Abstract
Autonomous sensory meridian response (ASMR) is characterized by a tingling sensation with a feeling of relaxation and a state of flow. We explore the neural underpinnings and comorbidities of ASMR and related phenomena with altered sensory processing. These phenomena include sensory processing sensitivity (SPS), synaesthesia, Alice in Wonderland syndrome and misophonia. The objective of this article is to uncover the shared neural substrates and distinctive features of ASMR and its counterparts. ASMR, SPS and misophonia exhibit common activations in the brain regions associated with social cognition, emotion regulation and empathy. Nevertheless, ASMR responders display reduced connectivity in the salience network (SN), while individuals with SPS exhibit increased connectivity in the SN. Furthermore, ASMR induces relaxation and temporarily reduces symptoms of depression, in contrast to SPS and misophonia, which are linked to depression. These observations lead us to propose that ASMR is a distinct phenomenon owing to its attention dispatch mechanism and its connection with emotion regulation. We suggest that increased activations in the insula, along with reduction in connectivity within the salience and default mode networks in ASMR responders, may account for their experiences of relaxation and flow states.
This article is part of the theme issue ‘Sensing and feeling: an integrative approach to sensory processing and emotional experience’.
Keywords: autonomous sensory meridian response, sensory processing sensitivity, insula, salience network, default mode network
1. Introduction
Autonomous sensory meridian response (ASMR) is a nonclinical term to describe a multidimensional phenomenon primarily triggered by auditory and audio-visual stimuli [1,2]. Individuals who experience ASMR describe a tingling sensation, originating in the head and spreading down to the body, accompanied by a feeling of relaxation and a mental state known as ‘flow’ [1,3]. In a flow state, a person is fully engrossed in an activity, free from self-consciousness, deeply focused and experiencing a distortion of time [4].
The estimated prevalence of ASMR ranges from 23.5 to 28% [5,6]. The common ASMR triggers include auditory and audio-visual stimuli made with persons whispering, eating, and using objects to make sounds [2]. Low-pitched sounds with dark timbre are effective in inducing ASMR [2,7].
ASMR can be regarded as an illusion or altered perception, transitioning from audio-visual information to tactile sensation. In this article, we compare ASMR with related sensory phenomena, including sensory processing sensitivity (SPS), synaesthesia, Alice in Wonderland syndrome (AIWS) and misophonia. Individuals with a SPS trait are easily overstimulated by external stimuli, and research has shown that their ASMR intensity is associated with SPS levels [8]. Synaesthesia is a phenomenon where a stimulus in one sensory modality elicits sensations in that modality and other modalities, and a higher prevalence of synaesthesia has been found among ASMR responders [1,6,9]. AIWS is a neurological condition known for distorted perception of the body and space, and it shares a distortion of time perception with ASMR. In addition, the prevalence of AIWS symptoms among ASMR responders (46.6%) is higher than the prevalence of AIWS in the general population (approx. 9%) [10–12]. Individuals with misophonia experience negative emotions and aversive responses elicited by specific auditory and visual stimuli. Given the overlap in triggers between ASMR and misophonia (e.g. eating sounds and repetitive tapping), ASMR and misophonia are suggested to share same mechanisms [13]. Although ASMR is linked to positive emotions, and misophonia is associated with negative emotions, there is a higher prevalence of misophonia among ASMR responders and vice versa [14–16].
This article reviews functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies that investigate the brain activations observed in these phenomena or in individuals who experience these phenomena, with a focus on the following brain regions: the insula and cingulate cortex (CC) related to social cognition and emotion regulation, the nucleus accumbens (NAc) of the reward system, and the sensory-motor system, including the premotor area (PMA), supplementary motor area (SMA) and somatosensory cortex.
Three large-scale brain networks in resting-state fMRI (rs-fMRI), including the salience network (SN), default mode network (DMN) and central executive network (CEN) (figure 1), are also compared between ASMR and its related phenomena. The SN is involved in detecting salient stimuli and switching between the DMN and the CEN [17]. The DMN is active when a person is at wakeful rest, such as during mind wandering and introspection. Meanwhile, the CEN is active when a person is engaged in specific tasks [18–22]. Through these comparisons, we aim to elucidate the similarities and differences in the brain circuits in ASMR and related phenomena. In addition to the brain circuits, we explore triggers, physiological responses, comorbidities and genetic predisposition associated with these phenomena.
Figure 1.
The framework of three large-scale networks in the human brain. The circles indicate important hubs in these networks. The salience network consists of the insula and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC). The central executive network includes the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and posterior parietal cortex (PPC), whereas the default mode network primarily comprises medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and posterior cingulate cortex (PCC).
To investigate brain activations and networks in these phenomena, we conducted a search on PubMed using keywords specific to each phenomenon, along with ‘fMRI’. In most cases, there are a handful of papers to review. Nevertheless, our search yielded numerous papers on synaesthesia. As many of these papers provide evidence supporting the theory of increased co-activations or connectivity across brain regions in synaesthetes, we only referred to some of the most cited papers that support and oppose this theory. Additionally, we searched for related literature using ‘electroencephalography (EEG)’ and ‘magnetoencephalography (MEG)’, but we found a very limited amount of literature using EEG and MEG to investigate these phenomena.
(a) . Autonomous sensory meridian response
ASMR elicits physiological responses in ASMR responders, including increased pupil diameters [23], enhanced skin conductance and reduced heart rate [24,25]. These observations suggest that ASMR induces arousal and relaxation among these individuals.
There are two types of fMRI studies in ASMR research: task-positive fMRI studies, which compare brain activations in ASMR responders and non-responders while they watch ASMR videos and control videos; and task-negative rs-fMRI studies, which compare brain functional connectivity between ASMR responders and non-responders. In task-positive fMRI studies, brain activations in the insula, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), NAc, secondary somatosensory cortex and SMA are more pronounced for ASMR responders when they experience a tingling sensation during ASMR videos than watching a fixation cross (namely, the control condition) [26]. In another study, while both ASMR responders and non-responders watched ASMR and control videos, ASMR responders showed more activity in the dACC, PMA and SMA in the ASMR condition than in the control condition, but non-responders only showed increased activity in the cuneus [27]. Auditory-only ASMR stimuli have been shown to increase activations of the insula and NAc compared with the resting condition [28]. The involvement of the insula and ACC suggests the significance of social cognition, emotion regulation and the perception of social pain in ASMR experience [29,30]. The insula, ACC and NAc are also activated during music-induced chills [31–34].
