Abstract
Developmental exposures to PCBs are implicated in the etiology of neurodevelopmental disorders (NDDs). This observation is concerning given the continued presence of PCBs in the human environment and the increasing incidence of NDDs. Previous studies reported that developmental exposure to legacy commercial PCB mixtures (Aroclors) or single PCB congeners found in Aroclors caused NDD-relevant behavioral phenotypes in animal models. However, the PCB congener profile in contemporary human samples is dissimilar to that of the legacy Aroclors, raising the question of whether human-relevant PCB mixtures similarly interfere with normal brain development. To address this question, we assessed the developmental neurotoxicity of the Fox River Mixture (FRM), which was designed to mimic the congener profile identified in fish from the PCB-contaminated Fox River that constitute a primary protein source in the diet of surrounding communities. Adult female C57BL/6 J mouse dams (8–10 weeks old) were exposed to vehicle (peanut oil) or FRM at 0.1, 1.0, or 6.0 mg/kg/d in their diet throughout gestation and lactation, and neurodevelopmental outcomes were assessed in their pups. Ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs) and measures of general development were quantified at postnatal day (P) 7, while performance in the spontaneous alternation task and the 3-chambered social approach/social novelty task was assessed on P35. Triiodothyronine (T3) and thyroxine (T4) were quantified in serum collected from the dams when pups were weaned and from pups on P28 and P35. Developmental exposure to FRM did not alter pup weight or body temperature on P7, but USVs were significantly decreased in litters exposed to FRM at 0.1 or 6.0 mg/kg/d in the maternal diet. FRM also impaired male and female pups’ performance in the social novelty task. Compared to sex-matched vehicles, significantly decreased social novelty was observed in male and female pups in the 0.1 and 6.0 mg/kg/d dose groups. FRM did not alter performance in the spontaneous alternation or social approach tasks. FRM increased serum T3 levels but decreased serum T4 levels in P28 male pups in the 1.0 and 6.0 mg/kg/d dose groups. In P35 female pups and dams, serum T3 levels decreased in the 6.0 mg/kg/d dose group while T4 levels were not altered. Collectively, these findings suggest that FRM interferes with the development of social communication and social novelty, but not memory, supporting the hypothesis that contemporary PCB exposures pose a risk to the developing brain. FRM had sex, age, and dose-dependent effects on serum thyroid hormone levels that overlapped but did not perfectly align with the FRM effects on behavioral outcomes. These observations suggest that changes in thyroid hormone levels are not likely the major factor underlying the behavioral deficits observed in FRM-exposed animals.
Keywords: Behavioral assessment, Communication, Developmental neurotoxicity, Polychlorinated biphenyls, Social behavior, Thyroid hormones
1. Introduction
Polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs) are pervasive environmental pollutants that have been the focus of neurotoxicological studies for decades. This research has identified the developing brain as a particularly vulnerable target of PCBs (Pessah et al., 2019). Epidemiological studies have linked early-life PCB exposures to adverse neurodevelopmental outcomes across domains related to cognition (e.g., IQ, language, memory, learning), and attention and executive function (Schantz et al., 2003; Sprowles et al., 2022; Boucher et al., 2009; Berghuis et al., 2018). These associations have been corroborated in experimental animal models (Schantz et al., 1996, 1995; Johansen et al., 2011). More recently, emerging epidemiological evidence has raised concerns about the potential for PCBs to increase the risk of neurodevelopmental disorders (NDDs), particularly attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and autism spectrum disorders (ASD) (Mendola et al., 2002; Bullert et al., 2021; Xu et al., 2023; Mehri et al., 2021). The few experimental animal studies that have examined the effects of developmental PCB exposures on NDD-relevant behavioral domains support these concerns (Johansen et al., 2014; Sethi et al., 2021; Jolous-Jamshidi et al., 2010).
With a few exceptions (Sethi et al., 2021; Granillo et al., 2019), most of the published human and animal literature describing PCB effects on NDD-relevant behaviors have focused on the Aroclors and other legacy commercial PCB mixtures or individual PCB congeners found in them (Hens and Hens, 2017; Klocke et al., 2020). However, the PCB congener profile in contemporary human samples is increasingly dissimilar from that of the legacy Aroclors (Li et al., 2022; Marek et al., 2014; Koh et al., 2016). This raises critical questions of whether the developmental neurotoxicity of the legacy commercial mixtures or single PCB congeners predict neurotoxic outcomes following exposure of the developing brain to PCB mixtures more representative of contemporary human exposures. This concern is heightened by the observation that the Fox River mixture (FRM), an experimental mixture based on the PCB congener profile found in fish from the Fox River, is more potent than Aroclor 1254 with respect to sensitization of the ryanodine receptor (Kostyniak et al., 2005), a molecular initiating event in PCB developmental neurotoxicity (Bal-Price et al., 2015).
The Fox River is a major tributary of Lake Michigan that flows across central and eastern Wisconsin and provides habitat for large populations of game fish that comprise the primary protein source for several communities in surrounding areas (Christensen et al., 2016; Steenport et al., 2000). Between 1954 and 1971, numerous factories and industries that discharged effluent into the lower Fox River used PCBs to manufacture carbonless copy paper (Imamoglu and Christensen, 2002). Despite attempts to decrease PCB contamination in the Fox River via biological degradation or dechlorination of PCBs in water and sediment (Imamoglu et al., 2004; Kaya et al., 2018), high concentrations of PCBs persist in the water column of the Fox River (Guo et al., 2017). Consumption of fish from the Fox River has been positively correlated with elevated serum levels of PCBs (Schantz et al., 2010) and impaired cognitive functioning (Sprowles et al., 2022) in adolescent humans. Comparable deficits in cognitive behavior have been documented in adult rodents exposed developmentally to FRM (Sable et al., 2006). However, to date, the effects of FRM on NDD-relevant behaviors in early life have not been explored.
To address this data gap, we assessed NDD-relevant behaviors in weanling mice exposed to FRM in the maternal diet throughout gestation and lactation. Since many PCBs have been shown to reduce circulating levels of thyroid hormone (Little et al., 2022), and reduced thyroid hormone levels can negatively impact brain development (O’Shaughnessy et al., 2018; Bernal, 2000), we also quantified the effect of developmental exposure to FRM on circulating levels of triiodothyronine (T3) and thyroxine (T4) in dams and postnatal pups.
2. Materials and methods
The ARRIVE guidelines were followed in reporting experimental materials and methods.
2.1. Materials
The Fox River PCB Mixture (FRM) is a synthetic PCB mixture containing 35 % Aroclor 1242, 35 % Aroclor 1248, 15 % Aroclor 1254, and 15 % Aroclor 1260 by weight (Kostyniak et al., 2005). All Aroclors for this study, including Aroclor 1242 (catalog #: C-242 N, lot #: 0114-A), Aroclor 1248 (catalog #: C-248 N, lot #: 1063–24B), Aroclor 1254 (catalog #: C-254 N, lot #: 124–191-B) and Aroclor 1260 (catalog #: C-260 N, lot #: 021–020–1 A) were purchased from AccuStandard, Inc. (New Haven, CT, USA). The FRM was prepared by adding 525 mg of Arclor1242, 525 mg of Aroclor 1248, and 225 mg of Aroclor 1254 to a vial containing 225 mg of Aroclor 1260. The mixture was kept at 30 °C for 5 min and homogenized by shaking. Three individual aliquots of the FRM used in this study were authenticated by gas chromatograph-tandem mass spectrometry as described (Li et al., 2022). The FRM was highly similar to the original FRM (Kostyniak et al., 2005), with a similarity coefficient cos Θ of 0.986 and a relative percent difference (RPD) of 22 % (Fig. 1). The FRM used in the studies reported here was also similar to the FRM used in our previous study (Li et al., 2022), with a cos Θ of 0.979 and a RPD of 40 %. A dataset reporting the authentication of the FRM and the comparison with earlier FRM preparations is available on Iowa Research Online (Li et al., 2023).
Fig. 1.
Comparison of PCB congener profiles of the (A) original FRM (Kostyniak et al., 2005) and (B) the FRM prepared for this study. The original FRM was analyzed by gas chromatography-electron capture detection, whereas the FRM for this project was analyzed by gas chromatograph-tandem mass spectrometry. The PCB profiles were collapsed to give the same list of single or co-eluting PCB congeners and allow a comparison of the PCB profiles. Only PCB congeners with a mass percentage of > 1 % are shown.
The FRM was dissolved in peanut oil at a stock concentration of 20 mg/ml and stored in amber glass vials at −20 °C. A 12.5 μL or 125 μL aliquot of 20 mg/ml peanut oil/FRM stock was mixed into 10 g of peanut butter with a metal spatula for 5 min daily before use. Organic peanut butter (Trader Joe’s, Monrovia, CA) and organic peanut oil (Spectrum Organic Products, LLC, Melville, NY) were purchased from Trader Joe’s (Seattle, WA).
2.2. Animals
All procedures involving animals were conducted in accordance with the NIH Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and were approved by the University of Washington, Seattle Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.
C57Bl/6 J WT mice were purchased from Jackson Labs (Sacramento, CA, United States). All animals were housed in clear plastic shoebox cages containing corn cob bedding and maintained on a 12 h light and dark cycle at 22 ± 2°C with 40–50 % humidity. Feed (Diet 5010 before mating, Diet 5021 during and following mating, LabDiet, Saint Louis, MO, United States) and water were available ad libitum.
