Skip to main content
PLOS Biology logoLink to PLOS Biology
. 2024 Sep 18;22(9):e3002653. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3002653

Functional network modules overlap and are linked to interindividual connectome differences during human brain development

Tianyuan Lei 1,2,3,4, Xuhong Liao 5,*, Xinyuan Liang 2,3,4, Lianglong Sun 2,3,4, Mingrui Xia 2,3,4, Yunman Xia 2,3,4, Tengda Zhao 2,3,4, Xiaodan Chen 2,3,4, Weiwei Men 6,7, Yanpei Wang 2, Leilei Ma 2, Ningyu Liu 2, Jing Lu 2, Gai Zhao 2, Yuyin Ding 2, Yao Deng 2, Jiali Wang 2, Rui Chen 2, Haibo Zhang 2, Shuping Tan 8, Jia-Hong Gao 6,7,9, Shaozheng Qin 2,3,4,10, Sha Tao 2, Qi Dong 2, Yong He 2,3,4,10,*
Editor: Claus C Hilgetag11
PMCID: PMC11441662  PMID: 39292711

Abstract

The modular structure of functional connectomes in the human brain undergoes substantial reorganization during development. However, previous studies have implicitly assumed that each region participates in one single module, ignoring the potential spatial overlap between modules. How the overlapping functional modules develop and whether this development is related to gray and white matter features remain unknown. Using longitudinal multimodal structural, functional, and diffusion MRI data from 305 children (aged 6 to 14 years), we investigated the maturation of overlapping modules of functional networks and further revealed their structural associations. An edge-centric network model was used to identify the overlapping modules, and the nodal overlap in module affiliations was quantified using the entropy measure. We showed a regionally heterogeneous spatial topography of the overlapping extent of brain nodes in module affiliations in children, with higher entropy (i.e., more module involvement) in the ventral attention, somatomotor, and subcortical regions and lower entropy (i.e., less module involvement) in the visual and default-mode regions. The overlapping modules developed in a linear, spatially dissociable manner, with decreased entropy (i.e., decreased module involvement) in the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, ventral prefrontal cortex, and putamen and increased entropy (i.e., increased module involvement) in the parietal lobules and lateral prefrontal cortex. The overlapping modular patterns captured individual brain maturity as characterized by chronological age and were predicted by integrating gray matter morphology and white matter microstructural properties. Our findings highlight the maturation of overlapping functional modules and their structural substrates, thereby advancing our understanding of the principles of connectome development.

Introduction

Childhood and adolescence is a period of transition from infancy to adulthood, which is critical for the maturation and improvement of motor, cognitive, emotional, and social functions [1,2]. During this period, the brain undergoes progressive and regressive maturation in its microscopic and macroscopic anatomy, such as increased myelination [3,4], synaptic pruning [46], and cortical thinning [4,79]. From the perspective of function, remarkable reconfigurations have also been observed for task-evoked regional activity [10,11] and task-free spontaneous activity [12]. These structural and functional changes are critical for improvements in children’s cognitive and behavioral performance [10,11,13]. Notably, the periods of childhood and adolescence are also critical windows for the onset of many psychiatric disorders [14]. Exploring brain developmental principles in children and adolescents would provide insights into understanding not only cognitive and behavioral growth but also neurodevelopmental disorders.

Over the past 2 decades, neuroimaging-based connectomics has provided a valuable framework for investigating the developmental principles of brain function [1517]. The functional modular structure, which is characterized by dense within-module connections and sparse between-module connections, has attracted great attention [1821]. The modular structure of the brain is particularly important for global network communications, as it can facilitate efficient information segregation and integration with low wiring costs [20,22]. Several studies have reported age-related changes in modular organization with development [15,16,2325]. Specifically, functional modular organization is already present in fetuses [26] and neonates [27,28]; in this configuration, the modules in the primary cortex show an adult-like topography, and the modules in the association cortex are far from mature. The modular architecture further undergoes an elaborate reconfiguration from childhood to adulthood [15,16,23,24]. Its spatial layout shifts from an anatomical proximity to a spatially distributed and functionally related configuration [25]. These changes have been linked to the development of individual cognition and behavior, such as cognitive control [24] and general cognitive function [23].

Despite this substantial progress, previous connectome development studies have focused primarily on modular structure without spatial overlap (i.e., hard assignment), implicitly assuming that each brain node belongs to 1 single functional module. This assumption may be problematic because the modular structures of real-world networks, such as cooperation networks in social systems and protein networks in nature, generally show overlapping properties [29,30]. This overlapping modular framework in complex networks provides important insights into the potential diverse functional roles of nodes in the network. Several recent functional network studies have also reported overlapping modular organization in adults [3137], indicating that brain regions are not restricted to one specific module. In particular, the overlap between functional modules is spatially heterogeneous [3235], with higher overlapping regions being crucial for intermodule communication [38] and global network efficiency [33]. The presence of overlapping modules is also related to spatial heterogeneity in functional diversity and neurocognitive flexibility [32,33,39], and changes with aging [39,40]. However, the network growth principle of the overlapping modular organization in the functional connectome and its association with gray and white matter features remain unknown.

To fill these knowledge gaps, we investigated the development of the overlapping modular architecture of functional connectomes during childhood and adolescence and examined their associations with gray and white matter features using a large longitudinal multimodal neuroimaging data set from 305 typically developing children (aged 6 to 14 years, 491 scans in total) [4143]. Specifically, we investigated the development of the overlapping functional modules using resting-state fMRI (rsfMRI) data and further revealed the underlying structural substrates of the overlapping modules using structural and diffusion MRI data. In the present study, we identified overlapping functional modules using an edge-centric module detection approach [44], which makes no assumptions about the probabilistic membership of nodes. This approach provides an intuitive way to assign nodes to various communities based on the module affiliations of their edges (referred to as nodal soft partitioning) [45]. The overlapping extent of each brain node in the module affiliations was quantified with an entropy measure [34]. We then examined the development in the overlapping extent of children’s brain networks at the global, system, and nodal levels, and further assessed whether the spatial pattern of the modular overlap can predict brain maturity. Finally, we investigated the potential structural substrates involved in the development of overlapping functional modules.

Results

We leveraged longitudinal rsfMRI data from 305 children (aged 6 to14 y, 491 scans), including 3 repeated scans from 47 children, 2 repeated scans from 92 children, and 1 scan from 166 children (Fig 1A). For comparison purposes, we also included cross-sectional rsfMRI data from a group of healthy adults (n = 61, aged 18 to 29 y). Both children and adults were scanned using the same scanner with identical protocols. All MR images used here underwent strict quality control (see Materials and methods). We identified the overlapping modules in individual- and group-level functional networks using an edge-centric module detection algorithm [30,44] (Fig 1B). Briefly, we first constructed a traditional functional network comprising nodal regions and interregional connections (i.e., edges). Then, we constructed a weighted edge-based brain graph that represented the similarity of connectivity profiles between edges (see Materials and methods). Finally, we identified module affiliations of each node according to the module assignments of its edges in the corresponding edge graph. A measure of entropy was used to estimate the extent of modular overlap for each node by quantifying the distribution of module affiliations of the edges attached to this node [34] (Fig 1B).

Fig 1. Data information and schematic diagram of the overlapping modular architecture based on the edge-centric module detection.

Fig 1

(A) Age distribution of longitudinal rsfMRI scans of children. (B) (i) Traditional brain functional connectivity network. In this network, each node denotes a brain region of interest, and each link denotes the interregional functional connectivity. (ii) Edge graph corresponding to a given functional network. In this graph, each node denotes an edge in the functional network, and each link is defined as the similarity between edges in the connectivity profiles using Tanimoto coefficient [30,46]. For 2 given edges eik and ejk that share a common node k, the interedge similarity was estimated as the similarity of connectivity profiles between node i and node j, wherein ai represents the modified connectivity profile of node i, aiaj represents the dot product of 2 vectors ai and aj, and |ai|2 denotes the sum of the squared weights of all connections of node i. (iii) Edge-centric module detection. Each edge is assigned to a specific module based on the Louvain algorithm [47]. (iv) Definition of regional module overlap. Each nodal region was assigned to one or more modules due to the diverse module affiliations of its edges. A measure of entropy was employed to quantify the extent of module overlap of each brain node by measuring the distribution of the module affiliations of its edges [34]. rsfMRI, resting-state fMRI.

Spatial topography of the overlapping functional modules in children and adults

We first identified the overlapping functional modules in healthy young adults, which serves as a reference for exploring the development of the overlapping modules in children. We found 7 modules in the weighted edge-based brain graph of the adult group (Fig 2A) and further showed the corresponding topographic distribution of each module (Fig 2B and 2C). These functional modules showed substantial spatial overlap, as characterized by 73% of the nodal regions belonging to 2 or more modules (Fig 2B). Module I was mainly located in the medial and lateral prefrontal and parietal cortex, and lateral temporal cortex; module II was mainly located in the primary motor and somatosensory cortices; module III was mainly located in the insula, supramarginal gyrus, superior temporal gyrus, and paracentral lobule; module IV was mainly located in the cingulate gyrus and the subcortical area; module V was located in the visual cortex and the superior parietal lobule; module VI was located in the middle frontal gyrus and superior parietal lobule; and module VII was primarily located in the temporal pole, hippocampus, and amygdala (Fig 2C).

Fig 2. Overlapping modular architecture of group-level brain functional networks in the adult cohort and the child subgroups.

Fig 2

(A) Modular organization in the weighted edge graph in adults. The edges were sorted according to their module affiliations. Each element denotes the interedge similarity in their connectivity profiles. (B) Distribution of the involved module number across brain nodes. Notably, 27% of the nodes belonged to one module, and 73% of the nodes belonged to 2 or more modules. (C) Topographic distributions of 7 functional modules. For each module, nodal values represent the proportion of edges assigned to that module. (D) Spatial similarity of functional module maps between the child subgroups and the adult group. Each line represents the age-dependent similarity for a particular module. (E) System-dependent spatial distributions of functional modules. For each functional module of each subgroup, we calculated the percentage of nodes distributed in eight systems, including 7 functional systems [48] and the subcortical area [49]. Given a prior system, the bar chart shows the percentage of nodes located in this system for 8 child subgroups with a one-year interval (i.e., 6–7 yrs, 7–8 yrs, 8–9 yrs, 9–10 yrs, 10–11yrs, 11–12 yrs, 12–13 yrs, and 13–14 yrs) and the adult cohort. In (C) and (D), cortical data were mapped on the brain surface using BrainNet Viewer software [52]. The data underlying this figure can be found at https://osf.io/qfcyu/. VIS, visual; SM, somatomotor; DA, dorsal attention; VA, ventral attention; LIM, limbic; FP, frontoparietal; DM, default-mode; SUB, subcortical; yrs, years.

We further divided all the children’s rsfMRI scans into 8 subgroups with a one-year interval, and the adult group was set as the ninth subgroup for comparison. We identified the overlapping modular architecture for each subgroup of children based on the rsfMRI data. The modules in each child subgroup were matched with those in the adult subgroup. In general, most functional modules in the child subgroups showed high spatial similarity with those in the adult subgroup (Pearson’s correlation rs: mean ± SD = 0.75 ± 0.02, range: 0.25 to 0.95) (Fig 2D). Initial inspection revealed that the spatial similarity with the adult group tended to increase with age for all modules, except modules V and VI. For each module, the spatial distribution of nodes among prior functional systems [48,49] was largely consistent across all child subgroups and the adult cohort (Fig 2E). Module I mainly involved the default-mode and frontoparietal systems; module II mainly involved the somatomotor, ventral attention, and dorsal attention systems; module III mainly involved the somatomotor and ventral attention systems; module IV mainly involved the ventral attention, frontoparietal, and default-mode systems, and the subcortical area; module V mainly involved the visual and dorsal attention systems; module VI mainly involved the visual, somatomotor, dorsal attention, and ventral attention systems; and module VII mainly involved the limbic and default-mode systems and the subcortical area. Interestingly, we found that the nodal regions contained in each module mainly belong to the functional systems located in the adjacent hierarchy [50,51], regardless of the subgroup.

Development of the overlapping functional modules during childhood and adolescence

We employed the mixed effects model [53,54] to quantify the longitudinal changes in the overlapping modular structure during childhood and adolescence. At the global level, the number of modules in the edge-based brain graphs significantly decreased with age (linear model, t = −2.09, p = 0.037), and the modularity tended to increase with age (linear model, t = 1.89, p = 0.059) (Fig 3A). The global entropy (i.e., mean nodal entropy across the brain) did not show significant correlation with age (t = −1.54, p = 0.12). At the regional level, the spatial topography of the nodal overlap (i.e., entropy) of every child subgroup was highly similar to that of the adult subgroup (Pearson’s correlation rs ranged from 0.74 to 0.91). Specifically, for each age subgroup, regions with higher levels of module overlap (i.e., higher entropy) were located mainly in the insula, supramarginal gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus, somatosensory cortex, anterior cingulate gyrus, superior temporal gyrus, and subcortical regions (e.g., putamen), and regions with lower levels of module overlap (i.e., lower entropy) were located mainly in the visual cortex, angular gyrus, and posterior cingulate gyrus (Fig 3B). Statistical analysis revealed that nodal entropy in 7 nodal regions showed significant linear changes with age (FDR-corrected p < 0.05, Fig 3C). These regions showed dissociable developmental patterns, with significant increases in entropy (i.e., increased module involvement) mainly in the superior and inferior parietal lobules and lateral prefrontal cortex and significant decreases in entropy (i.e., decreased module involvement) mainly in the ventral prefrontal cortex, dorsal medial prefrontal cortex, and putamen.

Fig 3. Longitudinal development of overlapping functional modules at the global and nodal levels.

Fig 3

(A) Left: Age effect on the number of modules. Right: Age effect on modularity in the edge graph. (B) Spatial patterns of functional module overlap (i.e., nodal entropy) across the brain for each child subgroup and for the adult group. (C) Spatial distribution of regions showing significant developmental changes in nodal entropy. Age effects are displayed in terms of t values (FDR-corrected p < 0.05, corresponding to uncorrected p < 0.0014). In (A) and (C), the boxplot represents the distribution of the adult group for reference. The blue lines connecting scattered points represent longitudinal scans of the same child. The adjusted value denotes the measure of interest corrected for sex, head motion, and random age effects. The data underlying this figure can be found at https://osf.io/qfcyu/. yrs, years; LPFC, lateral prefrontal cortex; SPL, superior parietal lobule; IPL, inferior parietal lobule; MPFC, medial prefrontal cortex; VPFC, ventral prefrontal cortex.

