Skip to main content
Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. Revue Canadienne de Psychiatrie logoLink to Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. Revue Canadienne de Psychiatrie
. 2024 Nov 4;70(3):229–239. doi: 10.1177/07067437241293985

Formal Thought Disorders and Neurocognition in Treatment-Resistant Schizophrenia: Trouble du cours de la pensée et neurocognition dans la schizophrénie réfractaire

Mohammed Alarabi 1,2, Leah Burton 1, Valerie Powell 1, Tanner Isinger 1, Sri Mahavir Agarwal 1,3,4,*,, Gary Remington 1,3,4,5,*,✉,
PMCID: PMC11562935  PMID: 39497431

Abstract

Objective

Formal thought disorders (FTDs), a core feature of schizophrenia, have been subdivided into positive and negative types, and are clinically assessed by examining speech (objective) or patient introspection (subjective). Despite being associated with poorer treatment response and worse outcomes, FTDs have been understudied in patients with schizophrenia, in particular treatment-resistant schizophrenia (TRS) or schizoaffective disorder. We aimed to explore the relationship between the severity of positive and negative FTDs and neurocognition as well as social/occupational functioning in this clinical subgroup.

Method

This was a retrospective chart review conducted at the Clozapine Clinic at the Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Toronto, Canada. We reviewed charted standardized assessment of FTDs using the Thought and Language Disorder (TALD) scale, neurocognition using the Brief Cognitive Assessment Tool for Schizophrenia (B-CATS), and functioning using the Social and Occupational Functioning Assessment Scale (SOFAS) between October 2022 and June 2023. Following the original factor structure of the TALD, we computed 4- factor scores that combined positive or negative and objective or subjective FTDs. We then explored the correlation between the scores from each TALD factor and the neurocognition and functioning scores.

Results

We analysed data for 23 outpatients on clozapine. After the Bonferroni adjustment, total TALD scores, indicating overall severity of FTDs, were strongly and inversely correlated with SOFAS scores (p < 0.001). A strong inverse correlation was found between the objective positive TALD factor and Letter-Number Span verbal working memory scores, r(21) = −0.63, p < 0.001.

Conclusions

Our results demonstrate the strong relationship between FTDs, neurocognition, and social/occupational functioning in a sample of TRS outpatients. Within the cognitive domains assessed, verbal working memory impairment had the strongest correlation with positive FTDs, such as derailment or tangentiality. These findings highlight the value of employing standardized psychopathological scales for FTDs in clinical practice.

Keywords: schizophrenia, psychopathology, neurocognition, formal thought disorder

Introduction

Formal thought disorders (FTDs) comprise a group of abnormalities in thought form, language production, and communication.1,2 Objective FTDs are observable on clinical examination of speech, distinguishing them from subjective FTDs, which are disturbances in thought flow and communication experienced subjectively by the individual and reported to the examiner.3,4 FTDs have been further subdivided into positive and negative. Positive FTDs include derailment, tangentiality, and pressured speech, indicating an excess or distortion of thought processes and speech. Conversely, negative FTDs indicate a deficit in thought and speech production, as represented by poverty of speech, slowed thinking, or blocking.5,6 These psychopathologies are thought to be integral to the clinical syndrome of schizophrenia.79 FTDs can also be found in other neuropsychiatric syndromes, including mania and epilepsy; however, they tend to be more persistent in patients with schizophrenia.10,11 In addition, the presence and severity of FTDs may be predictive of treatment-resistant schizophrenia (TRS)1214 TRS describes a subgroup of patients with schizophrenia who continue to suffer from positive symptoms after 2 or more trials of antipsychotic medications, 15 and has been proposed as a distinct subtype of schizophrenia.16,17 Patients with TRS have been found to have more severe neurocognitive deficits compared to other patients with schizophrenia. 18

The cognitive mechanism underlying FTDs is not fully understood. Nevertheless, it has been hypothesized that FTDs may be a manifestation of neurocognitive deficits which are commonly found in patients with schizophrenia.19,20 Both executive dysfunction, which impacts high-order cognition such as working memory, and semantic network impairment, which influences aspects of verbal fluency, have been linked to the etiology of FTDs in schizophrenia.21,22 Furthermore, it has been found that performance on executive functioning tests can better differentiate schizophrenia patients with and without FTDs compared to their performance on semantic tests. 23 These findings support the role of neurocognitive mechanisms in FTDs, particularly the hypothesis that impairments in executive functions that govern planning, editing, and monitoring of thoughts underlie the pathogenesis of clinically assessed FTDs. 24

Given the paucity of literature on the relationship between FTDs and neurocognitive measures in TRS, and the association of TRS with more severe FTDs and neurocognitive deficits, we aimed to investigate this relationship in this clinical population. Accordingly, our primary objective was to explore the relationship between the severity of positive and negative, objective and subjective, FTDs and neuropsychological test scores in patients with TRS. Our secondary objective was to investigate the relationship between FTD severity and social as well as occupational functioning.

