Bajcsy Ruzena, Posner Aaron, Gur Raquel E. A method for obtaining 3-dimensional facial expressions and its standardization for use in neurocognitive studies. J Neurosci Methods. 2002 Apr 15;115(2):137–143. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0270(02)00006-7.S0165027002000067 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Ochoa S, Usall J, Cobo J, Labad X, Kulkarni J. Gender differences in schizophrenia and first-episode psychosis: a comprehensive literature review. Schizophr Res Treatment. 2012;2012:916198. doi: 10.1155/2012/916198. doi: 10.1155/2012/916198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Riecher-Rössler A, Butler S, Kulkarni J. Sex and gender differences in schizophrenic psychoses-a critical review. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2018;21(6):627–648. doi: 10.1007/s00737-018-0847-9.10.1007/s00737-018-0847-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Seeman MV. Does gender influence outcome in schizophrenia? Psychiatr Q. 2019;90(1):173–184. doi: 10.1007/s11126-018-9619-y.10.1007/s11126-018-9619-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Li X, Zhou W, Yi Z. A glimpse of gender differences in schizophrenia. Gen Psychiatr. 2022;35(4):e100823. doi: 10.1136/gpsych-2022-100823. https://europepmc.org/abstract/MED/36118418 .gpsych-2022-100823 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Mote J, Kring AM. Facial emotion perception in schizophrenia: does sex matter? World J Psychiatry. 2016;6(2):257–268. doi: 10.5498/wjp.v6.i2.257. https://www.wjgnet.com/2220-3206/full/v6/i2/257.htm . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Fernando P, Sommer IEC, Hasan A. Do we need sex-oriented clinical practice guidelines for the treatment of schizophrenia? Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2020;33(3):192–199. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000597.00001504-202005000-00003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Cowman M, Holleran L, Lonergan E, O'Connor K, Birchwood M, Donohoe G. Cognitive predictors of social and occupational functioning in early psychosis: a systematic review and meta-analysis of cross-sectional and longitudinal data. Schizophr Bull. 2021;47(5):1243–1253. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbab033. https://europepmc.org/abstract/MED/33761534 .6185136 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Schmidt SJ, Mueller DR, Roder V. Social cognition as a mediator variable between neurocognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: empirical review and new results by structural equation modeling. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37 Suppl 2(Suppl 2):S41–S54. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr079. https://europepmc.org/abstract/MED/21860046 .sbr079 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Shamseer L, Moher D, Clarke M, Ghersi D, Liberati A, Petticrew M, Shekelle P, Stewart LA, PRISMA-P Group Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015: elaboration and explanation. BMJ. 2015;350:g7647. doi: 10.1136/bmj.g7647. http://www.bmj.com/lookup/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=25555855 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Moher D, Shamseer L, Clarke M, Ghersi D, Liberati A, Petticrew M, Shekelle P, Stewart LA, PRISMA-P Group Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement. Syst Rev. 2015;4(1):1. doi: 10.1186/2046-4053-4-1. https://systematicreviewsjournal.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/2046-4053-4-1 .2046-4053-4-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Clark JM, Sanders S, Carter M, Honeyman D, Cleo G, Auld Y, Booth D, Condron P, Dalais C, Bateup S, Linthwaite B, May N, Munn J, Ramsay L, Rickett K, Rutter C, Smith A, Sondergeld P, Wallin M, Jones M, Beller E. Improving the translation of search strategies using the polyglot search translator: a randomized controlled trial. J Med Libr Assoc. 2020;108(2):195–207. doi: 10.5195/jmla.2020.834. https://europepmc.org/abstract/MED/32256231 .jmla-108-195 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Covidence Covidence systematic review software Melbourne, Australia: Veritas Health Innovation. 2023. [2024-08-22]. https://www.covidence.org/
  • 42.Page MJ, Moher D, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, Shamseer L, Tetzlaff JM, Akl EA, Brennan SE, Chou R, Glanville J, Grimshaw JM, Hróbjartsson A, Lalu MM, Li T, Loder EW, Mayo-Wilson E, McDonald S, McGuinness LA, Stewart LA, Thomas J, Tricco AC, Welch VA, Whiting P, McKenzie JE. PRISMA 2020 explanation and elaboration: updated guidance and exemplars for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ. 2021;372:n160. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n160. http://www.bmj.com/lookup/pmidlookup?view=long&pmid=33781993 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Barker TH, Stone JC, Sears K, Klugar M, Tufanaru C, Leonardi-Bee J, Aromataris E, Munn Z. The revised JBI critical appraisal tool for the assessment of risk of bias for randomized controlled trials. JBI Evidence Synthesis. 2023;21(3):494–506. doi: 10.11124/JBIES-22-00430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Guyatt G, Oxman AD, Akl EA, Kunz R, Vist G, Brozek J, Norris S, Falck-Ytter Y, Glasziou P, DeBeer H, Jaeschke R, Rind D, Meerpohl J, Dahm P, Schünemann HJ. GRADE guidelines: 1. Introduction—GRADE evidence profiles and summary of findings tables. J Clin Epidemiol. 2011;64(4):383–394. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.04.026.S0895-4356(10)00330-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Multimedia Appendix 1

Draft MEDLINE search—PubMed interface.

Data Availability Statement

Access to research data is restricted and governed by the Queensland Health Government; however, extracted data may be available upon request. All researchers will have access to the final data set. Extracted data will be made publicly available as multimedia appendices.


Articles from JMIR Research Protocols are provided here courtesy of JMIR Publications Inc.

RESOURCES