While the activations in the insula and dACC, the key hubs in the SN, increase in ASMR responders when they watch ASMR videos, rs-fMRI studies showed less connectivity in the precuneus-centred DMN, SN and CEN (except in the left middle cingulate cortex) in ASMR responders than in non-responders [35,36]. Another study revealed higher functional connectivity seeded from the posterior cingulate cortex, a major hub of the DMN, to the superior and middle temporal gyri, cuneus, and lingual gyrus when comparing ASMR videos with resting state [37]. In addition to SN, DMN and CEN, other networks such as the sensorimotor and visual networks have been compared between ASMR responders and non-responders. The results show reduced functional connectivity in these two networks in ASMR responders [36].
The EEG studies exploring brain waves in different frequency ranges show increased alpha-wave activities when ASMR responders experience ASMR responses (i.e. tingling sensation) to ASMR stimuli [38,39]. Interestingly, footstep sounds, a less common ASMR trigger, were also observed to increased alpha-wave activities, especially among those who were less sensitive to these sounds and have a positive attitude toward them [40]. These findings indicate facilitation of attention-directed information and inhibition of irrelevant information [41]. This implication is consistent with the fMRI finding of insula activation in ASMR experiences, as the insula plays an important role in salience detection [17,42–44].
Although ASMR and autism share auditory sensitivity, there is no evidence supporting the association between ASMR and autism [45]. ASMR videos have been reported to alleviate symptoms of depression and anxiety [1,46].
(b) . Sensory processing sensitivity
SPS is defined as a personality trait characterized by being overstimulated by external stimuli, especially the positive ones, with heightened emotional responses [47,48]. SPS exhibits a prevalence ranging from 15 to 31% [48,49]. The Highly Sensitive Person (HSP) scale [49] has been used to measure the level of SPS, in addition to the Orienting Sensitivity subscale of the Adult Temperament Questionnaire [50]. The HSP scale comprises three subscales: low sensory threshold (LST), ease of excitation (EOE) and aesthetic sensitivity (AES) [51]. Genetic and twin studies suggest that SPS is influenced by genetic and environmental factors, with the AES subscale differentiating from the LST and EOE subscales [52,53]. The intensity of ASMR is associated with the AES subscale [8], while autism symptoms, alexithymia, anxiety and depression are linked to the EOE and LST subscales [54,55].
There are various triggers for individuals with SPS. In a study subjecting a group of adolescents to a stress test, their changes in heart rate, heart rate variability and skin conductance were not correlated with SPS, but their perceived stress was associated with SPS [56]. The relationship between SPS and subjective perception of stress indicates that the insula, especially the anterior insula, may play an important role in SPS. The anterior insula has been argued to integrate information from the internal and external stimuli, and its activity is associated with subjective feeling [42].
Several fMRI studies have explored the relationship between SPS levels and brain activations by using various stimuli. The SPS levels were positively correlated with reaction times to detect major and minor changes in visual stimuli and activations in the right claustrum, bilateral temporal cortex, bilateral precuneus and right inferior parietal cortex [57]. Another study observed the association between SPS levels and activations in the CC and PMA by using partner's and strangers’ faces with different emotions [58]. In addition, the primary somatosensory cortex and ventral tagmental area (VTA) were specifically activated by comparing the brain activations for partner's and strangers’ happy face. In another study, wherein the participants were touched by an experimenter, SPS levels were linked to brain activations in the insula [59]. Collectively, these findings in fMRI studies highlight the brain regions related to social cognition and empathy for social stimuli, such as the insula and CC, and those related to the reward pathway for positive stimuli, such as the VTA.
An rs-fMRI study has shown that the SPS levels were associated with enhanced brain connectivity within the SN and the dorsal attention network, as well as the limbic network [60]. These findings suggest an important role of attention and emotion regulation in SPS.
2. Synaesthesia
Synaesthesia is a phenomenon wherein a stimulus for a sensory modality elicits sensations in that modality and other modalities. The prevalence of various forms of synaesthesia is 4.4%, and the estimated prevalence of grapheme–colour synaesthesia, in which letters and numbers elicit colour perception, is 1.1% [61]. Although the presence of synaesthesia in family members suggests a hereditary connection [62,63], a twin study demonstrates that synaesthesia is a heritable condition that is influenced by epigenetic and environmental factors [64]. Previous studies have reported a higher prevalence of synaesthesia in the autism population compared with the neurotypical population. This association between autism and synaesthesia is believed to be mediated by attention to details and abnormal sensory processing [65–69].
Numerous studies have explored the neural basis of synaesthesia and identified activation in the sensory cortices beyond the stimulated modality and increased connectivity in sensory networks [70–73]. An rs-fMRI study revealed increased intranetwork connectivity from the medial and the lateral visual networks to the right frontoparietal network in grapheme–colour synaesthetes [74]. Nevertheless, ongoing debate persists about these findings and whether we should consider alternative neural models [75]. For example, the report of activation in the insula and CC in synaesthesia should be noted [76].
Mirror-touch synaesthesia, which involves experiencing tactile sensations on their own bodies upon seeing someone else touched [77], has been reported in approximately 1.6% of the population [78]. Some researchers debate about whether this phenomenon is a synaesthesia or vicarious experience, an empathetic state induced by seeing someone's sensations, emotions and actions [79]. Activations in the somatosensory cortex in mirror-touch synaesthetes may be related to empathy [80–83]. Additionally, mirror-touch synaesthetes exhibit higher autistic trait scores compared with the control group [84].
(a) . Alice in Wonderland syndrome
AIWS is a neurological condition characterized by distorted perception of the body, objects, space and time [10,85–87]. These perceptual distortions can manifest across the visual (e.g. dysmorphopsia, macropsia, micropsia and teleopsia), auditory and somaesthetic domains (e.g. macropsia, micropsia, teleopsia, macrosomatognosia, microsomatognosia and deceleration/acceleration of psychological time) [10,87,88] and have been observed in various clinical conditions, including migraine [88–90], epilepsy [91], infection [92–100] and concussion [101]. AIWS has also been found among people who use certain medication, such as 5-HT2 antagonist, and during intoxication with some substances, such as cannabis [10].
A lesion-mapping study identified local maxima across the lesions in the right occipital lobe, primarily driven by those with AIWS symptoms in the visual domain [102]. Those patients who had AIWS symptoms only in the somaesthetic domain exhibited lesions in the limbic system, including the insula, thalamus, CC, hippocampus and parahippocampus. This lesion study did not include participants perceiving time distortion, but the insula has been argued to be involved in time perception [103].
An imaging study that compared migraine patients with AIWS, migraine patients with typical aura, and healthy individuals showed that all migraine patients (with and without AIWS) had a lower functional connectivity in the lateral and the medial visual networks and higher functional connectivity in the SN and DMN compared with the healthy individuals [104].
3. Misophonia
Misophonia describes the phenomenon of experiencing negative emotions and aversive responses triggered by specific auditory and visual stimuli. Given the lack of well defined criteria for misophonia, estimates of its prevalence vary widely, ranging from 5.9 to 49.1% [16,105–109]. Although misophonia is not included in the Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM), researchers have proposed diagnostic criteria of misophonia to address this issue [110–112]. Misophonia typically involves excessive and disproportional negative emotions, often leading affected individuals to avoid the triggering stimuli. Furthermore, individuals with misophonia exhibit physiological responses when exposed to such triggers, including increased skin conductance, elevated heart rate and an increased low-frequency component of heart rate variability [113–117]. These physiological responses indicate heightened sympathetic tones.
fMRI studies have established a connection between misophonia and increased brain activation in the insula, auditory cortex, temporal cortex, CC, medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and PMA. Furthermore, misophonia is linked to increased functional connectivity between the insula and the mPFC, the key hubs in the SN and the DMN [114,115,117], indicating the link between the SN and the DMN. On the other hand, the association between misophonia and increased brain activations in the orofacial motor area remains debated [118,119].
Misophonia has been found to be comorbid with various psychiatric conditions, including obsessive–compulsive personality disorder, mood disorders, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, autism, tinnitus, hyperacusis, post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), eating disorders, migraine with visual aura, anxiety sensitivity, and hypersensitivity [15,109,120–123].
4. Discussion
This review encompasses the identification of the common and distinctive neural circuits associated with ASMR and related sensory phenomena, including SPS, synaesthesia, AIWS and misophonia, to shed light on their underlying neural mechanisms. Among these phenomena, ASMR and SPS share activation in the reward pathway. Activation in the NAc are observed in ASMR, while activations in the VTA are observed in SPS. The involvement of these reward-related regions in ASMR is consistent with experiences of music-induced pleasure and frisson, characterized by the sensation of goose-bumps or shivers down the spine [33,34]. ASMR and mirror-touch synaesthesia share activation in the somatosensory cortex. While the role of the somatosensory cortex in mirror-touch synaesthesia is suggested to be linked to empathy, the role of the somatosensory cortex in ASMR may be linked to sound localization and head orientation [124,125].
The insula, a pivotal brain region, emerges as a shared activation site in ASMR, SPS, AIWS (in the somaesthetic domain) and misophonia. The insula has connections with the cingulate, frontal, parietal, temporal and occipital cortices and subcortical structures, such as the amygdala and NAc [126–129]. The insula plays an important role in novel stimuli detection and is part of the SN, alongside the dACC and amygdala. In addition to salience detection, the insula is involved in visceral sensations, autonomic control, affective processes, sensorimotor processes, auditory processing, taste, olfaction and pain. The insula is proposed to be the centre of awareness and the interoceptive template for a ‘feeling’ [42–44]. The insula and ACC are closely connected and usually activate together, and they play an important role in social pain and empathy [130,131]. In summary, activation in the insula observed in ASMR, SPS, AIWS and misophonia is associated with peculiar perception and emotional responses observed in these phenomena.
Diverging from the previously mentioned shared brain activations, rs-fMRI studies uncover unique facets of these phenomena. ASMR responders exhibit weaker connectivity in the SN despite strong activations in the key hubs of the SN, the insula and the dACC. This finding suggests a diminished capacity for attention switch between internal and external stimuli. Furthermore, non-synchronization of the SN with strongly activated insula and dACC may result in altered perception. By contrast, increased connectivity in the SN is observed in SPS and AIWS, potentially linked to their sensory sensitivity or distortion.
This review also explores the comorbidities associated with these phenomena. ASMR exhibits links to temporary reduction of anxiety and depression symptoms but not to autism. Meanwhile, SPS is associated with autism, anxiety and depression; synaesthesia with autism; AIWS with migraine; and misophonia with autism, anxiety, depression and migraine. In other words, ASMR is on the opposite of the phenomena SPS, synaesthesia and misophonia.
This review acknowledges certain limitations. Heterogeneity amongst the participants in ASMR, SPS, synaesthesia, AIWS and misophonia studies, stemming from symptom-based definitions and questionnaire assessments poses a significant limitation. The modest amount of literature on these phenomena (except synaesthesia) also exacerbates this limitation. Furthermore, the diversity of the stimuli employed in physiological and imaging studies, the relatively small sample sizes and variations in experimental paradigms contribute to the need for caution in drawing definitive conclusions. Accordingly, this article serves as a basis for further investigations aimed at unravelling the intricate correlations between ASMR-induced sensations and neural networks. Future studies should address standardized stimuli and psychophysical approaches to objectively identify the target group.
5. Conclusion
The culmination of previous studies demonstrates that ASMR is distinct from related phenomena with altered sensory processing, including SPS, synaesthesia, AIWS and misophonia. Nonetheless, ASMR, SPS and misophonia all involve activation of the insula and CC related to social cognition and emotion regulation. ASMR differentiates itself from SPS through its reduced connectivity within the SN. Reduced connectivity in the SN during ASMR experiences indicates a particular way to direct attention that links to the feeling of relaxation and the flow state.
Data accessibility
All data gathered for this review may be available upon request to the corresponding author.
Declaration of AI use
We have used AI-assisted technologies in creating this article.
Authors' contributions
I.-F.L.: conceptualization, data curation, investigation, methodology, resources, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing; H.M.K.: data curation, funding acquisition, investigation, resources, writing—review and editing.
Both authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be held accountable for the work performed herein.
Conflict of interest declaration
We declare that we have no competing interests.
Funding
H.M.K. was supported by Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS) KAKENHI grants (nos 20H01789 and 22K18659).
References
- 1.Barratt EL, Davis NJ. 2015. Autonomous sensory meridian response (ASMR): a flow-like mental state. PeerJ 3, e851. ( 10.7717/peerj.851) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Barratt EL, Spence C, Davis NJ. 2017. Sensory determinants of the autonomous sensory meridian response (ASMR): understanding the triggers. PeerJ 5, e3846. ( 10.7717/peerj.3846) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Roberts N, Beath A, Boag S. 2019. Autonomous sensory meridian response: scale development and personality correlates. Psychol. Conscious. Theory Res. Pract. 6, 22-39. ( 10.1037/cns0000168) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Jackson SA, Marsh HW. 1996. Development and validation of a scale to measure optimal experience: the flow state scale. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol 18, 17-35. ( 10.1123/jsep.18.1.17) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Roberts N, Beath A, Boag S. 2020. A mixed-methods examination of autonomous sensory meridian response: comparison to frisson. Conscious. Cogn. 86, 103046. ( 10.1016/j.concog.2020.103046) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Poerio GL, Ueda M, Kondo HM. 2022. Similar but different: high prevalence of synesthesia in autonomous sensory meridian response (ASMR). Front. Psychol. 13, 990565. ( 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.990565) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Koumura T, Nakatani M, Liao H-I, Kondo HM. 2021. Dark, loud, and compact sounds induce frisson. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 74, 1140-1152. ( 10.1177/1747021820977174) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Poerio GL, Mank S, Hostler TJ. 2022. The awesome as well as the awful: heightened sensory sensitivity predicts the presence and intensity of autonomous sensory meridian response (ASMR). J. Res. Pers. 97, 104183. ( 10.1016/j.jrp.2021.104183) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Gillmeister H, Succi A, Romei V, Poerio GL. 2022. Touching you, touching me: higher incidence of mirror-touch synaesthesia and positive (but not negative) reactions to social touch in autonomous sensory meridian response. Conscious. Cogn. 103, 103380. ( 10.1016/j.concog.2022.103380) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Blom JD. 2016. Alice in Wonderland syndrome. Neurol. Clin. Pract 6, 259-270. ( 10.1212/CPJ.0000000000000251) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Bedwell SA, Butcher I. 2020. The co-occurrence of Alice in Wonderland syndrome and autonomous sensory meridian response. PsyPag Q. 114, 18-26. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Abe K, Oda N, Araki R, Igata M. 1989. Macropsia, micropsia, and episodic illusions in Japanese adolescents. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 28, 493-496. ( 10.1097/00004583-198907000-00004) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.McGeoch PD, Rouw R. 2020. How everyday sounds can trigger strong emotions: ASMR, misophonia and the feeling of wellbeing. Bioessays 42, 2000099. ( 10.1002/bies.202000099) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.McErlean ABJ, Banissy MJ. 2018. Increased misophonia in self-reported autonomous sensory meridian response. PeerJ 6, e5351. ( 10.7717/peerj.5351) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Rouw R, Erfanian M. 2018. A large-scale study of misophonia. J. Clin. Psychol. 74, 453-479. ( 10.1002/jclp.22500) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Jakubovski E, Müller A, Kley H, de Zwaan M, Müller-Vahl K. 2022. Prevalence and clinical correlates of misophonia symptoms in the general population of Germany. Front. Psychiatry 13, 1012424. ( 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.1012424) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Menon V, Uddin LQ. 2010. Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct. Funct. 214, 655-667. ( 10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Shen HH. 2015. Resting-state connectivity. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, 14 115-14 116. ( 10.1073/pnas.1518785112) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Seeley WW. 2019. The salience network: a neural system for perceiving and responding to homeostatic demands. J. Neurosci. 39, 9878-9882. ( 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1138-17.2019) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Sridharan D, Levitin DJ, Menon V. 2008. A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 12 569-12 574. ( 10.1073/pnas.0800005105) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Goulden N, Khusnulina A, Davis NJ, Bracewell RM, Bokde AL, McNulty JP, Mullins PG. 2014. The salience network is responsible for switching between the default mode network and the central executive network: replication from DCM. Neuroimage 99, 180-190. ( 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.052) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME. 2005. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 102, 9673-9678. ( 10.1073/pnas.0504136102) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Valtakari NV, Hooge ITC, Benjamins JS, Keizer A. 2019. An eye-tracking approach to autonomous sensory meridian response (ASMR): the physiology and nature of tingles in relation to the pupil. PLoS ONE 14, e0226692. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0226692) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Poerio GL, Blakey E, Hostler TJ, Veltri T. 2018. More than a feeling: autonomous sensory meridian response (ASMR) is characterized by reliable changes in affect and physiology. PLoS ONE 13, e0196645. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0196645) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Engelbregt HJ, Brinkman K, van Geest CCE, Irrmischer M, Deijen JB. 2022. The effects of autonomous sensory meridian response (ASMR) on mood, attention, heart rate, skin conductance and EEG in healthy young adults. Exp. Brain Res. 240, 1727-1742. ( 10.1007/s00221-022-06377-9) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Lochte BC, Guillory SA, Richard CAH, Kelley WM. 2018. An fMRI investigation of the neural correlates underlying the autonomous sensory meridian response (ASMR). BioImpacts 8, 295-304. ( 10.15171/bi.2018.32) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Smith SD, Fredborg BK, Kornelsen J. 2019. A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation of the autonomous sensory meridian response. PeerJ 7, e7122. ( 10.7717/peerj.7122) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Sakurai N, Ohno K, Kasai S, Nagasaka K, Onishi H, Kodama N. 2021. Induction of relaxation by autonomous sensory meridian response. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 15, 761621. ( 10.3389/fnbeh.2021.761621) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Tchalova K, Eisenberger NI. 2015. How the brain feels the hurt of heartbreak: examining the neurobiological overlap between social and physical pain. In Brain mapping (ed. Stein J), pp. 15-20. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Apps MAJ, Rushworth MFS, Chang SWC. 2016. The anterior cingulate gyrus and social cognition: tracking the motivation of others. Neuron 90, 692-707. ( 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.04.018) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Blood AJ, Zatorre RJ. 2001. Intensely pleasurable responses to music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and emotion. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98, 11 818-11 823. ( 10.1073/pnas.191355898) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Chanda ML, Levitin DJ. 2013. The neurochemistry of music. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 179-193. ( 10.1016/j.tics.2013.02.007) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Koelsch S. 2013. Emotion and music. In The Cambridge handbook of human affective neuroscience (eds Armony J, Vuilleumier P), pp. 286-303. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 34.Koelsch S. 2014. Brain correlates of music-evoked emotions. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 170-180. ( 10.1038/nrn3666) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Smith SD, Fredborg BK, Kornelsen J. 2017. An examination of the default mode network in individuals with autonomous sensory meridian response (ASMR). Social Neurosci. 12, 361-365. ( 10.1080/17470919.2016.1188851) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Smith SD, Fredborg BK, Kornelsen J. 2019. Atypical functional connectivity associated with autonomous sensory meridian response: an examination of five resting-state networks. Brain Connect. 9, 508-518. ( 10.1089/brain.2018.0618) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Lee S, Kim J, Tak S. 2020. Effects of autonomous sensory meridian response on the functional connectivity as measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 14, 154. ( 10.3389/fnbeh.2020.00154) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Fredborg BK, Champagne-Jorgensen K, Desroches A, Smith S. 2021. An electroencephalographic examination of the autonomous sensory meridian response (ASMR). Conscious. Cogn. 87, 103053. ( 10.1016/j.concog.2020.103053) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Swart TR, Banissy MJ, Hein TP, Bruña R, Pereda E, Bhattacharya J. 2022. ASMR amplifies low frequency and reduces high frequency oscillations. Cortex 149, 85-100. ( 10.1016/j.cortex.2022.01.004) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Frescura A, Lee P-J, Jeong J-H, Soeta Y. 2023. EEG alpha wave responses to sounds from neighbours in high-rise wood residential buildings. Build. Environ. 242, 110560. ( 10.1016/j.buildenv.2023.110560) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Klimesch W, Sauseng P, Hanslmayr S. 2007. EEG alpha oscillations: the inhibition–timing hypothesis. Brain Res. Rev. 53, 63-88. ( 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.06.003) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Craig AD, Craig AD. 2009. How do you feel?-now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 59-70. ( 10.1038/nrn2555) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Craig AD. 2011. Significance of the insula for the evolution of human awareness of feelings from the body. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1225, 72-82. ( 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.05990.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Strigo IA, Craig AD. 2016. Interoception, homeostatic emotions and sympathovagal balance. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 371, 20160010. ( 10.1098/rstb.2016.0010) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Tada K, Hasegawa R, Kondo HM. 2022. Sensitivity to everyday sounds: ASMR, misophonia, and autistic traits. Jap. J. Psychol. 93, 263-269. ( 10.4992/jjpsy.93.21319) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Hu MQ, Li HL, Huang SQ, Jin YT, Wang SS, Ying L, Qi YY, Yu X, Zhou Q. 2022. Reduction of psychological cravings and anxiety in women compulsorily isolated for detoxification using autonomous sensory meridian response (ASMR). Brain Behav. 12, e2636. ( 10.1002/brb3.2636) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Acevedo B, Aron E, Pospos S, Jessen D. 2018. The functional highly sensitive brain: a review of the brain circuits underlying sensory processing sensitivity and seemingly related disorders. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 373, 20170161. ( 10.1098/rstb.2017.0161) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Lionetti F, Aron A, Aron EN, Burns GL, Jagiellowicz J, Pluess M. 2018. Dandelions, tulips and orchids: evidence for the existence of low-sensitive, medium-sensitive and high-sensitive individuals. Transl. Psychiatry 8, 24. ( 10.1038/s41398-017-0090-6) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Aron EN, Aron A. 1997. Highly Sensitive Person Scale (HSPS). APA PsycTests. ( 10.1037/t00299-000) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Evans D, Rothbart M. 2007. Developing a model for adult temperament. J. Res. Pers. 41, 868-888. ( 10.1016/j.jrp.2006.11.002) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Smolewska KA, McCabe SB, Woody EZ. 2006. A psychometric evaluation of the Highly Sensitive Person Scale: the components of sensory-processing sensitivity and their relation to the BIS/BAS and ‘Big Five’. Pers. Individ. Diff. 40, 1269-1279. ( 10.1016/j.paid.2005.09.022) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Assary E, Zavos HMS, Krapohl E, Keers R, Pluess M. 2021. Genetic architecture of environmental sensitivity reflects multiple heritable components: a twin study with adolescents. Mol. Psychiatry 26, 4896-4904. ( 10.1038/s41380-020-0783-8) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Chen C, Chen C, Moyzis R, Stern H, He Q, Li H, Li J, Zhu B, Dong Q. 2011. Contributions of dopamine-related genes and environmental factors to highly sensitive personality: a multi-step neuronal system-level approach. PLoS ONE 6, e21636. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0021636) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Liss M, Mailloux J, Erchull MJ. 2008. The relationships between sensory processing sensitivity, alexithymia, autism, depression, and anxiety. Pers. Individ. Diff. 45, 255-259. ( 10.1016/j.paid.2008.04.009) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Yano K, Oishi K. 2018. The relationships among daily exercise, sensory-processing sensitivity, and depressive tendency in Japanese university students. Pers. Individ. Diff. 127, 49-53. ( 10.1016/j.paid.2018.01.047) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Weyn S, et al. 2022. Individual differences in environmental sensitivity at physiological and phenotypic level: two sides of the same coin? Int. J. Psychophysiol. 176, 36-53. ( 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2022.02.010) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Jagiellowicz J, Xu X, Aron A, Aron E, Cao G, Feng T, Weng X. 2011. The trait of sensory processing sensitivity and neural responses to changes in visual scenes. Social Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 6, 38-47. ( 10.1093/scan/nsq001) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Acevedo BP, Aron EN, Aron A, Sangster M, Collins N, Brown LL. 2014. The highly sensitive brain: an fMRI study of sensory processing sensitivity and response to others' emotions. Brain Behav. 4, 580-594. ( 10.1002/brb3.242) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Schaefer M, Kühnel A, Gärtner M. 2022. Sensory processing sensitivity and somatosensory brain activation when feeling touch. Scient. Rep. 12, 12024. ( 10.1038/s41598-022-15497-9) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Acevedo BP, Santander T, Marhenke R, Aron A, Aron E. 2021. Sensory processing sensitivity predicts individual differences in resting-state functional connectivity associated with depth of processing. Neuropsychobiology 80, 185-200. ( 10.1159/000513527) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Simner J, Mulvenna C, Sagiv N, Tsakanikos E, Witherby SA, Fraser C, Scott K, Ward J. 2006. Synaesthesia: the prevalence of atypical cross-modal experiences. Perception 35, 1024-1033. ( 10.1068/p5469) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Baron-Cohen S, Burt L, Smith-Laittan F, Harrison J, Bolton P. 1996. Synaesthesia: prevalence and familiality. Perception 25, 1073-1079. ( 10.1068/p251073) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Tilot AK, Kucera KS, Vino A, Asher JE, Baron-Cohen S, Fisher SE. 2018. Rare variants in axonogenesis genes connect three families with sound–color synesthesia. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 115, 3168-3173. ( 10.1073/pnas.1715492115) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Bosley HG, Eagleman DM. 2015. Synesthesia in twins: incomplete concordance in monozygotes suggests extragenic factors. Behav. Brain Res. 286, 93-96. ( 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.02.024) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Neufeld J, Roy M, Zapf A, Sinke C, Emrich HM, Prox-Vagedes V, Dillo W, Zedler M. 2013. Is synesthesia more common in patients with Asperger syndrome? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 847. ( 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00847) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Ward J, Hoadley C, Hughes JEA, Smith P, Allison C, Baron-Cohen S, Simner J. 2017. Atypical sensory sensitivity as a shared feature between synaesthesia and autism. Scient. Rep. 7, 41155. ( 10.1038/srep41155) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.van Leeuwen TM, van Petersen E, Burghoorn F, Dingemanse M, van Lier R. 2019. Autistic traits in synaesthesia: atypical sensory sensitivity and enhanced perception of details. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 374, 20190024. ( 10.1098/rstb.2019.0024) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.van Leeuwen TM, Neufeld J, Hughes J, Ward J. 2020. Synaesthesia and autism: different developmental outcomes from overlapping mechanisms? Cogn. Neuropsychol. 37, 433-449. ( 10.1080/02643294.2020.1808455) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.van Leeuwen TM, Wilsson L, Norrman HN, Dingemanse M, Bölte S, Neufeld J. 2021. Perceptual processing links autism and synesthesia: a co-twin control study. Cortex 145, 236-249. ( 10.1016/j.cortex.2021.09.016) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Nunn JA, et al. 2002. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of synesthesia: activation of V4/V8 by spoken words. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 371-375. ( 10.1038/nn818) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Rouw R, Scholte HS. 2007. Increased structural connectivity in grapheme-color synesthesia. Nat. Neurosci 10, 792-797. ( 10.1038/nn1906) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Terhune DB, Tai S, Cowey A, Popescu T, Cohen Kadosh R. 2011. Enhanced cortical excitability in grapheme-color synesthesia and its modulation. Curr. Biol. 21, 2006-2009. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2011.10.032) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Sinke C, Neufeld J, Emrich HM, Dillo W, Bleich S, Zedler M, Szycik GR. 2012. Inside a synesthete's head: a functional connectivity analysis with grapheme-color synesthetes. Neuropsychologia 50, 3363-3369. ( 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.09.015) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Dovern A, Fink GR, Fromme ACB, Wohlschläger AM, Weiss PH, Riedl V. 2012. Intrinsic network connectivity reflects consistency of synesthetic experiences. J. Neurosci. 32, 7614-7621. ( 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5401-11.2012) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Hupé J-M, Dojat M. 2015. A critical review of the neuroimaging literature on synesthesia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9, 103. ( 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00103) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Melero H, Peña-Melián Á, Ríos-Lago M, Pajares G, Hernández-Tamames JA, Álvarez-Linera J. 2013. Grapheme-color synesthetes show peculiarities in their emotional brain: cortical and subcortical evidence from VBM analysis of 3D-T1 and DTI data. Exp. Brain Res. 227, 343-353. ( 10.1007/s00221-013-3514-4) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Ward J, Banissy MJ. 2015. Explaining mirror-touch synesthesia. Cogn. Neurosci. 6, 118-133. ( 10.1080/17588928.2015.1042444) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Banissy MJ, Kadosh RC, Maus GW, Walsh V, Ward J. 2009. Prevalence, characteristics and a neurocognitive model of mirror-touch synaesthesia. Exp. Brain Res. 198, 261-272. ( 10.1007/s00221-009-1810-9) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Rothen N, Meier B. 2013. Why vicarious experience is not an instance of synesthesia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 128. ( 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00128) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Blakemore S-J, Tavassoli T, Calò S, Thomas RM, Catmur C, Frith U, Haggard P. 2006. Tactile sensitivity in Asperger syndrome. Brain Cogn. 61, 5-13. ( 10.1016/j.bandc.2005.12.013) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Bolognini N, Miniussi C, Gallo S, Vallar G. 2013. Induction of mirror-touch synaesthesia by increasing somatosensory cortical excitability. Curr. Biol. 23, R436-R437. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2013.03.036) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Bolognini N, Rossetti A, Fusaro M, Vallar G, Miniussi C. 2014. Sharing social touch in the primary somatosensory cortex. Curr. Biol. 24, 1513-1517. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2014.05.025) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Holley SI, Fryer AA, Haycock JW, Grubb SEW, Strange RC, Hoban PR. 2007. Differential effects of glutathione S-transferase pi (GSTP1) haplotypes on cell proliferation and apoptosis. Carcinogenesis 28, 2268-2273. ( 10.1093/carcin/bgm135) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Baron-Cohen S, Robson E, Lai M-C, Allison C. 2016. Mirror-touch synaesthesia is not associated with heightened empathy, and can occur with autism. PLoS ONE 11, e0160543. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0160543) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Farooq O, Fine EJ. 2017. Alice in Wonderland syndrome: a historical and medical review. Pediatr. Neurol. 77, 5-11. ( 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2017.08.008) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Blom JD, Nanuashvili N, Waters F. 2021. Time distortions: a systematic review of cases characteristic of Alice in Wonderland syndrome. Front. Psychiatry 12, 668633. ( 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.668633) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Blom JD. 2021. Charles Dodgson and Alice in Wonderland syndrome. Lancet Neurol. 20, 890-891. ( 10.1016/S1474-4422(21)00338-0) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Smith RA, Wright B, Bennett S. 2015. Hallucinations and illusions in migraine in children and the Alice in Wonderland syndrome. Arch. Dis. Childh. 100, 296-298. ( 10.1136/archdischild-2013-305283) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Golden GS. 1979. The Alice in Wonderland Syndrome in juvenile migraine. Pediatrics 63, 517-519. ( 10.1542/peds.63.4.517) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Beh SC, Masrour S, Smith SV, Friedman DI. 2018. Clinical characteristics of Alice in Wonderland syndrome in a cohort with vestibular migraine. Neurol. Clin. Pract. 8, 389-396. ( 10.1212/CPJ.0000000000000518) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Zwijnenburg PJG, Wennink JMB, Laman DM, Linssen WHJP. 2002. Alice in Wonderland syndrome: a clinical presentation of frontal lobe epilepsy. Neuropediatrics 33, 53-55. ( 10.1055/s-2002-23599) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Copperman SM. 1977. ‘Alice in Wonderland’ syndrome as a presenting symptom of infectious mononucleosis in children. Clin. Pediatr. 16, 143-146. ( 10.1177/000992287701600205) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Eshel GM, Eyov A, Lahat E, Brauman A. 1987. Alice in Wonderland syndrome, a manifestation of acute Epstein-Barr virus infection. Pediatr. Infect. Dis. J. 6, 68. ( 10.1097/00006454-198701000-00018) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Liaw S-B, Shen E-Y. 1991. Alice in Wonderland syndrome as a presenting symptom of EBV infection. Pediatr. Neurol 7, 464-466. ( 10.1016/0887-8994(91)90032-G) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Cinbis M, Aysun S. 1992. Alice in Wonderland syndrome as an initial manifestation of Epstein-Barr virus infection. Br. J. Ophthalmol. 76, 316. ( 10.1136/bjo.76.5.316) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Wang S-M, Liu C-C, Chen Y-J, Chang Y-C, Huang C-C. 1996. Alice in Wonderland syndrome caused by coxsackievirus B1. Pediatr. Infect. Dis. J. 15, 470-471. ( 10.1097/00006454-199605000-00022) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Soriani S, Faggioli R, Scarpa P, Borgna-Pignatti C. 1998. Alice in Wonderland’ syndrome and varicella. Pediatr. Infect. Dis. J. 17, 935-936. ( 10.1097/00006454-199810000-00026) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Augarten A, Aderka D. 2011. Alice in Wonderland syndrome in H1N1 influenza: case report. Pediatr. Emerg. Care 27, 120. ( 10.1097/PEC.0b013e318209af7a) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Paniz-Mondolfi AE, et al. 2018. Alice in Wonderland syndrome: a novel neurological presentation of Zika virus infection. J. Neurovirol 24, 660-663. ( 10.1007/s13365-018-0645-1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Naarden T, Ter Meulen BC, Van der Weele SI, Blom JD. 2019. Alice in Wonderland syndrome as a presenting manifestation of Creutzfeldt-Jakob Disease. Front. Neurol. 10, 473. ( 10.3389/fneur.2019.00473) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Vasudevan G, Farooq O, Fine E. 2021. Alice in Wonderland syndrome with pediatric concussion and synesthesia: a unique occurrence. Neurology 96, 4223. ( 10.1212/WNL.96.15_supplement.4223) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Piervincenzi C, Petsas N, Giannì C, DiPiero V, Pantano P. 2022. Alice in Wonderland syndrome: a lesion mapping study. Neurol. Sci. 43, 3321-3332. ( 10.1007/s10072-021-05792-0) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Craig AD. 2009. Emotional moments across time: a possible neural basis for time perception in the anterior insula. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 364, 1933-1942. ( 10.1098/rstb.2009.0008) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Piervincenzi C, Petsas N, Viganò A, Mancini V, Mastria G, Puma M, Giannì C, DiPiero V, Pantano P. 2023. Functional connectivity alterations in migraineurs with Alice in Wonderland syndrome. Neurol. Sci 44, 305-317. ( 10.1007/s10072-022-06404-1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Wu MS, Lewin AB, Murphy TK, Storch EA. 2014. Misophonia: incidence, phenomenology, and clinical correlates in an undergraduate student sample. J. Clin. Psychol. 70, 994-1007. ( 10.1002/jclp.22098) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Zhou X, Wu MS, Storch EA. 2017. Misophonia symptoms among Chinese university students: incidence, associated impairment, and clinical correlates. J. Obsess. Compuls. Relat. Disord. 14, 7-12. ( 10.1016/j.jocrd.2017.05.001) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Naylor J, Caimino C, Scutt P, Hoare DJ, Baguley DM. 2021. The prevalence and severity of misophonia in a UK undergraduate medical student population and validation of the Amsterdam Misophonia Scale. Psychiatr. Q. 92, 609-619. ( 10.1007/s11126-020-09825-3) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Aryal S, Prabhu P. 2022. Misophonia: prevalence, impact and co-morbidity among Mysore University students in India - a survey. Neurosci. Res. Notes 5, 161. ( 10.31117/neuroscirn.v5i4.161) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Brennan CR, Lindberg RR, Kim G, Castro AA, Khan RA, Berenbaum H, Husain FT. 2023. Misophonia and hearing comorbidities in a collegiate population. Ear Hear. 45, 390-399. ( 10.1097/AUD.0000000000001435) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Schröder A, Vulink N, Denys D. 2013. Misophonia: diagnostic criteria for a new psychiatric disorder. PLoS ONE 8, e54706. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0054706) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Dozier TH, Lopez M, Pearson C. 2017. Proposed diagnostic criteria for misophonia: a multisensory conditioned aversive reflex disorder. Front. Psychol. 8, 1975. ( 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01975) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.American Psychiatric Association. 2013. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders 5th edn. Washington, DC: APA. [Google Scholar]
- 113.Edelstein M, Brang D, Rouw R, Ramachandran VS. 2013. Misophonia: physiological investigations and case descriptions. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 296. ( 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00296) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Kumar S, et al. 2017. The brain basis for misophonia. Curr. Biol. 27, 527-533. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2016.12.048) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Grossini E, et al. 2022. Misophonia: analysis of the neuroanatomic patterns at the basis of psychiatric symptoms and changes of the orthosympathetic/parasympathetic balance. Front. Neurosci. 16, 827998. ( 10.3389/fnins.2022.827998) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Siepsiak M, Vrana SR, Rynkiewicz A, Rosenthal MZ, Dragan WŁ. 2023. Does context matter in misophonia? A multi-method experimental investigation. Front. Neurosci. 16, 880853. ( 10.3389/fnins.2022.880853) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Schröder A, van Wingen G, Eijsker N, San Giorgi R, Vulink NC, Turbyne C, Denys D. 2019. Misophonia is associated with altered brain activity in the auditory cortex and salience network. Scient. Rep. 9, 7542. ( 10.1038/s41598-019-44084-8) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Kumar S, Dheerendra P, Erfanian M, Benzaquén E, Sedley W, Gander PE, Lad M, Bamiou DE, Griffiths TD. 2021. The motor basis for misophonia. J. Neurosci. 41, 5762-5770. ( 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0261-21.2021) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Hansen HA, Stefancin P, Leber AB, Saygin ZM. 2022. Neural evidence for non-orofacial triggers in mild misophonia. Front. Neurosci. 16, 880759. ( 10.3389/fnins.2022.880759) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Erfanian M, Kartsonaki C, Keshavarz A. 2019. Misophonia and comorbid psychiatric symptoms: a preliminary study of clinical findings. Nord. J. Psychiatry 73, 219-228. ( 10.1080/08039488.2019.1609086) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Jager I, de Koning P, Bost T, Denys D, Vulink N. 2020. Misophonia: phenomenology, comorbidity and demographics in a large sample. PLoS ONE 15, e0231390. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0231390) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Andermane N, Bauer M, Simner J, Ward J. 2023. A symptom network model of misophonia: from heightened sensory sensitivity to clinical comorbidity. J. Clin. Psychol. 79, 2364-2387. ( 10.1002/jclp.23552) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Rinaldi LJ, Simner J, Koursarou S, Ward J. 2023. Autistic traits, emotion regulation, and sensory sensitivities in children and adults with misophonia. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 53, 1162-1174. ( 10.1007/s10803-022-05623-x) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Niven EC, Scott SK. 2021. Careful whispers: when sounds feel like a touch. Trends Cogn. Sci. 25, 645-647. ( 10.1016/j.tics.2021.05.006) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Wu C, Stefanescu RA, Martel DT, Shore SE. 2015. Listening to another sense: somatosensory integration in the auditory system. Cell Tissue Res. 361, 233-250. ( 10.1007/s00441-014-2074-7) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Ghaziri J, et al. 2017. The corticocortical structural connectivity of the human insula. Cereb. Cortex 27, 1216-1228. ( 10.1093/cercor/bhv308) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Uddin LQ, Nomi JS, Hébert-Seropian B, Ghaziri J, Boucher O. 2017. Structure and function of the human insula. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 34, 300-306. ( 10.1097/WNP.0000000000000377) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Ghaziri J, et al. 2018. Subcortical structural connectivity of insular subregions. Scient. Rep. 8, 8596. ( 10.1038/s41598-018-26995-0) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Zhang Y, Zhou W, Wang S, Zhou Q, Wang H, Zhang B, Huang J, Hong B, Wang X. 2019. The roles of subdivisions of human insula in emotion perception and auditory processing. Cereb. Cortex 29, 517-528. ( 10.1093/cercor/bhx334) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Singer T, Seymour B, O'Doherty J, Kaube H, Dolan RJ, Frith CD. 2004. Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science 303, 1157-1162. ( 10.1126/science.1093535) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Masten CL, Morelli SA, Eisenberger NI. 2011. An fMRI investigation of empathy for ‘social pain’ and subsequent prosocial behavior. Neuroimage 55, 381-388. ( 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.060) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
All data gathered for this review may be available upon request to the corresponding author.