At the system level, two-way repeated analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed that nodal entropy exhibited a system-dependent distribution (system effect: F(7,77) = 117.30, p < 0.0001; age subgroup effect: F(7,77) = 1.21, p = 0.31; interaction effect: F(49,539) = 1.44, p = 0.03) (Fig 4A). Specifically, the ventral attention and somatomotor systems showed greater module overlap (i.e., higher entropy), while the visual and default-mode systems showed lower module overlap (i.e., lower entropy). Quantitative analysis revealed that the entropy of the dorsal attention system significantly increased with age (linear model, t = 2.44, p = 0.015), while the entropy in the subcortical area significantly decreased with age (linear model, t = −3.13, p = 0.0019, Fig 4B).

Fig 4. Longitudinal development of the overlapping functional modules at the system level.

Fig 4

(A) Distribution of nodal entropy within each functional system for each child subgroup and for the adult group. Given a prior system, the bar chart shows the distribution of the average entropy of this system across individuals for 8 child subgroups with a one-year interval (i.e., 6–7 yrs, 7–8 yrs, 8–9 yrs, 9–10 yrs, 10–11yrs, 11–12 yrs, 12–13 yrs, and 13–14 yrs) and the adult cohort. Here, the circle with a dot denotes the median, and the box denotes the interquartile range. (B) Two functional systems showing significant developmental changes in functional module overlap (i.e., nodal entropy). The boxplot represents the distribution of the adult group for reference. Short lines connecting scattered points represent longitudinal scans of the same child. The adjusted value denotes the measure of interest corrected for sex, head motion, and random age effects. The data underlying this figure can be found at https://osf.io/qfcyu/. VIS, visual; SM, somatomotor; DA, dorsal attention; VA, ventral attention; LIM, limbic; FP, frontoparietal; DM, default-mode; SUB, subcortical; yrs, year.

Association of cognitive functions with developmental changes in overlapping modules

We then performed a meta-analysis using the NeuroSynth database [55] to explore the potential cognitive significance associated with developmental changes in the overlapping modules. The statistical map of the developmental changes in nodal entropy was divided into 10 bins with decreasing age-related T values. We found that the nodal regions that increased with age (bins of 0%–10% and 10%–20%), such as the lateral prefrontal cortex, superior parietal lobule, and inferior parietal lobule, were mainly involved in the cognitive terms “motor imagery,” “visual perception,” “spatial,” and “eye movements” (Fig 5). Nodal regions showing decreases with age (bins of 80%–90% and 90%–100%), such as the ventral prefrontal cortex, the dorsal medial prefrontal cortex, and the putamen, were mainly associated with the cognitive terms of “speech,” “word form,” “sound,” and “auditory.”

Fig 5. Cognitive function decoding of brain regions.

Fig 5

Nodal regions were sorted in descending order according to developmental changes in the functional module overlap. The results were obtained based on the information in the NeuroSynth meta-analytic database [55]. The significance level of the spatial similarity was assessed using permutation tests (n = 10,000) that corrected for spatial autocorrelation [56]. The data underlying this figure can be found at https://osf.io/qfcyu/. *, pperm < 0.05; **, pperm < 0.01; ***, pperm < 0.001.

Predicting chronological age from spatial topography of nodal overlap

We investigated whether the spatial topography of nodal overlap in the network modules could be used to predict individual chronological age. Linear support vector regression (SVR) was used with 10-fold cross-validation (Fig 6A). To avoid the possibility of data leakage that could occur by including scans from the same children in both the training and test sets, a total of 305 rsfMRI scans from independent subjects were selected for use in the prediction analysis. For a given individual with longitudinal scans, only one scan was randomly selected from the individual. The age prediction analysis was repeated 1,000 times with random scan selection. We found that the spatial patterns of nodal entropy significantly predicted individual chronological age (r = 0.14 ~ 0.52, all pperms < 0.05; Fig 6B). The significance level of the prediction accuracy was assessed with permutation tests (n = 10,000) in which the original age was shuffled across children. Regions with high contributions were primarily located in the dorsal attention, ventral attention, and default-mode systems (Fig 6C). Furthermore, we found that the nodal contribution weights showed a significant positive correlation with the developmental changes in nodal entropy in terms of age-related t values (Pearson’s correlation r = 0.48, pperm < 0.0001; Fig 6D). The significance level of the spatial similarity was assessed using permutation tests (n = 10,000) to correct for spatial autocorrelation [56]. This result suggests that brain regions showing age-related changes play a crucial role in predicting chronological age.

Fig 6. Age prediction based on spatial patterns of nodal overlap.

Fig 6

(A) Schematic representation of the SVR prediction model based on 10-fold cross-validation. (B) Frequency polygon for age prediction accuracy using the 10-fold SVR model for 1,000 times of randomly selected samples (blue histogram). For each time of random sampling, rsfMRI scans were randomly selected from 305 independent subjects. The gray frequency polygon in the inset displays the null distribution of prediction accuracy based on the permutation tests (n = 10,000) by randomly sampling the scans and shuffling the original ages across the scans. The red line in the blue histogram indicates the significant level (p < 0.05) derived from the null distribution. (C) Spatial distributions of the nodal contribution in the prediction model. Left: Contribution weight at the regional level. Right: Contribution weight at the system level. The nodal contribution weights were obtained by averaging the contribution weights across 1,000 times of randomly selected samples. Positive and negative weights were separately averaged within each system. (D) Left: Spatial pattern of age effect of on nodal overlap in terms of t values. Right: Nodal contribution weight shows a significant positive correlation with the development of nodal overlap based on Pearson’s correlation analysis. The significance level of the similarity was assessed using the permutation tests (n = 10,000) to correct for spatial autocorrelation [56]. The data underlying this figure can be found at https://osf.io/qfcyu/. Pos, positive; Neg, negative; VIS, visual; SM, somatomotor; DA, dorsal attention; VA, ventral attention; LIM, limbic; FP, frontoparietal; DM, default-mode; SUB, subcortical; SVR, support vector regression.

Predicting individual spatial topography of overlapping functional modules from structural brain features

We finally investigated whether structural features were related to the overlapping modules in children. In this analysis, we included 446 high-quality rsfMRI, structural and diffusion MRI scans from 279 children (aged 6 to 14 years, F/M = 138/141), with 3 repeated scans from 42 children, 2 repeated scans from 83 children, and 1 scan from 154 children. For each brain node of each child, we obtained 6 structural features, including 5 morphological measurements (cortical volume, thickness, curvature, folding index, and surface area) using structural MRI data and 1 white matter microstructural measure (fractional anisotropy (FA) strength) using diffusion MRI data. We initially examined the spatial similarity between nodal entropy in the overlapping functional modules and each structural feature within each scan using the univariate Pearson’s correlation. Cortical thickness showed a positive correlation with nodal entropy, while the other structural features exhibited negative correlations (S1 Fig). Next, the individual spatial pattern of nodal entropy was predicted by integrating all structural brain features using the SVR model. We found that the structural features significantly predicted the individual spatial pattern of nodal entropy for 95% of the scans (424/446), with a significance level of pperm < 0.05. The prediction accuracy varied across scans (mean ± SD: 0.35 ± 0.10), while the maximum prediction accuracy reached 0.59 (Fig 7A). The prediction contributions varied across structural features, with cortical thickness showing the greatest contribution (Fig 7B). Fig 7C and 7D show the prediction accuracy and contributing features for a representative child’s scan.

Fig 7. Prediction of individual spatial patterns of nodal module overlap from structural brain features in children.

Fig 7

(A) Frequency polygon for prediction accuracy of all rsfMRI scans. The inset in the upper left corner denotes the null distribution of the prediction accuracy based on permutation tests. To assess the statistical significance of the prediction accuracy, we generated a null distribution of accuracy using permutation tests by shuffling the original entropy values across nodes for each scan 100 times, thus leading to 44,600 (446 scans × 100 times) permutation instances in total. The red line denotes the 95% significance level in the null distribution. (B) Prediction contribution of different structural features in the prediction model of all rsfMRI scans. This histogram displays the mean contribution across subjects for different anatomical features, with each bar representing the mean accompanied by its standard deviation. (C) Accuracy of nodal entropy prediction for a representative child’s scan. (D) Spatial distributions of anatomical features for a representative child’s scan. In (C) and (D), the representative scan was selected as the scan that showed the highest prediction accuracy for the individual map of nodal module overlap (i.e., nodal entropy). The data underlying this figure can be found at https://osf.io/qfcyu/. CT, cortical thickness; SA, surface area; FI, folding index; FA, fractional anisotropy; CC, cortical curvature; CV, cortical volume.

Validation results

We first validated the procedures used for the estimation of nodal overlap in the functional module affiliations. Specifically, we re-estimated the nodal overlap in module affiliations in the adult group by considering the influence of several network construction and analysis strategies, including (i) spatial resolution of the parcellation (i.e., Schaefer-100) (S2 Fig); (ii) network thresholding strategies (i.e., 10% and 20%) (S3A and S3B Fig); (iii) module detection algorithms (S3C Fig); and (iv) measures for nodal overlap estimation (i.e., involved number) (S3D Fig). We found that the spatial pattern of nodal entropy remained almost unchanged in different cases, suggesting the robustness of the topography of the overlapping modular architecture.

Subsequently, the development of nodal entropy was validated by employing different network thresholding strategies (i.e., 10%, 20%, and 30%) (S4 Fig) and the generalized additive mixed model (GAMM) for estimating age effects (S5 Fig). The spatial patterns of age effects on nodal entropy remained highly similar to the main results, with spatial correlations ranging from 0.70 to 0.98. Furthermore, a more stringent head motion control strategy was implemented by excluding rsfMRI scans with mFD > 0.2 mm (retaining 355 scans from 233 children). The age effect map exhibited a high degree of spatial correlation (r = 0.89, pperm < 0.0001) with the main results (S6 Fig). These sensitive analyses indicate that the age effects on nodal entropy were robust across methodological variations.

Discussion

This study employs a large longitudinal multimodal MRI data set and an edge-centric module detection approach to elucidate the spatially dissociable development of overlapping functional modular organization and their structural associations during childhood and adolescence. Specifically, we observed the presence of an adult-like, spatially inhomogeneous nodal modular overlap in children. The nodal modular overlap (i.e., nodal entropy) developed with remarkable spatial heterogeneity, with significant decreases in the ventral and medial prefrontal cortex and putamen and significant increases in the superior and inferior parietal lobules and the lateral prefrontal cortex. These changes suggest an age-related reorganization in intermodule integration. The spatial patterns of nodal overlap were found to predict individual chronological age and were associated with morphological and white matter features, particularly cortical thickness. These findings highlight the gradual maturation of the overlapping modular architecture and its potential structural substrates during childhood and adolescence, providing novel insights into the developmental rules of the human brain.

The functional network exhibits an intricate community structure with multi-scale organization in both adults [57] and youths [58], and its spatial topography varies across individuals [21,36,37]. In this study, we used the novel framework of overlapping modular architectures, which provides an intuitive representation of the functional interactions between modules and the multiple roles of brain regions [59,60] and deepens the understanding of the organizational principles of the human brain [3135]. Compared to previous studies on overlapping modules in adults [3135] and the elderly [40], our research focuses on the development of overlapping functional modular organization during childhood and adolescence. We found that each functional module comprised nodal regions involved in at least 2 prior functional systems, regardless of the age subgroup. These systems contained in the same functional module have usually been found to be located in adjacent functional gradients [50]. This may be partially attributable to the fact that regions locating in proximate functional hierarchy tend to show strong functional interactions [58]. Taken together, these findings indicate that the overlapping modular architecture captures intersystem interactions at similar hierarchical levels. Interestingly, most of the prior functional systems were involved in 3 or more overlapping modules, except for the visual and limbic systems, suggesting diverse functional roles or functional differentiation within each brain system. Specifically, the default-mode regions were found to be involved in three overlapping modules (i.e., modules I, IV, and VII), which is in line with the previous identification of 3 subsystems within the default-mode system in both children [42] and adults [61]. From a dynamic perspective, functional modular organization spontaneously reconfigures on a short time scale (e.g., seconds) with regions switching among modules [21]. The observed spatial overlap of functional modules may be a summary of the dynamic reorganization at a long-term scale. However, the relationship between the overlapping and dynamic modular architectures warrants further investigation.

We observed that the topographical distribution of the module overlap in children and adolescents showed an adult-like spatially inhomogeneous pattern. This suggests that the overlapping modular architecture is taking shape in children and adolescents. High nodal overlap was primarily located in the ventral attention, somatomotor, and medial and inferior frontal regions, which is consistent with the findings of a recent study in adults [34]. The high nodal overlap of the ventral attention system may be due to its involvement in multiple general domain categories, as revealed in a meta-analytic study [59], including cognition (e.g., attention), perception (e.g., somesthesis), and action domains (e.g., imagination). During a movie watching task, the module overlap level of the ventral attention system increased significantly [34], further indicating that the module overlap of the ventral attention system may capture regional involvement in task transitions. The high nodal overlap in the sensorimotor regions may indicate a high level of between-module integration of this system, which aligns with previous findings that sensorimotor networks exhibit greater between-network coupling in comparison to association cortices [58,62]. The high nodal entropy in these regions may be attributed, in part, to their tendency to exhibit nested multi-scale organization, as demonstrated in previous studies [63]. Despite the highly specialized functions of sensorimotor regions [50,60], the potential functional significance of the high overlap in these regions warrants further investigation.