Methods

This was a retrospective chart review that received approval from The Centre for Addiction and Mental Health (CAMH) Research Ethics Board on November 6, 2023. The review included patients in the Clozapine Clinic at CAMH, Toronto, Canada, who have undergone an assessment of FTDs and neuropsychological testing as part of their outpatient care during the period from October 1, 2022, until June 1, 2023. Consent for data collection was not obtained from the participants as the collected data were anonymized and had been charted as part of routine clinical care. The waiver of consent was approved by the CAMH Research Ethics Board. Psychopathology and functional assessments were completed as a component of good clinical practice by the covering psychiatrists (MA and reviewed with SMA) whereas the neuropsychological tests were completed independently by trained nursing staff in the clinic (LB and VP). The data were collected from the patient's electronic medical records and included clinico-demographic characteristics (e.g., sex, age, and diagnosis) in addition to psychopathological, neuropsychological, and functional assessment scores.

Psychopathology Assessment

FTD severity is commonly assessed using clinician-rated scales. 25 Most rating scales, however, do not adequately address subjectively reported FTDs, which constitute an important but neglected aspect of FTDs.26,27 Kircher et al. 28 reviewed the literature and existing FTD scales, and with this as background developed the 30-item Thought and Language Disorder (TALD) scale to introduce a more comprehensive tool for the assessment of objectively observed and subjectively reported FTDs. 28 The TALD is a reliable and valid tool for the assessment of FTDs and correlates strongly with other measures of schizophrenia psychopathology including other scales that measure FTDs. Due to these advantages, the scale was adopted to assess FTD severity in our Clozapine Clinic. The scale was completed for each patient following at least a 50 min psychiatric interview during which psychopathologies were explored and clarified. Following the scale manual, objective FTDs were rated based on their presence and impact on communication during the interview. Subjective FTDs, on the other hand, were explored using semistructured questions to determine their presence and severity during the preceding 24 h based on the associated distress. Each item on the scale was given a score: 0 not present, 1 doubtful, 2 mild, 3 moderate, and 4 severe according to the scale manual. In addition, all included patients were assessed using the 24-item Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale Expanded Version (BPRS-E), 29 utilizing the anchor scores, which assign a score for reported and observed psychopathologies between 1 and 7. 30

Neurocognition and Social/Occupational Functioning Assessment

Neurocognition was assessed by the Letter-Number Span test for verbal working memory, semantic (categorical) and phonological (letter) fluency tasks for verbal fluency, and the symbol coding test for cognitive processing speed. For the verbal fluency tasks, the category used was animals, and the letters were “F” and “S.” These neuropsychological tests are part of the Brief Cognitive Assessment Tool for Schizophrenia (B-CATS),31,32 which explores similar domains to and correlates highly with the MATRICS Consensus Cognitive Battery, 33 considered to be the gold-standard assessment tool of neurocognition in schizophrenia. Social and occupational functioning was assessed using the Social and Occupational Functioning Assessment Scale (SOFAS). 34

Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

Inclusion criteria included being able to communicate in English, age 18 to 65, capacity to undergo neuropsychological testing, and a clinical diagnosis of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edition, with treatment resistance to 2 or more antipsychotics documented on clinical charts. Exclusion criteria included a diagnosis of moderate or severe intellectual disability, major or minor neurocognitive disorder such as Alzheimer's or unspecified dementia, and moderate to severe substance use disorders. Patients for whom neurocognitive assessment using the B-CATS was not documented within 3 months of FTD assessment were excluded. All but 2 of the included patients underwent neuropsychological testing on the same visit FTDs were assessed.