2.3. Exposure paradigm
A schematic of the experimental paradigm is shown in Fig. 2. Adult female nulliparous C57BL/6J mice (8–10 weeks old) were randomly divided into one of four experimental groups using a random number generator (GraphPad Prism 8 software- San Diego, CA, RRID: SCR_002798): vehicle (peanut oil in peanut butter); FRM at 0.1 mg/kg/d; FRM at 1 mg/kg/d; or FRM at 6.0 mg/kg/d. All animals were weighed daily before feeding and the amount of peanut butter was adjusted accordingly to account for weight changes throughout pregnancy and lactation. Two weeks prior to mating, dams were housed two per cage, and PCB dosing was initiated. Individual animals were placed in a clean cage containing a sterile weigh boat holding the FRM-peanut butter mixture. Animals were monitored to ensure the dose was fully consumed before they were placed back in the home cage. An age-matched male was placed in the cage for 7 d during mating. During this period, females were checked for the presence of a copulatory plug, which was considered gestational day 0. After mating, dams were housed singly and with their pups after parturition. At postnatal day 2 (P2), pups were culled or cross-fostered within dose groups to ensure all litters consisted of 6–8 pups. One male and one female pup from each litter were euthanized on P4 to collect brains for caspase-3 analyses (these data will be reported in a companion paper). After weaning at P28, pups were group-housed with same-sex littermates. One male and one female pup from each litter and their respective dams were euthanized on P28; on P35, one male and one female pup from each litter were euthanized to collect brain tissues for immunohistological analyses of neuroinflammation and neurogenesis (these data will be reported in a companion paper) and serum for thyroid hormone measurement. The pups euthanized on P35 were tested in several behavioral paradigms before euthanasia (Fig. 2B).
Fig. 2.
Schematic of experimental design. (A) Dams were exposed to the Fox River PCB Mixture (FRM) throughout gestation and lactation via ingestion of peanut butter containing the FRM at varying doses. (B) Dams were exposed to FRM starting 2 weeks before mating and exposure continued through gestation (G) and lactation until pups were weaned on postnatal day (P) 28. Ultrasonic vocalizations were assessed at P7, and the spontaneous alternation task and social approach/novelty (SA/SN) task were both conducted at P35. Serum was collected from dams on P28 and from pups on P28 and P35 for thyroid hormone measurements.
2.4. Behavioral Tests
Ultrasonic vocalizations were recorded using an Avisoft UltraSoundGate microphone and Avisoft Recorder USGH software (version 4.2, Avisoft Bioacoustics, Glienicke, Germany). Spectrograms were analyzed using Avisoft SASLab Pro software (version 5.2, Avisoft Bioacoustics) as previously described (Brielmaier et al., 2012; Berg et al., 2018). Since ultrasonic vocalizations can vary within a litter (Rieger and Dougherty, 2016), all pups in the litter (n=6–8 pups per litter) were analyzed. Briefly, pups at P7 were removed from their home cage, and placed singly in a separate container with corn cob bedding within a sound-attenuating chamber equipped with an Avisoft UltraSoundGate microphone. Ultrasonic vocalizations were recorded for a total of 3 min. After each recording period, body mass and temperature were recorded, after which pups were returned to their home cage. The temperature of the room was maintained at 22 ± 2°C. An experienced individual blinded to exposure group manually quantified ultrasonic calls. The total number of calls per pup over the 3 min recording period was averaged across all pups within the litter. Each litter was assigned a single total number of ultrasonic vocalizations (e.g., the average USV value of each litter was the statistical unit of measure). One-way ANOVA was used to detect differences in calls with FRM dose as the variable (p≤ 0.05). Multiple comparisons were assessed using the Tukey post-hoc test (p≤ 0.05).
Spatial memory was assessed at P35 using the spontaneous alternation task (y-maze) performed as previously described (Adhikari et al., 2022; Kraeuter et al., 2019). The Y-shaped apparatus was made of non-reflective matte white finished acrylic ¼” thick (P95 White, Tap Plastics, Sacramento, CA, USA). The 3 arms (33.5 cm length x 6 cm width x 23 cm height) were separated by 120° between each other. Subjects were placed at the end of the designated start arm facing the center of the Y, and the sequence of entries into each arm during the 8 min test period was recorded by a camera mounted above the apparatus. An investigator blinded to exposure group scored transitions between the three arms. The percentage of spontaneous alternations was calculated as the number of triads (entries into each of the three different arms of the maze in a sequence of three without returning to a previously visited arm) relative to the number of entries. Two-way ANOVA was used to detect differences in alternation with dose and sex as the variables (p≤ 0.05). Multiple comparisons were assessed using the Tukey post-hoc test (p≤ 0.05).
Sociability and social novelty were measured using the social approach and social novelty test at P35. Mice were given 3 h rest between the spontaneous alternation task and the social approach/social novelty task to reduce the potential effects of stress on the latter task. For this test, we used a three-chambered social box constructed from white plastic with removable doors as described previously (Silverman et al., 2010; Keil et al., 2019). Mice were moved into the behavioral testing room (lux level 110) in their home cage and allowed at least 30 min to acclimate prior to testing. The sociability assay consisted of one 5 min period and three 10-min periods for a total of 35 min. First, animals were placed in the center of the three-chambered arena for a 5 min acclimation period. Second, in the habituation phase, the doors were removed, and mice were allowed free exploration of the entire arena for 10 min. If an animal failed to explore during this time, they were removed from the study. Third, animals were allowed to interact for 10 min with either an empty, upside-down wire pencil cup (object) or an identical cup containing a novel age- and sex-matched “stranger mouse”. The two cups were placed on opposite sides of the arena and the middle chamber was left empty. Placement of the empty cup and the cup with the mouse was counterbalanced between the left and right sides of the apparatus to eliminate side bias. Finally, the empty cup was replaced with an identical cup containing a novel age- and sex- matched WT “stranger mouse” and the test subject was given 10 min to interact with the novel and familiar stranger mice. The placement of the novel and familiar mouse was counterbalanced between the left and right sides. Both the area and the cups were cleaned with 70 % ethanol and allowed to dry completely between each subject to eliminate scent cues. Animals used as stranger mice were habituated to sitting under the wire cup for 30 min every day for 3 days prior to the testing phase to avoid erratic behavior during the testing phase of the assay. Behavior was recorded using Noldus EthoVision XT (version 11.0), an automated tracking and analysis software (Noldus Information Technology Inc., Leesburg, VA) and analyzed by an investigator blind to exposure group. Paired t-tests were used to detect within-group differences in sniff time, number of interactions, and time in chamber (p≤ 0.05). One-way ANOVA was used to detect between-group differences in the time spent in the middle chamber (p≤ 0.05). Two-way ANOVA was used to detect between-group differences in the number of entries with dose and sex as the variables (p≤ 0.05). Multiple comparisons were assessed using the Tukey post-hoc test (p≤ 0.05).
2.5. Thyroid hormone assay
Levels of triiodothyronine (T3) and thyroxine (T4) were measured in maternal serum collected from dams (which were 4 months old at the time of collection) at the conclusion of the FRM dosing period, and in serum from P28 and P35 pups. Blood was collected during the second half of the animals’ 12-hour light cycle. Animals were anesthetized with 4 % isoflurane in medical grade oxygen and once determined to be properly anesthetized as determined by lack of response to toe pinch and tail pinch, blood was collected via cardiac stick using a 5 ml luer lock syringe (Becton Dickinson, San Jose, CA) fitted with a 30 gauge BD PrecisionGlide Needle (Becton Dickinson, San Jose, CA). Blood was transferred to a BD vacutainer blood collection tube (4 ml, Becton Dickinson, San Jose, CA) and 30 min after collection, centrifuged at 2000 x g for 10 min at 4°C. The supernatant (serum), was collected and placed into 2 ml cryogenic vials (ThermoFisher) and frozen at −80°C. T3 (Invitrogen, Carlsbad, CA, USA; Cat# EIAT3C) and T4 levels (Invitrogen; Cat# EIAT4C) were determined using enzyme-linked immunosorbent assays (ELISA) per the manufacturer’s instructions. Concentrations of T3 and T4 hormones in serum were measured at 450 nm absorbance on a spectrophotometer microplate reader (BioTek Instruments, Winooski, VT). Samples were run in duplicates, and values were normalized to blank controls within each plate. Two-way ANOVA was used to detect differences in T3 or T4 levels with dose and sex as the variables. Multiple comparisons were assessed using the Tukey post-hoc test (p≤ 0.05).
3. Results
This study was part of a larger study designed to assess the effects of developmental exposure to the FRM on multiple endpoints, including molecular and cellular indices of neurodevelopment, the gut microbiome and microbial metabolites, and intestinal pathology. Here, we report the effects of developmental FRM exposure on behavioral endpoints in pups and circulating levels of thyroid hormones in the dams and pups.
3.1. Developmental exposure to FRM in the maternal diet altered ultrasonic vocalizations
To assess whether gestational and lactational exposure to FRM caused general maternal toxicity or interfered with overall pup development, we measured the weight of dams and pups. There was no effect of FRM on the weight of the dams measured on the final day of FRM exposure when pups were P28 (Fig. 3A). Similarly, FRM had no effect on pup weight at P7 (Fig. 3B), P28 (Fig. 3C) or P35 (Fig. 3D).