Some recent studies have identified fuzzy network affiliations of brain regions in children using the clustering or template matching approaches, but ignoring their developmental changes at this stage [36,64]. Here, we found the maturation in the nodal modular overlap (i.e., nodal entropy) is regionally heterogeneous. The decreased nodal overlap of subcortical areas reflects the gradually decreased involvement of the subcortical system in modules III and V, which are related to somatomotor, attention, and visual functions (Fig 2E). A recent study reported that the strength of cortico-subcortical functional connectivity varies with age, with increasing connections between subcortical and association regions and weakening connections between subcortical and primary regions [65]. The age-related decreased overlap of the subcortical regions suggests that the enhanced functional segregation between the subcortical area and the primary system may be more dominant. In the prefrontal cortex, we observed different age-related changes in nodal overlap for the lateral prefrontal regions and the dorsal medial prefrontal regions. The divergent changes might be due to the fact that these 2 regions belong to different sub-modules of the frontoparietal system [66]. The lateral prefrontal region strongly connects with the default-mode system that is primarily involved in the regulation of introspective processes, and the dorsal medial prefrontal region strongly connects with the dorsal attention system that is mainly involved in the regulation of visuospatial perceptual attention [66]. The significant prediction of an individual’s chronological age based on nodal overlap patterns further demonstrated substantial changes in the overlapping modular architecture during childhood and adolescence. Of note, during the adult lifespan, the age-dependent overlapping functional modules are associated with fluid cognition [40]. Further exploration is warranted regarding the potential relationship between the maturation of the overlapping functional modules and individual cognitive development.

The functional activity of the human brain is supported and sculpted by the underlying anatomical structure [67]. During childhood and adolescence, both gray matter and white matter of the brain undergo elaborate reconfiguration. The cortical surface area gradually reaches a peak, and the gray matter volume of the whole brain decreases [9]. Moreover, the white matter volume continues to increase [9,68]. Here, we found that these developing anatomical features of gray matter and white matter could be used to significantly predict the spatial patterns of functional module overlap at the individual level. Of all the measures considered, cortical thickness was found to make the largest contribution. Previous studies have documented that cortical thickness shows a remarkable thinning during childhood and adolescence [9,69,70]. The cortical thinning may be related to the microstructural changes within the cortical layers, such as synaptic pruning and intracortical myelination, as well as mechanical forces that affect cortical morphology [4]. Interestingly, a previous study has shown that the coordinated development of cortical thickness during adolescence shows a similar pattern of functional connectivity [71]. These results provide a link between the overlapping functional modular architecture and structural features in the brain.

Previous studies on overlapping modules in the human brain have primarily used node-centric methods [32,33,35,39], usually assuming probabilistic membership or maximal clique for nodes, which struggle to capture intricate overlapping configuration [72]. In this study, we employed an edge-centric algorithm [30,44] to identify the overlapping modular architecture for brain nodes, offering an intuitive and detailed representation of multiple module affiliations of nodes [30,34]. A recent study suggests that the overlapping modular architecture revealed by the edge-centric approach can be explained by surrogate interedge similarity derived from the node-level functional connectivity [73]. Further research is needed to explore the differences between these 2 approaches. Of note, identifying the overlapping modular architecture using the current edge-centric method faces several challenges. First, uncertainties remain regarding whether and how the functional correlation matrix is thresholded to construct a stable node-level functional network [74,75]. Second, this study only considers the relationships between edge pairs that share common nodes, neglecting potential indirect relationships between edges. Higher-order relationships between edges could be considered in future studies.

Several issues need to be further considered. First, inner-scan head motion introduces a spatially inhomogeneous bias in functional connectivity estimation, which is age dependent [76]. To reduce the potential influence of head motion, we excluded rsfMRI scans with extensive head motion and further included 24 head parameters, global brain signals, and “bad” time points during nuisance regression [77,78]. The individual head motion parameters (i.e., mFD) were also included in the mixed effects model when assessing the age effects [77,79]. When performing a more stringent head motion control, the developmental effects of nodal entropy remained almost unchanged. These results suggest that the head motion has a limited effect on our findings. Nevertheless, it would be valuable to further refine head motion correction methods to mitigate the residual influence of head motion on functional network development. Second, this study used a large longitudinal data set of children and adolescents, but the age range was limited to school-age children. Previous studies have already identified changes in the overlapping modular properties of the human brain during aging [39,40]. Future studies should extend the period of interest by adding more data sets to chart a lifespan trajectory. Third, the structure–function association was established using an SVR prediction analysis. However, the spatial distribution of the overlapping modules in functional networks does not align consistently with that in structural networks [44,80]. Further work is needed to elucidate how structural brain networks shape functional overlapping modules during development by establishing computational network models, such as communication models [81] and large-scale dynamic modeling [82]. Finally, we explored the typical developmental changes in the overlapping modules in children and adolescents. Since adolescence is the most common period for the onset of mental disorders [14], the overlapping modular organization may be altered in individuals with neurodevelopmental disorders, such as autism spectrum disorders or attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Investigating the overlapping modular organization in these disorders may deepen insights into the pathological mechanism underlying atypical development.

Methods

Participants

A longitudinal multimodal MRI data set of 360 healthy children was obtained from the Children School Functions and Brain Development Project (Beijing Cohort) [4143]. All participants were cognitively normal, did not use psychotropic medication, and had no history of severe traumatic brain injury. These children underwent longitudinal rsfMRI, structural and diffusion MRI scans at intervals of approximately 1 year. We performed strict quality control on the rsfMRI data and excluded scans with field map errors, excessive head motion (see “Data preprocessing”), excessive “bad” time points, or T1 artifacts. Finally, 491 scans of 305 children (aged 6 to 14 years, F/M = 143/162) remained, including 3 scans from 47 children, 2 scans from 92 children, and 1 scan from 166 children. Notably, 45 rsfMRI scans were further excluded from the subsequent structural association analysis due to the poor quality of the corresponding dMRI data (see “Data preprocessing”). The remained 446 rsfMRI scans from 279 children (aged 6 to 14 years, F/M = 138/141) were used in the structure–function association analysis, including 3 scans from 42 children, 2 scans from 83 children, and 1 scan from 154 children. In addition, we employed rsfMRI scans of 61 healthy young adults (aged 18 to 29 years, F/M = 37/24) for comparison. All participants or their parents/guardians provided written informed consent. This study was designed and performed according to the principles of the Helsinki Declaration and was approved by the Ethics Committee of Beijing Normal University (approval No. IRB_A_0004_2019001).

Imaging acquisition

Multimodal magnetic resonance images were acquired on a 3T Siemens Prisma scanner with a 64-channel head coil at the Center for Magnetic Resonance Imaging Research at Peking University. Both children and adults underwent multimodal scanning using the following protocols.

  • (i) Functional MRI. Resting-state scans were acquired using an echo-planar imaging sequence: repetition time (TR) = 2,000 ms, echo time (TE) = 30 ms, flip angle = 90°, field of view (FOV) = 224 × 224 mm2, acquisition matrix = 64 × 64, slice number = 33, and slice thickness/gap = 3.5/0.7 mm. All participants were asked to fixate on a bright crosshair displayed in the center of the scanner screen. The total duration of the rsfMRI scans was 8 min (i.e., 240 volumes).

  • (ii) Field maps for functional MRI. The scans were acquired using a 2D dual gradient-echo sequence: TR = 400 ms, TE1 = 4.92 ms, TE2 = 7.38 ms, flip angle = 60°, FOV = 224 × 224 mm2, acquisition matrix = 64 × 64, slice number = 33, and slice thickness/gap = 3.5/0.7 mm.

  • (iii) T1-weighted structural MRI. The scans were acquired using a sagittal 3D magnetization prepared rapid acquisition gradient-echo (MPRAGE) sequence: TR = 2,530 ms, TE = 2.98 ms, flip angle = 7°, FOV = 256 × 224 mm2, acquisition matrix = 256 × 224, inversion time = 1,100 ms, slice number = 192, slice thickness = 1 mm, and bandwidth = 240 Hz/Px.

  • (iv) Diffusion MRI. The scans were acquired using a single-shell high angular resolution diffusion imaging (HARDI) sequence [83,84]: TR = 7,500 ms, TE = 64 ms, flip angle = 90°, FOV = 224 × 224 mm2, acquisition matrix = 112 × 112, slice number = 70, slice thickness = 2 mm, and bandwidth = 2,030 Hz/Px. The complete sequence consisted of 64 diffusion-weighted directions (b-value = 1,000 s/mm2) and 10 non-diffusion-weighted directions (b-value = 0 s/mm2). The use of multiple gradient directions allows more accurate detection of diffusion variations along different directions [83,84], thus resolving multiple fiber orientations within a single voxel and improving the accuracy of tractography.

Data preprocessing

  • (i) Functional MRI data. The functional images of all the children were preprocessed using SPM12 (https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm) and DPABI 3.0 [85]. First, we removed the first 10 volumes and performed slice-timing correction. Next, we applied field map correction to reduce geometric distortion and realigned the volumes over time. After realignment, 94 rsfMRI scans were excluded due to excessive head motion with a criterion of maximum head motion > 3 mm or 3° or mean framewise displacement (mFD) [86] >0.5 mm. Then, the functional images were coregistered with individual T1 images and spatially normalized to a custom pediatric template using a unified segmentation algorithm [87] with the following steps: (i) individual structural images were initially segmented into 3 tissue (i.e., gray matter, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid) probability maps by using the Chinese Pediatric Atlas (CHN-PD) (6 to 12 years) [88] as a reference; (ii) the resulting spatially normalized maps for each tissue type were averaged across scans to generate the custom tissue templates; (iii) individual structural images were segmented again using the custom tissue templates as a reference; and (iv) the functional images were spatially normalized using the transformation parameters estimated from the second segmentation of structural images. For the child cohort, custom tissue maps were used to improve the accuracy of spatial normalization. The normalized functional images were then resampled into 3-mm isotropic voxels and underwent spatial smoothing using a Gaussian kernel (full-width at half-maximum = 4 mm), linear detrending, and nuisance signal regression. A series of regressors were included in the nuisance regression, including 24 head motion parameters [89], “bad” time points with FD above 0.5 mm, white matter signals, cerebrospinal fluid signals, and global brain signals. Finally, we performed temporally band-pass filtering (0.01 to 0.1 Hz) on the images. For the adult cohort, the preprocessing procedures were conducted in the same way as for the child cohort, except that the functional images were normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) standard space.

  • (ii) T1-weighted structural MRI data. The T1-weighted structural images were preprocessed using FreeSurfer v6.0 [90]. First, we performed intensity normalization and removed nonbrain tissue using the HD-BET algorithm [91], during which the automatically extracted brain tissue maps replaced the default maps (i.e., “brainmask.mgz”) in FreeSurfer to improve accuracy. Next, we conducted tissue segmentation and cortical reconstruction on individual T1 images. Notably, the longitudinal processing stream of FreeSurfer was selected to obtain robust and reliable morphological measurements [92]. A trained researcher visually inspected the cortical reconstruction results to ensure that the correct boundaries were estimated. Finally, we obtained 5 local gray matter morphological features, including cortical volume, thickness, curvature, folding index, and surface area for each vertex.

  • (iii) Diffusion MRI data. The diffusion images were preprocessed using MRtrix 3.0 [93], FSL 6.0.1 (https://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/FSL), and ANTs [94]. First, the diffusion data were denoised, and Gibbs ringing artifacts were removed. Next, we corrected the eddy current-induced distortions, subject movement, and signal dropout for each scan using the FSL eddy tool [95]. Notably, 45 rsfMRI scans were further excluded from the structure–function association analysis due to missing images, excessive head motion (maximal motion >3 mm), or considerable signal dropout in the corresponding diffusion images. Then, field map correction was employed to reduce susceptibility by using FSL (epi_reg script). Finally, B1 field inhomogeneity was corrected with the N4 algorithm [96].

Identification of the overlapping modular architecture in functional networks

We identified the overlapping modular architecture in the brain functional networks by employing an edge-centric module detection algorithm [30,44] (Fig 1B). Briefly, we first constructed a traditional functional network comprising nodal regions and interregional connectivities (i.e., edges). Then, we constructed the corresponding weighted edge graph representing the similarity between edges. Finally, we identified the module affiliations of each nodal region according to the module assignments of its edges in the corresponding edge graph.

Functional network construction. We constructed a brain functional network comprising 232 nodal regions for each rsfMRI scan of each participant. In this functional network, cortical nodes were defined based on a recently developed functional parcellation comprising 200 cortical regions (i.e., Schaefer-200) [97], and subcortical nodes were defined according to a subcortical functional parcellation comprising 32 regions [49]. We extracted the mean time series for each nodal region and estimated Pearson’s correlation coefficients between every pair of nodes. Then, a weighted functional network was obtained by thresholding the correlation matrix with a density of 15% (i.e., 4,020 edges) to exclude potential spurious or weak correlations [98]. Negative correlations were not considered due to their controversial physiological interpretations [99,100].

Edge graph construction. For each functional network, we constructed a corresponding weighted edge graph that denoted the similarity between edges (Fig 1B). For simplicity and computation efficiency, we considered only directly connected edge pairs that shared one common node based on the hypothesis proposed in Ahn and colleagues [30], which captures the direct and dominant interaction between edges. The similarity between edges without common nodes was assumed to be zero. The Tanimoto coefficient [30,46] was used to incorporate the edge weight information. For a pair of edges eik and ejk that share a common node k, their similarity was defined based on the similarity in the connection profiles between nodes i and j:

Seik,ejk=aiajai2+aj2aiaj, (1)

where aiaj represents the dot product of the 2 vectors ai and aj, and |ai|2 denotes the sum of the squared weights of all connections of node i, and ai=A˜i1,A˜i2,,A˜iN represents a modified connectivity profile of node i. Specifically, each element A˜ij is defined as follows:

A˜ij=Aijijm=1NAimkii=j, (2)

where Aij is the functional connection strength between nodes i and j and ki is the number of edges of node i. Of note, nonzero diagonal elements were included in Eq (2) to make the similarity definition, i.e., Eq (1), applicable to extreme cases in which nodes i and j are directly connected.

Identifying the overlapping modular architecture based on the edge-centric module detection

Given a functional network of interest, we first detected the modular structure in the corresponding edge graph and then determined the module affiliations of each nodal region according to the module affiliations of its edges. Here, the Louvain algorithm [47] was employed to detect the modular architecture in the large-scale edge graph, and each edge was assigned to a specific module. Each nodal region was then assigned to one or more modules due to diverse module affiliations of its edges, leading to spatial overlap between functional modules. A nodal region whose edges were involved in 2 or more modules was defined as an overlapping region. Notably, the detected module partition in the edge graph varied slightly across each instance of detection due to the heuristic property of the Louvain algorithm. The module number of the functional network was defined as the module number that appeared most frequently among 100 instances, and the other measurements regarding the overlapping modular structure were taken as the average across 100 instances of identification.