Data Analysis

Statistical analysis was conducted using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS), Version 21.0. 35 Descriptive data were obtained through the means (M), standard deviations (SD), and frequencies (f). Medians (Mdn) and interquartile ranges (IQRs) were used for nonnormally distributed variables. Correlations were estimated using Pearson's correlation coefficient (r) for normally distributed continuous variables and the nonparametric Spearman's rank correlation coefficient (rs) for nonnormally distributed continuous variables. We followed the original factor structure of the TALD in computing the factor scores. 28 This factor structure included 4 factors: objective positive (15 items), objective negative (3 items), subjective positive (2 items), and subjective negative (7 items) FTDs. Each factor's relative score was obtained by dividing the sum of the individual scores by the number of items in each factor. 36 These factor scores have been utilized in other studies using the TALD.37,38 The internal consistency reliability of the factors was estimated by Cronbach's alpha using SPSS. Cronbach's alpha ranges from 0 to 1 with higher values indicating stronger relationships between the included items in each factor. The total BPRS-E score was calculated by adding the scores of the individual 24 items. As for the B-CATS, a total score was obtained by summing the score of the Letter-Number Span, Semantic (categorical) Verbal Fluency, Phonological Verbal Fluency (letter F), Phonological Verbal Fluency (letter S), and Symbol Coding test scores. Total Verbal Fluency was calculated by summing the 3 semantic and phonological fluency scores. To account for multiple bivariate analyses, a Bonferroni adjustment was applied to the level of significance to yield a level of significance when p < 0.0017 (0.05 out of 30).

Results

Clinico-Demographics

Out of 126 patients enrolled in the Clozapine Clinic, 23 patients met the inclusion criteria, having undergone the standardized assessment during the period from October 1, 2022, until June 1, 2023. Clinico-demographic data are summarized in Table 1. There were more male than female patients in our sample and the patients’ age ranged from 23 to 65 years. All patients in the sample were prescribed clozapine, and the majority were receiving clozapine monotherapy or cotreatment with antidepressants for depressive, anxiety, or obsessive–compulsive symptoms. None of the patients were receiving an adjunct Long-Acting Injectable antipsychotic.

Table 1.

Clinico-Demographics and Descriptive Analysis of Psychopathological, Neurocognitive, and Functional Assessment Scores (N = 23).

Characteristics and assessment measures
Sex
Male (f) 17
Female (f) 6
Age (M ± SD) 42.30 ± 11.32 years
Diagnosis
Schizophrenia (f) 19
Schizoaffective disorder (f) 4
Treatment regimen
Clozapine daily dose (M ± SD) 423.91 ± 172.80 mg/day
Clozapine monotherapy (f) 12
Adjunct antidepressant (f) 8
Adjunct mood stabilizer (f) 6
Adjunct antipsychotic (f) 2
Adjunct benzodiazepine (f) 4
Psychopathology (M ± SD)
TALD total score 23.65 ± 11.25
TALD objective positive factor (relative score) 0.79 ± 0.41
TALD objective negative factor (relative score) 1.16 ± 0.90
TALD subjective positive factor (relative score) 0.50 ± 1.50 a
TALD subjective negative Factor (relative score) 0.92 ± 0.69
BPRS-E total score 51.70 ± 11.62
Neurocognition
B-CATS total score 93.65 ± 24.27
B-CATS Letter-Number Span 9.91 ± 4.48
B-CATS semantic (categorical) verbal fluency 19.04 ± 6.34
B-CATS phonological verbal fluency (letter F) 11.70 ± 4.90
B-CATS phonological verbal fluency (letter S) 11.22 ± 4.50
B-CATS total verbal fluency 41.96 ± 13.10
B-CATS symbol coding 41.78 ± 12.44
Functioning
SOFAS 46.83 ± 16.69

Note. TALD = Thought and Language Disorder Scale; BPRS-E = Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale Expanded Version; B-CATS = Brief Cognitive Assessment Tool for Schizophrenia; SOFAS = Social and Occupational Functioning Assessment Scale. aMedian ± interquartile range.

FTDs Assessment

We analysed the frequency of individual TALD items in our sample. The most frequently encountered FTDs as mild, moderate, or severe were “poverty of content of speech” (f = 17) followed by “poverty of speech” and “tangentiality” (both f = 13). Clanging and echolalia were not encountered at all and were therefore excluded from the bivariate analysis along with verbigeration, which was only encountered in 1 patient. Figure 1 illustrates the frequency of formal thought and language disorders scored as mild, moderate, or severe according to the scale manual and grouped based on the factor structure of the TALD.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Frequency of formal thought and language disorders by Thought and Language Disorder Scale (TALD) items (N = 23): The TALD items have been grouped into 4 categories based on the factor structure of the scale. 28 Verbigeration, clanging, and echolalia have been excluded.