Fig. 3.
Exposure to the Fox River PCB mixture (FRM) had no significant effect on maternal or pup weights. (A) Dam weight was measured on P28, which corresponded to 4 months of age. Weight of the pups at (B) P7, the day of ultrasonic vocalization testing, (C) P28, the final day of PCB exposure and (B) P35 at the conclusion of behavioral testing. Data are presented as box and whisker plots in which the box indicates the 25th (lower) to 75th (upper) quartiles; the middle horizontal bar, the median; whiskers, the minimum and maximum values; and dots, the individual values (n=16–22 per dose group for pups, which included males and females; n= 16–22 per dose group for dams). There were no signfiicant differences from vehicle at p < 0.05 as determined using one-way ANOVA and post hoc Tukey’s test.
Ultrasonic vocalizations are commonly measured to assess early social communication (Sethi et al., 2021; Scattoni et al., 2009). An important component of communication between the dam and pup, pup ultrasonic vocalizations are whistle-like sounds at frequencies between 30 and 90 kHz (Grimsley et al., 2011; Branchi et al., 2001) emitted between P2 and P12 to orient maternal responsiveness to the pup for approach and/or retrieval (Cohen-Salmon et al., 1985; D’Amato et al., 2005; Yin et al., 2016). We tested the effect of gestational and lactational exposure to the FRM on the number of ultrasonic vocalizations at P7 in individual pups isolated from the dam immediately prior to testing (Fig. 4A). Male and female pups did not exhibit significantly different responses as measured by a two-way ANOVA with sex and dose group as variables (F=1.195, p=0.2769), therefore, male and female data were pooled and the average value of the entire litter was used as the statistical unit. Exposure to FRM at 6.0 mg/kg/d significantly decreased the number of vocalizations compared to vehicle controls (n=11–19 litters per dose group; F =3.069; p= 0.0362). There was a trend towards decreased vocalizations in the 0.1 mg/kg/d dose group compared to vehicle control but this did not reach statistical significance (p=0.1). Pup weight and temperature can have a significant effect on the number of vocalizations therefore Pearson’s correlation coefficient was computed to assess the linear relationship between the number of vocalizations and temperature (Fig. 3C) as well as the number of vocalizations and pup weight at P7 (Fig. 3D). Vocalizations and temperature have a weak positive association (r=0.3292, 95 %CI 0.06–0.55, p = 0.0151) whereas vocalizations and weight have a biologically negligible association (r=0.09, 95 %CI −0.18–0.35, p=0.51). Additionally, we found no significant effects of FRM on these endpoints (Figs. 3B and 4B) (F=0.5671, p=0.6391 and F=1.088, p=0.3681, respectfully), supporting the interpretation that FRM effects on vocalizations are likely not due to FRM effects on pup body temperature or weight.
Fig. 4.
Developmental exposure to the Fox River PCB mixture (FRM) altered social communication of P7 pups. (A) Each individual pup was placed into a soundproof chamber to record ultrasonic vocalizations for 3 min; the average of all pups in one litter was used as the statistical unit of measure. Data are presented as total number of vocalizations during the 3-min trial. (B) Temperature of pups measured immediately following isolation from the dam (n=11–19 litters per dose group, each point is one litter). Data are presented as box and whisker plots in which the box indicates the 25th (lower) to 75th (upper) quartiles; the middle horizontal bar, the median; whiskers, the minimum and maximum values; and dots, the individual values (n=11–19 litters per dose group, statistical unit is litter). *Significantly different from the vehicle at p < 0.05 as determined using one-way ANOVA and post hoc Tukey’s test. Pearson’s correlation was used to determine if there were linear relationships between (C) vocalizations and temperature of pups at P7 (or (D) vocalizations and weight of pups (n=54 pairs). Data are presented as scatterplots with each dot representing the average value of an individual litter.
3.2. Developmental exposure to FRM did not alter spontaneous alternation
The spontaneous alternation task assesses cognitive function through the lens of a spatial working and reference memory task (Kraeuter et al., 2019). Spatial navigation involves multiple cognitive functions, including but not limited to attention, memory, and decision-making (Berthoz and Viaud-Delmon, 1999; Brown and Chrastil, 2019), and deficits in these behaviors are associated with many NDDs (Faedda et al., 2022; Harper et al., 2023). Gestational and lactational exposure to FRM in the maternal diet did not significantly affect pup performance on the spontaneous alternation task (Fig. 5A) (F(3125)=0.2169, p=0.8845) and no significant sex differences were identified (F(1125)=2.367, p=0.1265). Similarly, the number of entries into the arms of the maze was not significantly altered by FRM exposure when compared to vehicle control using post hoc Tukey test (Fig. 5B)(F(3109)=1.163, p=0.3274), and there were no significant sex differences (F(1109)=1.445, p=0.2319).
Fig. 5.
Developmental exposure to FRM had no significant effect on performance of P35 pups in the spontaneous alternation (y-maze) task. Pups were placed into the y-maze apparatus to explore for 8 min. (A) The percentage of SAT (spontaneous alternating triads divided by the total number of entries). (B) Total number of entries between arms of the y-maze apparatus. Data are presented as box and whisker plots in which the box indicates the 25th (lower) to 75th (upper) quartiles; the middle horizontal bar, the median; whiskers, the minimum and maximum values; and dots, the individual values (n=15–22 pups per sex per exposure group). *Significantly different from vehicle at p<0.05 as determined by two-way ANOVA with sex and dose as the variables followed by Tukey’s post hoc analysis.
3.3. Developmental FRM exposure did not significantly affect social approach behavior
Deficits in social behavior are a core symptom of ASD and a component of many other NDDs (Association A.P. American psychiatric association. 2019; Lord et al., 2000). The three-chambered social approach task (Crawley, 2012; Nadler et al., 2004) was used to measure levels of sociability in FRM-exposed pups at P35. In this test, sociability is defined as the amount of time the test subject spends with an age- and sex-matched “stranger” mouse compared to an inanimate object, in this case, an empty inverted wire pencil cup (Silverman et al., 2012). Normal mice will spend significantly more time with the stranger mice than with the inanimate object. FRM-exposed groups performed similar to vehicle controls in all aspects of the social approach task. We found that FRM-exposed pups and vehicle controls of either sex spent significantly more time sniffing or interacting with the stranger mouse than with the empty cup (Fig. 6A, B). Additionally, in both FRM-exposed pups and sex-matched vehicle controls, the number of interactions (Fig. 6C, D) and time spent in the chamber (Fig. 6E, F) with the stranger mouse was significantly higher compared to the empty cup. The time spent in the middle chamber was analyzed using a one-way ANOVA and there were no significant differences in the males (F=1.508, p=0.2230) or females (F=1.153, p=0.3372), therefore, these were not included in further analysis. The total number of entries was quantified to rule out potential differences in overall activity between dose groups during the task. There were no significant differences between FRM-exposed pups and sex-matched vehicle controls (F(3120)=0.3553, p=0.7854). (Fig. 6G). Statistical analyses are reported in Table 1.
Fig. 6.
Developmental exposure to FRM in the maternal diet had no significant effect on social approach of P35 pups. During the social approach trial, pups were placed in a 3-chambered apparatus and following a period of habituation, were given 10 min to interact with an empty cup and a cup with an age- and sex-matched “stranger” mouse placed inside. Social approach data are presented as a comparison of the time spent sniffing the stranger mouse versus the empty cup in (A) males and (B) females (n=14–20 pups per sex per exposure group). The total number of interactions were quantified in (C) males and (D) females. The time spent in the chambers corresponding with the empty cup and stranger mouse were quantified in (E) males and (F) females. (G) Total number of entries into any of the chambers. Data are presented as box and whisker plots in which the box indicates the 25th (lower) to 75th (upper) quartiles; the middle horizontal bar, the median; whiskers, the minimum and maximum values; and dots, the individual values. (A-F) *Significantly different at p < 0.05 as determined using paired t-test. (G) *Significantly different at p < 0.05 from vehicle control as determined using two-way ANOVA and post hoc Tukey’s test.
Table 1.
Statistical analysis conducted on Social Approach and Social Novelty behavioral tests. Paired t-tests were used to compare time with the stranger mouse versus the empty cup for the social approach task and the novel mouse versus familiar mouse for the social novelty task.