Topography analysis of the overlapping modular architecture at the group level

To illustrate spatial patterns of the overlapping modular structure at different ages, we detected the modular architecture at the group level. All the children’s rsfMRI scans were divided into 8 subgroups with a one-year interval (i.e., 6 to 7 years, 7 to 8 years, 8 to 9 years, 9 to 10 years, 10 to 11 years, 11 to 12 years, 12 to 13 years, and 13 to 14 years), and the adult group was set as the ninth subgroup for comparison. A group-level weighted functional network was constructed for each subgroup by averaging individual functional correlation matrices followed by network thresholding (i.e., density = 15%). Then, we detected the modular structure in the corresponding edge graph for each subgroup with 100 instances of module detection. To obtain a stable module division, we conducted the following 2 steps: (i) computed the module co-occurrence matrix between each pair of edges [101], wherein each element denoted the proportion of instances in which a pair of edges were assigned to the same module; and (ii) applied the modular detection algorithm to the module co-occurrence matrix 100 times. We iterated these 2 steps until the module partition remained unchanged across multiple detection instances. The final version of the module partition of edges was used to infer the overlapping modular architecture of the group-level functional network.

Considering the spatial overlap of functional modules, we obtained a spatial map for each functional module separately. Nodal values in the map denote the proportion of edges assigned to this module. To compare the modules observed in each child subgroup with those in adults, we further matched the functional module maps between the adult subgroup and all the child subgroups. Given a module map of interest in the adult group, a matching module map was selected as the map showing the maximal similarity for each child subgroup. The spatial similarity between 2 maps was calculated as the Pearson’s correlation coefficient across nodal regions.

To further assess the functional system dependence of the overlapping modular structure, we mapped each cortical or subcortical node to one of the prior functional systems, including the visual, somatomotor, dorsal attention, ventral attention, limbic, frontoparietal, and default-mode systems [48] and the subcortical area [49]. For each module, we quantified its spatial pattern by estimating the percentage of nodes distributed in each functional system.

Measure of the extent of module overlap in brain regions

Given an overlapping modular architecture, a node may be involved in multiple modules due to the diverse module affiliations of its edges. Here, a measure of entropy was used to quantify the extent of module overlap for brain regions by examining the distribution of module affiliations of all edges attached to a node (Fig 1B). Given a node i, the entropy [34] was defined as follows:

Hi=k=1npiklog2pik, (3)

where n is the number of modules involved with this node, pik represents the proportion of its edges participating in module k, and k=1npik=1. Nodal entropy was further normalized to a range of 0 to 1 by dividing it by log2n [34]. A higher normalized entropy value indicates greater overlap, suggesting that the distribution of edges among different modules is more homogeneous and diverse. For each scan of each participant, we obtained a nodal module overlap map in terms of nodal entropy.

To illustrate the patterns of nodal overlap at different ages, a group-level entropy map was separately generated for each child subgroup and the adult group by averaging individual entropy maps within the subgroup. Then, we estimated the spatial similarity of the entropy maps between each of the child subgroups and the adult subgroup by calculating Pearson’s correlation coefficients across nodal regions. We further employed two-way repeated analysis of variance (ANOVA) models to explore whether the nodal overlap level was functional system-dependent for these subgroups (i.e., 8 child subgroups and the adult cohort). The functional systems were defined based on the prior 7 functional systems [48] and the subcortical area [49].

Analysis of developmental changes in the overlapping modular architecture

To explore the developmental changes in the overlapping modular architecture, we assessed age effects on a series of brain measures, including the module number, the modularity of the edge graph, and the nodal module overlap (i.e., nodal entropy) at the global, system, and nodal levels. For each brain measure of interest, the age effects were estimated by using a mixed effects model [53,54]. The mixed model is suitable for a longitudinal data set with irregular intervals between measurements and is applicable to cases of missing time points. The model parameters were estimated with the maximum likelihood method. Considering the potential linear and quadratic age effects, we employed both the linear model and the quadratic model of the age effects. The optimal model was selected for each brain measure based on the Akaike information criterion (AIC) [102]. Sex and the in-scanner head motion parameter (i.e., mFD) were included as covariates within each model.

The linear model was defined as follows:

yij=β0+bi+(βage+bage,i)ageij+βsexsexi+βmFDmFDij+εij. (4)

The quadratic model was defined as follows:

yij=β0+bi+(βage1+bage,i1)ageij+(βage2+bage,i2)ageij2+βsexsexi+βmFDmFDij+εij. (5)

In these equations, yij represents the brain measure of subject i at the jth scan, bi represents individual-specific intercept, βage represents the fixed effect, bage,i represents the random effect, and εij represents the residual. The fixed effect βage accounts for the population-level age effects, while the random effect bage,i accounts for individual variability in the age-related slope. The addition of the random effect allows the model to capture individual differences in developmental trajectories, thus providing a more accurate estimation of developmental changes. The fixed age effect was used to capture the developmental changes in the brain measure. For the nodal-level analyses, the significance level of the results was corrected for multiple comparisons across nodes using the false discovery rate (FDR) method [103].

Associations between cognitive functions and developmental changes in nodal overlap

To explore the cognitive significance of developmental changes in nodal overlap, we performed a meta-analysis using the NeuroSynth database (www.neurosynth.org) [55]. We first sorted the developmental changes in nodal entropy (i.e., age-related t values) in decreasing order and then divided the brain map into 10 bins (i.e., 10% apart as a box). Each bin was binarized to generate a brain mask. Next, we calculated the Pearson’s correlations between each mask and all the cognitive term maps available in the database. We selected the top 2 associated cognitive terms for each mask and removed 6 overlapping terms among the 10 masks. Finally, 14 cognitive terms were used to depict the distribution of cognitive functions across different levels of developmental changes. The significance level of each spatial similarity was assessed using permutation tests that corrected for spatial autocorrelation [56]. Specifically, given a cognitive term of interest, we constructed a null distribution of correlation coefficients for each percentile bin separately based on 10,000 permutations. For each permutation, we generated a surrogate map preserving the spatial autocorrelation of the original age effect map, and then calculated the association with that specific cognitive term for each percentile bin. We further compared each empirically observed value to the corresponding null distribution to derive the significance level.

Predicting chronological age from spatial patterns of nodal overlap

We used the SVR model with 10-fold cross-validation to test whether individual spatial patterns of nodal overlap could be used to predict a participant’s chronological age. The nodal entropy map was set as features for each participant. To avoid the possibility of data leakage that could occur by including longitudinal scans from the same children in both the training and test sets, we performed the 10-fold cross-validation using 305 rsfMRI scans that were selected from independent subjects. For children with longitudinal scans, only 1 scan was selected from each individual and the other scans were discarded. To reduce the potential bias, the 10-fold cross-validation analysis was repeated 1,000 times with random scan selection. The detailed prediction process for each time of 305 randomly selected scans is as follows: First, all the entropy maps were divided into 10 subsets with similar age distributions. This division strategy is better than the random splitting method, which can reduce the sampling bias among the subsets [104,105]. Of the 10 subsets, one was designated as the testing set, and the remaining 9 subsets were used as the training set. Next, we linearly scaled each feature across individuals between zero and one in the training set and applied the estimated scaling parameters to the testing set. Then, we trained the prediction model for individual chronological ages based on the training set. Finally, we quantified the prediction accuracy by calculating the Pearson’s correlation coefficients between the predicted scores (i.e., ages) obtained from the SVR model and the actual scores (i.e., chronological age). To assess the statistical significance of the prediction accuracy, we generated a null distribution of accuracy based on permutation tests (n = 10,000) by shuffling the actual scores across scans. To reduce the influence of confounding factors, we further corrected for sex, in-scanner head motion (i.e., mFD) and random age effects from the nodal entropy values prior to the prediction analysis. To determine the contribution of nodal features to the prediction model, we trained another SVR model using all the 305 scans to improve the estimation accuracy [104,105]. The resulting regression coefficients were regarded as the weights denoting the importance of all features. The final contribution weights of nodal regions were obtained by averaging the estimated weights over the 1,000 times of random sampling. To further clarify the system dependence of the weights, we also classified the nodal weights into 8 functional systems, including 7 cortical functional systems [48] and the subcortical area [49]. The positive and negative weights were separately averaged within each system. Here, the SVR model was implemented using the LIBSVM toolbox in MATLAB with the default parameters (https://www.csie.ntu.edu.tw/~cjlin/libsvm/) [106].

Analysis of nodal microstructure in the white matter structural network

In addition to local morphological measures, we also considered the potential influence of nodal microstructural properties in the white matter structural network. Structural networks were generated from the preprocessed diffusion images using DSI Studio (http://dsi-studio.labsolver.org). First, we tracked the fasciculus and obtained diffusion anisotropy parameters (i.e., FA). Specifically, we employed the generalized q-sampling imaging (GQI) algorithm [107] with a diffusion sampling length ratio of 1.25 for deterministic tractography. The Otsu threshold was 0.6. The tracking procedure was terminated if the turning angle was >45° or if the fibers reached the borders of the cerebrospinal fluid or subcortical areas. Ten million streamlines were generated with a step size of 0.625 mm, and only tracts with a length between 6~250 mm were retained for subsequent analysis. Next, the reconstructed streamlines were projected to the Schaefer-200 atlas, which has been registered to the native space. Finally, we calculated the FA values between every pair of nodes (i.e., the mean FA values along all reconstructed streamlines between 2 nodes) and further summed the FA values between each node and all the other nodes. This metric was defined as nodal FA strength, which indicates the microstructural property of the fiber bundles attached to a node [108].

Predicting individual nodal overlap maps from structural features

To evaluate whether the spatial pattern of the nodal overlap map was associated with anatomical architecture, we performed a prediction analysis based on the SVR model for each scan of each participant. We considered 5 morphological measurements (cortical volume, thickness, curvature, folding index, and surface area) in the gray matter and the FA strength in the white matter. For each node, morphological features were extracted as the average value within the nodal region based on the Schaefer-200 atlas, which has been registered to the native space. Prior to the prediction analysis, we first examined the spatial similarity between nodal entropy and each structural feature of interest using a univariate association analysis (i.e., Pearson’s correlation). Then, we performed the prediction using the same framework as that used in the age prediction mentioned above by integrating multiple structural features. Ten-fold cross-validation was also used here. In each validation instance, 10% of the nodal regions were designated as the testing set, and the remaining nodes were set as the training set. We predicted nodal entropy values for each scan by integrating anatomical features from both gray matter and white matter. To assess the statistical significance of the prediction accuracy, we generated a null distribution of prediction accuracy for the whole population using permutation tests. Specifically, we first generated a null distribution of accuracy by shuffling nodal entropy across nodes for each scan (n = 100), and then aggregated the null distributions across 446 scans, resulting in the final null distribution with 44,600 (i.e., 446 scans × 100 times) instances. The 95% significance level was determined according to the null distribution containing all 44,600 instances. To assess the prediction contribution of anatomical features, we trained another SVR model for each scan using all the nodal regions in the whole brain to improve estimation accuracy [104,105]. The resulting regression coefficients were regarded as the weights denoting the importance of all features.

Validation analyses

To ensure the robustness of the topography of the overlapping modular architecture, we investigated the potential influence of several network construction and analysis strategies in the adult cohort. Specifically, we examined the potential influence of functional parcellation for the nodal definition, network thresholding strategy, edge module detection algorithm, and nodal overlap estimation. First, we considered the spatial resolution of the functional parcellation. For the main analysis, we constructed a brain functional network comprising 200 cortical regions [97] and 32 subcortical regions [49]. To further validate the influence of spatial resolution, we reconstructed whole-brain functional networks, during which the cortical nodes were defined based on the Scheafer-100 atlas, which comprises 100 cortical regions [97]. Second, we examined the network thresholding density by obtaining weighted functional networks using 2 other network densities (i.e., 10% and 20%). Third, we explored the influence of the module detection algorithm. In addition to the Louvain algorithm [47], we employed the eigenspectral analysis method [109] to detect the modular structure of the edge graph. Finally, we quantified the nodal overlap level by considering the number of modules involved, which provides a more intuitive understanding. For each nodal region, we calculated the involved module number as the number of modules involved in its edges. The larger the involved number, the higher the nodal overlap in module affiliations.

To assess the robustness of the developmental effects of node entropy, we examined the potential impact of the network thresholding strategy, the statistical model for estimating age effects, and the head motion control strategy. First, as some weaker connections in functional networks play a key role in the development of human cognitive function [110], we examined age effects with a wider range of network thresholds, including 10%, 20%, and 30%. The latter threshold doubled the density of connecting edges used in the main results. Second, we used the GAMM [111] to assess the potentially complex nonlinear age effects. This model was implemented in R using the package mgcv. Third, to assess the potential effect of head motion, we performed a more stringent head motion control by excluding rsfMRI scans with mFD > 0.2 mm. Age effects were assessed using the mixed effects model based on the remaining scans.

Declaration of competing interests

The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Univariate relationship in spatial patterns between nodal entropy and structural brain features within each scan in children.

For each scan, we separately calculated Pearson’s correlation in spatial patterns between nodal entropy and each structural brain feature. (A) Frequency polygon of correlation coefficients for cortical thickness across rsfMRI scans. Compared to the null distribution, cortical thickness showed significant positive spatial correlations with nodal entropy in 17% of scans (74/446) (p < 0.05). (B) Frequency polygon of correlation coefficients for surface area in all rsfMRI scans. Compared to the null distribution, surface area showed significant negative correlations with nodal entropy in 44% of scans (196/446) (p < 0.05). (C) Frequency polygon of correlation coefficients for folding index in all rsfMRI scans. Compared to the null distribution, folding index showed significant negative correlations with nodal entropy in 46% of scans (207/446) (p < 0.05). (D) Frequency polygon of correlation coefficients for FA strength in all rsfMRI scans. Compared to the null distribution, FA strength showed significant negative correlations with nodal entropy in 5% of scans (24/446) (p < 0.05). (E) Frequency polygon of correlation coefficients for cortical curvature in all rsfMRI scans. Compared to the null distribution, cortical curvature showed significant negative correlations with nodal entropy in 30% of scans (136/446) (p < 0.05). (F) Frequency polygon of correlation coefficients for cortical volume in all rsfMRI scans. Compared to the null distribution, cortical volume showed significant negative correlations with nodal entropy in 17% of scans (76/446) (p < 0.05). In (A–F), the inset in the upper corner denotes the null distribution of the correlation coefficients. This null distribution was generated by aggregating the 100 permutation instances for each scan, resulting in a total of 44,600 permutation instances (446 scans × 100 times). For each permutation within each scan, a surrogate nodal entropy map was generated that preserved the spatial autocorrelation characteristics of the original nodal entropy map [56]. The red line denotes the 95% significance level in the null distribution.