Psychopathological Assessment Scores

Table 1 shows the descriptive analysis of psychopathological (TALD and BPRS-E), neurocognitive (B-CATS), and functional (SOFAS) assessment scores. The total TALD score provided an assessment of the general severity of FTDs for each patient. The total score ranged from 10 to 54 points. Estimating the internal consistency of the total TALD scale yielded a Cronbach's alpha of 0.78. Using the factor structure of the TALD, we calculated the relative score of positive and negative, objective and subjective, FTDs as 4 distinct factor scores. The Cronbach's alpha was 0.65 for the objective positive factor, 0.57 for the objective negative, 0.79 for the subjective positive, and 0.71 for the subjective negative factor. All factors followed a normal distribution except for the subjective positive TALD factor. The highest average relative scores were for the objective negative factor of the TALD, followed by the subjective negative factor (Table 1). The TALD total score and relative factor scores for each patient are detailed in Appendix 1. Male patients averaged higher scores on the subjective positive factor than female patients (t(20.99) = 2.92, p = 0.008). Otherwise, male and female patients had comparable scores on the TALD and its factors (p > 0.05). Total BPRS-E scores ranged from 32 to 74. Male patients had more severe psychopathologies, as assessed by the total BPRS-E (M = 55.12, SD = 10.82), compared to female patients (M = 42.00, SD = 8.17), t(21)  = 2.70, p = 0.014. There were no significant differences in psychopathology scores between patients on different drug regimens (p > 0.05).

Neurocognitive and Functional Assessment Scores

For neurocognitive measures, the minimum B-CATS total score was 44 and the maximum was 153. We did not find any differences between male and female patients on any of the B-CATS measures (p > 0.05). There was an inverse trend between age and the Letter-Number Span score, r(21) = −0.52, p = 0.011. The level of functioning, as scored by the SOFAS, ranged from 75 (indicating some difficulty in social, occupational, or school functioning but generally functioning well) to a score of 20 (which indicates inability to function independently and occasionally failing to maintain minimal personal hygiene). We did not find a difference between male and female patients in SOFAS scores (p > 0.05).

Bivariate Analysis

The bivariate analysis is detailed in Table 2, which shows the correlation coefficients of psychopathological, neurocognitive, and functional assessment scores in our sample. Overall, more severe FTDs, as scored by the TALD total score, were correlated with lower total B-CATS scores. After the Bonferroni adjustment, however, the only significant correlation between TALD factors and neuropsychological test scores was found for the objective positive factor and the Letter-Number Span verbal working memory, r(21) =  −0.63, p < 0.001. Figure 2A shows a scatterplot of objective positive TALD factor scores by the Letter-Number Span verbal working memory scores, demonstrating the strong inverse correlation. More severe psychopathology, as assessed by total BPRS-E scores, was strongly and inversely correlated with both total B-CATS scores and Symbol Coding processing speed scores. Higher TALD and BPRS-E total scores were both strongly correlated with lower functional assessment scores. Figure 2B illustrates the relationship between total TALD scores and SOFAS scores.

Table 2.

Correlation Coefficient Matrix Between Formal Thought and Language Disorders, Neurocognition, and Functional Assessment Scores:.

Psychopathology Neurocognition (B-CATS) Functional assessment
B-CATS Total Score Letter-Number Span Total Verbal Fluency Symbol Coding SOFAS
TALD
 Total score –.530* –.563* –.341 –.473* .629**
 Objective positive –.467* .631** –.295 –.412 –.591*
 Objective negative –.259 –.128 –.276 –.168 –.294
 Subjective positive a –.329 –.161 –.194 –.362 –.208
 Subjective negative –.282 –.290 –.160 –.277 –.370
BPRS-E
 Total score .631** –.553* –.372 .640** .682**

Note. Pearson's correlation coefficient unless otherwise specified. aSpearman's correlation coefficient, *significant at p < 0.05, **significant at p < 0.0017 (Bonferroni-adjusted). TALD = Thought and Language Disorder Scale; BPRS-E = Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale Expanded Version; B-CATS = Brief Cognitive Assessment Tool for Schizophrenia; SOFAS = Social and Occupational Functioning Assessment Scale.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Relationship between formal thought and language disorders, verbal working memory, and functional assessment scores. (A) Scatterplot of objective positive TALD factor scores by the B-CATS Letter-Number Span verbal working memory test scores. (B) Scatterplot of total TALD scores by SOFAS scores.

Note. TALD = Thought and Language Disorder Scale; B-CATS = Brief Cognitive Assessment Tool for Schizophrenia; SOFAS = Social and Occupational Functioning Assessment Scale.

Discussion

This chart review examined the relationship between FTD severity and neurocognitive measures in a sample of outpatients with TRS. Our results demonstrate the relationship between neurocognition and severity of FTDs in schizophrenia. We found a strong inverse correlation between verbal working memory and objectively assessed positive FTDs such as derailment, tangentiality, or poverty of content of speech. Moreover, the overall severity of FTDs was associated with worse social and occupational functioning. These findings are discussed in the context of the existing literature on FTDs and their relationship to neurocognitive measures in schizophrenia.

The relationship we found between FTDs and neuropsychological test scores is in line with the literature investigating symptom domains and their relationship to neurocognition. Indeed, a large meta-analysis of studies has shown that neurocognition is more linked to disorganization than to other positive symptoms such as delusions or hallucinations. 39 This further justifies positioning FTDs as distinct from other positive symptoms, in line with numerous factor analyses separating disorganization from reality distortion.4042

We did not find a relationship between FTDs and semantic or verbal fluency test scores, in contrast to published reports on the link between semantic memory and FTDs in schizophrenia. For example, the severity of FTDs has been correlated with semantic memory deficits. 43 Horn et al. 44 compared the difference between patients with schizophrenia and normal controls in the semantic network on functional magnetic resonance imaging and found that these differences correlated with increasing severity of FTDs. The nonsignificant correlation between semantic verbal fluency and FTDs in our sample could be due to the small sample size or low variability in performance.

However, in the current study, we found a strong correlation between objectively assessed positive FTDs and verbal working memory, a correlation not found for other neurocognitive domains in our sample. This finding aligns with the existing literature implicating executive dysfunction as underlying positive FTDs. For example, executive functioning was more closely associated with improvement in FTDs than semantic function in a sample of patients with first-episode schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. 45 Moreover, executive dysfunction at the onset of schizophrenia was found to be predictive of FTD persistence after 1 year of follow-up. 46 Furthermore, currently available evidence from neuroimaging studies does not indicate that aberrations in the brain's language network are specific to FTDs in schizophrenia, nor does it rule out the relevance of other brain regions to these psychopathologies. 47 Could executive dysfunction be the underlying abnormality affecting patients’ performance on verbal fluency tasks? It has been suggested that semantic fluency impairment itself might be explained by executive dysfunction in working memory, which, in turn, affects cognitive search and retrieval strategies in patients with schizophrenia. 48 However, this finding is challenged by evidence showing that executive functioning in schizophrenia is not related to semantic memory function as assessed by semantic fluency tasks. 49

What about the relationship between neurocognitive measures and subdivisions of FTDs? In a systematic review of reports on 2805 patients, Bora et al. 50 investigated the correlates of FTDs in schizophrenia and found that executive dysfunction correlated with both positive and negative FTDs. They found that deficits in working memory and planning correlated with both types of FTDs, while fluency deficits were only associated with negative FTDs. The correlation between positive FTDs and working memory was found in our sample, but not the relationship between negative FTDs and verbal fluency impairments.

Our results demonstrate the relationship between social and occupational functioning and severity of FTDs. In turn, FTD severity has been previously linked to measures of real-world outcomes.51,52 In addition to predicting lower social and occupational functioning, FTDs have been shown to predict more frequent relapses and rehospitalizations.53,54 Moreover, previous studies have found that negative FTDs and their severity are associated with poorer treatment response and worse long-term outcomes.55,56 In our sample, however, the relationship between subjective negative FTDs, such as blocking, and functioning was moderate but not significant.

The limitations of this chart review include its retrospective design and small sample size. A convenience sampling strategy was used based on suitability and availability to undergo the standardized assessment during routine clinical care. In addition, the associations are based on cross-sectional assessments. The sample contained more male patients than female, which may have influenced our results. Despite these limitations, our findings support the value of standardized psychopathological assessments as part of good clinical practice. In our sample of TRS outpatients, the current analysis demonstrated a strong relationship between FTDs, neurocognition, and social and occupational functioning. Particularly, the results show how the overall severity of FTDs in patients with TRS is correlated with lower functional assessment scores, and that impairment in verbal working memory is strongly correlated with more severe positive FTDs. Large, longitudinal studies are needed to shed light on how the relationship between FTDs and neurocognition evolves over time in patients with TRS.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to all staff working in the clozapine clinic at CAMH for their efforts in implementing measurement-based care.

Appendix 1. TALD Total Score and Relative Factor Scores by Patient (N = 23).

Patient TALD total score Objective positive factor Objective negative factor Subjective positive factor Subjective negative factor
First 17.00 0.33 1.33 0.00 1.14
Second 30.00 0.87 1.33 0.50 1.71
Third 14.00 0.27 1.33 1.50 0.43
Fourth 33.00 0.87 2.33 0.00 1.86
Fifth 19.00 0.73 1.00 1.00 0.43
Sixth 22.00 0.67 1.33 2.50 0.43
Seventh 33.00 0.53 2.33 2.50 1.86
Eighth 15.00 0.67 0.00 0.00 0.71
Ninth 29.00 0.93 1.33 0.00 1.57
Tenth 10.00 0.27 2.00 0.00 0.00
Eleventh 24.00 0.67 3.00 0.00 0.71
Twelfth 34.00 1.47 0.00 0.00 1.29
Thirteenth 23.00 1.13 0.00 1.00 0.57
Fourteenth 13.00 0.27 1.00 1.00 0.57
Fifteenth 13.00 0.67 0.33 0.00 0.29
Sixteenth 28.00 0.93 2.00 0.50 1.00
Seventeenth 19.00 0.47 0.33 0.00 1.57
Eighteenth 16.00 1.00 0.33 0.00 0.00
Nineteenth 44.00 1.47 2.67 3.00 1.14
Twentieth 54.00 1.80 0.67 3.50 2.57
Twenty-first 31.00 1.07 1.00 3.00 0.86
Twenty-second 11.00 0.40 1.00 0.00 0.29
Twenty-third 12.00 0.60 0.00 1.00 0.14

Note. TALD = Thought and Language Disorder Scale. The total TALD score is calculated by summing the score of all 30 items on the scale. Each factor's relative score was calculated by dividing the sum of the individual scores by the number of items in each factor: 15 items for the objective positive factor, 3 items for the objective negative factor, 2 items for the subjective positive factor, and 7 items for the subjective negative factor.

Footnotes

Data Access: Anonymized datasets used and analysed in the current study can be requested by contacting the corresponding authors.

Declaration of Conflicting Interest: The authors declared the following potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: SMA has served as a consultant for HLS Therapeutics and Boehringer Ingelheim Canada. GR has received research support through HLS Therapeutics.

Funding: MA is supported by SABIC Psychological Health Research and Applications Chair, Department of Psychiatry, College of Medicine, Deanship of Post Graduate Teaching, King Saud University, and the Saudi Arabian Cultural Mission in Canada. SMA is supported, in part, by an Academic Scholars Award from the Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto and the CAMH Discovery Fund.

References

  • 1.Çokal D, Sevilla G, Jones WS, et al. The language profile of formal thought disorder. NPJ Schizophr. 2018;4(1):1-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Hart M, Lewine RRJ. Rethinking thought disorder. Schizophr Bull. 2017;43(3):514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Barrera A, McKenna PJ, Berrios GE. Two new scales of formal thought disorder in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2008;157(1–3):225-234. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Parnas J, Møller P, Kircher T, et al. EASE: examination of anomalous self-experience. Psychopathology. 2005;38(5):236-258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 1987;13(2):261-276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Kircher T, Bröhl H, Meier F, et al. Formal thought disorders: from phenomenology to neurobiology. Lancet Psychiatry. 2018;5(6):515-526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.DeLisi LE. Speech disorder in schizophrenia: review of the literature and exploration of its relation to the uniquely human capacity for language. Schizophr Bull. 2001;27(3):481-496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Covington MA, He C, Brown C, et al. Schizophrenia and the structure of language: the linguist’s view. Schizophr Res. 2005;77(1):85-98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bleuler E. Dementia Praecox or the Group of Schizophrenias. Translated by Joseph Zinkin. New York (NY): International Universities Press; 1950. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Roche E, Creed L, Macmahon D, et al. The epidemiology and associated phenomenology of formal thought disorder: a systematic review. Schizophr Bull. 2015;41(4):951. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Yalincetin B, Bora E, Binbay T, et al. Formal thought disorder in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2017;185:2-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Barone A, De Prisco M, Altavilla B, et al. Disorganization domain as a putative predictor of treatment resistant schizophrenia (TRS) diagnosis: a machine learning approach. J Psychiatr Res. 2022;155:572-578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Ortiz BB, Higuchi CH, Noto C, et al. A symptom combination predicting treatment-resistant schizophrenia—a strategy for real-world clinical practice. Schizophr Res. 2020;218:195-200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Ortiz BB, de Filho araújo GM, de Alencar Araripe Neto AG, et al. Is disorganized schizophrenia a predictor of treatment resistance? Evidence from an observational study. Braz J Psychiatry. 2013;35(4):432-434. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Potkin SG, Kane JM, Correll CU, et al. The neurobiology of treatment-resistant schizophrenia: paths to antipsychotic resistance and a roadmap for future research. NPJ Schizophr. 2020;6(1):1-10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Farooq S, Agid O, Foussias G, et al. Using treatment response to subtype schizophrenia: proposal for a new paradigm in classification. Schizophr Bull. 2013;39(6):1169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Nucifora FC, Woznica E, Lee BJ, et al. Treatment resistant schizophrenia: clinical, biological, and therapeutic perspectives. Neurobiol Dis. 2019;131:104257. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Frydecka D, Beszłej JA, Gościmski P, et al. Profiling cognitive impairment in treatment-resistant schizophrenia patients. Psychiatry Res. 2016;235:133-138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Bora E, Murray RM. Meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in ultra-high risk to psychosis and first-episode psychosis: do the cognitive deficits progress over, or after, the onset of psychosis? Schizophr Bull. 2014;40(4):744-755. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Bora E, Yucel M, Pantelis C. Cognitive functioning in schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder and affective psychoses: meta-analytic study. Br J Psychiatry. 2009;195(6):475-482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Stirling J, Hellewell J, Blakey A, et al. Thought disorder in schizophrenia is associated with both executive dysfunction and circumscribed impairments in semantic function. Psychol Med. 2006;36(4):475-484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Tan EJ, Rossell SL. Language comprehension and neurocognition independently and concurrently contribute to formal thought disorder severity in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2019;204:133-137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Barrera A, McKenna PJ, Berrios GE. Formal thought disorder in schizophrenia: An executive or a semantic deficit? Psychol Med. 2005;35(1):121-132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.McGrath J, Scheldt S, Hengstberger P, et al. Thought disorder and executive ability. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 1997;2(4):303-314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Andreasen NC. Scale for the assessment of thought. language, and communication (TLC). https://academic.oup.com/schizophreniabulletin/article/12/3/473/1866041 (1986).
  • 26.Parnas J, Sass LA, Zahavi D. Rediscovering psychopathology: the epistemology and phenomenology of the psychiatric object. Schizophr Bull. 2013;39(2):270-277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Sumner PJ, Rossell SL, Tan EJ. On the assessment of subjective formal thought disorder. Schizophr Res. 2021;228:58-59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Kircher T, Krug A, Stratmann M, et al. A rating scale for the assessment of objective and subjective formal thought and language disorder (TALD). Schizophr Res. 2014;160(1–3):216-221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Lukoff D, Liberman RP, Nuechterlein KH. Symptom monitoring in the rehabilitation of schizophrenic patients. Schizophr Bull. 1986;12(4):578-602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Ventura J, Lukoff D, Nuechterlein KH, et al. Appendix 1: Brief psychiatric rating scale (BPRS) expanded version (4.0) scales, anchor points and administration manual. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 1993;3:227-244. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Hurford IM, Ventura J, Marder SR, et al. A 10-min measure of global cognition: Validation of the Brief Cognitive Assessment Tool for Schizophrenia (B-CATS). Schizophr Res. 2018;195:327-333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Hurford IM, Marder SR, Keefe RSE, et al. A brief cognitive assessment tool for schizophrenia: Construction of a tool for clinicians. Schizophr Bull. 2011;37(3):538-545. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Nuechterlein KH, Green MF, Kern RS, et al. The MATRICS consensus cognitive battery, part 1: test selection, reliability, and validity. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165(2):203-213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Goldman HH, Skodol AE, Lave TR. Revising axis V for DSM-IV: A review of measures of social functioning. Am J Psychiatry. 1992;149(9):1148-1156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.IBM SPSS Inc. SPSS Statistics for Windows. IBM Corp Released 2012; Version 201-8.
  • 36.DiStefano C, Zhu M, Mîndrilǎ D. Understanding and using factor scores: considerations for the applied researcher. Practical Assessment, Research and Evaluation. 14(20). [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Maderthaner L, Pavlidou A, Lefebvre S, et al. Neural correlates of formal thought disorder dimensions in psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2023;49(Suppl_2):S104-S114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Nagels A, Fährmann P, Stratmann M, et al. Distinct neuropsychological correlates in positive and negative formal thought disorder syndromes: the thought and language disorder scale in endogenous psychoses. Neuropsychobiology. 2016;73(3):139-147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Ventura J, Thames AD, Wood RC, et al. Disorganization and reality distortion in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of the relationship between positive symptoms and neurocognitive deficits. Schizophr Res. 2010;121(1–3):1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Arndt S, Andreasen NC, Flaum M, et al. A longitudinal study of symptom dimensions in schizophrenia: prediction and patterns of change. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52(5):352-360. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Andreasen NC, Arndt S, Alliger R, et al. Symptoms of schizophrenia. Methods, meanings, and mechanisms. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52(5):341-351. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Salokangas RKR. Symptom dimensions and outcome in schizophrenia. World Psychiatry. 2003;2(3):172-178. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Tan EJ, Neill E, Rossell SL. Assessing the relationship between semantic processing and thought disorder symptoms in schizophrenia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2015;21(8):629-638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Horn H, Jann K, Federspiel A, et al. Semantic network disconnection in formal thought disorder. Neuropsychobiology. 2012;66(1):14-23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Goldstein RZ, Giovannetti T, Schullery M, et al. Neurocognitive correlates of response to treatment in formal thought disorder in patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol. 2002;15(2):88-98. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Xu JQ, Hui CLM, Longenecker J, et al. Executive function as predictors of persistent thought disorder in first-episode schizophrenia: a one-year follow-up study. Schizophr Res. 2014;159(2–3):465-470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Cavelti M, Kircher T, Nagels A, et al. Is formal thought disorder in schizophrenia related to structural and functional aberrations in the language network? A systematic review of neuroimaging findings. Schizophr Res. 2018;199:2-16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Berberian AA, Moraes G V, Gadelha A, et al. Is semantic verbal fluency impairment explained by executive function deficits in schizophrenia? Braz J Psychiatry. 2016;38(2):121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Neill E, Gurvich C, Rossell SL. Category fluency in schizophrenia research: Is it an executive or semantic measure? Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2014;19(1):81-95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Bora E, Yalincetin B, Akdede BB, et al. Neurocognitive and linguistic correlates of positive and negative formal thought disorder: A meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2019;209:2-11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Rocca P, Galderisi S, Rossi A, et al. Disorganization and real-world functioning in schizophrenia: results from the multicenter study of the Italian network for research on psychoses. Schizophr Res. 2018;201:105-112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Galderisi S, Rossi A, Rocca P, et al. The influence of illness-related variables, personal resources and context-related factors on real-life functioning of people with schizophrenia. World Psychiatry. 2014;13(3):275. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Oeztuerk OF, Pigoni A, Antonucci LA, et al. Association between formal thought disorders, neurocognition and functioning in the early stages of psychosis: a systematic review of the last half-century studies. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2022;272(3):381-393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Marggraf MP, Lysaker PH, Salyers MP, et al. The link between formal thought disorder and social functioning in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Eur Psychiatry. 2020;63(1):e34. doi: 10.1192/J.EURPSY.2020.30 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Wilcox J, Winokur G, Tsuang M. Predictive value of thought disorder in new-onset psychosis. Compr Psychiatry. 2012;53(6):674-678. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Cuesta MJ, Peralta V, De Leon J. Schizophrenic syndromes associated with treatment response. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 1994;18(1):87-99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. Revue Canadienne de Psychiatrie are provided here courtesy of SAGE Publications

RESOURCES