Task |
Sex |
Exposure Group |
Sniff Time |
# Interactions |
Chamber Time |
||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
t-value | DF | p-value | t-value | DF | p-value | t-value | DF | p-value | |||
Social Approach | Male | Vehicle | 5.59 | 14 | <0.0001 | 5.38 | 13 | 0.0001 | 5.17 | 11 | 0.0003 |
0.1 mg/kg/d | 5.51 | 13 | 0.0001 | 3.66 | 12 | 0.0033 | 4.227 | 11 | 0.0014 | ||
1.0 mg/kg/d | 7.35 | 14 | <0.0001 | 5.65 | 13 | <0.0001 | 5.24 | 13 | 0.0002 | ||
6.0 mg/kg/d | 6.51 | 22 | <0.0001 | 6.08 | 21 | <0.0001 | 4.97 | 19 | <0.0001 | ||
Female | Vehicle | 4.16 | 13 | 0.0012 | 1.19 | 13 | 0.2547 | 2.21 | 11 | 0.0492 | |
0.1 mg/kg/d | 4.34 | 13 | 0.0008 | 3.15 | 13 | 0.0076 | 6.78 | 10 | <0.0001 | ||
1.0 mg/kg/d | 4.19 | 16 | 0.0008 | 2.86 | 14 | 0.0126 | 2.95 | 14 | 0.0105 | ||
6.0 mg/kg/d | 5.89 | 20 | <0.0001 | 6.54 | 21 | <0.0001 | 5.44 | 14 | <0.0001 | ||
Social Novelty | Male | Vehicle | 4.17 | 12 | 0.0013 | 4.31 | 12 | 0.001 | 3.26 | 11 | 0.0076 |
0.1 mg/kg/d | 1.76 | 13 | 0.1023 | 2.767 | 13 | 0.016 | 1.04 | 11 | 0.3208 | ||
1.0 mg/kg/d | 3.74 | 8 | 0.0057 | 2.879 | 8 | 0.0205 | 2.35 | 9 | 0.0434 | ||
6.0 mg/kg/d | 1.26 | 14 | 0.2292 | 3.076 | 14 | 0.0082 | 2.56 | 14 | 0.0224 | ||
Female | Vehicle | 3.79 | 14 | 0.002 | 3.05 | 14 | 0.0086 | 4.25 | 11 | 0.0014 | |
0.1 mg/kg/d | 1.46 | 13 | 0.1685 | 1.71 | 13 | 0.1126 | 0.88 | 10 | 0.4011 | ||
1.0 mg/kg/d | 3.41 | 15 | 0.0039 | 4.162 | 15 | 0.0008 | 4.05 | 14 | 0.0012 | ||
6.0 mg/kg/d | 0.08 | 20 | 0.94 | 0.62 | 20 | 0.5421 | 1.12 | 18 | 0.2764 |
3.4. Developmental exposure to FRM significantly altered social novelty and memory
The social novelty test was conducted to determine whether developmental exposure to FRM in the maternal diet altered social learning and preference for social novelty (Nadler et al., 2004; Moy et al., 2004; Yang et al). In this test, preference for social novelty and social memory is defined as the test subject spending significantly more time with a novel stranger mouse than the previously introduced, familiar stranger mouse (Silverman et al., 2010). Male (Fig. 7A) and female (Fig. 7B) pups in the 0.1 and 6.0 mg/kg/d FRM dose groups showed significantly decreased preference for social novelty compared to sex-matched vehicle controls as indicated by no significant differences in the time spent sniffing the novel versus familiar stranger mouse. Measurement of the number of interactions with the novel versus familiar stranger mouse revealed significant differences between dose groups amongst female, but not male, pups (Figs. 7C and 7D). Specifically, female pups in the 0.1 and 6.0 mg/kg/d dose group showed a significantly decreased preference for social novelty (Fig. 7D). Relative to sex-matched controls, the amount of time spent in the chamber with the novel versus familiar stranger mouse was not significantly different in male pups exposed to FRM at 0.1 mg/kg/d in the maternal diet (Fig. 7E) or in female pups exposed to FRM at 0.1 or 6.0 mg/kg/d in the maternal diet (Fig. 7F). The time spent in the middle chamber was analyzed using a one-way ANOVA and there were no significant differences in the males (F=2.401, p=0.0808) or females (F=0.7021, p=0.555), therefore, these were not included in further analysis. There were no significant differences in the number of total entries made by FRM-exposed pups of either sex compared to sex-matched vehicle controls (Fig. 7G)(F(3106)=1.274, p=0.2872). Statistical analyses are reported in Table 1.
Fig. 7.
Dose- and sex-dependent effects of developmental exposure to FRM on social novelty trial conducted at P35. The social novelty trial was conducted immediately following the conclusion of the social approach trial using the same 3-chambered apparatus. Animals were given 10 min to interact with the same stranger mouse used in the social approach trial and a cup with a novel stranger mouse. (A, B) The amount of time spent with the novel mouse compared to the familiar stranger mouse in males (A) and females (B). (C, D) The total number of interactions during the social novelty trial in males (C) and females (D). (E, F) The time spent in the chambers corresponding with the familiar stranger mouse and novel stranger mouse in males (E) and females (F). (G) Total number of entries into all chambers in males and females. Data are presented as box and whisker plots in which the box indicates the 25th (lower) to 75th (upper) quartiles; the middle horizontal bar, the median; whiskers, the minimum and maximum values; and dots, the individual values (n=14–21 pups per sex per dose group). (A-F) *Significantly different at p < 0.05 as determined using paired t-test. (G) *Significantly different at p < 0.05 from vehicle control as determined using two-way ANOVA and post hoc Tukey’s test.
3.5. Dose, sex, and age-dependent effects of developmental FRM exposure on circulating levels of thyroid hormones
In P28 males, serum T3 levels (Fig. 8A) were significantly increased while serum T4 levels (Fig. 8B) were significantly decreased in the 1.0 and 6.0 mg/kg/d dose groups (F=2.796, p=0.0432, F=2.825, p=0.0412). In contrast, there were no significant effects of FRM on T3 or T4 levels in P28 females (Fig. 8A, B). At P35, FRM had no significant effect on serum T3 levels in males, but significantly decreased serum T3 levels in females in the 6.0 mg/kg/d dose groups (Fig. 8C) (F=6.401, p=0.0012). The FRM did not significantly alter serum T4 levels at P35 in either males or females (Fig. 8D) (F=0.4866, p=0.6935). Measurement of thyroid hormone levels in serum collected from dams at weaning (when pups were P28), revealed a significant decrease in T3 levels in the 6.0 mg/kg/d dose group (Fig. 8E) (F=3.686, p=0.0314), but no significant effects of FRM on T4 levels (Fig. 8F) (F=1.959, p=0.1584).
Fig. 8.
Dose-, sex- and age-dependent effects of developmental exposure to FRM on serum thyroid hormone levels. Serum was collected from pups upon euthanasia at P28 (A, B), P35 (C, D) and from dams when pups were weaned at P28 (E, F). All serum samples were analyzed for levels of triiodothyronine (T3) (A, C, E) and thyroxine (T4) (G, B, F) at P35 was analyzed for (C) T3 and (D) T4 hormones. Dam serum was collected at the P28 tissue collection timepoint and was analyzed for (E) T3 and (F) T4 (n=4–7 per dose group). Data are presented as box and whisker plots in which the box indicates the 25th (lower) to 75th (upper) quartiles; the middle horizontal bar, the median; whiskers, the minimum and maximum values; and dots, the individual values (n=5–8 pups per sex per dose group; n = 4–7 dams per dose group). *Significantly different from the vehicle at p < 0.05; pup data were analyzed using two-way ANOVA with sex and dose as variables, and post hoc Tukey’s test; dam data were analyzed using one-way ANOVA and post hoc Tukey’s test.
Pearson correlation was used to assess whether there was a linear relationship between the serum T3 and T4 levels of P35 pups and the behavioral endpoints measured in this study. Ultrasonic vocalizations were not correlated with T3 levels (Fig. 9A) (r=0.1658, 95 %CI −0.1536–0.4539, p=0.3066) or T4 levels (Fig. 9B) (r=0.2579, 95 % CI −0.0582–0.5271, p=0.1081). There were also no significant correlations between T3 levels and spontaneous alternation performance (Fig. 9C) (r=0.0763, 95 % CI −0.2410–0.3788, p=0.64), social approach behavior (Fig. 9E) (r=−0.1959, 95 % CI −0.4783–0.1231, p=0.2256), or social novelty behavior (Fig. 9G) (r=0.1669, 95 % CI −0.1710–0.4697, p=0.3307). Similarly, T4 levels also did not correlate with the spontaneous alternation (Fig. 9D) (r=−0.1187, 95 % CI −0.4148–0.2003, p=0.4658), social approach (Fig. 9F) (r=0.0549, 95 % CI −0.2611–0.3602, p=0.7365), or social novelty behavioral tasks (Fig. 9H) (r=0.05191, 95 % CI −0.2814–0.3741, p=0.7637).
Fig. 9.
FRM effects on thyroid hormone are not correlated with FRM effects on behavior. Pearson correlation analyses were used to assess linear relationships between serum triiodothyronine (T3) levels (A, C, E, G) or serum thyroxine (T4) levels (B, D, F, H) at P35 with ultrasonic vocalizations (A, B) measured as the number of calls during the 3 minute trial, spontaneous alternation (C, D) presented as the percent spontaneous alternation (spontaneous alternating triads divided by the total number of entries), social approach (E, F) presented as percent of total sniff time spent with the stranger mouse, or social novelty (G, H) presented as percent of total sniff time spent with the novel mouse. Data are presented as scatterplots (n=36–40 pairs).
4. Discussion
It has previously been reported that consumption of PCB-contaminated fish from the Fox River is associated with cognitive deficits in adolescent humans (Sprowles et al., 2022) and in adult mice exposed developmentally to FRM (Sable et al., 2006). Our findings from this studies extend these observations by demonstrating effects of developmental exposure to the FRM, a human-relevant PCB mixture, altered early communication and social novelty tasks in weanling mice. Additionally, we found that exposure to the FRM altered serum thyroid hormone levels in pups and dams, but these changes were inconsistent and exhibited dose-response relationships that varied from those of the FRM-associated behavioral effects. These observations provide further support for the hypothesis that PCBs are putative risk factors for NDDs, and in particular, ADHD and ASD (Keil-Stietz and Lein, 2023; Panesar et al., 2020).
Social behaviors like communication, sociability, and social memory are critical cognitive abilities that are impaired in humans diagnosed with NDDs (Shultz et al., 2018; Khalil et al., 2018; Harpin et al., 2016) and in rodent models of NDDs (Moy et al., 2004; Ricceri et al., 2007; Kight et al., 2021; Silverman and Ellegood, 2018). Our data demonstrate that exposure to the FRM in the maternal diet during gestation and lactation caused deficits in human-relevant social behaviors in weanling mice. Communication is a complex social behavior mediated by multiple brain regions, including the medial prefrontal cortex, amygdala, and anterior cingulate cortex (Lovinger, 2008; Gan-Or and London, 2023; Frith, 2007). In early life, communication aids in maternal and paternal care; in later life, communication is key to developing and maintaining social relationships (Portfors, 2007; Motomura et al., 2002). To assess early-life communication, we measured the ultrasonic vocalizations of P7 pups following maternal isolation and found that developmental exposure to FRM caused non-monotonic dose-dependent deficits. The number of USVs can be negatively impacted by decreased pup weight or temperature, which are reflective of general development and maternal care, respectively (Hahn and Lavooy, 2005). These endpoints did not differ between exposure groups, indicating that the decreased USVs observed in FRM-exposed pups are likely due to the direct effects of PCBs on neural circuits that regulate this form of communication. These findings are consistent with previous reports that ultrasonic vocalizations were significantly decreased in P7 mice developmentally exposed to the MARBLES PCB mixture (Sethi et al., 2021), which replicates the congener profile of the 12 most abundant PCBs detected in the serum of Northern California women at increased risk for having a child diagnosed with an NDD (Sethi et al., 2019). Ultrasonic vocalizations were also decreased in P10 rat pups following developmental exposure to a mixture of PCB 47 and PCB 77 at 25 ppm, but not 12.5 ppm (Krishnan et al., 2014).
Sociability and social memory are two other important measures of social behavior that are often assessed in neurotoxicological studies (Grandjean and Landrigan, 2014; Miodovnik et al., 2011). Using the three-chambered social approach task, we found no effects of developmental exposure to FRM on general sociability in the mice at P35 (Silverman et al., 2010). This observation is consistent with a previous study of Sprague Dawley rats that reported exposure to Aroclor 1221 in the maternal diet during gestation and lactation did not alter social approach in adult animals (Reilly et al., 2015). In contrast, in mice exposed to the MARBLES mix in the maternal diet during gestation and lactation, sociability was decreased in male, but not female, weanling mice exposed to this PCB mixture at 0.1, but not 1.0 or 6.0 mg/kg/d (Sethi et al., 2021). These observations suggest that PCB mixtures with differing congener profiles may differentially alter sociability.
In contrast to social approach, the social novelty task measures preference for social novelty as a type of working social memory. We found that developmental exposure to FRM significantly decreased social novelty in male and female pups in the 0.1 and 6.0 mg/kg/d dose groups. When the social novelty data were analyzed as the number of interactions or the time spent in each of the chambers, males had no significant group differences, but amongst females, these measures were significantly decreased in the same dose groups that showed decreased social novelty. These findings suggest that developmental exposure to FRM had stronger effects on social novelty in females than males. Importantly, we observed no effects of FRM exposure on the number of entries and number of interactions, indicating that dose group differences in the social novelty test were not due to effects of FRM on activity or interest levels. While studies assessing the effects of developmental PCB exposure on working social memory are limited, similar deficits in social novelty have been reported in adult rats exposed developmentally to Aroclor 1221 (Reilly et al., 2015).
Spatial memory and navigation are highly dependent on executive function skills, including attention, visual discrimination, perception, and sensory discrimination (Faedda et al., 2022). These skills are often disrupted following exposures to PCBs (Bastien et al., 2022). However, we found that developmental exposure to the FRM did not cause deficits in performance in the spontaneous alternation task, which is a test of spatial working memory. The lack of effect of the FRM on this behavior was not due to possible FRM effects on activity level since we observed no significant differences in the number of entries between exposure groups. This finding further confirms that FRM effects on social novelty are not due to alterations in the level of exploration. To our knowledge, there are no published studies of PCB effects on performance in the y-maze, but studies that employed alternative tests of spatial memory, including the radial arm maze or Morris Water maze, have similarly reported no or minimal effects of developmental PCB exposure. For example, Sprague-Dawley rats exposed throughout gestation and lactation to PCB 28, PCB 118, or PCB 153 exhibited no significant differences in latency to complete the 8-arm radial maze, with the exception of males exposed to PCB 153 at a high dose of 64 mg/kg/d, which were impaired relative to vehicle controls (Schantz et al., 1995). In a later study, this same group assessed coplanar PCBs and found small deficits in spatial memory using the radial arm maze (Schantz et al., 1996). Varying impacts on performance in the Morris water maze have been reported by three different labs that studied Long-Evans rats exposed throughout gestation and lactation to Aroclor 1254 in the maternal diet. Two studies observed no impairment in Morris water maze performance in 3-month-old animals exposed to Aroclor 1254 at 6.0 mg/kg/d (Gilbert et al., 2000) or 8 mg/kg Aroclor 1254 (Zahalka et al., 2001). However, a third independent study found that weanling rats exposed to Aroclor 1254 in the maternal diet at 1, but not 6, mg/kg/d were significantly impaired in the Morris water maze task (Yang et al., 2009). Collectively, these findings suggest the possibility that the y-maze is not sensitive enough to reveal spatial memory deficits and/or that PCB effects on spatial memory are congener-specific and exhibit a non-monotonic dose-dependent relationship.
It is widely posited that PCBs interfere with normal neurodevelopment by decreasing circulating levels of thyroid hormone (Gauger et al., 2004). This premise originated from the well-documented importance of thyroid hormone in brain development (Bernal, 2000; Prezioso et al., 2018), as well as evidence that PCBs can decrease blood levels of thyroid hormones in humans (Curtis et al., 2019; Berlin et al., 2021) and experimental animal models (Sethi et al., 2021; Martin and Klaassen, 2010; Kato et al., 2003). However, there is also evidence that not all developmental PCB exposures decrease blood levels of thyroid hormones (Sethi et al., 2021; Yang et al., 2009). Here, we observed that gestational and lactational exposure to FRM resulted in significant sex, age–and dose-dependent changes in serum thyroid hormone levels in the dams and pups. Most of the significant changes were observed in the 1.0 and 6.0 mg/kg/d dose groups but the magnitude of the change did not increase with increasing dose. Male pups exhibited increased T3 levels and decreased T4 levels at P28, but no changes relative to vehicle controls at P35. Conversely, dams and female pups exhibited decreased T3 levels at P35. These sex-, age and dose-dependent patterns of FRM effects on thyroid hormone levels overlap but do not perfectly align with the FRM effects on behavioral outcomes. This was confirmed by correlation analysis that failed to identify any significant correlative relationships between FRM effects on thyroid hormone levels and FRM effects on behavioral outcomes. These analyses suggest that changes in thyroid hormone levels are not the major factor underlying the behavioral deficits observed in FRM-exposed animals.
The lack of a significant correlation between FRM effects on behavior and circulating levels of thyroid hormone are consistent with previous observations of animal models of developmental PCB exposure (Pessah et al., 2019). For example, developmental exposure to Aroclor 1254 caused spatial memory deficits in weanling rats independent of effects on serum thyroid hormone levels (Yang et al., 2009), and investigations of weanling mice exposed developmentally to the MARBLES PCB mixture revealed no correlation between altered thyroid hormone levels and behavioral deficits in USVs, repetitive behavior or sociability in MARBLES-exposed mice (Sethi et al., 2021). Moreover, neither the MARBLES PCB mixture nor any of the individual congeners in the MARBLES mixture exhibited agonistic or antagonistic activity on the thyroid hormone receptor (THR) as determined using a THR reporter cell line (Sethi et al., 2019). However, we cannot rule out the possibility that thyroid hormone-mediated mechanisms contribute to the behavioral deficits observed in mice developmentally exposed to FRM since individual PCB congeners found in the Aroclors used to make the FRM have been shown to alter THR-mediated signaling (Kostyniak et al., 2005). This possibility is further supported by reports that developmental exposure to Aroclor 1254 altered the expression of TH-responsive genes in the brain of weanling rats independent of significant changes in circulating levels of thyroid hormone in the dam or pups (Gauger et al., 2004). Further study is required to address this possibility in the mouse model of developmental FRM exposure.
Overall, our findings suggest that developmental exposure to FRM in the maternal diet may increase risk of deficits in NDD-relevant behaviors. Confidence in this conclusion is increased by the strong study design, including the inclusion of appropriate controls for each behavioral test to rule out non-specific effects of FRM that could have influenced behavioral outcomes, such as maternal toxicity, and FRM effects on activity and interest levels. However, it remains possible that PCBs altered motor capabilities (Roegge and Schantz, 2006; Gaum et al., 2021) or attention (Johansen et al., 2014; Lombardo et al., 2015), which could interfere with performance in the three-chambered task. Another limitation of this study is that we did not assess anxiety-like behavior or stress levels in these mice, which if elevated, can also alter social behavior (Bell et al., 2016; Orito et al., 2007). Finally, we conducted two behavior tests on the same day, which in spite of the 3-hour break between the tests to minimize effects of the first test on the second, could have produced additional stress to the animals (Lassarén et al., 2023).
Despite the fact that Aroclors were utilized to mimic the congener-profile of the human-relevant Fox River PCB mixture, the behavioral responses we’ve seen following developmental FRM exposure do not directly mirror the effects described in studies that assessed the developmental neurotoxicity of individual Aroclors. This suggests that risk assessments based on individual Aroclors may not fully capture the developmental neurotoxic potential of current human PCB exposures, and, therefore, further assessment of FRM and other human-relevant PCB mixtures in relevant animal models will be necessary to identify which PCBs are of most concern, and to understand how PCBs interact to modulate neurotoxic outcomes. Given the variable developmental neurotoxicity of PCB mixtures with different congener profiles, it is striking that the human epidemiological studies are largely consistent in reporting negative impacts of developmental PCB exposure on behavioral and cognitive deficits (Pessah et al., 2019).
Acknowledgements
A sincere thank you to Anthony Valenzuela for assistance with travel coordination and intellectual contributions and Jessie Badley for intellectual contributions. We would also like to thank Sarah Kim, Kyle Kim, and Joe Lim for assistance with tissue collections. This work was supported by the National Institute of Environmental Health [grant numbers R01 ES014901, R01 ES031098 and P30 ES023513]. The contents of this study do not necessarily represent the official views of the NIEHS, and the NIEHS does not endorse the purchase of any commercial products or services mentioned in the publication. Dr. Ilknur Dursun was supported by the Scientific and Technological Research Council of Turkey under the program TUBITAK 2219-International Postdoctoral Research Fellowship (TUBITAK 2219–1059B192000996).
Footnotes
CRediT authorship contribution statement
Ana Cristina Grodzki: Writing – review & editing, Methodology, Investigation. Ilknur Dursun: Writing – review & editing, Methodology, Investigation. Felipe da Costa Souza: Writing – review & editing, Methodology, Investigation. Rebecca J. Wilson: Writing – original draft, Visualization, Methodology, Investigation, Data curation. Youjun P. Suh: Writing – review & editing, Methodology, Investigation. Pamela J Lein: Writing – review & editing, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Conceptualization. Julia Y. Cui: Writing – review & editing, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Conceptualization. Hans-Joachim Lehmler: Writing – review & editing, Supervision, Resources, Project administration, Funding acquisition, Conceptualization.
Declaration of Competing Interest
The authors declare the following financial interests/personal relationships which may be considered as potential competing interests: Pamela J. Lein was hired as an expert witness by lawyers representing a group of plaintiffs alleging they were harmed by exposure to PCBs in school air. In that capacity, she testified as an expert witness on PCB neurotoxicity. The defendant was Pharmacia, a successor company to Monsanto.
Data Availability
Data will be made available on request.
References
- Adhikari A, Buchanan FKB, Fenton TA, Cameron DL, Halmai JANM, Copping NA, et al. , 2022. Touchscreen cognitive deficits, hyperexcitability and hyperactivity in males and females using two models of Cdkl5 deficiency. Hum. Mol. Genet 31 (18), 3032–3050 doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddac091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Association A.P. American psychiatric association. 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Bal-Price A, Crofton KM, Sachana M, Shafer TJ, Behl M, Forsby A, et al. , 2015. Putative adverse outcome pathways relevant to neurotoxicity. Crit. Rev. Toxicol 45 (1), 83–91 doi: 10.3109/10408444.2014.981331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bastien K, Muckle G, Ayotte P, Courtemanche Y, Dodge NC, Jacobson JL, et al. , 2022. Associations between developmental exposure to environmental contaminants and spatial navigation in late adolescence. N. Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev 2022 (181–182), 11–35. Epub 20220831. doi: 10.1002/cad.20478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bell MR, Thompson LM, Rodriguez K, Gore AC, 2016. Two-hit exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls at gestational and juvenile life stages: 1. Sexually dimorphic effects on social and anxiety-like behaviors. Horm. Behav 78, 168–177. Epub 20151122. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.11.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berg EL, Copping NA, Rivera JK, Pride MC, Careaga M, Bauman MD, et al. , 2018. Developmental social communication deficits in the Shank3 rat model of phelan-mcdermid syndrome and autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res 11 (4), 587–601. Epub 20180129. doi: 10.1002/aur.1925. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berghuis SA, Van Braeckel KNJA, Sauer PJJ, Bos AF, 2018. Prenatal exposure to persistent organic pollutants and cognition and motor performance in adolescence. Environ. Int 121 (Pt 1)), 13–22. Epub 20180829. doi: 10.1016/j.envint.2018.08.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berlin M, Barchel D, Brik A, Kohn E, Livne A, Keidar R, et al. , 2021. Maternal and newborn thyroid hormone, and the association with polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs) burden: the EHF (Environmental Health Fund) birth cohort. Front. Pedia 9, 705395. Epub 20210913. doi: 10.3389/fped.2021.705395. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernal J, 2000. Thyroid Hormones in Brain Development and Function. In: Feingold KR, Anawalt B, Blackman MR, Boyce A, Chrousos G, Corpas E, et al. (Eds.), Endotext. MDText.com, Inc.,Copyright © 2000–2023, MDText.com, Inc., South Dartmouth (MA). [Google Scholar]
- Berthoz A, Viaud-Delmon I, 1999. Multisensory integration in spatial orientation. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol 9 (6), 708–712 doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(99)00041-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boucher O, Muckle G, Bastien CH, 2009. Prenatal exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls: a neuropsychologic analysis. Environ. Health Perspect. 117 (1), 7–16. Epub 20080814. doi: 10.1289/ehp.11294. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Branchi I, Santucci D, Alleva E, 2001. Ultrasonic vocalisation emitted by infant rodents: a tool for assessment of neurobehavioural development. Behav. Brain Res 125 (1–2), 49–56 doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(01)00277-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brielmaier J, Matteson PG, Silverman JL, Senerth JM, Kelly S, Genestine M, et al. , 2012. Autism-relevant social abnormalities and cognitive deficits in engrailed-2 knockout mice. PLoS One 7 (7), e40914. Epub 20120719. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0040914. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown TI, Chrastil ER, 2019. Editorial: spatial navigation: memory mechanisms and executive function interactions. Front. Hum. Neurosci 13, 202. Epub 20190613. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bullert AJ, Doorn JA, Stevens HE, Lehmler HJ, 2021. The effects of polychlorinated biphenyl exposure during adolescence on the nervous system: a comprehensive review. Chem. Res Toxicol 34 (9), 1948–1952. Epub 20210907. doi: 10.1021/acs.chemrestox.1c00226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christensen KY, Thompson BA, Werner M, Malecki K, Imm P, Anderson HA, 2016. Levels of persistent contaminants in relation to fish consumption among older male anglers in Wisconsin. Int J. Hyg. Environ. Health 219 (2), 184–194. Epub 20151110. doi: 10.1016/j.ijheh.2015.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen-Salmon C, Carlier M, Roubertoux P, Jouhaneau J, Semal C, Paillette M, 1985. Differences in patterns of pup care in mice. V–Pup ultrasonic emissions and pup care behavior. Physiol. Behav 35 (2), 167–174 doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(85)90331-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crawley JN, 2012. Translational animal models of autism and neurodevelopmental disorders. Dialog-. Clin. Neurosci 14 (3), 293–305 doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2012.14.3/jcrawley. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Curtis SW, Terrell ML, Jacobson MH, Cobb DO, Jiang VS, Neblett MF, et al. , 2019. Thyroid hormone levels associate with exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls and polybrominated biphenyls in adults exposed as children. Environ. Health 18 (1), 75. Epub 20190823. doi: 10.1186/s12940-019-0509-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D’Amato FR, Scalera E, Sarli C, Moles A, 2005. Pups call, mothers rush: does maternal responsiveness affect the amount of ultrasonic vocalizations in mouse pups? Behav. Genet 35 (1), 103–112 doi: 10.1007/s10519-004-0860-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faedda N, Piccardi L, Boccia M, Bartonek Å, Guariglia C, 2022. Editorial: spatial navigation and neurodevelopmental disorders. Front. Psychiatry 13, 875868. Epub 20220324. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.875868. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frith CD, 2007. The social brain? Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci 362 (1480), 671–678 doi: 10.1098/rstb.2006.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gan-Or B, London M, 2023. Cortical circuits modulate mouse social vocalizations. Sci. Adv 9 (39), eade6992. Epub 20230929. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.ade6992. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gauger KJ, Kato Y, Haraguchi K, Lehmler HJ, Robertson LW, Bansal R, et al. , 2004. Polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs) exert thyroid hormone-like effects in the fetal rat brain but do not bind to thyroid hormone receptors. Environ. Health Perspect. 112 (5), 516–523 doi: 10.1289/ehp.6672. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaum PM, Kuczynski I, Schettgen T, Putschögl FM, Kraus T, Fimm B, et al. , 2021. Adverse health effects of PCBs on fine motor performance - analysis of a neurophysiological pathway in the HELPcB surveillance program. Neurotoxicology 84, 146–154. Epub 20210324. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2021.03.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilbert ME, Mundy WR, Crofton KM, 2000. Spatial learning and long-term potentiation in the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus in animals developmentally exposed to Aroclor 1254. Toxicol. Sci 57 (1), 102–111 doi: 10.1093/toxsci/57.1.102. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grandjean P, Landrigan PJ, 2014. Neurobehavioural effects of developmental toxicity. Lancet Neurol 13 (3), 330–338. Epub 20140217. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(13)70278-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Granillo L, Sethi S, Keil KP, Lin Y, Ozonoff S, Iosif AM, et al. , 2019. Polychlorinated biphenyls influence on autism spectrum disorder risk in the MARBLES cohort. Environ. Res 171, 177–184. Epub 20181225. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2018.12.061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grimsley JM, Monaghan JJ, Wenstrup JJ, 2011. Development of social vocalizations in mice. PLoS One 6 (3), e17460. Epub 20110309. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017460. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guo J, Romanak K, Westenbroek S, Li A, Kreis RG, Hites RA, et al. , 2017. Updated polychlorinated biphenyl mass budget for lake michigan. Environ. Sci. Technol 51 (21), 12455–12465. Epub 20171017. doi: 10.1021/acs.est.7b02904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hahn ME, Lavooy MJ, 2005. A review of the methods of studies on infant ultrasound production and maternal retrieval in small rodents. Behav. Genet 35 (1), 31–52 doi: 10.1007/s10519-004-0854-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harper KM, Harp SJ, Moy SS, 2023. Prenatal stress unmasks behavioral phenotypes in genetic mouse models of neurodevelopmental disorders. Front Behav. Neurosci 17, 1271225. Epub 20230922. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2023.1271225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harpin V, Mazzone L, Raynaud JP, Kahle J, Hodgkins P, 2016. Long-term outcomes of ADHD: a systematic review of self-esteem and social function. J. Atten. Disord 20 (4), 295–305. Epub 20130522. doi: 10.1177/1087054713486516. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hens B, Hens L, 2017. Persistent threats by persistent pollutants: chemical nature, concerns and future policy regarding pcbs-what are we heading For? Toxics 6 (1). Epub 20171221. doi: 10.3390/toxics6010001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Imamoglu I, Christensen ER, 2002. PCB sources, transformations, and contributions in recent Fox River, Wisconsin sediments determined from receptor modeling. Water Res 36 (14), 3449–3462 doi: 10.1016/s0043-1354(02)00050-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Imamoglu I, Li K, Christensen ER, McMullin JK, 2004. Sources and dechlorination of polychlorinated biphenyl congeners in the sediments of Fox River, Wisconsin. Environ. Sci. Technol 38 (9), 2574–2583 doi: 10.1021/es035165x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johansen EB, Knoff M, Fonnum F, Lausund PL, Walaas SI, Wøien G, et al. , 2011. Postnatal exposure to PCB 153 and PCB 180, but not to PCB 52, produces changes in activity level and stimulus control in outbred male Wistar Kyoto rats. Behav. Brain Funct 7, 18. Epub 20110526. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-7-18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johansen EB, Fonnum F, Lausund PL, Walaas SI, Bærland NE, Wøien G, et al. , 2014. Behavioral changes following PCB 153 exposure in the spontaneously hypertensive rat - an animal model of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Behav. Brain Funct 10 (1). Epub 20140109. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-10-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jolous-Jamshidi B, Cromwell HC, McFarland AM, Meserve LA, 2010. Perinatal exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls alters social behaviors in rats. Toxicol. Lett 199 (2), 136–143. Epub 20100909. doi: 10.1016/j.toxlet.2010.08.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kato Y, Haraguchi K, Yamazaki T, Ito Y, Miyajima S, Nemoto K, et al. , 2003. Effects of polychlorinated biphenyls, kanechlor-500, on serum thyroid hormone levels in rats and mice. Toxicol. Sci 72 (2), 235–241 doi: 10.1093/toxsci/kfg025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaya D, Imamoglu I, Sanin FD, Sowers KR, 2018. A comparative evaluation of anaerobic dechlorination of PCB-118 and Aroclor 1254 in sediment microcosms from three PCB-impacted environments. J. Hazard Mater. 341, 328–335. Epub 20170803. doi: 10.1016/j.jhazmat.2017.08.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keil KP, Sethi S, Lein PJ, 2019. Sex-dependent effects of 2,2′,3,5′,6-pentachlorobiphenyl on dendritic arborization of primary mouse neurons. Toxicol. Sci 168 (1), 95–109 doi: 10.1093/toxsci/kfy277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keil-Stietz K, Lein PJ, 2023. Gene×environment interactions in autism spectrum disorders. Curr. Top. Dev. Biol 152, 221–284. Epub 20221219. doi: 10.1016/bs.ctdb.2022.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Khalil R, Tindle R, Boraud T, Moustafa AA, Karim AA, 2018. Social decision making in autism: On the impact of mirror neurons, motor control, and imitative behaviors. CNS Neurosci. Ther 24 (8), 669–676. Epub 20180702. doi: 10.1111/cns.13001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kight KE, Argue KJ, Bumgardner JG, Bardhi K, Waddell J, McCarthy MM, 2021. Social behavior in prepubertal neurexin 1α deficient rats: a model of neurodevelopmental disorders. Behav. Neurosci 135 (6), 782–803. Epub 20210729. doi: 10.1037/bne0000482. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klocke C, Sethi S, Lein PJ, 2020. The developmental neurotoxicity of legacy vs. contemporary polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs): similarities and differences. Environ. Sci. Pollut. Res Int 27 (9), 8885–8896. Epub 20191112. doi: 10.1007/s11356-019-06723-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koh WX, Hornbuckle KC, Marek RF, Wang K, Thorne PS, 2016. Hydroxylated polychlorinated biphenyls in human sera from adolescents and their mothers living in two U.S. Midwestern communities. Chemosphere 147, 389–395. Epub 20160115. doi: 10.1016/j.chemosphere.2015.12.113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kostyniak PJ, Hansen LG, Widholm JJ, Fitzpatrick RD, Olson JR, Helferich JL, et al. , 2005. Formulation and characterization of an experimental PCB mixture designed to mimic human exposure from contaminated fish. Toxicol. Sci 88 (2), 400–411. Epub 20050921. doi: 10.1093/toxsci/kfi338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kraeuter AK, Guest PC, Sarnyai Z, 2019. The Y-maze for assessment of spatial working and reference memory in mice. Methods Mol. Biol 1916, 105–111 doi: 10.1007/978-1-4939-8994-2_10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krishnan D, Cromwell HC, Meserve L, 2014. Effects of polychlorinated biphenyl (PCB) exposure on response perseveration and ultrasonic vocalization emission in rat during development. Endocr. Disruptors 2 (1), e969608. [Google Scholar]
- Lassarén P, Conley G, Boucher ML, Conley AN, Morriss NJ, Qiu J, et al. , 2023. Optimizing choice and timing of behavioral outcome tests after repetitive mild traumatic brain injury: a machine learning-based approach on multiple pre-clinical experiments. J. Neurotrauma 40 (15–16), 1762–1778. Epub 20230321. doi: 10.1089/neu.2022.0486. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li X, Lim JJ, Wang K, Prasad B, Bhatt DK, Cui JY, et al. , 2022. The disposition of polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs) differs between germ-free and conventional mice. Environ. Toxicol. Phar 92, 103854 doi: 〈 10.1016/j.etap.2022.103854〉. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li X, Hefti MM, Marek RF, Hornbuckle KC, Wang K, Lehmler HJ, 2022. Assessment of polychlorinated biphenyls and their hydroxylated metabolites in postmortem human brain samples: age and brain region differences. Environ. Sci. Technol 56 (13), 9515–9526. Epub 20220603. doi: 10.1021/acs.est.2c00581. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li X, Suh YP, Cui JY, Lehmler H-J, 2023. Dataset for Characterization of the Fox River Mixture (FRM). University of Iowa; (dataset). 10.25820/data.006235. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Little CC, Barlow J, Alsen M, van Gerwen M, 2022. Association between polychlorinated biphenyl exposure and thyroid hormones: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Environ. Sci. Health C. Toxicol. Carcinog 40 (3–4), 248–267. Epub 20221214. doi: 10.1080/26896583.2022.2149213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lombardo JP, Berger DF, Hunt A, Carpenter DO, 2015. Inhalation of polychlorinated biphenyls (PCB) produces hyperactivity in rats. J. Toxicol. Environ. Health A 78 (18), 1142–1153. Epub 20150923. doi: 10.1080/15287394.2015.1060913. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lord C, Cook EH, Leventhal BL, Amaral DG, 2000. Autism spectrum disorders. Neuron 28 (2), 355–363 doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)00115-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lovinger DM, 2008. Communication networks in the brain: neurons, receptors, neurotransmitters, and alcohol. Alcohol Res Health 31 (3), 196–214. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marek RF, Thorne PS, DeWall J, Hornbuckle KC, 2014. Variability in PCB and OH-PCB serum levels in children and their mothers in urban and rural U.S. communities. Environ. Sci. Technol 48 (22), 13459–13467. Epub 20141029. doi: 10.1021/es502490w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin L, Klaassen CD, 2010. Differential effects of polychlorinated biphenyl congeners on serum thyroid hormone levels in rats. Toxicol. Sci 117 (1), 36–44. Epub 20100623. doi: 10.1093/toxsci/kfq187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mehri F, Bashirian S, Khazaei S, Jenabi E, 2021. Association between pesticide and polychlorinated biphenyl exposure during pregnancy and autism spectrum disorder among children: a meta-analysis. Clin. Exp. Pedia 64 (6), 286–292. Epub 20200727. doi: 10.3345/cep.2020.00864. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mendola P, Selevan SG, Gutter S, Rice D, 2002. Environmental factors associated with a spectrum of neurodevelopmental deficits. Ment. Retard Dev. Disabil. Res Rev 8 (3), 188–197 doi: 10.1002/mrdd.10033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miodovnik A, Engel SM, Zhu C, Ye X, Soorya LV, Silva MJ, et al. , 2011. Endocrine disruptors and childhood social impairment. Neurotoxicology 32 (2), 261–267. Epub 20101221. doi: 10.1016/j.neuro.2010.12.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Motomura N, Shimizu K, Shimizu M, Aoki-Komori S, Taniguchi K, Serizawa I, et al. , 2002. A comparative study of isolation-induced ultrasonic vocalization in rodent pups. Exp. Anim 51 (2), 187–190 doi: 10.1538/expanim.51.187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moy SS, Nadler JJ, Perez A, Barbaro RP, Johns JM, Magnuson TR, et al. , 2004. Sociability and preference for social novelty in five inbred strains: an approach to assess autistic-like behavior in mice. Genes Brain Behav 3 (5), 287–302. 10.1111/j.1601-1848.2004.00076.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nadler JJ, Moy SS, Dold G, Trang D, Simmons N, Perez A, et al. , 2004. Automated apparatus for quantitation of social approach behaviors in mice. Genes Brain Behav 3 (5), 303–314 doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2004.00071.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orito K, Gotanda N, Murakami M, Ikeda T, Egashira N, Mishima K, et al. , 2007. Prenatal exposure to 3,3′,4,4′,5-pentachlorobiphenyl (PCB126) promotes anxiogenic behavior in rats. Tohoku J. Exp. Med 212 (2), 151–157 doi: 10.1620/tjem.212.151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Shaughnessy KL, Wood CR, Ford RL, Kosian PA, Hotchkiss MG, Degitz SJ, et al. , 2018. Thyroid hormone disruption in the fetal and neonatal rat: predictive hormone measures and bioindicators of hormone action in the developing cortex. Toxicol. Sci 166 (1), 163–179 doi: 10.1093/toxsci/kfy190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panesar HK, Kennedy CL, Keil Stietz KP, Lein PJ, 2020. Polychlorinated Biphenyls (PCBs): risk Factors for Autism Spectrum Disorder? Toxics 8 (3). Epub 20200917. doi: 10.3390/toxics8030070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pessah IN, Lein PJ, Seegal RF, Sagiv SK, 2019. Neurotoxicity of polychlorinated biphenyls and related organohalogens. Acta Neuropathol. 138 (3), 363–387. Epub 20190411. doi: 10.1007/s00401-019-01978-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Portfors CV, 2007. Types and functions of ultrasonic vocalizations in laboratory rats and mice. J. Am. Assoc. Lab Anim. Sci 46 (1), 28–34. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Prezioso G, Giannini C, Chiarelli F, 2018. Effect of thyroid hormones on neurons and neurodevelopment. Horm. Res Paediatr. 90 (2), 73–81. Epub 20180829. doi: 10.1159/000492129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reilly MP, Weeks CD, Topper VY, Thompson LM, Crews D, Gore AC, 2015. The effects of prenatal PCBs on adult social behavior in rats. Horm. Behav 73, 47–55. Epub 20150618. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.06.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ricceri L, Moles A, Crawley J, 2007. Behavioral phenotyping of mouse models of neurodevelopmental disorders: relevant social behavior patterns across the life span. Behav. Brain Res 176 (1), 40–52. Epub 20060922. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.08.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rieger MA, Dougherty JD, 2016. Analysis of within subjects variability in mouse ultrasonic vocalization: pups exhibit inconsistent, state-like patterns of call production. Front Behav. Neurosci 10, 182. Epub 20160928. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00182. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roegge CS, Schantz SL, 2006. Motor function following developmental exposure to PCBS and/or MEHG. Neurotoxicol Teratol 28 (2), 260–277. Epub 20060217. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2005.12.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sable HJ, Powers BE, Wang VC, Widholm JJ, Schantz SL, 2006. Alterations in DRH and DRL performance in rats developmentally exposed to an environmental PCB mixture. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 28 (5), 548–556. Epub 20060630. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2006.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scattoni ML, Crawley J, Ricceri L, 2009. Ultrasonic vocalizations: a tool for behavioural phenotyping of mouse models of neurodevelopmental disorders. Neurosci. Biobehav Rev 33 (4), 508–515. Epub 20080813. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schantz SL, Moshtaghian J, Ness DK, 1995. Spatial learning deficits in adult rats exposed to ortho-substituted PCB congeners during gestation and lactation. Fundam. Appl. Toxicol. 26 (1), 117–126 doi: 10.1006/faat.1995.1081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schantz SL, Seo BW, Moshtaghian J, Peterson RE, Moore RW, 1996. Effects of gestational and lactational exposure to TCDD or coplanar PCBs on spatial learning. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 18 (3), 305–313 doi: 10.1016/s0892-0362(96)90033-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schantz SL, Widholm JJ, Rice DC, 2003. Effects of PCB exposure on neuropsychological function in children. Environ. Health Perspect. 111 (3), 357–576 doi: 10.1289/ehp.5461. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schantz SL, Gardiner JC, Aguiar A, Tang X, Gasior DM, Sweeney AM, et al. , 2010. Contaminant profiles in Southeast Asian immigrants consuming fish from polluted waters in northeastern Wisconsin. Environ. Res 110 (1), 33–39 doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2009.09.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sethi S, Morgan RK, Feng W, Lin Y, Li X, Luna C, et al. , 2019. Comparative analyses of the 12 most abundant PCB congeners detected in human maternal serum for activity at the thyroid hormone receptor and ryanodine receptor. Environ. Sci. Technol 53 (7), 3948–3958. Epub 20190319. doi: 10.1021/acs.est.9b00535. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sethi S, Keil Stietz KP, Valenzuela AE, Klocke CR, Silverman JL, Puschner B, et al. , 2021. Developmental exposure to a human-relevant polychlorinated biphenyl mixture causes behavioral phenotypes that vary by sex and genotype in juvenile mice expressing human mutations that modulate neuronal calcium. Front Neurosci 15, 766826. Epub 20211206. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.766826. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shultz S, Klin A, Jones W, 2018. Neonatal transitions in social behavior and their implications for autism. Trends Cogn. Sci 22 (5), 452–469. Epub 20180330. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.02.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silverman JL, Ellegood J, 2018. Behavioral and neuroanatomical approaches in models of neurodevelopmental disorders: opportunities for translation. Curr. Opin. Neurol 31 (2), 126–133 doi: 10.1097/WCO.0000000000000537. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silverman JL, Yang M, Lord C, Crawley JN, 2010. Behavioural phenotyping assays for mouse models of autism. Nat. Rev. Neurosci 11 (7), 490–502 doi: 10.1038/nrn2851. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silverman JL, Smith DG, Rizzo SJ, Karras MN, Turner SM, Tolu SS, et al. , 2012. Negative allosteric modulation of the mGluR5 receptor reduces repetitive behaviors and rescues social deficits in mouse models of autism. Sci. Transl. Med 4 (131), 131ra51 doi: 10.1126/scitranslmed.3003501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sprowles JLN, Monaikul S, Aguiar A, Gardiner J, Monaikul N, Kostyniak P, et al. , 2022. Associations of concurrent PCB and PBDE serum concentrations with executive functioning in adolescents. Neurotoxicol Teratol. 92, 107092. Epub 20220423. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2022.107092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steenport DM, Anderson HA, Hanrahan LP, Falk C, Draheim LA, Kanarek MS, et al. , 2000. Fish consumption habits and advisory awareness among Fox River anglers. WMJ 99 (8), 43–46. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu K, Li Z, Qiao J, Wang S, Xie P, Zong Z, et al. , 2023. Persistent organic pollutants exposure and risk of autism spectrum disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Environ. Pollut. 336, 122439. Epub 20230822. doi: 10.1016/j.envpol.2023.122439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang D, Kim KH, Phimister A, Bachstetter AD, Ward TR, Stackman RW, et al. , 2009. Developmental exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls interferes with experience-dependent dendritic plasticity and ryanodine receptor expression in weanling rats. Environ. Health Perspect. 117 (3), 426–435. Epub 20080912. doi: 10.1289/ehp.11771. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang M, Silverman JL, Crawley JN Automated three-chambered social approach task for mice. Curr Protoc Neurosci 2011;Chapter 8:Unit 8.26. doi: 10.1002/0471142301.ns0826s56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yin X, Chen L, Xia Y, Cheng Q, Yuan J, Yang Y, et al. , 2016. Maternal deprivation influences pup ultrasonic vocalizations of C57BL/6J Mice. PLoS One 11 (8), e0160409. Epub 20160823. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0160409. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zahalka EA, Ellis DH, Goldey ES, Stanton ME, Lau C, 2001. Perinatal exposure to polychlorinated biphenyls Aroclor 1016 or 1254 did not alter brain catecholamines nor delayed alternation performance in Long-Evans rats. Brain Res. Bull. 55 (4), 487–500 doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(01)00548-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Data will be made available on request.