(TIF)

pbio.3002653.s001.TIF (1.6MB, TIF)
S2 Fig. Spatial patterns of nodal module overlap (i.e., nodal entropy) at different spatial resolutions.

For the adult cohort, the overlapping modular architecture was separately detected in the group-level functional networks obtained from different functional parcellations. (A) Nodal overlap in the functional networks with a coarse parcellation. This network comprised 100 cortical nodes obtained from the Schaefer-100 atlas [97] and 32 subcortical regions [49]. (B) Nodal overlap in the functional networks with a fine parcellation (i.e., main results). This network comprised 200 cortical nodes obtained from the Schaefer-200 atlas [97] and 32 subcortical regions [49]. (C) Extent of nodal overlap for 8 systems at different spatial resolutions. Similar distributions of nodal overlap were observed between the 2 parcellations. VIS, visual; SM, somatomotor; DA, dorsal attention; VA, ventral attention; LIM, limbic; FP, frontoparietal; DM, default-mode; SUB, subcortical.

(TIF)

S3 Fig. Spatial patterns of nodal module overlap (i.e., nodal entropy) for different network analysis strategies and their relationships with the main results.

All of these analyses were specified to the group-level functional network for the adult cohort. (A) Network density of 10% for functional network construction. (B) Network density of 20% for functional network construction. (C) Eigenspectral analysis for module detection in the edge graph. (D) Number of involved modules was used to quantify the extent of node module overlap. (E) Main result as a reference. In each case, all the network construction and analysis strategies were set to be the same as those in the main analysis, except for the strategy of interest. All correlations were assessed with Pearson’s correlation across nodal regions. The significance of spatial similarity was assessed by comparing the observed value to a null distribution generated through 10,000 permutations that retained the spatial autocorrelation characteristics of the nodal entropy map of the original adult cohort in the main results.

(TIF)

pbio.3002653.s003.TIF (5.4MB, TIF)
S4 Fig. Spatial patterns of developmental changes in nodal entropy for different thresholding strategies.

For each rsfMRI scan of children, the brain functional network was generated with different network thresholding strategies. (A) Network density of 10% for functional network construction. (B) Network density of 20% for functional network construction. (C) Network density of 30% for functional network construction. (D) Network density of 15% (i.e., main result as a reference). In each case, all the network construction and analysis strategies were set to be the same as those in the main analysis, except for the strategy of interest. All correlations were assessed using Pearson’s correlation across nodal regions. The significance of spatial similarity was assessed by comparing the observed value to a null distribution generated through 10,000 permutations that retained the spatial autocorrelation characteristics of the original age effect map (t-value map) in the main results.

(TIF)

pbio.3002653.s004.TIF (4.1MB, TIF)
S5 Fig. Spatial patterns of developmental changes in nodal entropy using different statistical models.

(A) Age effects on nodal entropy using a mixed effects model. (B) Age effects on nodal entropy using a generalized additive mixed effects model. (C) Spatial correlation of age effects on nodal entropy between 2 statistical methods. The significance of spatial similarity was assessed by comparing the observed value to a null distribution generated through 10,000 permutations that maintained the spatial autocorrelation characteristics of the original age effect map (i.e., p-value map) obtained from the MEM in the main results. MEM, mixed effects model; GAMM, generalized additive mixed model.

(TIF)

pbio.3002653.s005.TIF (2.1MB, TIF)
S6 Fig. Effects of head motion parameters on developmental changes of nodal entropy.

(A) Age effects on head motion parameters. Pearson’s correlation analysis revealed that individual age and mFD values showed a significant but weak correlation. (B) Left: Age effects on nodal entropy. Right: Head motion effects on nodal entropy. The influence of age and head motion on nodal entropy was evaluated using a mixed effects model. In this model, age was designated as the independent variable, while the head motion parameter (i.e., mFD) was included as a covariate. The variable of sex was also included in the model as a covariate. (C) Left: Spatial pattern of age effects on nodal module overlap with a head motion control strategy of mFD < 0.5 mm (main results). Middle: Spatial pattern of age effects on nodal module overlap with a more strict head motion control strategy with mFD < 0.2 mm. Right: Spatial correlation of age effects on nodal entropy between 2 head motion control strategies. The significance of the spatial similarity was assessed by comparing the observed value to a null distribution generated through 10,000 permutations that retained the spatial autocorrelation characteristics of the original age effect map (t-value map) in the main results. mFD, mean framewise displacement.

(TIF)

pbio.3002653.s006.TIF (3.8MB, TIF)

Acknowledgments

We thank the National Center for Protein Sciences at Peking University in Beijing, China for assistance on data acquisition.

Abbreviations

AIC

Akaike information criterion

FDR

false discovery rate

FOV

field of view

GQI

generalized q-sampling imaging

HARDI

high angular resolution diffusion imaging

mFD

mean framewise displacement

MPRAGE

magnetization prepared rapid acquisition gradient-echo

rsfMRI

resting-state fMRI

SVR

support vector regression

Data Availability

All code and data used in the analyses are available in the Zenodo database (https://osf.io/qfcyu/; doi: 10.17605/OSF.IO/QFCYU) and on GitHub (https://github.com/helab207/Development-of-the-overlapping-modular-structure-in-human-brain-functional-networks).

