Abstract
Background
The ability to relearn a lost skill is critical to motor recovery after a stroke. Previous studies indicate that stroke typically affects the processes underlying motor control and execution but not the learning of those skills. However, these studies could be confounded by the presence of significant motor impairments. Furthermore, prior research involving the upper extremity indicates that stroke survivors have an advantage in offline motor learning when compared with controls. However, this has not been examined using motor acuity tasks (i.e., tasks focusing on the quality of executed actions) that have direct functional relevance to rehabilitation.
Objective
Investigate how stroke affects leg motor skill learning during walking in stroke survivors.
Methods
Twenty-five participants (10 stroke; 15 controls) were recruited for this prospective, case-control study. Participants learned a novel foot-trajectory tracking task on two consecutive days while walking on a treadmill. The task necessitated greater hip and knee flexion during the swing phase of the gait. Online learning was measured by comparing tracking error at the beginning and end of each practice session, offline (rest-driven) learning was measured by comparing the end of the first practice session to the beginning of the second, and retention was measured by comparing the beginning of the first practice session to the beginning of the second. Online learning, offline learning, and retention were compared between the stroke survivors and uninjured controls.
Results
Stroke survivors improved their tracking performance on the first day (p = 0.033); however, the amount of learning in stroke survivors was lower in comparison with the control group on both days (p ≤ 0.05). Interestingly, stroke survivors showed higher offline learning gains when compared with uninjured controls (p = 0.011).
Conclusions
Even stroke survivors with no perceivable motor impairments have difficulty acquiring new motor skills related to walking, which may be related to the underlying neural damage caused at the time of stroke. Furthermore, stroke survivors may require longer training with adequate rest to acquire new motor skills.
Keywords: Skill acquisition, Error-based learning, Motor task, Hemiparesis, Consolidation
Background
Stroke is a major cause of adult disability worldwide, affecting millions of people each year [1, 2]. Common motor impairments after stroke include weakness on one side of the body [3], difficulty coordinating movements [4, 5], and loss of balance [6]. These impairments often result in disabilities that restrict the mobility and independence of stroke survivors in their daily activities, which in turn highlights the need for effective rehabilitation techniques that can improve walking ability. Current approaches to gait recovery after stroke often involve task-specific training with assistive devices and interactive technologies [7, 8]. However, despite their effectiveness, these methods are no more beneficial than conventional rehabilitation in most clinical trials [9–11]. Therefore, there is a critical need for new therapies that can facilitate gait recovery after stroke.
A key to developing effective rehabilitation interventions after stroke is through the application of motor learning principles. Although the importance of incorporating motor learning principles into stroke rehabilitation programs has been repeatedly emphasized [12, 13], there still remains a large gap in our understanding of how learning and rehabilitation processes are interlinked in clinical populations [14]. There is some evidence that acquiring new skills can activate neuroplastic mechanisms in the brain and that the process of learning a new motor skill shares similarities with relearning lost motor skills following a stroke [12, 15]. Therefore, studying motor learning deficits after a stroke can provide a better understanding of the specific mechanisms of neurophysiological recovery, which could aid in the development of more effective interventions.
However, the effect of stroke on motor skill learning is difficult to estimate, as there is limited research on this topic and previous research has yielded conflicting results. For example, some studies suggest that stroke primarily affects the processes underlying motor control and execution, while leaving the learning of motor skills intact [16–19]. However, a recent study revealed that the extent of motor learning deficits following a stroke is dependent on the severity of motor impairment [20]. It is important to note that a major challenge in establishing evidence of learning deficits is that performance deficits can be misinterpreted as learning deficits [21, 22]. This is supported by the observation that error-based learning capacity—learning driven by error relative to a desired action or goal—in stroke survivors is comparable to neurologically intact adults when motor execution deficits are controlled for during the experiment [16, 19]. However, many of these prior studies, for good reasons, have focused on goal or action selection (i.e., where to move to or what movement can achieve the chosen goal) with less emphasis on motor acuity (i.e., the quality of the executed movements) [23]. More importantly, the experimental tasks are often restricted to a single degree of freedom (DOF) movement, thereby making it challenging to generalize these findings to complex multi-DOF movements (e.g., gait) and limiting their functional relevance to rehabilitation.
Another challenge in determining the effect of stroke on motor skill learning is that learning is mediated by both online and offline processes that may be differentially affected by stroke. Specifically, changes in performance can be due to learning during practice or from periods of rest between practice when information is consolidated and committed to long-term memory [24]. Changes due to practice are typically measured by evaluating the change in performance from the beginning to the end of training (i.e., online learning), while consolidation is measured by evaluating changes in performance from the end of training to the beginning of a follow-up session conducted in the following days (i.e., offline gains/learning) [25]. The summation of these two processes can be evaluated by retention of learning, which is measured as a change from the beginning of training to the follow-up session. Previous studies have shown that stroke survivors improve their performance on motor skills following a period of sleepful rest, while uninjured controls reduce their performance. Interestingly, these same learning gains are not observed in stroke survivors who rest for an equivalent time interval without sleep [26–28]. As such, it seems possible that stroke survivors undergoing interventions involving learning may benefit from periods of sleep between practice sessions. Furthermore, it is possible that this advantage in offline gains is masking online deficits in studies that only measure retention. However, as mentioned above, these contributions of offline gains to skill learning in stroke survivors have not been investigated in complex, lower-extremity tasks that have relevance to gait rehabilitation. As a result, it is currently unclear how stroke affects motor learning and whether learning deficits are present in individuals with minimal impairment when performing functional lower-extremity tasks such as walking.
Therefore, the purpose of this study was to evaluate the extent of motor learning deficits in chronic stroke survivors using a functional leg motor skill learning task. To minimize the impact of paresis/weakness on our findings, we specifically recruited stroke survivors with minimal impairment. To comprehensively understand the effect of stroke on motor learning, we examined both online (i.e., changes that occur during practice within the same day) and offline (i.e., changes that occur after practice during periods of no practice between days) learning. To address the issue of task relevance to day-to-day activities, the task required participants to learn a gait pattern that required 30% greater hip and knee flexion during the swing phase, which has been previously shown to be highly relevant in rehabilitation training for addressing stiff knee gait after stroke [29]. We hypothesized that stroke survivors with mild motor impairments would exhibit significant deficits in both online and offline learning and retention of motor skills during walking when compared with uninjured controls.
Methods
Participants
A total of 25 adults (10 individuals with stroke and 15 elderly uninjured controls, Table 1) participated in this study. Older adults of similar age to the stroke survivors were recruited as controls to minimize age as a confounding factor in our analysis. This sample size provided us with a power β > 80% to detect statistical significance with a conservative effect size of n2 = 0.3 [30, 31] at a significance level of α = 0.05 (computed in G*power 3.1.9.6, Test family: F tests, ANOVA: Repeated measures, within-between interaction). Participants in the control group were also part of a different study that investigated the effects of aging on motor learning [31]. All participants were right leg dominant based on their preferred leg to kick a ball [32]. Stroke survivors were included in the study if they (1) had a radiologically (CT or MRI) confirmed ischemic or hemorrhagic stroke at least 6 months prior to the study, (2) had no significant cognitive deficits (Mini-Mental State Examination [MMSE] Score ≥ 22), (3) had no documented major sensory or proprioceptive deficits (determined via self-assessment and medical chart review during screening), (4) had no major motor deficits (determined by lower-extremity Fugl-Meyer ≥ 27 or 78% of the total scale of 34 [33]), (5) were able to walk independently with or without assistive devices, (6) had no history of uncontrolled diabetes or hypertension, and (7) had no major orthopaedic issues or range of motion deficits. Control participants were included in the study if they (1) had no significant cognitive deficits (MMSE ≥ 22), (2) had no significant orthopaedic or neurological issues, and (3) had no history major medical conditions, including uncontrolled diabetes or hypertension. We measured the stroke survivors’ lower extremity motor impairment with the lower-extremity Fugl-Meyer scale (LE-FM, 32.1 ± 2.2, range: 27–34, one participant was not measured). All participants were tested at a single laboratory within the University of Michigan and signed a written informed consent prior to participation that was approved by the University of Michigan Human Subjects Institutional Review Board.
Table 1.
Group | Sex | Age (year) | Mass (kg) | Height (m) | MMSE score | LE-FM score | Gait speed | Sleep quality |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Stroke |
5 females, 5 males |
58.3 ± 8.9 | 76.5 ± 20.5 | 1.7 ± 0.1 | 28.3 ± 2.2 | 32.1 ± 2.2 | 1.3 ± 0.2 | 2.5 ± 2.2 |
Control |
10 females, 5 males |
65.3 ± 2.9 | 72.0 ± 13.2 | 1.6 ± 0.1 | 29.3 ± 0.8 | N/A | 1.24* | 3.5 ± 2.9 |
* indicates that data is taken from normative data set (Kasovic et al. 2021 [49], “Normative Data for Gait Speed and Height Norm Speed in ≥ 60-Year-Old Men and Women”)
Experimental protocol
Participants learned a foot-trajectory tracking task on two consecutive days that were separated by about 24 h (Fig. 1(A)). Participants performed this task with their affected leg while walking on a motorized treadmill that was set to move at a constant speed of 0.89 m/s (2 mph, selected to minimize fatigue and align with previous studies using this paradigm [31, 32, 34, 35]) and wearing the same foot- and leg-wear (i.e., shorts or spandex). The foot-trajectory tracking task required participants to adjust their hip and knee angles during the swing phase of walking to match a target trajectory projected onto a computer monitor placed in front of them. On both days, the experiment consisted of four phases: baseline, pre-test (Pre), training (Tr), and post-test (Post) (Fig. 1(B)). During baseline, participants walked normally on the treadmill for one minute. During the pre-test, the participants performed the foot-trajectory tracking task for one minute and their initial performance on the task was evaluated. Training consisted of repeated practice of the foot-trajectory tracking task. Participants completed eight blocks of practice, with each block lasting one minute and separated by one minute of rest where the treadmill was stopped. In the post-test, the participants again performed the foot-trajectory tracking task for one minute and changes in target-tracking performance were assessed. For stroke participants, the more-affected side was used as the training leg (3 left leg and 7 right leg), and for control participants, the training leg for each participant was determined randomly (7 left leg and 8 right leg). In the post-test, the participants’ final performance was evaluated by assessing their final target-tracking error. Participants were evaluated on the task on two subsequent days so that we could examine the contribution of rest between days to learning.
Foot-trajectory tracking task
A custom-designed, real-time motion tracking system, developed using LabVIEW 2011 and NI Vision Assistant (National Instruments Corp., Austin, TX, USA), was used for the motor learning task [36]. The system consisted of a camera (C920 Pro HD Logitech Webcam, Logitech, San Jose, CA, USA) and computed the sagittal plane hip and knee kinematics during walking by tracking three 19 mm retroreflective markers positioned on the participants’ greater trochanter, lateral epicondyle of the femur, and lateral malleolus of the ankle. The target template trajectory for the foot-trajectory tracking task was created based on the participants’ sagittal plane hip and knee kinematics data obtained during baseline walking. The target trajectory was generated by scaling (1.3×) the hip and knee angles during swing phase of the normal walking trial and projecting this template in the end-point space, specifically the trajectory of the ankle relative to the hip on the sagittal plane (Fig. 1(C)). This was achieved using the following forward kinematic equation:
Where xa and ya are the x and y positions of the ankle lateral malleolus relative to the hip, l1 is the distance between hip and knee markers (i.e., thigh segment), l2 is the distance between knee and ankle markers (i.e., shank segment), θh and θk are the anatomical hip and knee angles. The target template was smoothed using a Hanning window to prevent abrupt scaling at the beginning and end of the swing phase. The template trajectory was then displayed concurrently with the participant’s actual foot trajectory on a computer monitor positioned in front of the participant. Participants were instructed to try and match the target template trajectory as best as they can during the swing phase of their gait. Additionally, they were asked not to alter the normal gait patterns of the opposite leg that was not involved in the foot-trajectory tracking task.
Data analyses
The performance on the foot-trajectory tracking task (i.e., how closely the participant’s actual trajectory matched the target trajectory spatially) was evaluated by computing the tracking error for each block. Tracking error was calculated as the difference in area (i.e., non-overlapping area) in pixels between the participant’s actual foot trajectory and the target template trajectory for each stride (Fig. 1(D)). This method of error computation was selected to align with previous work and is useful to prevent positive and negative error cancellation when examining complex shapes [31, 32, 34, 35, 37]. This stride-by-stride tracking error was then expressed as a percentage of the area within the participant’s target template and averaged across strides for each block. Normalizing to each participant’s target template accounts for differences in target templates between participants [31, 32, 34, 35]. For the purposes of this study, four performance metrics were derived from the tracking error data on Day 1 and Day 2: (1) online learning (Day 1), (2) online learning (Day 2), (3) offline learning, and (4) retention (Fig. 1(B)). The amount of online learning on Day 1 (D1) and Day 2 (D2) was evaluated by comparing the tracking error during Pre blocks on Day 1 (D1-Pre) and Day 2 (D2-Pre) to Post blocks on Day 1 (D1-Post) and Day 2 (D2-Post), respectively. The amount of offline learning was evaluated by computing the difference in tracking error during the Pre block on Day 2 (D2-Pre) from the Post block on Day 1 (D1-Post). It is important to note that the offline gain metric is measuring the change in performance between days and is therefore examining the contribution of prolonged rest to learning. The amount of retention was evaluated by comparing the tracking error during the Pre block on Day 1 (D1-Pre) to Pre block on Day 2 (D2-Pre).
Statistical analysis
All statistical analyses were performed in IBM SPSS for Windows Version 27 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL). A two-sample t-test was used to determine if there were any initial performance differences between the two groups. To evaluate if stroke affected the online learning and retention processes, we tested the differences in the amount of online learning (changes in tracking error on Day 1 and Day 2: D1-Post relative to D1-Pre and D2-Post relative to D2-Pre) and retention (initial tracking error on Day 2 relative to initial tracking error on Day 1: D2-Pre relative to D1-Pre) between the stroke and the control participants using repeated measures analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with block as within-subject factor, group as between-subjects factor, and the appropriate Pre block as the covariate (e.g., D1-Pre was used as a covariate for D1 online learning and retention and D2-Pre was used as a covariate for D2 online learning). To evaluate if stroke affected the consolidation process, we tested the differences in the amount of offline learning (changes in tracking error from the end of Day 1 to the beginning of Day 2: D2-Pre – D1-Post) between the stroke and the control participants using a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with group as the between-subjects factor. These analyses are depicted graphically in Fig. 1(B). A significant interaction effect was followed by appropriate post-hoc analysis with Sidak correction. All analyses used a significance level of = 0.05.
Robustness check analysis
Robustness checks were also performed by evaluating the results in relative terms (i.e., % baseline) and via exponential curve-fitting in addition to the above method [38]. For the relative analysis, we normalized the tracking error as a percentage of their respective baseline values (e.g., Day 1 online learning = [D1-Post/D1-Pre] × 100; offline learning = [D2-Pre/D1-Post] × 100) and compared those values between groups using two-sample t-tests with bootstrapping (10,000 iterations). For the curve-fitting analysis, we used a custom MATLAB program (R2019b, MathWorks, Natick, MA, USA) to fit an exponential function to each participant’s performance using a trust-region-reflective least-squares algorithm and the following expression.
Here, and are the participant’s measured and estimated tracking error, respectively, x(i) is the ith experimental block (e.g., x(0) = Pre, x(1) = TM-1, etc.), and a, b and c are function coefficients. In the context of this study, coefficients b and c can be thought of as the learning rate and asymptote, respectively, and a + c can be thought of as initial error. During fitting, the following bounds were applied on each coefficient to prevent convergence on unreasonable local minima: a [min(y) – min(y [8], y [9]), y [1] – min(y [8], y [9])], b [0, 5], and c [min(y [8], y [9]), max(y [8], y [9])]. We then repeated the ANCOVAs used in our primary statistical analyses (e.g., comparing D1-Pre to D1-Post with D1-Pre as a covariate) with the predicted error values and the coefficients. In the latter analysis of the coefficients, a + c was treated as the initial error and the covariate on each day, and was treated as the final error. To compare the learning rates (b) between groups, we also performed a one-way between-subjects ANOVAs on the values of b between groups on each day (note that ANOVA was used instead of ANCOVA for analysis of b because there is no appropriate covariate for this term). A significance level of α = 0.05 was used for all statistical analyses with appropriate post-hoc comparisons with Sidak adjustments.
Results
Day 1 online learning
Performance of a typical participant from each group is shown in Fig. 2, and average group performance on the tracking task across each block on days one and two is shown in Fig. 3. While there were no initial differences in tracking error between groups (t1,23 = 1.338; p = 0.194), there was a significant block × group interaction effect on the amount of online learning on Day 1 (F1,22 = 7.755; p = 0.011). Post-hoc analysis indicated that although both groups improved on tracking performance with practice on Day 1 (stroke: Δ = 3.2 ± 1.4%, p = 0.033; control: Δ = 8.4 ± 1.1%, p < 0.001; Figs. 3B and 4), the amount of tracking error at the end of practice on Day 1 was greater in stroke survivors when compared with the control group (19.6 ± 1.4% vs. 14.5 ± 1.1%, p = 0.033; Figs. 3B and 4). Note that the younger adult curve in Fig. 3 depicts data from a previous study using the same paradigm [31] to provide context in age-related effects on motor learning.
Day 2 online learning
There was a significant block × group interaction effect on the amount of online learning on Day 2 (F1,22 = 4.757; p = 0.040; Figs. 3B and 4). Post-hoc analysis indicated that although the control participants improved on tracking performance with practice on Day 2 (Δ = 4.4 ± 0.8%, p < 0.001; Figs. 3B and 4), the stroke participants did not (Δ = 1.5 ± 1.0%, p = 0.165; Figs. 3B and 4). The amount of tracking error at the end of practice on Day 2 was greater in stroke survivors when compared with the control group (16.3 ± 1.0% vs. 13.4 ± 0.8%, p = 0.046; Figs. 3B and 4).
Retention
Retention of performance of the tracking task is shown in Figs. 3C and 5A. There was a significant effect of block on the amount of retention after training (F1,22 = 5.403; p = 0.030). On average, there was a 5.0 ± 1.5% decrease in tracking error from D1-Pre to D2-Pre in stroke survivors and a 5.1 ± 1.9% decrease in tracking error from D1-Pre to D2-Pre in the control participants. However, there was no group or block × group interaction effect on the amount of retention after training.
Offline gains
Offline changes in the performance of the tracking task are shown in Figs. 3C and 5B. There was a significant effect of group on the amount of offline gains in motor performance (F1,23 = 7.602; p = 0.011). On average, stroke survivors experienced a 2.2 ± 1.6% reduction in tracking error from the end of Day 1 to the beginning of Day 2 (indicating offline gains), whereas control participants had a 3.6 ± 1.3% increase in tracking error during the same period (indicating offline loss).
Robustness check
Relative error check
There was a significant difference between groups in the amount of online learning on Day 1 (stroke: 83.6 ± 6.5%, control: 66.7 ± 4.8%, mean difference = 16.9 ± 7.5%, 95% bootstrapped confidence interval = 2.1–32.9%, p = 0.045), but the differences in the amount of online learning on Day 2 barely missed statistical significance (stroke: 93.2 ± 6.1%, control: 78.6 ± 4.4%, mean difference = 14.6 ± 7.4%, 95% bootstrapped confidence interval = − 0.1–29.1%, p = 0.066). There was also a significant difference between groups in the amount of offline learning (stroke: 92.7 ± 8.0%, control: 127.7 ± 8.1%, mean difference = − 35.1 ± 11.0%, 95% bootstrapped confidence interval = − 56.9–−12.9%, p = 0.007) but no difference between groups in the amount of retention (stroke: 75.6 ± 6.7%, control: 84.2 ± 7.4%, mean difference = − 8.6 ± 9.6%, 95% bootstrapped confidence interval = − 28.1–10.2%, p = 0.397).
Exponential-fit check
The results of the curve-fitting analysis were similar to those of the primary analysis and the first robustness check using relative changes (see Supplementary Material for quantitative results: https://osf.io/hzne3/). Specifically, we found that stroke survivors learned to a lesser extent than controls on Day 1 and Day 2 (Fig. 6(A-B)). There were no between-group differences in retention, but the stroke survivors had greater offline gains than controls (Fig. 6(C)). These robustness checks indicate that our findings were generally robust to different analyses, however the curve-fitting approach increased data variability, resulting in some metrics to fall below significance.
Discussion
The objective of this study was to examine the extent of motor learning deficits in stroke survivors with low impairment using a functional, lower-extremity task that focused on motor acuity during gait. We focused on motor acuity because the bulk of literature examining motor learning following stroke has focused on the stroke survivor’s ability to select the right action (e.g., amplitude, order in sequence) rather than their quality of movement execution (e.g., kinematics, variability) [23]. Furthermore, learning movement quality is critical for lower extremity tasks, especially in the context of rehabilitation where the quality of a stride is stressed rather than simply its completion [39]. Accordingly, stroke survivors and neurologically intact controls practiced walking on a treadmill with a new gait pattern in two separate sessions. To perform the new gait pattern, participants matched an ankle trajectory that necessitated 30% more hip and knee flexion during the swing phase. We found that stroke survivors showed a lower reduction in tracking error on both days (i.e., online learning deficits) when compared with the control group, who also are known to exhibit learning deficits due to the normal aging process (see Fig. 3 for comparison of both groups to cohort of young, uninjured controls taken from a previous study [31]). Another key finding was that stroke survivors showed offline performance gains between days while the control group showed offline performance loss, indicating that although stroke survivors have lower ability in online learning, they have an advantage in offline learning during a period of rest. Although our study was limited by a small sample size, our participants were quite homogenous in terms of impairment level, and our findings were consistent regardless of how the analyses were performed (i.e., absolute vs. relative learning), indicating the robustness of our results.
The first notable finding from this study is that although stroke survivors were able to learn the task and improve with practice, they showed less improvement in their tracking error as compared with the control group on both days. This finding indicates that stroke survivors have online motor learning deficits when compared with neurologically intact adults, which agrees with some prior studies [40, 41] but differs to some extent from other existing motor learning literature [16–19, 42]. It is likely that this divergence from previous literature results from our examination of mildly impaired individuals using a multi-DOF, functionally relevant motor acuity task during gait. For example, recent research suggests that reinforcement learning was impaired early after the stroke but not in the chronic phase, whereas error-based learning was unaffected after stroke at either time point when compared with controls [16]. However, our study indicates that online motor learning deficits are present even in chronic stroke survivors with minimal motor impairments when compared with controls. A key distinction between the two studies that could explain this discrepancy is the differences in the learning paradigm (skill learning in our study vs. visuomotor adaptation in the previous study). Further, gait is a highly practiced movement involving automatic processes such as balance and posture, and therefore invokes both conscious (e.g., corticospinal) and automatic (e.g., extrapyramidal) motor control pathways [43]. For participants, learning a novel gait pattern requires making conscious alterations to this highly practiced movement, which could influence both these conscious and automatic pathways. On the contrary, non-functional upper-extremity tasks are generally unpracticed motions that do not impact balance and posture and are therefore more likely governed primarily by the conscious motor control pathways [44]. Because these tasks likely require modulation of different motor control pathways, it is possible that they involve different learning mechanisms. Furthermore, even minimally impaired stroke survivors often have diminished balance that can influence their ability to prevent falls [45]. Therefore, it is possible that the observed learning deficits arose from stroke survivors’ resistance to deviate from their current gait pattern and balance on one leg to perform the necessary exploration of the motor control task space, which is necessary to learn a motor skill [46].
It is possible that some of the observed learning deficits could be attributed to stroke characteristics. For example, lesions in the prefrontal cortex could have a greater impact during early stages of learning and on motor consolidation processes [47, 48]. Similarly, lesions involving the extrapyramidal systems (e.g., basal ganglia) can disrupt implicit and explicit learning processes [47]. Unfortunately, lesion location information was not evaluated for this study, therefore it is difficult to determine if the observed results could be attributed to damage to specific pathways. Furthermore, other factors that interfere with performance, such as fatigue, differences in preferred gait speed, and affected side, could have contributed to this group difference in learning. However, these factors most likely had a minimal impact because of the following: (1) participants walked at a slow speed and received adequate rest between blocks to prevent fatigue, (2) treadmill speed was sufficiently lower than preferred gait speed for both groups and stroke participants’ gait speed was comparable to controls (Table 1 [49]), and (3) our previous studies with this paradigm have shown that learning is not affected by limb dominance [32]. Future studies should examine stroke-related learning deficits in walking tasks to fully assess the extent of stroke’s effect on learning.
Another interesting finding was that despite the deficits in online learning observed in stroke survivors, we did not detect any differences in skill retention between the groups. This occurred because of the differences in offline learning between groups—stroke survivors exhibited offline performance gains whereas the control group exhibited an offline performance loss. This finding aligns with previous research, highlighting the complex interplay between neurological damage and motor learning processes post-stroke [12, 50]. One potential mechanism that could explain decreased ‘online’ learning is ‘reactive inhibition’— the process where performance worsens over repeated executions of the task and improves with breaks [51]. Stroke survivors could exhibit increased reactive inhibition, which interferes in subsequent target-matching and thus decreases their online gains, but when the reactive inhibition dissipates, they “catch up” with the other group. This is typically observed in “massed” vs. “distributed” practice effects – the massed group shows poor online learning but then huge gains over the break [51]. To examine this issue further, we performed an exploratory analysis to determine if stroke survivors also demonstrated offline learning advantages over shorter rest periods. To address this, we computed micro-offline learning for each rest period, defined as the difference between the average tracking error on the first five strides of a target-matching block and the last five strides of the preceding block [52, 53]. Interestingly, there did not appear to be any systematic difference between groups in micro-offline learning (Fig. 7), suggesting that offline learning advantages only appear following longer periods of rest that include sleep.
In our study, it is possible that offline learning could be treated as a measure of sleep-dependent memory consolidation —a process of the central nervous system where recent memory traces are committed to long-term memory during rest—although this should be verified with future experiments with another group that undergoes matched intervals of rest without sleep [54]. Existing research examining this process has shown that stroke survivors demonstrate greater motor performance in upper extremity motor tasks following a period of sleep as compared with an equivalent period of wakefulness, but the same is not true for neurologically intact individuals [27, 28, 48, 55, 56]. Our results directly align with these prior findings and extend them to a functional lower-extremity task. This phenomenon may reflect heightened neural plasticity or alternative neural pathways recruited to compensate for damaged regions wherein post-stroke plasticity facilitates continued skill acquisition (or retention of skills acquired) during rest. Importantly, these findings indicate that stroke-induced neuroplastic changes can also lead to functionally beneficial adaptations apart from the commonly recognized maladaptive processes. Indeed, the observed online learning deficits in conjunction with the offline learning gains could evidence the detriments and benefits of the same repair process; albeit the precise mechanisms underlying these beneficial adaptations are not clear.
Several additional mechanisms could potentially explain why individuals with stroke may exhibit enhanced offline motor learning compared with older controls who do not demonstrate the same phenomenon. First, the brain undergoes significant neuroplastic changes after a stroke as it attempts to reorganize and compensate for the damaged areas [22]. It is possible that these neuroplastic changes enhance the brain’s ability to consolidate and retain motor memories during rest or sleep, leading to improved offline motor learning [50]. Second, stroke survivors often develop compensatory mechanisms (e.g., increased reliance on the premotor cortex or other undamaged regions) to overcome motor deficits [57, 58]. During rest periods, these compensatory mechanisms may be reinforced or optimized, resulting in enhanced offline motor learning [59]. Older controls, who have not experienced neurological damage, may lack the same need for compensatory mechanisms, and therefore, do not exhibit the same enhancement in offline motor learning. Third, stroke can disrupt normal sleep architecture, leading to alterations in sleep stages and patterns [60, 61]. Some studies suggest that certain sleep stages, particularly REM and slow-wave sleep, are crucial for motor memory consolidation [27, 62]. Changes in sleep architecture or enhanced sleep due to pharmacological effects (e.g., gabapentin improves slow wave sleep [63] and total sleep time [64]) post-stroke may create a more conducive environment for offline motor learning compared with older controls. Indeed, the stroke survivors who participated in our study reported superior sleep quality as compared to the controls (Table 1). Finally, aging is associated with decreased neuroplasticity due to alterations in GABAergic activity [65–67], which may impact the brain’s ability to learn and consolidate motor memories during sleep and may be offset by neuroplastic mechanisms following the stroke. It is to be noted though that the differences in online and offline learning observed in this study occurred despite participants being in the chronic period of stroke recovery (i.e., when changes in neuroplasticity are believed to have plateaued), indicating that the enhancement in offline learning could be a sustained phenomenon. Overall, one or many of the above mechanisms likely contributed to the observed differences in offline motor learning between individuals with stroke and older controls. However, it is important to note that retention was only evaluated after one day, so it is unclear if offline learning would have led to improved retention following the second day of training. Therefore, further research is needed to fully elucidate the underlying mechanisms and their implications for stroke rehabilitation.
The results of this study have meaningful implications for post-stroke gait rehabilitation. First, we found that stroke survivors learned to a lesser extent than neurologically intact individuals over the same period. Therefore, it is likely that, when learning or relearning a motor skill following a stroke, stroke survivors will need more practice to achieve similar performance levels as uninjured controls. This would also imply that interventions involving motor learning in stroke survivors should incorporate longer training than in neurologically intact adults. Additionally, we also found that stroke survivors demonstrate learning advantages over uninjured persons over a period of rest involving sleep. Therefore, interventions involving motor learning could likely improve outcomes if they include breaks between sessions where stroke survivors have the opportunity to rest and consolidate learning from practice. However, future research is necessary to fully elucidate the extent of learning deficits in stroke and how sleep can facilitate learning.
Conclusions
In summary, we investigated differences in learning a functional lower extremity motor skill between mildly impaired stroke survivors and neurologically intact individuals. We found that neurologically intact individuals showed greater motor performance with practice as compared to stroke survivors, but stroke survivors showed greater offline learning than neurologically intact individuals. These findings lend important insights into how stroke affects the learning process and may have potential implications for gait rehabilitation after stroke.
Acknowledgements
Not applicable.
Abbreviations
- DOF
Degree of Freedom
- D1
Day 1
- D2
Day 2
- Tr
Training
Author contributions
The authors confirm contribution to the paper as follows: Study conception and design: CK, RR, EPW; Data Collection: EPW, CRH; Analysis and interpretation of data: CK, TEA, RR; ESC, EPW, CRH; Major contributors to writing the manuscript and preparing figures: CK, TEA; Critically reviewing and editing for important intellectual content: RR, EPW, ESC, CRH. All authors reviewed the results, approved the final version of the manuscript, and agreed to be personally accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.
Funding
This work was partly supported by the National Institutes of Health (R41-HD111289) and the National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship Program (DGE #1256260).
Data availability
No datasets were generated or analysed during the current study.
Declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
All participants provided written consent to participation and the study conducted in accordance with the Belmont Report and the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the University of Michigan’s Institutional Review Board of the Medical School (IRBMED).
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Thomas E. Augenstein co-first author.
References
- 1.World Health Organization. The world health report 2002: reducing risks, promoting healthy life. World Health Organization; 2002.
- 2.Feigin VL, Brainin M, Norrving B, Martins S, Sacco RL, Hacke W, et al. World Stroke Organization (WSO): global stroke fact sheet 2022. Int J Stroke. 2022;17(1):18–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Wist S, Clivaz J, Sattelmayer M. Muscle strengthening for hemiparesis after stroke: a meta-analysis. Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2016;59(2):114–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Dewald JP, Pope PS, Given JD, Buchanan TS, Rymer WZ. Abnormal muscle coactivation patterns during isometric torque generation at the elbow and shoulder in hemiparetic subjects. Brain. 1995;118(Pt 2):495–510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Cruz TH, Dhaher YY. Evidence of abnormal lower-limb torque coupling after stroke: an isometric study. Stroke. 2008;39(1):139–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Tyson SF, Hanley M, Chillala J, Selley A, Tallis RC. Balance disability after stroke. Phys Ther. 2006;86(1):30–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Winstein CJ, Stein J, Arena R, Bates B, Cherney LR, Cramer SC, et al. Guidelines for adult Stroke Rehabilitation and Recovery: a Guideline for Healthcare professionals from the American Heart Association/American Stroke Association. Stroke. 2016;47(6):e98–169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Calabro RS, Cacciola A, Berte F, Manuli A, Leo A, Bramanti A, et al. Robotic gait rehabilitation and substitution devices in neurological disorders: where are we now? Neurol Sci. 2016;37(4):503–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Gibbons EM, Thomson AN, de Noronha M, Joseph S. Are virtual reality technologies effective in improving lower limb outcomes for patients following stroke - a systematic review with meta-analysis. Top Stroke Rehabil. 2016;23(6):440–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Mehrholz J, Thomas S, Kugler J, Pohl M, Elsner B. Electromechanical-assisted training for walking after stroke. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2020;10(10):CD006185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Hwang S, Song CS. Assistive Technology Involving Postural Control and Gait performance for adults with stroke: a systematic review and Meta-analysis. Healthc (Basel). 2023;11:15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Winstein C, Lewthwaite R, Blanton SR, Wolf LB, Wishart L. Infusing motor learning research into neurorehabilitation practice: a historical perspective with case exemplar from the accelerated skill acquisition program. J Neurol Phys Ther. 2014;38(3):190–200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Leech KA, Roemmich RT, Gordon J, Reisman DS, Cherry-Allen KM. Updates in Motor Learning: implications for physical therapist practice and education. Phys Ther. 2022;102(1). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 14.Ranganathan R, Doherty C, Gussert M, Kaplinski E, Koje M, Krishnan C. Scientific basis and active ingredients of current therapeutic interventions for stroke rehabilitation. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2022;40(2):97–107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Levin MF, Demers M. Motor learning in neurological rehabilitation. Disabil Rehabil. 2021;43(24):3445–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Branscheidt M, Hadjiosif AM, Anaya MA, Keller J, Widmer M, Runnalls KD et al. Reinforcement Learning Is Impaired in the Sub-acute Post-stroke Period. bioRxiv. 2023.
- 17.Riga A, Gathy E, Ghinet M, De Laet C, Bihin B, Regnier M, et al. Evidence of Motor Skill Learning in Acute Stroke patients without lesions to the Thalamus and Internal Capsule. Stroke. 2022;53(7):2361–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Baguma M, Yeganeh Doost M, Riga A, Laloux P, Bihin B, Vandermeeren Y. Preserved motor skill learning in acute stroke patients. Acta Neurol Belg. 2020;120(2):365–74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Winstein CJ, Merians AS, Sullivan KJ. Motor learning after unilateral brain damage. Neuropsychologia. 1999;37(8):975–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Gerardin E, Bontemps D, Babuin NT, Herman B, Denis A, Bihin B, et al. Bimanual motor skill learning with robotics in chronic stroke: comparison between minimally impaired and moderately impaired patients, and healthy individuals. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2022;19(1):28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Wong AL, Jax SA, Smith LL, Buxbaum LJ, Krakauer JW. Movement Imitation via an Abstract trajectory representation in dorsal Premotor Cortex. J Neurosci. 2019;39(17):3320–31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Krakauer JW, Carmichael ST. Broken movement: the neurobiology of motor recovery after stroke. MIT Press; 2022.
- 23.Krakauer JW, Hadjiosif AM, Xu J, Wong AL, Haith AM. Motor Learning. Compr Physiol. 2019;9(2):613–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Krakauer JW. Motor learning: its relevance to stroke recovery and neurorehabilitation. Curr Opin Neurol. 2006;19(1):84–90. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Dayan E, Cohen LG. Neuroplasticity subserving motor skill learning. Neuron. 2011;72(3):443–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Siengsukon C, Boyd LA. Sleep enhances off-line spatial and temporal motor learning after stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2009;23(4):327–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Siengsukon CF, Boyd LA. Sleep enhances implicit motor skill learning in individuals poststroke. Top Stroke Rehabil. 2008;15(1):1–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Siengsukon CF, Boyd LA. Sleep to learn after stroke: implicit and explicit off-line motor learning. Neurosci Lett. 2009;451(1):1–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Sulzer JS, Gordon KE, Dhaher YY, Peshkin MA, Patton JL. Preswing knee flexion assistance is coupled with hip abduction in people with stiff-knee gait after stroke. Stroke. 2010;41(8):1709–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Krishnan C, Ranganathan R, Dhaher YY, Rymer WZ. A pilot study on the feasibility of robot-aided leg motor training to facilitate active participation. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(10):e77370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Krishnan C, Washabaugh EP, Reid CE, Althoen MM, Ranganathan R. Learning new gait patterns: age-related differences in skill acquisition and interlimb transfer. Exp Gerontol. 2018;111:45–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Krishnan C, Ranganathan R, Tetarbe M. Interlimb transfer of motor skill learning during walking: no evidence for asymmetric transfer. Gait Posture. 2017;56:24–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Woytowicz EJ, Rietschel JC, Goodman RN, Conroy SS, Sorkin JD, Whitall J, et al. Determining levels of Upper Extremity Movement Impairment by applying a cluster analysis to the Fugl-Meyer Assessment of the Upper Extremity in Chronic Stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2017;98(3):456–62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Krishnan C. Learning and interlimb transfer of new gait patterns are facilitated by distributed practice across days. Gait Posture. 2019;70:84–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Krishnan C, Dharia AK, Augenstein TE, Washabaugh EP, Reid CE, Brown SR, et al. Learning new gait patterns is enhanced by specificity of training rather than progression of task difficulty. J Biomech. 2019;88:33–7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Krishnan C, Washabaugh EP, Seetharaman Y. A low cost real-time motion tracking approach using webcam technology. J Biomech. 2015;48(3):544–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Augenstein TE, Remy CD, Claflin ES, Ranganathan R, Krishnan C. Teaching motor skills without a motor: a semi-passive robot to facilitate learning. IEEE Trans Haptics. 2023;17(3). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 38.Ranganathan R, Lee MH, Krishnan C. Ten guidelines for designing motor learning studies. Braz J Mot Behav. 2022;16(2):112–33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Harvey RL, Macko RF, Stein J, Winstein CJ, Phd P, Zorowitz RD. Stroke recovery and rehabilitation: Demos Medical Publishing; 2008.
- 40.Vidoni ED, Boyd LA. Preserved motor learning after stroke is related to the degree of proprioceptive deficit. Behav Brain Funct. 2009;5:36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Wadden KP, Asis K, Mang CS, Neva JL, Peters S, Lakhani B, et al. Predicting Motor sequence learning in individuals with chronic stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2017;31(1):95–104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Russo C, Veronelli L, Casati C, Monti A, Perucca L, Ferraro F, et al. Explicit motor sequence learning after stroke: a neuropsychological study. Exp Brain Res. 2021;239(7):2303–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Takakusaki K. Functional neuroanatomy for posture and Gait Control. J Mov Disord. 2017;10(1):1–17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Rong D, Zhang M, Ma Q, Lu J, Li K. Corticospinal tract change during motor recovery in patients with medulla infarct: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Biomed Res Int. 2014;2014:524096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Krasovsky T, Lamontagne A, Feldman AG, Levin MF. Reduced gait stability in high-functioning poststroke individuals. J Neurophysiol. 2013;109(1):77–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Lokesh R, Ranganathan R. Differential control of task and null space variability in response to changes in task difficulty when learning a bimanual steering task. Exp Brain Res. 2019;237(4):1045–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Dahms C, Brodoehl S, Witte OW, Klingner CM. The importance of different learning stages for motor sequence learning after stroke. Hum Brain Mapp. 2020;41(1):270–86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Gomez Beldarrain M, Astorgano AG, Gonzalez AB, Garcia-Monco JC. Sleep improves sequential motor learning and performance in patients with prefrontal lobe lesions. Clin Neurol Neurosurg. 2008;110(3):245–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Kasovic M, Stefan L, Stefan A. Normative data for Gait Speed and Height Norm Speed in >/= 60-Year-old men and women. Clin Interv Aging. 2021;16:225–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Backhaus W, Kempe S, Hummel FC. The effect of sleep on motor learning in the aging and stroke population - a systematic review. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2016;34(1):153–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Gupta MW, Rickard TC. Dissipation of reactive inhibition is sufficient to explain post-rest improvements in motor sequence learning. NPJ Sci Learn. 2022;7(1):25. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Bonstrup M, Iturrate I, Thompson R, Cruciani G, Censor N, Cohen LG. A Rapid form of offline consolidation in Skill Learning. Curr Biol. 2019;29(8):1346–51. e4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Cubillos LH, Augenstein TE, Ranganathan R, Krishnan C. Breaking the barriers to designing online experiments: a novel open-source platform for supporting procedural skill learning experiments. Comput Biol Med. 2023;154:106627. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Born J, Wilhelm I. System consolidation of memory during sleep. Psychol Res. 2012;76(2):192–203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Backhaus W, Braass H, Gerloff C, Hummel FC. Can Daytime Napping assist the process of skills Acquisition after Stroke? Front Neurol. 2018;9:1002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Brearley DJ. The potential role of Sleep in Post-stroke Motor Learning. Int Neuropsychiatric Disease J. 2023;20(3):93–100. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Johansen-Berg H, Rushworth MF, Bogdanovic MD, Kischka U, Wimalaratna S, Matthews PM. The role of ipsilateral premotor cortex in hand movement after stroke. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99(22):14518–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Miyai I, Yagura H, Oda I, Konishi I, Eda H, Suzuki T, et al. Premotor cortex is involved in restoration of gait in stroke. Ann Neurol. 2002;52(2):188–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Kantak SS, Stinear JW, Buch ER, Cohen LG. Rewiring the brain: potential role of the premotor cortex in motor control, learning, and recovery of function following brain injury. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2012;26(3):282–92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Gottselig JM, Bassetti CL, Achermann P. Power and coherence of sleep spindle frequency activity following hemispheric stroke. Brain. 2002;125(Pt 2):373–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Vock J, Achermann P, Bischof M, Milanova M, Muller C, Nirkko A, et al. Evolution of sleep and sleep EEG after hemispheric stroke. J Sleep Res. 2002;11(4):331–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Pace M, Camilo MR, Seiler A, Duss SB, Mathis J, Manconi M et al. Rapid eye movements sleep as a predictor of functional outcome after stroke: a translational study. Sleep. 2018;41(10). [DOI] [PubMed]
- 63.Foldvary-Schaefer N, De Leon Sanchez I, Karafa M, Mascha E, Dinner D, Morris HH. Gabapentin increases slow-wave sleep in normal adults. Epilepsia. 2002;43(12):1493–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Rosenberg RP, Hull SG, Lankford DA, Mayleben DW, Seiden DJ, Furey SA, et al. A randomized, double-blind, single-dose, placebo-controlled, multicenter, polysomnographic study of gabapentin in transient insomnia induced by sleep phase advance. J Clin Sleep Med. 2014;10(10):1093–100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Rozycka A, Liguz-Lecznar M. The space where aging acts: focus on the GABAergic synapse. Aging Cell. 2017;16(4):634–43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Blicher JU, Near J, Naess-Schmidt E, Stagg CJ, Johansen-Berg H, Nielsen JF, et al. GABA levels are decreased after stroke and GABA changes during rehabilitation correlate with motor improvement. Neurorehabil Neural Repair. 2015;29(3):278–86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Paparella I, Vanderwalle G, Stagg CJ, Maquet P. An integrated measure of GABA to characterize post-stroke plasticity. Neuroimage Clin. 2023;39:103463. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
No datasets were generated or analysed during the current study.