Funding Statement

The study was supported by the grant from the National Key R&D Program of China (https://service.most.gov.cn/index/) (grant 2018YFA0701402 to Y.H.), the National Natural Science Foundation of China (https://www.nsfc.gov.cn/) (grants 82021004, 81620108016 to Y.H., grants 81971690, 11835003 to X.L., grants 31221003, 31521063 to Q.D., and grant 81801783 to T.Z.), the Beijing Brain Initiative of the Beijing Municipal Science & Technology Commission (https://kw.beijing.gov.cn/) (grant Z181100001518003 to S.T.), and the Tang Scholar Award of Beijing Normal University (http://www.tangfoundation.org.cn/) (grant to X.L.). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Paus T. Mapping brain maturation and cognitive development during adolescence. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9(2):60–8. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.008 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Berk LE. Development through the lifespan (7th). London: Pearson; 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Miller DJ, Duka T, Stimpson CD, Schapiro SJ, Baze WB, McArthur MJ, et al. Prolonged myelination in human neocortical evolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109(41):16480–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1117943109 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Norbom LB, Ferschmann L, Parker N, Agartz I, Andreassen OA, Paus T, et al. New insights into the dynamic development of the cerebral cortex in childhood and adolescence: Integrating macro-and microstructural MRI findings. Prog Neurobiol. 2021;204:102109. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2021.102109 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Stephan AH, Barres BA, Stevens B. The complement system: an unexpected role in synaptic pruning during development and disease. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2012;35:369–89. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113810 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Sakai J. Core Concept: How synaptic pruning shapes neural wiring during development and, possibly, in disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2020;117(28):16096–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2010281117 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Walhovd KB, Fjell AM, Giedd J, Dale AM, Brown TT. Through thick and thin: a need to reconcile contradictory results on trajectories in human cortical development. Cereb Cortex. 2017;27(2):1472–81. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv301 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Liang X, Sun L, Liao X, Lei T, Xia M, Duan D, et al. Structural connectome architecture shapes the maturation of cortical morphology from childhood to adolescence. Nat Commun. 2024;15(1):784. doi: 10.1038/s41467-024-44863-6 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bethlehem RAI, Seidlitz J, White SR, Vogel JW, Anderson KM, Adamson C, et al. Brain charts for the human lifespan. Nature. 2022;604(7906):525–33. doi: 10.1038/s41586-022-04554-y . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Morita T, Asada M, Naito E. Contribution of neuroimaging studies to understanding development of human cognitive brain functions. Front Hum Neurosci. 2016;10:464. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00464 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Casey BJ, Giedd JN, Thomas KM. Structural and functional brain development and its relation to cognitive development. Biol Psychol. 2000;54(1–3):241–57. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(00)00058-2 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Long X, Benischek A, Dewey D, Lebel C. Age-related functional brain changes in young children. Neuroimage. 2017;155:322–30. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.04.059 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Casey B, Tottenham N, Liston C, Durston S. Imaging the developing brain: what have we learned about cognitive development? Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9(3):104–10. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.01.011 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Paus T, Keshavan M, Giedd JN. Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9(12):947–57. doi: 10.1038/nrn2513 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Vértes PE, Bullmore ET. Annual research review: growth connectomics—the organization and reorganization of brain networks during normal and abnormal development. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2015;56(3):299–320. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12365 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Cao M, Huang H, Peng Y, Dong Q, He Y. Toward developmental connectomics of the human brain. Front Neuroanat. 2016;10:25. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2016.00025 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Grayson DS, Fair DA. Development of large-scale functional networks from birth to adulthood: a guide to the neuroimaging literature. Neuroimage. 2017;160:15–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.079 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.He Y, Wang J, Wang L, Chen Z, Yan C, Yang H, et al. Uncovering intrinsic modular organization of spontaneous brain activity in humans. PLoS ONE. 2009;4(4):e5226. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005226 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Meunier D, Lambiotte R, Bullmore ET. Modular and hierarchically modular organization of brain networks. Front Neurosci. 2010;4:200. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2010.00200 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Sporns O, Betzel RF. Modular brain networks. Annu Rev Psychol. 2016;67:613–40. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033634 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Liao X, Cao M, Xia M, He Y. Individual differences and time-varying features of modular brain architecture. Neuroimage. 2017;152:94–107. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.02.066 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Sporns O. Structure and function of complex brain networks. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2013;15(3):247–262. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2013.15.3/osporns . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Gu S, Satterthwaite TD, Medaglia JD, Yang M, Gur RE, Gur RC, et al. Emergence of system roles in normative neurodevelopment. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2015;112(44):13681–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1502829112 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Marek S, Hwang K, Foran W, Hallquist MN, Luna B. The contribution of network organization and integration to the development of cognitive control. PLoS Biol. 2015;13(12):e1002328. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002328 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Fair DA, Cohen AL, Power JD, Dosenbach NUF, Church JA, Miezin FM, et al. Functional brain networks develop from a “local to distributed” organization. PLoS Comput Biol. 2009;5(5):e1000381. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000381 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Turk E, van den Heuvel MI, Benders MJ, de Heus R, Franx A, Manning JH, et al. Functional connectome of the fetal brain. J Neurosci. 2019;39(49):9716–24. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2891-18.2019 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.van den Heuvel MP, Kersbergen KJ, de Reus MA, Keunen K, Kahn RS, Groenendaal F, et al. The neonatal connectome during preterm brain development. Cereb Cortex. 2015;25(9):3000–13. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu095 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Gao W, Alcauter S, Smith JK, Gilmore JH, Lin W. Development of human brain cortical network architecture during infancy. Brain Struct Funct. 2015;220(2):1173–86. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0710-3 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Palla G, Derényi I, Farkas I, Vicsek T. Uncovering the overlapping community structure of complex networks in nature and society. Nature. 2005;435(7043):814–8. doi: 10.1038/nature03607 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Ahn YY, Bagrow JP, Lehmann S. Link communities reveal multiscale complexity in networks. Nature. 2010;466(7307):761–4. doi: 10.1038/nature09182 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Pessoa L. Understanding brain networks and brain organization. Phys Life Rev. 2014;11(3):400–35. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2014.03.005 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Najafi M, McMenamin BW, Simon JZ, Pessoa L. Overlapping communities reveal rich structure in large-scale brain networks during rest and task conditions. Neuroimage. 2016;135:92–106. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.04.054 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Lin Y, Ma J, Gu Y, Yang S, Li LMW, Dai Z. Intrinsic overlapping modular organization of human brain functional networks revealed by a multiobjective evolutionary algorithm. Neuroimage. 2018;181:430–45. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.07.019 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Faskowitz J, Esfahlani FZ, Jo Y, Sporns O, Betzel RF. Edge-centric functional network representations of human cerebral cortex reveal overlapping system-level architecture. Nat Neurosci. 2020;23(12):1644–54. doi: 10.1038/s41593-020-00719-y . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Yeo BT, Krienen FM, Chee MW, Buckner RL. Estimates of segregation and overlap of functional connectivity networks in the human cerebral cortex. Neuroimage. 2014;88:212–27. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.046 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Hermosillo RJM, Moore LA, Feczko E, Miranda-Dominguez O, Pines A, Dworetsky A, et al. A precision functional atlas of personalized network topography and probabilities. Nat Neurosci. 2024;27(5):1000–13. doi: 10.1038/s41593-024-01596-5 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Dworetsky A, Seitzman BA, Adeyemo B, Neta M, Coalson RS, Petersen SE, et al. Probabilistic mapping of human functional brain networks identifies regions of high group consensus. Neuroimage. 2021;237:118164. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118164 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Wu K, Taki Y, Sato K, Sassa Y, Inoue K, Goto R, et al. The overlapping community structure of structural brain network in young healthy individuals. PLoS ONE. 2011;6(5):e19608. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0019608 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Gu Y, Li L, Zhang Y, Ma J, Yang C, Xiao Y, et al. The overlapping modular organization of human brain functional networks across the adult lifespan. Neuroimage. 2022;253:119125. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119125 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Shankar A, Tanner JC, Mao T, Betzel RF, Prakash RS. Edge-community entropy is a novel neural correlate of aging and moderator of fluid cognition. J Neurosci. 2024;44(25). doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1701-23.2024 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Lei T, Liao X, Chen X, Zhao T, Xu Y, Xia M, et al. Progressive stabilization of brain network dynamics during childhood and adolescence. Cereb Cortex. 2022;32(5):1024–39. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab263 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Fan F, Liao X, Lei T, Zhao T, Xia M, Men W, et al. Development of the default-mode network during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal resting-state fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2021;226:117581. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117581 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Xia Y, Xia M, Liu J, Liao X, Lei T, Liang X, et al. Development of functional connectome gradients during childhood and adolescence. Sci Bull (Beijing). 2022;67(10):1049–61. doi: 10.1016/j.scib.2022.01.002 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.de Reus MA, Saenger VM, Kahn RS, van den Heuvel MP. An edge-centric perspective on the human connectome: link communities in the brain. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2014;369(1653). doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0527 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Betzel RF, Faskowitz J, Sporns O. Living on the edge: network neuroscience beyond nodes. Trends Cogn Sci. 2023;27(11):1068–84. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2023.08.009 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Tanimoto TT. Elementary mathematical theory of classification and prediction. 1958. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Blondel VD, Guillaume JL, Lambiotte R, Lefebvre E. Fast unfolding of communities in large networks. J Stat Mech. 2008;2008(10):P10008. doi: 10.1088/1742-5468/2008/10/P10008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Yeo BTT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Lashkari D, Hollinshead M, et al. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 2011;106(3):1125–65. doi: 10.1152/jn.00338.2011 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Tian Y, Margulies DS, Breakspear M, Zalesky A. Topographic organization of the human subcortex unveiled with functional connectivity gradients. Nat Neurosci. 2020;23(11):1421–32. doi: 10.1038/s41593-020-00711-6 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Margulies DS, Ghosh SS, Goulas A, Falkiewicz M, Huntenburg JM, Langs G, et al. Situating the default-mode network along a principal gradient of macroscale cortical organization. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2016;113(44):12574–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1608282113 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Sydnor VJ, Larsen B, Bassett DS, Alexander-Bloch A, Fair DA, Liston C, et al. Neurodevelopment of the association cortices: patterns, mechanisms, and implications for psychopathology. Neuron. 2021;109(18):2820–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.06.016 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Xia M, Wang J, He Y. BrainNet Viewer: a network visualization tool for human brain connectomics. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(7):e68910. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0068910 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Laird NM, Ware JH. Random-effects models for longitudinal data. Biometrics. 1982;38(4):963–74. doi: 10.2307/2529876 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Diggle P, Kenward MG. Informative drop-out in longitudinal data-analysis. J R Stat Soc Ser C Appl Stat. 1994;43(1):49–93. doi: 10.2307/2986113 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Yarkoni T, Poldrack RA, Nichols TE, Van Essen DC, Wager TD. Large-scale automated synthesis of human functional neuroimaging data. Nat Methods. 2011;8(8):665–70. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1635 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Burt JB, Helmer M, Shinn M, Anticevic A, Murray JD. Generative modeling of brain maps with spatial autocorrelation. Neuroimage. 2020;220:117038. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117038 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Betzel RF, Bassett DS. Multi-scale brain networks. Neuroimage. 2017;160:73–83. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.006 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Pines AR, Larsen B, Cui Z, Sydnor VJ, Bertolero MA, Adebimpe A, et al. Dissociable multi-scale patterns of development in personalized brain networks. Nat Commun. 2022;13(1):2647. doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-30244-4 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Anderson ML, Kinnison J, Pessoa L. Describing functional diversity of brain regions and brain networks. Neuroimage. 2013;73:50–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.071 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Yeo BTT, Krienen FM, Eickhoff SB, Yaakub SN, Fox PT, Buckner RL, et al. Functional specialization and flexibility in human association cortex. Cereb Cortex. 2015;25(10):3654–72. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu217 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Buckner RL, DiNicola LM. The brain’s default network: updated anatomy, physiology and evolving insights. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2019;20(10):593–608. doi: 10.1038/s41583-019-0212-7 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Luo AC, Sydnor VJ, Pines A, Larsen B, Alexander-Bloch AF, Cieslak M, et al. Functional connectivity development along the sensorimotor-association axis enhances the cortical hierarchy. Nat Commun. 2024;15(1):3511. doi: 10.1038/s41467-024-47748-w . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Power JD, Cohen AL, Nelson SM, Wig GS, Barnes KA, Church JA, et al. Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron. 2011;72(4):665–78. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.006 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Tooley UA, Bassett DS, Mackey AP. Functional brain network community structure in childhood: Unfinished territories and fuzzy boundaries. Neuroimage. 2022;247:118843. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118843 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Vasa F, Romero-Garcia R, Kitzbichler MG, Seidlitz J, Whitaker KJ, Vaghi MM, et al. Conservative and disruptive modes of adolescent change in human brain functional connectivity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2020;117(6):3248–53. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1906144117 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Dixon ML, De La Vega A, Mills C, Andrews-Hanna J, Spreng RN, Cole MW, et al. Heterogeneity within the frontoparietal control network and its relationship to the default and dorsal attention networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2018;115(7):E1598–E607. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1715766115 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Suarez LE, Markello RD, Betzel RF, Misic B. Linking structure and function in macroscale brain networks. Trends Cogn Sci. 2020;24(4):302–15. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.01.008 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, Jeffries NO, Castellanos FX, Liu H, Zijdenbos A, et al. Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2(10):861–3. doi: 10.1038/13158 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Wierenga LM, Langen M, Oranje B, Durston S. Unique developmental trajectories of cortical thickness and surface area. Neuroimage. 2014;87:120–6. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.010 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Fjell AM, Grydeland H, Krogsrud SK, Amlien I, Rohani DA, Ferschmann L, et al. Development and aging of cortical thickness correspond to genetic organization patterns. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2015;112(50):15462–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1508831112 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Raznahan A, Lerch JP, Lee N, Greenstein D, Wallace GL, Stockman M, et al. Patterns of coordinated anatomical change in human cortical development: a longitudinal neuroimaging study of maturational coupling. Neuron. 2011;72(5):873–84. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.09.028 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Fortunato S, Hric D. Community detection in networks: A user guide. Phys Rep. 2016;659:1–44. doi: 10.1016/j.physrep.2016.09.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Novelli L, Razi A. A mathematical perspective on edge-centric brain functional connectivity. Nat Commun. 2022;13(1):2693. doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-29775-7 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Bullmore ET, Bassett DS. Brain graphs: graphical models of the human brain connectome. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2011;7:113–40. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-040510-143934 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Liao X, Vasilakos AV, He Y. Small-world human brain networks: perspectives and challenges. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;77:286–300. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.03.018 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Satterthwaite TD, Wolf DH, Ruparel K, Erus G, Elliott MA, Eickhoff SB, et al. Heterogeneous impact of motion on fundamental patterns of developmental changes in functional connectivity during youth. Neuroimage. 2013;83:45–57. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.045 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Ciric R, Wolf DH, Power JD, Roalf DR, Baum GL, Ruparel K, et al. Benchmarking of participant-level confound regression strategies for the control of motion artifact in studies of functional connectivity. Neuroimage. 2017;154:174–87. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.020 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Parkes L, Fulcher B, Yucel M, Fornito A. An evaluation of the efficacy, reliability, and sensitivity of motion correction strategies for resting-state functional MRI. Neuroimage. 2018;171:415–36. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.12.073 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Cui Z, Li H, Xia CH, Larsen B, Adebimpe A, Baum GL, et al. Individual variation in functional topography of association networks in youth. Neuron. 2020;106(2):340–53 e8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.01.029 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Faskowitz J, Yan X, Zuo XN, Sporns O. Weighted stochastic block models of the human connectome across the life span. Sci Rep. 2018;8(1):12997. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-31202-1 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Avena-Koenigsberger A, Misic B, Sporns O. Communication dynamics in complex brain networks. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2018;19(1):17–33. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.149 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Breakspear M. Dynamic models of large-scale brain activity. Nat Neurosci. 2017;20(3):340–52. doi: 10.1038/nn.4497 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Descoteaux M. High angular resolution diffusion imaging (HARDI). Wiley encyclopedia of electrical and electronics engineering. 2015:1–25. doi: 10.1002/047134608X.W8258 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Tuch DS, Reese TG, Wiegell MR, Makris N, Belliveau JW, Wedeen VJ. High angular resolution diffusion imaging reveals intravoxel white matter fiber heterogeneity. Magn Reson Med. 2002;48(4):577–82. doi: 10.1002/mrm.10268 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Yan C, Wang X, Zuo X, Zang Y. DPABI: data processing & analysis for (resting-state) brain imaging. Neuroinformatics. 2016;14(3):339–51. doi: 10.1007/s12021-016-9299-4 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage. 2012;59(3):2142–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Unified segmentation. Neuroimage. 2005;26(3):839–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Zhao T, Liao X, Fonov VS, Wang Q, Men W, Wang Y, et al. Unbiased age-specific structural brain atlases for Chinese pediatric population. Neuroimage. 2019;189:55–70. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.01.006 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Friston KJ, Williams S, Howard R, Frackowiak RSJ, Turner R. Movement-related effects in fMRI time-series. Magn Reson Med. 1996;35(3):346–55. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910350312 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Fischl B. FreeSurfer Neuroimage 2012;62(2):774–81. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.021 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Isensee F, Schell M, Pflueger I, Brugnara G, Bonekamp D, Neuberger U, et al. Automated brain extraction of multisequence MRI using artificial neural networks. Hum Brain Mapp. 2019;40(17):4952–64. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24750 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Reuter M, Fischl B. Avoiding asymmetry-induced bias in longitudinal image processing. Neuroimage. 2011;57(1):19–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.076 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Tournier JD, Smith R, Raffelt D, Tabbara R, Dhollander T, Pietsch M, et al. MRtrix3: A fast, flexible and open software framework for medical image processing and visualisation. Neuroimage. 2019;202:116137. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116137 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Avants BB, Tustison NJ, Song G, Cook PA, Klein A, Gee JC. A reproducible evaluation of ANTs similarity metric performance in brain image registration. Neuroimage. 2011;54(3):2033–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.025 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Andersson JLR, Sotiropoulos SN. An integrated approach to correction for off-resonance effects and subject movement in diffusion MR imaging. Neuroimage. 2016;125:1063–78. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.019 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Tustison NJ, Avants BB, Cook PA, Zheng Y, Egan A, Yushkevich PA, et al. N4ITK: improved N3 bias correction. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 2010;29(6):1310–20. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2010.2046908 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Schaefer A, Kong R, Gordon EM, Laumann TO, Zuo XN, Holmes AJ, et al. Local-global parcellation of the human cerebral cortex from intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Cereb Cortex. 2018;28(9):3095–114. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx179 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Rubinov M, Sporns O. Complex network measures of brain connectivity: uses and interpretations. Neuroimage. 2010;52(3):1059–69. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Fox MD, Zhang DY, Snyder AZ, Raichle ME. The global signal and observed anticorrelated resting state brain networks. J Neurophysiol. 2009;101(6):3270–83. doi: 10.1152/jn.90777.2008 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Murphy K, Fox MD. Towards a consensus regarding global signal regression for resting state functional connectivity MRI. Neuroimage. 2017;154:169–73. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.052 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Braun U, Schafer A, Walter H, Erk S, Romanczuk-Seiferth N, Haddad L, et al. Dynamic reconfiguration of frontal brain networks during executive cognition in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2015;112(37):11678–83. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1422487112 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Akaike H. A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Trans Automat Contr. 1974;19(6):716–23. doi: 10.1109/TAC.1974.1100705 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc B Methodol. 1995;57(1):289–300. doi: 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Cui Z, Gong G. The effect of machine learning regression algorithms and sample size on individualized behavioral prediction with functional connectivity features. Neuroimage. 2018;178:622–37. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.06.001 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Cui Z, Stiso J, Baum GL, Kim JZ, Roalf DR, Betzel RF, et al. Optimization of energy state transition trajectory supports the development of executive function during youth. Elife. 2020;9:e53060. doi: 10.7554/eLife.53060 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Chang C-C, Lin C-J. LIBSVM: a library for support vector machines. ACM Trans Intell Syst Technol. 2011;2(3):1–27. doi: 10.1145/1961189.1961199 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Yeh FC, Wedeen VJ, Tseng WY. Generalized q-sampling imaging. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 2010;29(9):1626–35. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2010.2045126 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Alexander AL, Hurley SA, Samsonov AA, Adluru N, Hosseinbor AP, Mossahebi P, et al. Characterization of cerebral white matter properties using quantitative magnetic resonance imaging stains. Brain Connect. 2011;1(6):423–46. doi: 10.1089/brain.2011.0071 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Newman ME. Modularity and community structure in networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103(23):8577–82. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0601602103 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Santarnecchi E, Galli G, Polizzotto NR, Rossi A, Rossi S. Efficiency of weak brain connections support general cognitive functioning. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35(9):4566–82. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22495 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Lin X, Zhang D. Inference in generalized additive mixed models by using smoothing splines. J R Stat Soc Series B Stat Methodology. 1999;61(2):381–400. doi: 10.1111/1467-9868.00183 [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Christian Schnell, PhD

2 May 2024

Dear Dr He,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript entitled "Development of the overlapping network modules in the human brain" for consideration as a Research Article by PLOS Biology.

Your manuscript has now been evaluated by the PLOS Biology editorial staff as well as by an academic editor with relevant expertise and I am writing to let you know that we would like to send your submission out for external peer review.

However, before we can send your manuscript to reviewers, we need you to complete your submission by providing the metadata that is required for full assessment. To this end, please login to Editorial Manager where you will find the paper in the 'Submissions Needing Revisions' folder on your homepage. Please click 'Revise Submission' from the Action Links and complete all additional questions in the submission questionnaire.

Once your full submission is complete, your paper will undergo a series of checks in preparation for peer review. After your manuscript has passed the checks it will be sent out for review. To provide the metadata for your submission, please Login to Editorial Manager (https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology) within two working days, i.e. by May 04 2024 11:59PM.

If your manuscript has been previously peer-reviewed at another journal, PLOS Biology is willing to work with those reviews in order to avoid re-starting the process. Submission of the previous reviews is entirely optional and our ability to use them effectively will depend on the willingness of the previous journal to confirm the content of the reports and share the reviewer identities. Please note that we reserve the right to invite additional reviewers if we consider that additional/independent reviewers are needed, although we aim to avoid this as far as possible. In our experience, working with previous reviews does save time.

If you would like us to consider previous reviewer reports, please edit your cover letter to let us know and include the name of the journal where the work was previously considered and the manuscript ID it was given. In addition, please upload a response to the reviews as a 'Prior Peer Review' file type, which should include the reports in full and a point-by-point reply detailing how you have or plan to address the reviewers' concerns.

During the process of completing your manuscript submission, you will be invited to opt-in to posting your pre-review manuscript as a bioRxiv preprint. Visit http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/preprints for full details. If you consent to posting your current manuscript as a preprint, please upload a single Preprint PDF.

Feel free to email us at plosbiology@plos.org if you have any queries relating to your submission.

Kind regards,

Christian

Christian Schnell, PhD

Senior Editor

PLOS Biology

cschnell@plos.org

Decision Letter 1

Christian Schnell, PhD

6 Jun 2024

Dear Dr He,

Thank you for your patience while your manuscript "Development of the overlapping network modules in the human brain" was peer-reviewed at PLOS Biology. It has now been evaluated by the PLOS Biology editors, an Academic Editor with relevant expertise, and by several independent reviewers.

In light of the reviews, which you will find at the end of this email, we would like to invite you to revise the work to thoroughly address the reviewers' reports.

As you will see below, the reviewers are overall interested in your study and identified many strengths. However, there are also a couple of concerns, some of which represent quite a methodological challenge. In addition to the other concerns, we think that it is also important to address the comments on the claim of novelty, while demonstrating substantial advance by the current findings.

Given the extent of revision needed, we cannot make a decision about publication until we have seen the revised manuscript and your response to the reviewers' comments. Your revised manuscript is likely to be sent for further evaluation by all or a subset of the reviewers.

We expect to receive your revised manuscript within 3 months. Please email us (plosbiology@plos.org) if you have any questions or concerns, or would like to request an extension.

At this stage, your manuscript remains formally under active consideration at our journal; please notify us by email if you do not intend to submit a revision so that we may withdraw it.

**IMPORTANT - SUBMITTING YOUR REVISION**

Your revisions should address the specific points made by each reviewer. Please submit the following files along with your revised manuscript:

1. A 'Response to Reviewers' file - this should detail your responses to the editorial requests, present a point-by-point response to all of the reviewers' comments, and indicate the changes made to the manuscript.

*NOTE: In your point-by-point response to the reviewers, please provide the full context of each review. Do not selectively quote paragraphs or sentences to reply to. The entire set of reviewer comments should be present in full and each specific point should be responded to individually, point by point.

You should also cite any additional relevant literature that has been published since the original submission and mention any additional citations in your response.

2. In addition to a clean copy of the manuscript, please also upload a 'track-changes' version of your manuscript that specifies the edits made. This should be uploaded as a "Revised Article with Changes Highlighted" file type.

*Re-submission Checklist*

When you are ready to resubmit your revised manuscript, please refer to this re-submission checklist: https://plos.io/Biology_Checklist

To submit a revised version of your manuscript, please go to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/ and log in as an Author. Click the link labelled 'Submissions Needing Revision' where you will find your submission record.

Please make sure to read the following important policies and guidelines while preparing your revision:

*Published Peer Review*

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out. Please see here for more details:

https://blogs.plos.org/plos/2019/05/plos-journals-now-open-for-published-peer-review/

*PLOS Data Policy*

Please note that as a condition of publication PLOS' data policy (http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/data-availability) requires that you make available all data used to draw the conclusions arrived at in your manuscript. If you have not already done so, you must include any data used in your manuscript either in appropriate repositories, within the body of the manuscript, or as supporting information (N.B. this includes any numerical values that were used to generate graphs, histograms etc.). For an example see here: http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pbio.1001908#s5

*Blot and Gel Data Policy*

We require the original, uncropped and minimally adjusted images supporting all blot and gel results reported in an article's figures or Supporting Information files. We will require these files before a manuscript can be accepted so please prepare them now, if you have not already uploaded them. Please carefully read our guidelines for how to prepare and upload this data: https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/figures#loc-blot-and-gel-reporting-requirements

*Protocols deposition*

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols

Thank you again for your submission to our journal. We hope that our editorial process has been constructive thus far, and we welcome your feedback at any time. Please don't hesitate to contact us if you have any questions or comments.

Sincerely,

Christian

Christian Schnell, PhD

Senior Editor

PLOS Biology

cschnell@plos.org

------------------------------------

REVIEWS:

Reviewer #1 (Adam Pines): This is a nice paper with several beneficial contributions. The description of the need for studying overlap in networks is very well-articulated, as are the methods. The longitudinal design of the study is another advantage, and the publicly available code is a clear strength.

I do have several comments, which are listed below. I'm suggesting major revisions, but I think they are minor as far as major revisions go.

Major Comments:

1. Figure 5 is interesting, but these correlations are quite low, particularly for brain maps. Please use something like you did for figure 6 (i.e., Burt et al 2020 or the spin test for parcels) to correct brain map associations by accounting for spatial autocorrelation. It's not clear that any of these associations are above chance, after accounting for spatial autocorrelation.

2. Please clarify if entire subjects are held out of train-test in figure 6. It is essential to not include the same subjects (across different observations) across train-test splits to avoid leakage.

3. The multimodal and cognitive aspects are good efforts but are a bit oversold. They might be interesting enough to warrant inclusion as either a main text or supplemental figure, but I don't think its fair to say the authors have "provided novel insights into the neural mechanism underlying individual cognitive development.", especially because the structure-function association is multivariate rather than tractable to a more straightforward univariate correspondence. What are the mechanisms, and furthermore, where are the individual-level measures of cognition? Please clarify or temper these claims.

4. Maximum head motion > 3 mm or 3° or mean framewise displacement (mFD) > 0.5 mm: seems like an incredibly liberal threshold. Could the authors provide a surface/parcel plot of entropy associations with FD parallel to their surface/parcel plot of entropy associations with age? I would imagine there are a ton of residual motion effects, and it would be useful for the reader to know whether or not this is the case, particularly if they can compare effect sizes/prominence relative to those reported for age.

Minor Comments:

1. The authors appear to make two claims of primacy that are unsubstantiated. Despite citing work from the same lab, I assume the authors were unaware of our study on overlapping functional modular organization in children and adolescents when they claimed "our study is the first to uncover overlapping functional modular organization in children and adolescents" (See Pines et al., Nature comms, 2022). We also take a personalized approach, directly tie individual level cognitive effects in, developed a generalized estimating equations approach to run stats on individual locations belonging to multiple networks, and explicitly quantify network edge development as a function of the hierarchical similarity (hierarchical distance) as alluded to by the current authors in this manuscript's discussion. You are not obligated to cite us, but the claim of primacy is false and should be removed. Further, although not in developing individuals, this paper is similar enough to "Edge-Community Entropy is a Novel Neural Correlate of Aging and Moderator of Fluid Cognition", now out in J Neurosci, where that work probably should be cited. The second unsubstantiated claim of primacy, although indirect, is "However, until now, there has been no gold standard for evaluating the quality of the detected overlapping functional modular structure in the human brain." I'm not sure the authors meant it this way, but it sounds like they are implying that they pioneered an approach to evaluate the quality of overlapping functional modular structure in the human brain. Unless I missed something substantial in the paper, this appears to be taking credit for the development of the approach, which by my understanding was done by the Betzel lab (Faskowitz et al), not the current authors. Apologies if I'm misunderstanding this claim, but I would at least clarify it.

2. Reliance on linear models might be one reason the authors are not returning a ton of significant results in figure 3. There's every reason to believe development is non-linear in this age range, as many of the authors' citations point to (see citation 9 for a particularly compelling example). I think the results they returned are real, but choice of linear mixed effect models seems like a suboptimal fit to the data at hand compared to something like generalized additive mixed effect models. It's not 100% necessary for this paper to make a contribution to the literature, but seems like a clear step forward.

3. I would recommend describing what went up and down in the sentences: "Statistical analysis revealed that seven nodal regions showed significant linear changes with age (pFDR corrected < 0.0014, Figure 3C). These regions showed dissociable age-related changes, with significant increases mainly located in the superior and inferior parietal lobules and the lateral prefrontal cortex and significant decreases mainly located in the ventral and medial prefrontal cortex and the putamen." I assume it's entropy but that's never actually stated in results or the figure caption.

4. Could the authors allocate a sentence or two to describing why they are fitting age as a random effect rather than purely as a fixed effect with random effects for "subname"?

5. I might avoid using the phrase "brain age", as this paper does not seem to be about discrepancies between predicted and actual age (which is a good thing!). Brain age literature is rife with problems (see Pitfalls in brain age analyses. Human brain mapping) that this current paper is not subject to.

6. It's not clear that you aren't using topological and topographical interchangeably. It's largely a stylistic choice to be consistent with that or not, and clarifying or standardizing your usage of these words is not something that will change my accept/non-accept opinion. However, some believe these refer to entirely different constructs (topology being purely in graph-space, topography representing a spatial distribution).

7. This is also just my opinion and not something I would suggest rejecting this paper for, but I don't think this reasoning is entirely coherent: "The high nodal overlap in the sensorimotor regions indicates the functional diversity of this system, which is consistent with its involvement in multiple modules (Figure 2E). In addition to the widely known sensorimotor functions [54], these regions may be involved in other cognitive functions or behaviors, such as attention allocation [55], speech perception [56], and emotional regulation". In many papers, including the Margulies PNAS paper cited several times, there is a clear case for sensorimotor regions being the most specialized for one specific modality out of all cortical regions (i.e., unimodal). The authors are by no means obligated to take the same explanation we have in similar papers (Pines et al., Nature comms, 2022, Luo et al., Nature comms, 2024), but as is I find their logic inconsistent with the citations they invoke.

8. Edge-centric analyses are an important attempt to get at different aspects of network organization, but are subject to important limitations. Please consider the work "A mathematical perspective on edge-centric brain functional connectivity" in nature comms in either your introduction or discussion.

9. I can't find a protocol paper or more detail on it, but it's odd that a HARDI scan would have ordinary diffusion weightings only. Would the authors mind confirming that only b=0 and b=1,000 were used? Or citing a protocol paper if one exists?

10. "Then, a weighted functional network was obtained by thresholding the correlation matrix with a density 5 of 15% (i.e., 4020 edges) to exclude potential spurious or weak correlations." This seems antagonistic to the idea of analyzing overlapping associations, which as the authors show, often fall into secondary or tertiary "network memberships" or more broadly FC strengths. Given that the data is already heavily reduced to 232 parcels, there doesn't seem to be any computational need for this step. Can the authors justify this choice? I do appreciate the inclusion of S2A and S2B to show similarity in the relative (although not absolute) distribution of entropy across regions across thresholds.

11. "To assess the statistical significance of the prediction accuracy, we generated a null distribution of accuracy using permutation tests (n = 100) by shuffling the actual entropy across nodes for each scan." Is n=100 correct? Looks like 10k in main text, and the p value for 7b indicates 10k permutations (because it can't be more precise than p<0.01 with 100 tests). If I'm simply misunderstanding, it would be great if the authors could clarify this.

12. Further, it's not clear that leave-region-out is a valid approach due to spatial autocorrelation. Please use leave entire-subjects-out if you want to include this analysis, or explain to me why that is not possible/valid in this instance. In my opinion, univariate associations between entropy and structure metrics are more informative than multivariate regression here, particularly if the authors are trying to claim something mechanistic.

Reviewer #2: In this work, the authors use a large longitudinal sample of children ages 6-14 years of age to look at how overlapping module assignment in edge-centric functional networks varies with age. They find that areas that show high and low overlap in assignment are relatively stable with age, with higher overlap in VA and SM networks and lower in VIS and DM. Some particular regions show age-related changes, such that their module assignment becomes more (PFC) or less (parietal cortex) stable with age. The authors also conduct ML analyses using SVR to show that module assignment overlap can predict age, and that regional variation in module overlap is associated most strongly with regional variation in patterns of cortical thickness.

The study has several strengths, including a large longitudinal sample and novel methods, and I very much appreciate the set of validation analyses. There are some weaknesses, specifically around clarity and interpretation of findings as well as methodological assumptions, that could be remedied. I've included specific comments on these aspects below.

Major comments

Intro is well-motivated for a study focusing on soft partitioning of functional networks and overlap of network assignments…but needs more motivation/explanation as to why taking an edge-centric approach in addition to a soft partitioning approach is necessary. It seems the authors are taking two steps from what has previously been done, rather than the one that they motivate in the intro, which makes comparison to previous studies harder.

Similarly, in the discussion, the authors frame their findings in the context of previous work on overlapping functional modular organization, but don't mention how their methods differ significantly (in the focus on edges) from other prior work that used nodal FC but characterized overlapping modular architecture.

There has been some work using soft partitioning methods that capture overlap in the age range studied that is not covered in the intro. Tooley et al 2022 (Neuroimage) and Faskowitz et al. 2018 (Sci Reports) come to mind. Dworetsky et al. 2021 (Neuroimage) and Hermosillo 2024 (Nat Neuro) might also be relevant, though in adults.

The reader needs clearer explanation of what goes into constructing a weighted edge graph, and perhaps an addition to Fig 1. Given that an edge represents the similarity of fluctuations in BOLD signal between two regions, what does the weighted edge graph represent? What is being evaluated for similarity between edges? I had to go look at Faskowitz 2020 to remind myself.

Thresholding at an arbitrary density is problematic for interpretation of results. Other work using edge-centric methods does not necessitate a specific threshold or the removal of negative edges before constructing an weighted edge matrix. This simply indicates that all results are likely based on only examining the strongest connections, while there is good evidence that weak connections might be changing during development and play an important role. The authors should conduct supplemental analyses using fully weighted matrices and/or matrices at a wider variety of densities to investigate this.

"For simplicity, we considered directly connected edges that shared at least one common node, and the similarity between edges without common nodes was assumed to be zero. "

This seems like an extremely strong assumption, and not one that I'm familiar with in other work. No rationale is provided or supplemental analyses showing that this is a reasonable assumption to make are included.

Is the Tanimoto coefficient a typical way to assess edge similarity? I see no mention of it in Faskowitz et al (2020). Or is this paper also introducing a different way of assessing edge similarity than has been previously used? If so, why?

If the main paper finding is related to the nodal entropy map (which the validation analyses suggest is the case), I'd suggest emphasizing the interpretation of this specific finding in the abstract and first paragraph of discussion in a clearer way. Findings that there are decreases in nodal overlap in module affiliations in PFC imply what for brain development? To my mind, it implies that PFC is becoming more homogenously affiliated, or more stable/less flexible in its network affiliation, with age. Some phrases of this nature would be helpful for a reader not versed in soft-partitioning methods.

Minor comments

Some clearer takeaways in the abstract might be appreciate by readers (e.g. what does regional inhomogeneity in module overlap mean for children's brain networks?)

The values of edge correlations in Fig 2a seem quite low, compared to the corresponding figure in Faskowitz 2020, and this figure further reinforces that setting many edge similarities to 0 without calculating them may be a strong assumption.

Would Fig 1d be better off as a graph with differently colored or weighted lines? It is initially difficult to interpret what the reader is trying to understand from the colors and sizes of circles.

It is quite interesting that looking at Fig 3b and comparing to adults, it seems that the overall trend might be decreasing entropy with age (both in insular areas and subcortex); this would imply that module assignments based on edge similarity are more stable in adults than children. Did the authors examine average whole-brain entropy (or cortical vs subcortical) associations with age?

The claim on page 13 line 27 seems quite strong, given some of the other research that exists already.

I'd find the title easier to parse as "Development of overlapping network modules in the human brain". I would be more specific than "signatures" when describing associations with brain structure.

Decision Letter 2

Christian Schnell, PhD

19 Aug 2024

Dear Dr He,

Thank you for your patience while we considered your revised manuscript "Development of overlapping network modules in the human brain" for publication as a Research Article at PLOS Biology. This revised version of your manuscript has been evaluated by the PLOS Biology editors, the Academic Editor and one of the original reviewers.

Based on the reviews and on our Academic Editor's assessment of your revision, we are likely to accept this manuscript for publication, provided you satisfactorily address the remaining points raised by the reviewers (specifically, Reviewer 1's comment regarding the legend of Figure 6B). Please also make sure to address the following data and other policy-related requests:

* We would like to suggest a different title to improve readability: "Functional network modules overlap and are linked to interindividual connectome differences during human brain development"

* Please include information in the Methods section whether the study has been conducted according to the principles expressed in the Declaration of Helsinki.

* Can you please check that the following blurb is correct: "Network modules in the human brain have mostly been considered to be non-overlapping during development. This neuroimaging study of functional connectome maturation during childhood and adolescence in over 300 children reveals its overlapping architecture, which is associated with structural properties."

* DATA POLICY:

You may be aware of the PLOS Data Policy, which requires that all data be made available without restriction: http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/data-availability. For more information, please also see this editorial: http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1001797

Note that we do not require all raw data. Rather, we ask that all individual quantitative observations that underlie the data summarized in the figures and results of your paper be made available in one of the following forms:

1) Supplementary files (e.g., excel). Please ensure that all data files are uploaded as 'Supporting Information' and are invariably referred to (in the manuscript, figure legends, and the Description field when uploading your files) using the following format verbatim: S1 Data, S2 Data, etc. Multiple panels of a single or even several figures can be included as multiple sheets in one excel file that is saved using exactly the following convention: S1_Data.xlsx (using an underscore).

2) Deposition in a publicly available repository. Please also provide the accession code or a reviewer link so that we may view your data before publication.

Regardless of the method selected, please ensure that you provide the individual numerical values that underlie the summary data displayed in the following figure panels as they are essential for readers to assess your analysis and to reproduce it: 4A, 6C, and 7B.

NOTE: the numerical data provided should include all replicates AND the way in which the plotted mean and errors were derived (it should not present only the mean/average values).

Please also ensure that figure legends in your manuscript include information on where the underlying data can be found, and ensure your supplemental data file/s has a legend.

Please ensure that your Data Statement in the submission system accurately describes where your data can be found.

* CODE POLICY

Per journal policy, if you have generated any custom code during the course of this investigation, please make it available without restrictions. Please ensure that the code is sufficiently well documented and reusable, and that your Data Statement in the Editorial Manager submission system accurately describes where your code can be found.

Please note that we cannot accept sole deposition of code in GitHub, as this could be changed after publication. However, you can archive this version of your publicly available GitHub code to Zenodo. Once you do this, it will generate a DOI number, which you will need to provide in the Data Accessibility Statement (you are welcome to also provide the GitHub access information). See the process for doing this here: https://docs.github.com/en/repositories/archiving-a-github-repository/referencing-and-citing-content

As you address these items, please take this last chance to review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the cover letter that accompanies your revised manuscript.

We expect to receive your revised manuscript within two weeks.

To submit your revision, please go to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/ and log in as an Author. Click the link labelled 'Submissions Needing Revision' to find your submission record. Your revised submission must include the following:

- a cover letter that should detail your responses to any editorial requests, if applicable, and whether changes have been made to the reference list

- a Response to Reviewers file that provides a detailed response to the reviewers' comments (if applicable, if not applicable please do not delete your existing 'Response to Reviewers' file.)

- a track-changes file indicating any changes that you have made to the manuscript.

NOTE: If Supporting Information files are included with your article, note that these are not copyedited and will be published as they are submitted. Please ensure that these files are legible and of high quality (at least 300 dpi) in an easily accessible file format. For this reason, please be aware that any references listed in an SI file will not be indexed. For more information, see our Supporting Information guidelines:

https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/supporting-information

*Published Peer Review History*

Please note that you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out. Please see here for more details:

https://blogs.plos.org/plos/2019/05/plos-journals-now-open-for-published-peer-review/

*Press*

Should you, your institution's press office or the journal office choose to press release your paper, please ensure you have opted out of Early Article Posting on the submission form. We ask that you notify us as soon as possible if you or your institution is planning to press release the article.

*Protocols deposition*

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols

Please do not hesitate to contact me should you have any questions.

Sincerely,

Christian

Christian Schnell, PhD

Senior Editor

cschnell@plos.org

PLOS Biology

------------------------------------------------------------------------

Reviewer remarks:

Reviewer #1: The authors demonstrated a strong effort in addressing our comments. I am convinced of their findings and commend their work. I also appreciate them clarifying and explaining several points to me.

I'm suggesting acceptance, but here are two remaining minor points for the authors to consider.

1. The caption for Figure 6B isn't clear. The gray frequency polygon is referenced, but not the blue one. I assume the first sentence after (B) refers to blue, but simply adding an equivalent sentence structure for the blue frequency polygon would make the difference between what they are intended to display more clear.

2. I'm not a fan of displaying only p-values from models as they have done in figure S5. Because it's a supplementary analysis and because GAMMs don't have straightforward coefficients, it's not a deal-breaker as is. But I would avoid doing that in future work.

Best of luck to the authors moving forward.

Decision Letter 3

Christian Schnell, PhD

29 Aug 2024

Dear Dr He,

Thank you for the submission of your revised Research Article "Functional network modules overlap and are linked to interindividual connectome differences during human brain development" for publication in PLOS Biology. On behalf of my colleagues and the Academic Editor, Claus Hilgetag, I am pleased to say that we can in principle accept your manuscript for publication, provided you address any remaining formatting and reporting issues. These will be detailed in an email you should receive within 2-3 business days from our colleagues in the journal operations team; no action is required from you until then. Please note that we will not be able to formally accept your manuscript and schedule it for publication until you have completed any requested changes.

Please take a minute to log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/, click the "Update My Information" link at the top of the page, and update your user information to ensure an efficient production process.

PRESS

We frequently collaborate with press offices. If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper at this point, to enable them to help maximise its impact. If the press office is planning to promote your findings, we would be grateful if they could coordinate with biologypress@plos.org. If you have previously opted in to the early version process, we ask that you notify us immediately of any press plans so that we may opt out on your behalf.

We also ask that you take this opportunity to read our Embargo Policy regarding the discussion, promotion and media coverage of work that is yet to be published by PLOS. As your manuscript is not yet published, it is bound by the conditions of our Embargo Policy. Please be aware that this policy is in place both to ensure that any press coverage of your article is fully substantiated and to provide a direct link between such coverage and the published work. For full details of our Embargo Policy, please visit http://www.plos.org/about/media-inquiries/embargo-policy/.

Thank you again for choosing PLOS Biology for publication and supporting Open Access publishing. We look forward to publishing your study. 

Sincerely, 

Christian

Christian Schnell, PhD

Senior Editor

PLOS Biology

cschnell@plos.org

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Fig. Univariate relationship in spatial patterns between nodal entropy and structural brain features within each scan in children.

    For each scan, we separately calculated Pearson’s correlation in spatial patterns between nodal entropy and each structural brain feature. (A) Frequency polygon of correlation coefficients for cortical thickness across rsfMRI scans. Compared to the null distribution, cortical thickness showed significant positive spatial correlations with nodal entropy in 17% of scans (74/446) (p < 0.05). (B) Frequency polygon of correlation coefficients for surface area in all rsfMRI scans. Compared to the null distribution, surface area showed significant negative correlations with nodal entropy in 44% of scans (196/446) (p < 0.05). (C) Frequency polygon of correlation coefficients for folding index in all rsfMRI scans. Compared to the null distribution, folding index showed significant negative correlations with nodal entropy in 46% of scans (207/446) (p < 0.05). (D) Frequency polygon of correlation coefficients for FA strength in all rsfMRI scans. Compared to the null distribution, FA strength showed significant negative correlations with nodal entropy in 5% of scans (24/446) (p < 0.05). (E) Frequency polygon of correlation coefficients for cortical curvature in all rsfMRI scans. Compared to the null distribution, cortical curvature showed significant negative correlations with nodal entropy in 30% of scans (136/446) (p < 0.05). (F) Frequency polygon of correlation coefficients for cortical volume in all rsfMRI scans. Compared to the null distribution, cortical volume showed significant negative correlations with nodal entropy in 17% of scans (76/446) (p < 0.05). In (A–F), the inset in the upper corner denotes the null distribution of the correlation coefficients. This null distribution was generated by aggregating the 100 permutation instances for each scan, resulting in a total of 44,600 permutation instances (446 scans × 100 times). For each permutation within each scan, a surrogate nodal entropy map was generated that preserved the spatial autocorrelation characteristics of the original nodal entropy map [56]. The red line denotes the 95% significance level in the null distribution.

    (TIF)

    pbio.3002653.s001.TIF (1.6MB, TIF)
    S2 Fig. Spatial patterns of nodal module overlap (i.e., nodal entropy) at different spatial resolutions.

    For the adult cohort, the overlapping modular architecture was separately detected in the group-level functional networks obtained from different functional parcellations. (A) Nodal overlap in the functional networks with a coarse parcellation. This network comprised 100 cortical nodes obtained from the Schaefer-100 atlas [97] and 32 subcortical regions [49]. (B) Nodal overlap in the functional networks with a fine parcellation (i.e., main results). This network comprised 200 cortical nodes obtained from the Schaefer-200 atlas [97] and 32 subcortical regions [49]. (C) Extent of nodal overlap for 8 systems at different spatial resolutions. Similar distributions of nodal overlap were observed between the 2 parcellations. VIS, visual; SM, somatomotor; DA, dorsal attention; VA, ventral attention; LIM, limbic; FP, frontoparietal; DM, default-mode; SUB, subcortical.

    (TIF)

    S3 Fig. Spatial patterns of nodal module overlap (i.e., nodal entropy) for different network analysis strategies and their relationships with the main results.

    All of these analyses were specified to the group-level functional network for the adult cohort. (A) Network density of 10% for functional network construction. (B) Network density of 20% for functional network construction. (C) Eigenspectral analysis for module detection in the edge graph. (D) Number of involved modules was used to quantify the extent of node module overlap. (E) Main result as a reference. In each case, all the network construction and analysis strategies were set to be the same as those in the main analysis, except for the strategy of interest. All correlations were assessed with Pearson’s correlation across nodal regions. The significance of spatial similarity was assessed by comparing the observed value to a null distribution generated through 10,000 permutations that retained the spatial autocorrelation characteristics of the nodal entropy map of the original adult cohort in the main results.

    (TIF)

    pbio.3002653.s003.TIF (5.4MB, TIF)
    S4 Fig. Spatial patterns of developmental changes in nodal entropy for different thresholding strategies.

    For each rsfMRI scan of children, the brain functional network was generated with different network thresholding strategies. (A) Network density of 10% for functional network construction. (B) Network density of 20% for functional network construction. (C) Network density of 30% for functional network construction. (D) Network density of 15% (i.e., main result as a reference). In each case, all the network construction and analysis strategies were set to be the same as those in the main analysis, except for the strategy of interest. All correlations were assessed using Pearson’s correlation across nodal regions. The significance of spatial similarity was assessed by comparing the observed value to a null distribution generated through 10,000 permutations that retained the spatial autocorrelation characteristics of the original age effect map (t-value map) in the main results.

    (TIF)

    pbio.3002653.s004.TIF (4.1MB, TIF)
    S5 Fig. Spatial patterns of developmental changes in nodal entropy using different statistical models.

    (A) Age effects on nodal entropy using a mixed effects model. (B) Age effects on nodal entropy using a generalized additive mixed effects model. (C) Spatial correlation of age effects on nodal entropy between 2 statistical methods. The significance of spatial similarity was assessed by comparing the observed value to a null distribution generated through 10,000 permutations that maintained the spatial autocorrelation characteristics of the original age effect map (i.e., p-value map) obtained from the MEM in the main results. MEM, mixed effects model; GAMM, generalized additive mixed model.

    (TIF)

    pbio.3002653.s005.TIF (2.1MB, TIF)
    S6 Fig. Effects of head motion parameters on developmental changes of nodal entropy.

    (A) Age effects on head motion parameters. Pearson’s correlation analysis revealed that individual age and mFD values showed a significant but weak correlation. (B) Left: Age effects on nodal entropy. Right: Head motion effects on nodal entropy. The influence of age and head motion on nodal entropy was evaluated using a mixed effects model. In this model, age was designated as the independent variable, while the head motion parameter (i.e., mFD) was included as a covariate. The variable of sex was also included in the model as a covariate. (C) Left: Spatial pattern of age effects on nodal module overlap with a head motion control strategy of mFD < 0.5 mm (main results). Middle: Spatial pattern of age effects on nodal module overlap with a more strict head motion control strategy with mFD < 0.2 mm. Right: Spatial correlation of age effects on nodal entropy between 2 head motion control strategies. The significance of the spatial similarity was assessed by comparing the observed value to a null distribution generated through 10,000 permutations that retained the spatial autocorrelation characteristics of the original age effect map (t-value map) in the main results. mFD, mean framewise displacement.

    (TIF)

    pbio.3002653.s006.TIF (3.8MB, TIF)
    Attachment

    Submitted filename: ResponsesToReviewers.docx

    pbio.3002653.s007.docx (9.9MB, docx)
    Attachment

    Submitted filename: ResponsesToReviewers.docx

    pbio.3002653.s008.docx (17.8KB, docx)

    Data Availability Statement

    All code and data used in the analyses are available in the Zenodo database (https://osf.io/qfcyu/; doi: 10.17605/OSF.IO/QFCYU) and on GitHub (https://github.com/helab207/Development-of-the-overlapping-modular-structure-in-human-brain-functional-networks).


    Articles from PLOS Biology are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES