Skip to main content
Science Advances logoLink to Science Advances
. 2024 May 15;10(20):eadn3028. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.adn3028

Nature and human well-being: The olfactory pathway

Gregory N Bratman 1,2,3,*, Cecilia Bembibre 4, Gretchen C Daily 5,6,7, Richard L Doty 8, Thomas Hummel 9, Lucia F Jacobs 10, Peter H Kahn Jr 1,2, Connor Lashus 1, Asifa Majid 11, John D Miller 12, Anna Oleszkiewicz 9,13, Hector Olvera-Alvarez 14, Valentina Parma 15, Anne M Riederer 3, Nancy Long Sieber 16, Jonathan Williams 17,18, Jieling Xiao 19, Chia-Pin Yu 20,21, John D Spengler 16
PMCID: PMC11809653  PMID: 38748806

Abstract

The world is undergoing massive atmospheric and ecological change, driving unprecedented challenges to human well-being. Olfaction is a key sensory system through which these impacts occur. The sense of smell influences quality of and satisfaction with life, emotion, emotion regulation, cognitive function, social interactions, dietary choices, stress, and depressive symptoms. Exposures via the olfactory pathway can also lead to (anti-)inflammatory outcomes. Increased understanding is needed regarding the ways in which odorants generated by nature (i.e., natural olfactory environments) affect human well-being. With perspectives from a range of health, social, and natural sciences, we provide an overview of this unique sensory system, four consensus statements regarding olfaction and the environment, and a conceptual framework that integrates the olfactory pathway into an understanding of the effects of natural environments on human well-being. We then discuss how this framework can contribute to better accounting of the impacts of policy and land-use decision-making on natural olfactory environments and, in turn, on planetary health.


A conceptual framework that integrates the olfactory pathway into research on nature and human well-being is presented.

INTRODUCTION

The olfactory system has evolved to detect a vast range of airborne chemicals (1, 2). These molecules, known as odorants, act as a type of volatile intermediary. They are emitted from a source, transported through the air, and provide information to an organism via olfactory perception (3). Odorants vary in their physiochemical properties, including molecular structure, functional groups, vapor pressure, and solubility (4, 5) (see Box 1). In addition to differences in individual molecules, the trace gas composition of air that human beings breathe contains perpetually changing ratios and concentrations of these chemicals. This results in a range of olfactory environments that, while invisible, are dynamic, potentially highly potent, and consistently experienced by human beings to varying degrees of awareness (6).

Box 1. Anatomical substrates, molecular underpinnings, and evolutionary function.

Molecular structure

A current debate in the olfaction literature is the degree to which variance in perception is explained by the structure and composition of the odor molecule itself (4, 99, 230, 231). Much is still unknown regarding the ways in which the brain creates and/or draws meaning from smells, and how these meanings vary (or not) according to structural diversity, composition, and other molecular features (6, 232). Methods and insights from organic chemistry provide a substantial amount of knowledge regarding the intrinsic properties (e.g., chemoinformatic features) of volatile compounds and their reaction products, but an understanding of the underlying biological and psychological processes related to human perception of these molecules is still evolving (8, 44, 233237).

Odorous compounds can be measured and described consistently, but the character and dimensions of quality, intensity, and hedonic tone that follow from their processing by the olfactory system depends at least in part on the specific nose, brain, and experiences of the individual perceiving them (5, 75, 238242). Nonetheless, substantial progress has been made in understanding the basic elements of olfactory transduction, including the identification of gene families that express olfactory receptors (243).

Evolutionary function

Olfaction provides a major input to the limbic system, which subserves multiple brain functions in mammals, primarily memory and emotion (9, 58). This system is responsible for the primary affective responses that vertebrates have to their environments, and this critical survival function is reflected in its evolutionary history and development (58, 244246). Along with influences on affect, olfaction plays a pivotal role in spatial orientation to odor plumes and trails (247) and has influenced hippocampal evolution (248253). In these and other ways, olfactory cognition is deeply embedded in extended, complex olfactory landscapes (244).

Most living organisms rely on chemical senses (including the olfactory, gustatory, and trigeminal systems) for critical information about their environment. These influences span from spatial navigation to dietary choice to social organization (7). In mammals, after odorants bind to ciliated surfaces of olfactory receptor neurons located in the upper recesses of the nasal cavity, action potentials are generated that propagate along these neurons to the olfactory bulbs. From there, information is sent to brain structures critical for memory, affect, and a range of behaviors, including emotional responses mediated by the amygdala and other parts of the limbic system (8, 9). A complex interacting neural network is involved in these processes, including important associations with the hippocampus and the orbitofrontal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (1013).

OLFACTION AND HUMAN WELL-BEING

Human beings typically have an excellent sense of smell, even when compared to a number of other animals, including mice and some canids (1417). Yet, olfaction has been undervalued as a sensory pathway for human experience in Western cultures (14, 18, 19). Recent studies found that a sample of adults in the UK valued their sense of smell less than vision, hearing, touch, and taste (20), and many US college students from a separate sample stated that they would rather lose their sense of smell than their phone or hair (21). In addition, linguistic analyses of the most common word choices for descriptions of perceptual experiences reveal that they are often dominated by those related to vision (22, 23).

Independent of judgments regarding its utility, however, it is clear that human beings relate to surrounding environments in important ways through olfaction (10). This is particularly salient with a global perspective. For example, odors found in nature play an important role in many Indigenous cultures around the world (24, 25). A nomadic hunter-gatherer group from southern Thailand known as the Maniq routinely use smell to make foraging decisions and judge the medicinal properties of herbs. For the Maniq, smells are directly related to their sources, so unpleasant odors can be indicative of danger while pleasant ones are perceived to be protective and beneficial (26). Among a range of diverse communities across the globe and throughout different periods of history, odor has been considered a marker of health as well as individual and group identity and status—and is also closely tied to other aspects of social interactions, including friendship and marriage (27).

In addition to providing crucial contextual information, the human well-being impacts associated with olfactory processing of odorants are substantial (28). Olfactory function is associated with quality of and satisfaction with life (2933). The sense of smell is tightly related to the limbic system (which evolved from the olfactory cortex) and to psychological processes (e.g., associations and memories) that play an integral role in everyday human functioning (3436). This sensory system influences emotions, emotion regulation, cognitive function, social interactions, dietary choices, stress responses, and depressive symptoms, along with other core dimensions of life (10, 28, 3740).

Volatile organic compounds (VOCs) and other airborne molecules can affect well-being through two primary olfactory pathways: (i) conscious (i.e., suprathreshold) perception of odors at varying degrees of awareness caused by the nervous system processing of these molecules (thereby classifying them as “odorants”) and (ii) nonconscious (i.e., subthreshold) processing of these molecules by the olfactory system that an individual does not perceive. These pathways may interact in complex ways—pleasant associations with smells that are physiologically harmful may provide a positive affective experience (41). For example, a positive childhood memory of woodsmoke associated with social connection can simultaneously produce adverse inflammatory effects via neuroimmunological or respiratory reactions (42). Similar complexities can exist with the use of scented candles (43). Distinguishing affective and physiological responses to airborne molecules via the olfactory pathway is therefore critical, as well as assessing aspects of the perceptions of odorants that take place at various levels of awareness (see Box 2).

Box 2. Odor perception.

For detection of odorants to be possible, the concentration of volatile molecules must exist at or above a threshold level for an organism. In human beings, biological, cultural, demographic, and environmental (both physical and social) factors affect the ability to detect and identify odors over time (18, 254259). Certain odorants bind to nociceptors on the trigeminal nerve and typically give rise to sensations of pungency (e.g., spices) or irritation (e.g., vinegar or acrolein). These sensations, combined with signals carried on the olfactory nerve, contribute to the perception of odor as well. “Olfactory sensitivity” refers to an organism’s ability to detect very low odorant concentrations. Operationally, it is defined as the lowest concentration of an odorant that an individual can consciously perceive (260). Age, gender, bodily state, learned behaviors, and abilities regarding directed attention to smell influence olfactory thresholds and, thus, awareness of an odor. Relevant environmental factors include ambient air pollution, concentrations and rates of change of airborne chemicals, the degree to which they are distinguishable from the background, humidity, distance, and other elements of a given geographic location (e.g., altitude) (116). ”Olfactory discrimination” is the process of distinguishing smells from each other without the requirement of identifying the odor. Olfactory discrimination can occur between chemicals at concentrations too low to be readily identified, i.e., where only a nuance is perceived, as well as between chemicals at higher concentrations where distinct odor experiences arise (135, 259263). ”Olfactory identification” describes the ability to associate an odor with a name and is largely dependent on language and experience (24, 264). ”Odor memory” is the ability to recall experiencing an odor and is critical for odor perception, since, without memory, one cannot identify or discriminate between odors. Measures of odor detection, discrimination, identification, and memory are not mutually exclusive and often depend on the same underlying physiological substrates (265). For example, if sensitivity is markedly decreased, then the ability to identify and remember odors is also compromised. On the other hand, in some disorders such as chronic rhinosinusitis, odor sensitivity can be decreased while odor identification is still intact (174).

Awareness

Conscious awareness of odors is not a sufficient and necessary condition for many adaptive, olfactory-mediated functions (approaching, avoiding, and navigating) (10, 64). Subconscious olfactory processing of odorants may also influence physiology, mood, behavior, cognition, and social interactions (16, 151, 235, 266, 267). One primary function of olfaction may be to notice changes in the environment and to take appropriate action in response to this perception (268, 269). These phenomena can take place in an ongoing, continuous manner, even if explicit appraisal is missing until change occurs (16). In this way, human beings can be aware of smells without paying attention to them—an arena of experience that lies in between subconscious processing and explicit appraisal but that nevertheless has repercussions for a “state of being” (10). As another example, the olfactory vector hypothesis posits that a category of subthreshold impacts includes effects from aerosolized toxins, xenobiotics, and viruses that can “enter the brain via the nose”—traveling directly from peripheral tissue to the CNS and leading to neuroinflammation and other adverse effects (270). Olfactory receptor neurons are unique in their high level of exposure to the environment, providing a pathway through which volatile molecules can cause neuroinflammation through bypassing the blood-brain barrier (271, 272).

In general, there appears to be a core universal bedrock to olfactory processing—an initial affective component that is prelinguistic, preceding cognitive appraisal but still based on suprathreshold perception (18, 19, 44, 45). This early, preverbal affective reaction is likely independent of later processing that is informed by top-down processes and subjective experience. The latter category of appraisals contributes to evaluation and judgment about odor (un)pleasantness and is influenced by preference, culture, associations, prior experience, multisensory context, and other factors. These secondary responses to odors can also vary according to culture and individual-level factors such as genetics, age, and gender (18, 46). In this way, the “hard-wired” spectrum of (un)pleasantness that exists for initial affective reactions to odors may differ from higher-order responses that are open to influence from cognitive processes (e.g., preference for the smell of rotten meat is modulated by culture) (47).

NATURE, OLFACTION, AND HUMAN WELL-BEING

Whether in ancient or modern times, in rural hinterlands or megacities, human beings have always lived in complex interrelationships with nature. The importance of this relationship is emphasized in Indigenous Knowledge and by research in environmental anthropology (4853). Along with these perspectives, Western-based knowledge systems and approaches to conservation have noted the degree to which we depend on tangible and intangible benefits from nature, some coproduced by people, to survive and flourish (5456). However, much of the Western research specific to the impacts of nature contact on human well-being has focused on the causal mechanisms tied to the visual system (57, 58)—including the foundations of influential theories in environmental psychology (59, 60). These theories have motivated hypotheses and study designs in nature and health (61), such as a highly influential study on the impacts of views from a hospital window on recovery after surgery, for example (62). This perspective extends to much of psychology research in general, which often focuses on the visual versus other sensory pathways as well (63, 64).

Here, we expand these considerations to include the olfactory pathway. This is a unique and important factor in the relationship of nature with human well-being, especially given the evolutionary history of olfactory responses to natural environments. There are human subjective olfactory experiences and affective responses that are specific to the natural world (58). These come from potted plants and private gardens, public greenspaces, the sea, wilderness, and other natural areas. Sailors crossing vast ocean expanses report smelling land long before seeing it. Smells of nature may be tied to an individual’s sense of place and call to mind associations or memories of specific natural landscapes (65).

These phenomena have been studied through a variety of methodologies. For example, in-person interviews centered on the sensory experiences of participants during walks have been used to assess smellscapes (i.e., the aspects of the environment that are perceived by human beings as smells, influenced by past experiences and spatial context, and that may affect individuals’ thoughts and feelings) (6668). This approach has often focused on the built environment (6772) and is now being applied to gardens, woodlands, and other natural environments (73, 74).

Research in olfactory heritage emphasizes aspects of smells that are related to cultural practices and spaces with unique and integral values for identity and place-making for communities (46, 69, 75). This includes the smells of nature (76, 77). For example, Sakura Blossom sites across Japan and lavender fields in France produce distinctive smells with acknowledged local significance and aesthetic values. These connections are related to the concept of olfactory heritage insofar as they are tied to dimensions of place-based identity for groups and individuals (46).

Other studies, predominantly from East Asia, demonstrate that the “immune system” of trees (i.e., the activation of volatile-mediated plant defenses to repel herbivores and pathogenic microbes) (78, 79) may have an impact on the immune system of human beings. Shinrin-yoku (also known as “forest bathing”) researchers have investigated whether exposures to VOCs produced in natural environments confer psychophysiological and neuroimmunological benefits through the olfactory pathway (8082). These studies focus primarily on chemical compounds known as terpenes that are naturally present in forests and have been found to be associated with short-term health outcomes in human beings (82, 83), including impacts on mood, stress, anxiety, and inflammation (80, 84, 85). It is not yet known whether these effects are due to conscious appraisals of smells (e.g., the aroma of pine) and/or to neuroimmunological responses that bypass awareness (e.g., anti-inflammatory biochemical processes specific to terpene exposures).

This emerging body of research on the ways in which natural environments affect human well-being through the olfactory pathway informs the consensus statements below. Bringing together expertise across the health, natural, and social sciences, we offer the statements and the conceptual framework that follows as a foundation upon which future research can expand—as a more complete understanding is built regarding the relationship between nature, olfaction, and human well-being. When considering natural olfactory environments, we include VOCs and other airborne molecules produced by nature that are classified as odorants (i.e., perceived as odors), as well as VOCs and other airborne molecules from nature that bypass conscious awareness but still influence human well-being through the olfactory system.

CONSENSUS STATEMENTS

#1: Human beings are embedded in complex, rich, and prolific olfactory environments—chemical contexts within which the natural world transmits information

Volatile chemical emissions convey a multitude of signals both within and between plants and animals—a key function for life in the natural world (86, 87). Olfactory environments play an essential role in microbial, plant, and animal communications (8890). Plants emit odorous volatiles that are crucial for defense against herbivores, pollination (reproduction), and animal navigation (79, (87, (91–(94). These compounds are then transformed through oxidation, increasing molecular diversity as the concentrations of primary emissions decay (95). Human beings are embedded in these contexts, likely affect them through the generation of our own oxidation fields (96), and yet consciously perceive only a small proportion of the total chemosensory communication that exists within them (5).

#2: Airborne chemicals from the natural environment affect human well-being through pathways specific to olfaction—initially perceived and later explicitly judged through both innate and acquired processes

The human olfactory system is intimately connected to the natural world. It involves a distinct avenue for perception via neuronal exposures to the airborne chemicals from nature, including those from vegetation, microbial communities, and bodies of water (97). Olfactory experiences of nature can include both pleasant and unpleasant odors and will be accompanied by different affective and autonomic responses accordingly. Natural olfactory environments—such as the smell of earth after rain or the aroma of pine trees during a forest walk—may instill a sense of connectedness and belonging within the larger natural world (73).

Reactions to natural odors contain dimensions of both affective and semantic valence—independent features that can be aligned or in conflict (98). Affective valence, the initial perception of (un)pleasantness, is relatively universal (19), possibly innate (99, 100), and refers here to initial responses to nature based on perceptions of potential benefit or harm (e.g., sustenance versus toxins). Semantic valence, the later explicit judgment of (un)pleasantness, is partly influenced by lived experience, culture, and social factors—and includes higher-order, top-down information processing about the ways in which nature relates to well-being (e.g., an association of the smell of the sea with restoration due to a cultural practice). These judgments about the smells of nature may be shared or differ across populations and cultures (44, 45, 101).

#3: Anthropogenic activity often negatively affects natural olfactory environments to the detriment of human well-being

Human action shapes natural olfactory environments in fundamental ways. Air pollution, climate change, deforestation, agricultural intensification, urbanization, and other dimensions of transforming and destroying nature continue to accelerate (102), negatively affecting natural olfactory environments (103105). This includes influencing the production of levels of ozone and other impacts on sensescapes (i.e., the combined multisensory characteristics of an environment in a given geographical area at a specific point in time) (106, 107) that are harmful to plants and animals, including human beings (108, 109). Typically, the natural world exists in a state of photochemical balance with the atmosphere, with atmospheric oxidants continually cleaning the air of odorous signals and maintaining chemical spatial gradients (95). For example, although the amount of VOCs emitted by forests changes strongly in accordance with temperature, humidity, and light, interactions with other atmospheric chemicals keep ambient ozone levels in forested areas remarkably low and stable. However, emissions from anthropogenic sources (particularly NOx) can disrupt this balance and subvert the capacity of the biosphere to control the atmospheric environment (110). As these natural olfactory environments are degraded and destroyed through atmospheric and land-use change, fewer opportunities for experiencing a diverse range of odorants of nature are available. This change will likely have corresponding negative consequences for human well-being.

#4: A better understanding of the relationship of human beings with natural olfactory environments can promote appreciation and revitalization of the natural world—and can thereby contribute to human well-being

As olfaction is typically undervalued and the benefits of natural settings are often overlooked (111), the loss of diversity of natural smellscapes is routinely ignored in large-scale assessments of the benefits of nature on human well-being. As urbanization continues, a shifting baseline and extinction of human experience with nature may be accompanied by a decreased capacity to recognize and appreciate the smells of the natural world (112, 113). Studies have demonstrated how contact with more biodiverse natural settings is associated with greater human well-being (114, 115), and this can extend to olfactory environments as well (73). With greater information about these benefits comes greater awareness of their loss, and the importance of actions to conserve and protect the olfactory environments of the natural world.

CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK

Building on these consensus statements, we propose a conceptual framework that recognizes the critical role that olfaction plays in the impact of the environment on human well-being (Fig. 1). While the focus of this framework is on the natural olfactory environment, the same principles and components can be applied to urban olfactory environments, including those that combine anthropogenic and natural emissions. We propose four components of this framework: (i) a characterization of the airborne chemical composition of the olfactory environment; (ii) determinants of sensitivity, discrimination, and identification that moderate perception of odors; (iii) determinants of the subjective experience that mediates the relationship of these olfactory perceptions with (iv) the well-being outcomes resulting from these processes.

Fig. 1. Conceptual framework of the pathway from exposure to natural olfactory environments to human well-being.

Fig. 1.

The olfactory environment is characterized by the concentrations and ratios of airborne chemicals. Dimensions of olfactory function (i.e., sensitivity, discrimination, and identification) are influenced by a variety of individual and environmental factors, which together moderate olfactory perception. Subjective experience is a mediator through which olfactory perceptions lead to well-being outcomes. Relevant determinants of this experience include individual preference, culture, association, prior experience, and multisensory context. Other pathways to well-being include those that occur below the threshold of perception (i.e., subthreshold) and those that occur via initial affective responses that are suprathreshold but independent of top-down processes related to subjective experience. These components lead to a variety of well-being outcomes, from broader dimensions such as quality of and satisfaction with life, to emotional responses and emotion regulation, cognitive function, influences on behavior (social interactions and dietary choices), stress, depressive symptoms, (anti-)inflammatory processes, and effects from exposures to pathogens. Together, these outcomes are the result of subthreshold biochemical processes, initial affective responses, and subjective appraisals of odors from nature. A variety of other pathways mediate the relationship between olfactory environments and human well-being, although they are not illustrated here. Photos credit: University of Washington.

It is important to note that not all factors involved in olfaction are illustrated here. Some are beyond the scope of this paper including, for example, the ways in which different levels of attention to odors can influence olfactory function itself (116), as well as a variety of other mediating pathways that lead from environmental exposures to well-being through the olfactory pathway.

Characterizing the olfactory environment

Natural olfactory environments are generated from forests, meadows, deserts, wetlands, lakes, rivers, oceans, and many other settings and forms of life, from microbes to mammals to giant sequoias. The constituents of olfactory environments are determined in part by the types and abundances of biological species contained within the landscape. In addition, when volatile molecules from nature are released from their sources, they mix with the local atmosphere and undergo photochemical oxidation over the course of minutes to hours (117, 118). These processes affect odor and scent concentration gradients, which have implications for the distances at which natural odorants are detectable. Individual airborne chemicals and the specific ratios and concentrations of mixtures in the air should be considered when characterizing the natural olfactory environment (see Box 3).

Box 3. Additional measurement considerations.

To characterize natural olfactory environments, interactions between natural ecosystems and the atmosphere should be considered (273). Forests emit an array of VOCs across a range of species as a function of light and temperature, with seasonal variation in strength and speciation (274). These compounds undergo oxidation in surrounding air through reactions with OH, O3, and NO3 (275278). Mass spectrometry methods coupled to gas chromatography or proton transfer reaction mass spectrometry measures are typically used to measure terpene concentrations in the air (279, 280), and the Model of Emissions of Gases and Aerosols from Nature has been used to estimate global terrestrial isoprene emissions (118).

Emissions from vegetation typically increase with temperature, often peak in the summer months, and can be affected by climate change (281284). These considerations, along with oxidation, fluid turbulence, and rates of mixture with surrounding air, should be considered when modeling the presence of the VOCs from nature that exist in the air (252, 285). In addition, many natural terpenes are chiral (286)—meaning that they exist in mirror image forms. Chirality expands chemical communication possibilities. The most abundant terpene measured in forests is alpha pinene. While (−) alpha pinene dominates the air of tropical forests, (+) alpha pinene is predominant in boreal forests (286). Although chirality may have an effect on insects (287), it is not yet known whether human beings react or respond to these changes.

Future efforts should also focus on expanding the tools available for measuring the presence of terpenes and other VOCs in ambient air to which human beings are exposed (e.g., through portable equipment that can be worn by individuals over the course of the day) and assessing absorbed dose of these VOCs in human beings (e.g., through serum collection and analysis) before, during, and after these exposures. In addition, accessibility to measurements of olfactory function should be increased. A lack of this availability often leaves individuals at a disadvantage to determine the level of their olfactory abilities. Without this insight, they may not know whether compensatory strategies to supplement lack of exposures or decreased processing of olfactory cues are necessary for their well-being.

Moderators of olfactory perception of natural olfactory environments

Exposure to the odorants of nature may be deliberate or incidental. Whether the smells of a natural olfactory environment fall above or below thresholds of perception will be determined by a variety of individual-level factors related to olfactory function, such as genetics, age, gender, baseline levels of stress, illness, and attention (119). Environmental factors such as air pollution, concentrations of chemical mixtures and their rates of change, distinction from surrounding background, humidity, and distance to perceiver will also influence threshold levels, as will properties of the compounds themselves (16, 120, 121).

In addition, the specific mixtures of natural and anthropogenically generated airborne chemicals need to be considered. Through masking of odors from vegetation via oxidation and other processes, air pollution and elevated levels of ozone have been shown to disrupt chemical signaling that pollinators use to locate flowers (105, 122126). Relatedly, air pollution has been shown to disrupt olfactory function in human beings (127131), as well as cause longer-term impacts through potential damage to cells in the olfactory epithelium (132, 133)—all relevant factors to include when considering the olfactory perception of natural odorants. In addition, recent work has demonstrated that human beings may have evolved to be more sensitive to naturally occurring chemical species with lighter molecular weights or those that decay most rapidly, leading to differential odor detection thresholds (134, 135). Mode of encounter (e.g., a burst versus a gradual appearance) is another relevant factor to include in this component (136).

Subjective experience as a mediator

The subjective experience of olfactory environments mediates the pathway from perception to well-being and can be influenced by an individual’s preference, culture, association, prior experience, and the concurrent multisensory stimuli that provide additional context (137). Different depths of engagement with the natural world exist for different individuals—a factor that will likely influence this mediation as well (138). First person descriptions can provide insight into the role that smell plays in the subjective experience of natural landscapes (73, 139). These experiences involve various levels of arousal, as well as affective and semantic valence, and can occur in the context of diverse activities—from prolonged park visits to smelling a garden or the sea from a window or rooftop (57). Human beings may also associate smell with natural landscapes differently, depending on specific types of nature. For example, sea and ocean are typically strongly associated with smell, but streams and rivers are often not (140). Some aspects of the moderators of olfactory perception may also influence subjective experience (e.g., genetics, age, and gender).

Well-being outcomes

Together, although the evidence base is still growing, exposure to nature via the olfactory pathway may affect a variety of different types of well-being. Impacts associated with olfactory processing of natural airborne chemicals can lead to beneficial affective outcomes or, in some cases, adverse ones (e.g., unpleasant memories associated with a negative prior nature experience). The association of olfaction with quality of and satisfaction with life (2933) may extend to experiences of the natural olfactory environment that are tied to connection with the natural world, individual and community identity, and sense of place (73, 76, 77). Studies have found that nature contact is associated with emotion, emotion regulation, and cognitive function (141, 142), and the substantial tie between olfaction and these outcomes as well (28, 143, 144) supports the possibility that the olfactory pathway may play an important role in these documented impacts from nature contact (145).

Research has shown that odors influence social interactions [e.g., tendencies to cooperate, select friends and mates (40, 146), shake hands (147), or influence parent-infant bonding (148, 149)]. It is therefore possible that natural olfactory environments may contain molecules that influence these interactions, including via subthreshold pathways that influence social preferences or aggression (150, 151). Orthonasal smelling yields anticipation for macronutrients, modifies food selection, and regulates appetite, even in the absence of physiological hunger. As with other odors that influence dimensions of dietary choices [e.g., food preference and quality of diet (152, 153), appetite (28), food flavor and enjoyment (154, 155)], those from nature may have a substantial influence on these choices by causing certain foods to be more appealing. For example, the scents of edible plants in a garden may increase the desire for fresh vegetables and fruits over processed options.

As with other affective outcomes, many studies have demonstrated an association of nature contact with stress responses and depressive symptoms (57). Evidence from olfactory function research (156158) supports the notion that the olfactory pathway may play one explanatory role for these effects and associations. Last, research from Shinrin-yoku supports the association of nature contact with anti-inflammatory outcomes via the olfactory pathway (80, 84).

Future studies will help to inform this emerging body of evidence. With these investigations, it will be important to consider additional moderators related to conscious appraisals of smells of nature, as well as subthreshold biochemical pathways to well-being from natural olfactory environments. Relatedly, there is growing evidence that the constituents of air pollution negatively affect health through inhalation, including via the olfactory pathway (159162) (see Box 4). Some of these constituents, impacts, and pathways may be relevant to future work that investigates the effect of natural olfactory environments on human well-being.

Box 4. Effects of air pollution on well-being.

Evidence from a variety of disciplines provides insight into the mechanisms through which the air that human beings breathe affects well-being. These include exposures to particulate matter (e.g., fine and ultrafine) and other harmful constituents of air pollution that come from tailpipe emissions, fuel refineries, transportation corridors, wildfire smoke, and other sources—all of which can affect well-being by increasing the risk for cardiovascular disease, dementia, anxiety, and depression via oxidative stress and other inflammatory mechanisms (160, 288290). It is also now recognized that air pollution exposure is a risk factor for diseases such as Alzheimer’s disease (291). Other support for the adverse impacts of human-generated airborne toxins comes from the fact that individuals living near major highways are at higher risk for Alzheimer’s disease, Parkinson’s disease, and multiple sclerosis (292).

Beyond the direct effects on human well-being linked to neurochemical and neuroendocrine changes, exposure to air pollution can also lead to other behavioral impacts. Specifically, increased inflammatory cytokine activity and systemic inflammation can be associated with “sickness behavior,” via a CNS pathway, characterized by withdrawal, symptoms related to depression, and hypervigilance or an increased awareness of threats (293295).

PRACTICAL APPLICATION

With continual integration of future findings, this conceptual framework can begin to inform decision-making efforts that account for the repercussions of landscape change for human well-being via associated impacts on natural olfactory environments. This approach follows a preliminary version of past examples in ecosystem service modeling and scenario generation, in which different potential futures are modeled and compared to inform decisions (57, 163167). Today, this approach underpins the transformation to nature-positive, inclusive development pathways being pioneered by cities, countries, multilateral development banks, and other partners (164, 168).

Recent efforts and evolving methodologies in mapping smells through the use of field olfactometry (169) may contribute to the spatial specificity of these scenarios as they relate to olfactory environments, as well as diffusion functions that account for distance decay from plants to individuals and other factors related to the “geography of smell” (58, 69, 170, 171). Analogous efforts exist with respect to examining soundscapes, including natural ones. For example, models that describe natural soundscape quality have been developed using specific indicator metrics that allow for quantification, mapping, and visualization of soundscapes—information that can then be integrated into development decisions (172). Eventually, it will be possible to create an array of projections (i.e., three-dimensional maps of smellscapes) that include both the adverse effects of air pollution and the beneficial effects of natural odorants when considering the repercussions of alternative development scenarios on human well-being (66, 173).

Together, these components allow for the incorporation of spatial and temporal factors in modeling the impacts of landscape change on olfactory environments. Future work to refine this model should continue to integrate interdisciplinary research from atmospheric chemistry, epidemiology, exposure science, neurobiology, ethnography, Indigenous Knowledge, and environmental psychology. To assure ecological validity and best reflect human-nature interactions, these potential avenues of research should examine the mechanisms of the olfactory pathway in the multisensory contexts in which they exist in the natural world.

FUTURE DIRECTIONS

Many understudied areas exist for future research on the impact of nature on human well-being through the olfactory pathway. Below, we outline some of these potential intersections and research frontiers.

Nature, enriched environments, and olfactory function

Loss of sense of smell (i.e., anosmia) (174) has been associated with decreased well-being through risk of exposure to chemical hazards and the inability to fully experience a variety of stimuli, social connections, and environments (28, 175, 176). Reduced olfactory function can also serve as an early indicator of such neurological diseases as Alzheimer’s disease and Parkinson’s disease and can be associated with depression (30, 32, 175, 177183). Idiopathic olfactory loss may precede the symptoms of neurodegenerative diseases by several years and has a distinctive clinical pattern in comparison to other instances of dysosmia (i.e., smell hallucinations or phantosmias). Relatedly, studies have demonstrated that improvements in smell after prior loss are associated with higher levels of well-being and reduced depressive symptoms (184187).

In recent years, a substantial portion of the global population has experienced an impaired sense of smell on at least a temporary basis due to COVID-19 infection—and the subsequent consequences of this on well-being are now being investigated (188192). In addition to disease, there is evidence that odor deprivation and consistent exposure to sterile, odor-deprived environments may diminish human olfactory function (193195). For example, clean room workers who spent significant portions of each day in rooms deliberately deprived of odorants exhibited decreased ability to discriminate between odors, as well as an elevated odor perception threshold. These effects were exacerbated with longer durations of time in the clean room environment (196). Conversely, enriched olfactory environments have been shown to provide support for a range of neurological challenges, including behavioral and cognitive outcomes evident in autism spectrum disorders (197). Findings like these suggest that exposure to a wide range of natural smells could act as a training mechanism—continuously maintaining and improving the olfactory system’s functional capabilities and, through this effect on function, contribute to increased human well-being. This is a vital arena for future research.

Environmental psychology theory

Studies that are motivated by two dominant theories from environmental psychology—attention restoration theory (ART) (59) and stress reduction theory (SRT) (198)—have focused primarily on the visual pathway as the one through which natural stimuli have a restorative effect on cognition and affect. However, the principles underlying these theories can be applied to natural olfactory stimuli as well (73). This approach may offer potential insight regarding causal mechanisms underlying the restorative effects of natural odors on human well-being.

For example, ART posits that individuals have a limited capacity for directed attention, a resource that allows for focus and concentration on a specific set of stimuli, while blocking out competing distractions (which are often present to a greater degree in urban environments). Natural environments are hypothesized to restore directed attention insofar as they contain qualities that are “softly fascinating” and give a sense of “being away,” among other factors. In these types of restorative environments, involuntary attention is engaged, which allows for a replenishment of directed attention and subsequent improvements in cognitive function (e.g., short-term working memory, concentration, and impulse inhibition) (199201). These findings align with studies that reveal an association of greater olfactory function and training with increased cognitive function (184, 185), as well as the literature that demonstrates the relationship of decreased olfactory function with decreased cognitive function and increased cognitive impairment (29, 202205).

With respect to smellscapes, it may be the case that many urban environments present a multitude of odors—a large proportion of them anthropogenically generated—that tax our directed attention as we attend to odors most relevant to specific situations and demands. As a human being moves through an urban landscape, there may be frequent and intense demands upon olfactory attention (e.g., vehicle exhaust, pizza, trash, cigarette smoke, and sewage). This contrasts with many natural environments, in which smellscapes are more constant and only sharp changes on the otherwise slowly changing chemical background are perceived. Movement through a natural landscape may therefore be less demanding on our olfactory attention. Important research foci include causes of odor fatigue (206), mental fatigue from odor exposures (207), and specificities of the environment that affect olfactory awareness and attention (64, 137) to examine whether human beings experience odor fatigue in urban environments. These studies could then investigate whether olfactory attention restoration can be fulfilled directly by exposure to natural olfactory environments in ways that are similar or complementary to the restoration that occurs via the visual pathway in ART studies.

Studies have demonstrated the potential of nature exposure to reduce stress in compelling ways (208). Roger Ulrich’s psychoevolutionary SRT posits that many natural environments restore and reduce acute and chronic stress in human beings through an initial, precognitive affective response and subsequent engagement of the parasympathetic nervous system. This theory may be related to initial affective responses to the smells of nature—those that have an impact on our well-being through a prelinguistic pathway, independent of our later semantic processing of these effects. Future research could examine this further by assessing the degree to which odorants from nature result in affective benefits through initial responses, and how these reactions do or do not differ from later subjective experiences (and resulting impacts on affect).

Biodiversity

The diversity and abundance of life strongly influence how and how well ecosystems support human well-being (209, 210). Biodiversity is also fundamental to the diversity of the human lived experience, including olfactory perceptions (211, 212). Contact with more biodiverse nature has been shown to benefit human well-being to larger degrees than less biodiverse nature (115, 213), and olfaction is an understudied potential pathway through which these benefits may occur (73). In addition, the plasticity of the olfactory system over the lifetime of an individual – and through the evolution of species over time – implies that as environments change, olfactory function may change as well.

Emerging evidence suggesting that anthropogenic ecosystem change may influence neurobiology (214) merits further attention to investigate whether decreased biodiversity of olfactory environments harms cognitive function and neurodevelopment in human beings. A related frontier concerns whether a narrowing or broadening of available odors of nature is associated with a contraction or expansion of corresponding vocabularies of smell, interactions with nature, and outcomes for human well-being (112, 215, 216). With respect to decision-making in this context, recent work demonstrated the importance of framing biodiversity in terms of positive or negative change (versus absolute numbers) and showed that positive emotional responses to gains in biodiversity may be greater in magnitude than negative emotional responses to biodiversity losses (217). These and related future findings can inform interventions designed to motivate protection and restoration of natural olfactory environments.

Breath

Great value would come from investigating the role of other physiological mechanisms that underlie the impacts of nature contact on human well-being via the olfactory pathway. For example, smells of nature may affect respiration in beneficial ways. In some languages, the concept of smell is closely linked with the concept of breath (218) and accounts from the Anangu Aboriginal people of Western Australia describe the post-rain smell of the desert as one that encourages ease of breathing (219). Olfactory perceptions of safety in the natural environment may signal to the central nervous system (CNS) that it is safe to breathe more deeply—a behavior that has been shown to reduce self-reported stress and levels of cortisol and slow heart rate (220). Imagery of pleasant versus unpleasant odors has been shown to cause deeper inhalation through the nose (i.e., “sniffs”) (221), and future research should test these effects using natural stimuli specifically.

Nasal microbiome

Last, the potential role of the nasal microbiome in mediating the relationship between nature exposure and human well-being merits further investigation (222). This community, consisting of billions of microbes, exists under persistent interaction with other microbial life as well as various mixtures of biogenic and anthropogenic airborne chemicals from the environment (223). Differences in nasal bacterial community composition and lower nasal microbiome diversity have been shown to be associated with decreased ability to discriminate odors (224, 225). These results suggest that the nasal microbiome could play a part in shaping an individual’s sense of smell. There is also emerging evidence that nasal microbiome composition may vary between those who grow up in a rural environment versus an urban one (226). Future research should build upon this foundation to explore the ways in which the composition and function of this microbiome may be influenced by natural olfactory environments.

CONCLUSION

Human beings are chemosensory communicators (40), a capacity we share with many other species in the natural world (227). We live in a reciprocal relationship with nature through interactions in the airborne chemical environment—a context that our actions can degrade (e.g., generating air pollution and losing forest/grassland cover via climate change–induced wildfire) or enhance (e.g., creating natural urban greenspaces and conserving large natural areas). Given rapid environmental change, it is urgent that we increase attention to the critical contributions that natural environments make to human olfactory experiences and well-being.

We also underscore the importance of including a full range of cultural contexts in this emerging field. As humanity becomes ever more urban and experiences ever less nature (57), we are cut off from an evolutionary library of olfactory experience. We are only beginning to learn about the complexities of the functioning of the human olfactory system. This includes the fact that olfactory receptors exist in other parts of the body outside of the nose, such as the skin, liver, prostate, and muscles (228). It unknown whether their expression levels are correlated with those within the olfactory neuroepithelium and the degree to which they also influence human health and well-being.

Understanding more about natural olfactory environments is important not only because of associations with human well-being but also because protecting chemosensory communication is core to protecting nature. Outside of an anthropocentric focus, there is a critical importance of the “volatilome” to the functioning of nature itself, and these concerns should inform preservation and conservation efforts (229). Recent activity to protect soundscapes and reduce light pollution in national parks could be expanded to include smellscapes, as has recently been done in certain parts of France through sensory law (https://www.assemblee-nationale.fr/dyn/15/dossiers/definition_protection_patrimoine_sensoriel_campagnes).

Together, the material reviewed in this paper provides a foundation for developing and implementing activities that account for the role of the olfactory pathway. We know very little about the intricacies and interactions that occur within natural olfactory environments. Their protection should be prioritized as we continue to uncover the complex ways in which they support the flourishing of the human and more-than-human world.

Acknowledgments

This paper was developed through a working group in which our coauthors from a variety of disciplines gathered to develop the ideas and consensus represented here. We also thank H. Frumkin and C. D. Simpson as well as C. Levy and other members of the Environment and Well-Being Lab at the University of Washington for support and related discussions. G.N.B. is grateful for support from the Walker Endowed Professorship, John D. Miller, and Craig McKibben and Sarah Merner.

Funding: Partial support for this research came from the University of Washington Center for Studies in Demography & Ecology, whose funders include a Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development research infrastructure grant (P2C HD042828) and multiple units at UW. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. This work was also supported by JPB Environmental Health Fellowship (to G.N.B.), Enlight Foundation and Winslow Foundation (to G.C.D.), Volkswagenstiftung (“Olfactorial Perceptronics”) (to T.H.), Army Research Office grant MURI 574723 (to L.F.J.), National Science Center OPUS grant 2020/39/B/HS6/01533 (to A.O.), Monell Starting Grant (to V.P.), Ministry of Science and Technology of Taiwan grant MOST 110-2410-H-002-146-MY3 (to C.-P.Y.), and was cofunded by the European Union (ERC, D2Smell, 101118977) (to J.W.). Views and opinions expressed are however those of the author(s) only and do not necessarily reflect those of the European Union or the European Research Council. Neither the European Union nor the granting authority can be held responsible for them.

Author contributions: G.N.B., C.B., G.C.D., R.L.D., T.H., L.F.J., P.H.K. Jr., C.L., A.M., J.D.M., A.O., H.O.-A., V.P., A.M.R., N.L.S., J.W., J.X., C.-P.Y., and J.D.S. wrote the paper.

Competing interests: Since 2020, T.H. did research together with and/or received funds from Sony, Stuttgart, Germany; Smell and Taste Lab, Geneva, Switzerland; Takasago, Paris, France; Baia Foods, Madrid, Spain; Primavera, Oy-Mittelberg, Germany; Firmenich, Geneva, Switzerland; and Novartis, Nürnberg, Germany. R.L.D. is a consultant to Destiny Pharma, Johnson and Johnson, Merck, and Satsuma Pharmaceuticals. He receives royalties from Cambridge University Press, Elsevier, Johns Hopkins University Press, McGraw-Hill, and John Wiley and Sons. He is president and major shareholder of Sensonics International, the manufacturer and distributor of smell and taste tests. All other authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Data and materials availability: All data needed to evaluate the conclusions in the paper are present in the paper.

REFERENCES AND NOTES

  • 1.Bear D. M., Lassance J. M., Hoekstra H. E., Datta S. R., The evolving neural and genetic architecture of vertebrate olfaction. Curr. Biol. 26, R1039–R1049 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.de March C. A., Matsunami H., Abe M., Cobb M., Hoover K. C., Genetic and functional odorant receptor variation in the Homo lineage. Iscience 26, 105908 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.K. A. Wilson, “The temporal structure of olfactory experience” in Theoretical Perspectives on Smell, A. Keller, B. D. Young, Eds. (Routledge, 2022), pp. 111–130. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Bierling A. L., Croy I., Hummel T., Cuniberti G., Croy A., Olfactory perception in relation to the physicochemical odor space. Brain Sci. 11, 563 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Mayhew E. J., Arayata C. J., Gerkin R. C., Lee B. K., Magill J. M., Snyder L. L., Little K. A., Yu C. W., Mainland J. D., Transport features predict if a molecule is odorous. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 119, e2116576119 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Young B. D., Escalon J. A., Mathew D., Odors: From chemical structures to gaseous plumes. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 111, 19–29 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Largey G. P., Watson D. R., The sociology of odors. Am. J. Sociol. 77, 1021–1034 (1972). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.R. L. Doty, Handbook of Olfaction and Gustation. (John Wiley & Sons, ed. 3rd, 2015). [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Catani M., Dell’Acqua F., De Schotten M. T., A revised limbic system model for memory, emotion and behaviour. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 1724–1737 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.B. C. Smith, “The role of smell in consciousness” in Theoretical Perspectives on Smell, A. Keller, B. D. Young, Eds. (Routledge, 2022), pp. 13–35. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Brann D. H., Datta S. R., Finding the brain in the nose. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 43, 277–295 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.M. T. Shipley, M. Ennis, A. C. Puche, “The olfactory system” in Neuroscience in Medicine, P. M. Conn, Ed. (Humana Press, 2008), pp. 611–622. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kjelvik G., Evensmoen H. R., Brezova V., Håberg A. K., The human brain representation of odor identification. J. Neurophysiol. 108, 645–657 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.McGann J. P., Poor human olfaction is a 19th-century myth. Science 356, eaam7263 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Sarrafchi A., Odhammer A. M., Hernandez Salazar L. T., Laska M., Olfactory sensitivity for six predator odorants in CD-1 mice, human subjects, and spider monkeys. PLOS ONE 8, e80621 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Sela L., Sobel N., Human olfaction: A constant state of change-blindness. Exp. Brain Res. 205, 13–29 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.M. Laska, “Human and animal olfactory capabilities compared” in Springer Handbook of Odor, A. Buettner, Ed. (Springer International Publishing, 2017), pp. 675–690. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Majid A., Human olfaction at the intersection of language, culture, and biology. Trends Cogn. Sci. 25, 111–123 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Majid A., Burenhult N., Stensmyr M., de Valk J., Hansson B. S., Olfactory language and abstraction across cultures. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 373, 20170139 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Enoch J., McDonald L., Jones L., Jones P. R., Crabb D. P., Evaluating whether sight is the most valued sense. JAMA Ophthalmol. 137, 1317–1320 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Herz R. S., Bajec M. R., Your money or your sense of smell? A comparative analysis of the sensory and psychological value of olfaction. Brain Sci. 12, 299 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.San Roque L., Kendrick K. H., Norcliffe E., Brown P., Defina R., Dingemanse M., Dirksmeyer T., Enfield N. J., Floyd S., Hammond J., Rossi G., Tufvesson S., van Putten S., Majid A., Vision verbs dominate in conversation across cultures, but the ranking of non-visual verbs varies. Linguistics 26, 31–60 (2015). [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Winter B., Perlman M., Majid A., Vision dominates in perceptual language: English sensory vocabulary is optimized for usage. Cognition 179, 213–220 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Majid A., Burenhult N., Odors are expressible in language, as long as you speak the right language. Cognition 130, 266–270 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.O'Meara C., Majid A., How changing lifestyles impact Seri smellscapes and smell language. Anthropol. Linguist. 58, 107–131 (2016). [Google Scholar]
  • 26.E. Wnuk, A. Majid, Revisiting the limits of language: The odor lexicon of Maniq. Cognition 131, 125–138 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 27.Boswell R., Scents of identity: Fragrance as heritage in Zanzibar. J. Contemp. Afr. Stud. 26, 295–311 (2008). [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Boesveldt S., Parma V., The importance of the olfactory system in human well-being, through nutrition and social behavior. Cell Tissue Res. 383, 559–567 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Papazian E. J., Pinto J. M., Olfactory loss and aging: Connections with health and well-being. Chem. Senses 46, bjab045 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Blomqvist E. H., Brämerson A., Stjärne P., Nordin S., Consequences of olfactory loss and adopted coping strategies. Rhinology 42, 189–194 (2004). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Coelho D. H., Reiter E. R., Budd S. G., Shin Y., Kons Z. A., Costanzo R. M., Quality of life and safety impact of COVID-19 associated smell and taste disturbances. Am. J. Otolaryngol. 42, 103001 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Croy I., Nordin S., Hummel T., Olfactory disorders and quality of life–An updated review. Chem. Senses 39, 185–194 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Miwa T., Furukawa M., Tsukatani T., Costanzo R. M., DiNardo L. J., Reiter E. R., Impact of olfactory impairment on quality of life and disability. Arch. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 127, 497–503 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Martiros N., Kapoor V., Kim S. E., Murthy V. N., Distinct representation of cue-outcome association by D1 and D2 neurons in the ventral striatum’s olfactory tubercle. eLife 11, e75463 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Smeets M. A., Dalton P. H., Evaluating the human response to chemicals: Odor, irritation and non-sensory factors. Environ. Toxicol. Pharmacol. 19, 581–588 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.LeDoux J., The amygdala. Curr. Biol. 17, R868–R874 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Krajnik J., Kollndorfer K., Notter L. A., Mueller C. A., Schopf V., The impact of olfactory dysfunction on interoceptive awareness. Psychophysiology 52, 263–268 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Mutic S., Parma V., Brünner Y. F., Freiherr J., You smell dangerous: Communicating fight responses through human chemosignals of aggression. Chem. Senses 41, 35–43 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.V. Parma, A. R. Gordon, C. Cecchetto, A. Cavazzana, J. N. Lundström, M. J. Olsson, “Processing of human body odors” in Springer Handbook of Odor, A. Buettner, Ed. (Springer International Publishing, 2017), pp. 127–128. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Loos H. M., Schaal B., Pause B. M., Smeets M. A. M., Ferdenzi C., Roberts S. C., de Groot J., Lübke K. T., Croy I., Freiherr J., Bensafi M., Hummel T., Havlíček J., Past, present, and future of human chemical communication research. Perspect. Psychol. Sci., 17456916231188147, (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.R. S. Herz, I know what I like: Understanding odor preferences. The smell culture reader, J. Drobnick, Ed. (Oxford, England: Berg Publishers, 2006), pp. 190–203. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Schwartz C., Bølling A. K., Carlsten C., Controlled human exposures to wood smoke: A synthesis of the evidence. Part. Fibre Toxicol. 17, 1–17 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Spence C., Using ambient scent to enhance well-being in the multisensory built environment. Front. Psychol. 11, 598859 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Arshamian A., Gerkin R. C., Kruspe N., Wnuk E., Floyd S., O’Meara C., Garrido Rodriguez G., Lundström J. N., Mainland J. D., Majid A., The perception of odor pleasantness is shared across cultures. Curr. Biol. 32, 2061–2066.e3 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Oleszkiewicz A., Schriever V. A., Valder C., Agosin E., Altundag A., Avni H., Cao van H., Cornejo C., Fishman G., Guarneros M., Gupta N., Kamel R., Knaapila A., Konstantinidis I., Landis B. N., Larsson M., Lundström J. N., Macchi A., Marino-Sanchez F., Mori E., Mullol J., Parma V., Propst E. J., Sandell M. A., Sorokowska A., Vodicka J., Hummel T., Gellrich J., Hedonic perception of odors in children aged 5-8 years is similar across 18 countries: Preliminary data. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 157, 111129 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Bembibre C., Strlič M., Smell of heritage: A framework for the identification, analysis and archival of historic odours. Herit. Sci. 5, 1–11 (2017). [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Yamin-Pasternak S., Kliskey A., Alessa L., Pasternak I., Schweitzer P., The rotten renaissance in the Bering Strait. Curr. Anthropol. 55, 619–646 (2014). [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Hill R., Adem Ç., Alangui W. V., Molnár Z., Aumeeruddy-Thomas Y., Bridgewater P., Tengö M., Thaman R., Adou Yao C. Y., Berkes F., Carino J., Carneiro da Cunha M., Diaw M. C., Díaz S., Figueroa V. E., Fisher J., Hardison P., Ichikawa K., Kariuki P., Karki M., Lyver P. O. B., Malmer P., Masardule O., Oteng Yeboah A. A., Pacheco D., Pataridze T., Perez E., Roué M. M., Roba H., Rubis J., Saito O., Xue D., Working with Indigenous, local and scientific knowledge in assessments of nature and nature’s linkages with people. Curr. Opin. Environ. Sustain. 43, 8–20 (2020). [Google Scholar]
  • 49.R. Kimmerer, Braiding Sweetgrass: Indigenous Wisdom, Scientific Knowledge and the Teachings of Plants. (Milkweed Editions, 2013). [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Pascual U., Adams W. M., Díaz S., Lele S., Mace G. M., Turnhout E., Biodiversity and the challenge of pluralism. Nat. Sustain. 4, 567–572 (2021). [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Tengo M., Austin B. J., Danielsen F., Fernandez-Llamazares A., Creating synergies between citizen science and Indigenous and local knowledge. Bioscience 71, 503–518 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.C. Folke, F. Berkes, Linking Social and Ecological Systems: Management Practices and Social Mechanisms For Building Resilience. (Cambridge University Press, 1998). [Google Scholar]
  • 53.F. Berkes, Sacred Ecology. (Taylor & Francis, 1999). [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Daily G. C., Söderqvist T., Aniyar S., Arrow K., Dasgupta P., Ehrlich P. R., Folke C., Jansson A. M., Jansson B. O., Kautsky N., Levin S., Lubchenco J., Mäler K. G., Simpson D., Starrett D., Tilman D., Walker B., The value of nature and the nature of value. Science 289, 395–396 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Díaz S., Pascual U., Stenseke M., Martín-López B., Watson R. T., Molnár Z., Hill R., Chan K. M. A., Baste I. A., Brauman K. A., Polasky S., Church A., Lonsdale M., Larigauderie A., Leadley P. W., van Oudenhoven A. P. E., van der Plaat F., Schröter M., Lavorel S., Aumeeruddy-Thomas Y., Bukvareva E., Davies K., Demissew S., Erpul G., Failler P., Guerra C. A., Hewitt C. L., Keune H., Lindley S., Shirayama Y., Assessing nature’s contributions to people. Science 359, 270–272 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Pascual U., Balvanera P., Díaz S., Pataki G., Roth E., Stenseke M., Watson R. T., Dessane E. B., Islar M., Kelemen E., Maris V., Quaas M., Subramanian S. M., Wittmer H., Adlan A., Ahn S., Al-Hafedh Y. S., Amankwah E., Asah S. T., Berry P., Yagi N., Valuing nature’s contributions to people: The IPBES approach. Curr. Opin. Environ. Sustain. 26, 7–16 (2017). [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Bratman G. N., Anderson C. B., Berman M. G., Cochran B., de Vries S., Flanders J., Folke C., Frumkin H., Gross J. J., Hartig T., Kahn P. H. Jr., Kuo M., Lawler J. J., Levin P. S., Lindahl T., Meyer-Lindenberg A., Mitchell R., Ouyang Z., Roe J., Scarlett L., Smith J. R., van den Bosch M., Wheeler B. W., White M. P., Zheng H., Daily G. C., Nature and mental health: An ecosystem service perspective. Sci. Adv. 5, eaax0903 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Press D., Minta S. C., The smell of nature: Olfaction, knowledge and the environment. Philos. Geogr. 3, 173–186 (2000). [Google Scholar]
  • 59.R. Kaplan, S. Kaplan, The Experience of Nature: A Psychological Perspective. (Cambridge University Press, 1989). [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Ulrich R. S., Visual landscapes and psychological well-being. Land. Res. 4, 17–23 (1979). [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Frumkin H., Bratman G. N., Breslow S. J., Cochran B., Kahn Jr P. H., Lawler J. J., Levin P. S., Tandon P. S., Varanasi U., Wolf K. L., Wood S. A., Nature contact and human health: A research agenda. Environ. Health Perspect. 125, 075001 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Ulrich R. S., View through a window may influence recovery from surgery. Science 224, 420–421 (1984). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Hutmacher F., Why is there so much more research on vision than on any other sensory modality? Front. Psychol. 10, 2246 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Keller A., Attention and olfactory consciousness. Front. Psychol. 2, 380 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.C. Brozzo, “Perfumes and the aesthetic appreciation of nature” in Theoretical Perspectives on Smell, A. Keller, B. D. Young, Eds. (Routledge, 2022). [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Xiao J., Tait M., Kang J., A perceptual model of smellscape pleasantness. Cities 76, 105–115 (2018). [Google Scholar]
  • 67.V. Henshaw, Urban Smellscapes: Understanding and Designing City Smell Environments. (Routledge, 2013). [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Porteous J. D., Smellscape. Prog. Phys. Geogr. 9, 356–378 (1985). [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Bembibre C., Strlic M., From smelly buildings to the scented past: An overview of olfactory heritage. Front. Psychol. 12, 718287 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Degen M. M., Rose G., The sensory experiencing of urban design: The role of walking and perceptual memory. Urban Stud. 49, 3271–3287 (2012). [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Xiao J., Aletta F., Radicchi A., McLean K., Shiner L. E., Verbeek C., Recent advances in smellscape research for the built environment. Front. Psychol. 12, 700514 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Xiao J., Tait M., Kang J., Understanding smellscapes: Sense-making of smell-triggered emotions in place. Emot. Space Soc. 37, 100710 (2020). [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Bentley P. R., Fisher J. C., Dallimer M., Fish R. D., Austen G. E., Irvine K. N., Davies Z. G., Nature, smells, and human wellbeing. Ambio 52, 1–14 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Palsdottir A. M., Spendrup S., Martensson L., Wendin K., Garden smellscape-experiences of plant scents in a nature-based intervention. Front. Psychol. 12, 667957 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Leemans I., Tullett W., Bembibre C., Marx L., Whiffstory: Using multidisciplinary methods to represent the olfactory past. Am. Hist. Rev. 127, 849–879 (2022). [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Y.-F. Tuan, Topophilia: A study of Environmental Perception, Attitudes, and Values. (Columbia Univ. Press, 1990). [Google Scholar]
  • 77.P. Rodaway, Sensuous Geographies: Body, Sense and Place. (Routledge, 2002). [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Hammerbacher A., Coutinho T. A., Gershenzon J., Roles of plant volatiles in defence against microbial pathogens and microbial exploitation of volatiles. Plant Cell Environ. 42, 2827–2843 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Brosset A., Blande J. D., Volatile-mediated plant-plant interactions: Volatile organic compounds as modulators of receiver plant defence, growth, and reproduction. J. Exp. Bot. 73, 511–528 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Wen Y., Yan Q., Pan Y., Gu X., Liu Y., Medical empirical research on forest bathing (Shinrin-yoku): A systematic review. Environ. Health Prev. Med. 24, 70 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Oh B., Lee K. J., Zaslawski C., Yeung A., Rosenthal D., Larkey L., Back M., Health and well-being benefits of spending time in forests: Systematic review. Environ. Health Prev. Med. 22, 71 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Yu C.-P., Lin C.-M., Tsai M.-J., Tsai Y.-C., Chen C.-Y., Effects of short forest bathing program on autonomic nervous system activity and mood states in middle-aged and elderly individuals. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 14, 897 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Li Q., Morimoto K., Kobayashi M., Inagaki H., Katsumata M., Hirata Y., Hirata K., Suzuki H., Li Y. J., Wakayama Y., Kawada T., Park B. J., Ohira T., Matsui N., Kagawa T., Miyazaki Y., Krensky A. M., Visiting a forest, but not a city, increases human natural killer activity and expression of anti-cancer proteins. Int. J. Immunopathol. Pharmacol. 21, 117–127 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Andersen L., Corazon S. S. S., Stigsdotter U. K. K., Nature exposure and its effects on immune system functioning: A systematic review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18, 1416 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Kuo M., How might contact with nature promote human health? Promising mechanisms and a possible central pathway. Front. Psychol. 6, 1093 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Kessler A., Mueller M. B., Kalske A., Chauta A., Volatile-mediated plant-plant communication and higher-level ecological dynamics. Curr. Biol. 33, R519–R529 (2023). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Tuteja N., Sopory S. K., Chemical signaling under abiotic stress environment in plants. Plant Signal. Behav. 3, 525–536 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Meredith L. K., Tfaily M. M., Capturing the microbial volatilome: An oft overlooked ‘ome’. Trends Microbiol. 30, 622–631 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Doty R. L., Odor-guided behavior in mammals. Experientia 42, 257–271 (1986). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.C. Mucignat-Caretta, Neurobiology of Chemical Communication. (CRC Press, 2014). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Takabayashi J., Shiojiri K., Multifunctionality of herbivory-induced plant volatiles in chemical communication in tritrophic interactions. Curr. Opin. Insect Sci. 32, 110–117 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Zannoni N., Wikelski M., Gagliardo A., Raza A., Kramer S., Seghetti C., Wang N., Edtbauer A., Williams J., Identifying volatile organic compounds used for olfactory navigation by homing pigeons. Sci. Rep. 10, 15879 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Wikelski M., Quetting M., Cheng Y., Fiedler W., Flack A., Gagliardo A., Salas R., Zannoni N., Williams J., Smell of green leaf volatiles attracts white storks to freshly cut meadows. Sci. Rep. 11, 12912 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Safi K., Gagliardo A., Wikelski M., Kranstauber B., How displaced migratory birds could use volatile atmospheric compounds to find their migratory corridor: A test using a particle dispersion model. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 10, 175 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.J. Williams, R. Koppmann, Volatile Organic Compounds in the Atmosphere, R. Koppmann, Ed. (Blackwell Publishing Ltd., 2007), pp. 1–32. [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Zannoni N., Lakey P. S. J., Won Y., Shiraiwa M., Rim D., Weschler C. J., Wang N., Ernle L., Li M., Bekö G., Wargocki P., Williams J., The human oxidation field. Science 377, 1071–1077 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Peters K., Steinberg P., The ocean in excess: Towards a more-than-wet ontology. Dialogues Hum. Geogr. 9, 293–307 (2019). [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Itkes O., Kron A., Affective and semantic representations of valence: A conceptual framework. Emotion Review 11, 283–293 (2019). [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Snitz K., Yablonka A., Weiss T., Frumin I., Khan R. M., Sobel N., Predicting odor perceptual similarity from odor structure. PLOS Comput. Biol. 9, e1003184 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Ustun B., Reissland N., Covey J., Schaal B., Blissett J., Flavor sensing in utero and emerging discriminative behaviors in the human fetus. Psychol. Sci. 33, 1651–1663 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Ferdenzi C., Roberts S. C., Schirmer A., Delplanque S., Cekic S., Porcherot C., Cayeux I., Sander D., Grandjean D., Variability of affective responses to odors: Culture, gender, and olfactory knowledge. Chem. Senses 38, 175–186 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Díaz S., Settele J., Brondízio E. S., Ngo H. T., Agard J., Arneth A., Balvanera P., Brauman K. A., Butchart S. H. M., Chan K. M. A., Garibaldi L. A., Ichii K., Liu J., Subramanian S. M., Midgley G. F., Miloslavich P., Molnár Z., Obura D., Pfaff A., Polasky S., Purvis A., Razzaque J., Reyers B., Chowdhury R. R., Shin Y. J., Visseren-Hamakers I., Willis K. J., Zayas C. N., Pervasive human-driven decline of life on Earth points to the need for transformative change. Science 366, eaax3100 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Yuan J. S., Himanen S. J., Holopainen J. K., Chen F., Stewart C. N. Jr., Smelling global climate change: Mitigation of function for plant volatile organic compounds. Trends Ecol. Evol. 24, 323–331 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Cordeiro G. D., Dötterl S., Global warming impairs the olfactory floral signaling in strawberry. BMC Plant Biol. 23, 549 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Chan J., Parasurama S., Atlas R., Xu R., Jongebloed U. A., Alexander B., Langenhan J. M., Thornton J. A., Riffell J. A., Olfaction in the Anthropocene: NO3 negatively affects floral scent and nocturnal pollination. Science 383, 607–611 (2024). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Bell S. L., Hickman C., Houghton F., From therapeutic landscape to therapeutic ‘sensescape’ experiences with nature? A scoping review. Wellbeing Space Soc. 4, 100126 (2023). [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Rosen R. S., Geographies in the American DeafWorld as institutional constructions of the deaf body in space: The sensescape model. Disability Soc. 33, 59–77 (2018). [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Lelieveld J., Evans J. S., Fnais M., Giannadaki D., Pozzer A., The contribution of outdoor air pollution sources to premature mortality on a global scale. Nature 525, 367–371 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Williams H. J., Sridhar V. H., Hurme E., Gall G. E. C., Borrego N., Finerty G. E., Couzin I. D., Galizia C. G., Dominy N. J., Rowland H. M., Hauber M. E., Higham J. P., Strandburg-Peshkin A., Melin A. D., Sensory collectives in natural systems. eLife 12, e88028 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Williams J., Keßel S. U., Nölscher A. C., Yang Y., Lee Y., Yáñez-Serrano A. M., Wolff S., Kesselmeier J., Klüpfel T., Lelieveld J., Shao M., Opposite OH reactivity and ozone cycles in the Amazon rainforest and megacity Beijing: Subversion of biospheric oxidant control by anthropogenic emissions. Atmos. Environ. 125, 112–118 (2016). [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Nisbet E. K., Zelenski J. M., Underestimating nearby nature: Affective forecasting errors obscure the happy path to sustainability. Psychol. Sci. 22, 1101–1106 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Kahn P. H. Jr., Ruckert J. H., Severson R. L., Reichert A. L., Fowler E., A nature language: An agenda to catalog, save, and recover patterns of human–nature interaction. Ecopsychology 2, 59–66 (2010). [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Kahn P. H. Jr., Friedman B., Gill B., Hagman J., Severson R. L., Freier N. G., Feldman E. N., Carrère S., Stolyar A., A plasma display window? The shifting baseline problem in a technologically mediated natural world. J. Environ. Psychol. 28, 192–199 (2008). [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Cox D. T. C., Shanahan D. F., Hudson H. L., Fuller R. A., Anderson K., Hancock S., Gaston K. J., Doses of nearby nature simultaneously associated with multiple health benefits. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 14, 172 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Fuller R. A., Irvine K. N., Devine-Wright P., Warren P. H., Gaston K. J., Psychological benefits of greenspace increase with biodiversity. Biol. Lett. 3, 390–394 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Oleszkiewicz A., Heyne L., Sienkiewicz-Oleszkiewicz B., Cuevas M., Haehner A., Hummel T., Odours count: Human olfactory ecology appears to be helpful in the improvement of the sense of smell. Sci. Rep. 11, 16888 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Atkinson R., Arey J., Gas-phase tropospheric chemistry of biogenic volatile organic compounds: A review. Atmos. Environ. 37, 197–219 (2003). [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Guenther A., Jiang X., Heald C. L., Sakulyanontvittaya T., Duhl T., Emmons L. K., Wang X., The model of emissions of gases and aerosols from nature version 2.1 (MEGAN2.1): An extended and updated framework for modeling biogenic emissions. Geosci. Model Dev. 5, 1471–1492 (2012). [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Ai Y., Yang J., Nie H., Hummel T., Han P., Increased sensitivity to unpleasant odor following acute psychological stress. Horm. Behav. 150, 105325 (2023). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Nagata Y., Takeuchi N., Measurement of odor threshold by triangle odor bag method. Odor Measurement Rev. 118, 118–127 (2003). [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Oleszkiewicz A., Rambacher L., Whitcroft K., Hummel T., The confounding effect of background odors on olfactory sensitivity testing. J. Neurosci. Methods 306, 88–91 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Ryalls J. M. W., Langford B., Mullinger N. J., Bromfield L. M., Nemitz E., Pfrang C., Girling R. D., Anthropogenic air pollutants reduce insect-mediated pollination services. Environ. Pollut. 297, 118847 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Blande J. D., Effects of air pollution on plant–insect interactions mediated by olfactory and visual cues. Curr. Opin. Environ. Sci. Health 19, 100228 (2021). [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Masui N., Agathokleous E., Mochizuki T., Tani A., Matsuura H., Koike T., Ozone disrupts the communication between plants and insects in urban and suburban areas: An updated insight on plant volatiles. J. Forest. Res. 32, 1337–1349 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Masui N., Shiojiri K., Agathokleous E., Tani A., Koike T., Elevated O3 threatens biological communications mediated by plant volatiles: A review focusing on the urban environment. Crit. Rev. Environ. Sci. Technol. 53, 1982–2001 (2023). [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Farre-Armengol G., Peñuelas J., Li T., Yli-Pirilä P., Filella I., Llusia J., Blande J. D., Ozone degrades floral scent and reduces pollinator attraction to flowers. New Phytol. 209, 152–160 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Guarneros M., Hummel T., Martinez-Gomez M., Hudson R., Mexico City air pollution adversely affects olfactory function and intranasal trigeminal sensitivity. Chem. Senses 34, 819–826 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Calderón-Garcidueñas L., Franco-Lira M., Henríquez-Roldán C., Osnaya N., González-Maciel A., Reynoso-Robles R., Villarreal-Calderon R., Herritt L., Brooks D., Keefe S., Palacios-Moreno J., Villarreal-Calderon R., Torres-Jardón R., Medina-Cortina H., Delgado-Chávez R., Aiello-Mora M., Maronpot R. R., Doty R. L., Urban air pollution: Influences on olfactory function and pathology in exposed children and young adults. Exp. Toxicol. Pathol. 62, 91–102 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Ajmani G. S., Suh H. H., Pinto J. M., Effects of ambient air pollution exposure on olfaction: A review. Environ. Health Perspect. 124, 1683–1693 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Ekstrom I. A., Rizzuto D., Grande G., Bellander T., Laukka E. J., Environmental air pollution and olfactory decline in aging. Environ. Health Perspect. 130, 27005 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Zhang Z., Rowan N. R., Pinto J. M., London N. R., Lane A. P., Biswal S., Ramanathan M. Jr., Exposure to particulate matter air pollution and anosmia. JAMA Netw. Open 4, e2111606–e2111606 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Hudson R., Arriola A., Martínez-Gómez M., Distel H., Effect of air pollution on olfactory function in residents of Mexico City. Chem. Senses 31, 79–85 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Kanninen K., Lampinen R., Rantanen L. M., Odendaal L., Jalava P., Chew S., White A. R., Olfactory cell cultures to investigate health effects of air pollution exposure: Implications for neurodegeneration. Neurochem. Int. 136, 104729 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Sinding C., Puschmann L., Hummel T., Is the age-related loss in olfactory sensitivity similar for light and heavy molecules? Chem. Senses 39, 383–390 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Williams J., Ringsdorf A., Human odour thresholds are tuned to atmospheric chemical lifetimes. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 375, 20190274 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.S. Balez, “Smell walks” in Experiential Walks for Urban Design, B. Piga, D. Siret, J.-P. Thibaud, Eds. (Springer, 2021), pp. 93–114. [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Smeets M. A., Schifferstein H. N., Boelema S. R., Lensvelt-Mulders G., The odor awareness scale: A new scale for measuring positive and negative odor awareness. Chem. Senses 33, 725–734 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Daly L., Shepard G. Jr., Magic darts and messenger molecules: Toward a phytoethnography of indigenous Amazonia. Anthropol. Today 35, 13–17 (2019). [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Koblet O., Purves R. S., From online texts to landscape character assessment: Collecting and analysing first-person landscape perception computationally. Landsc. Urban Plan. 197, 103757 (2020). [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Purves R. S., Striedl P., Kong I., Majid A., Conceptualizing landscapes through language: The role of native language and expertise in the representation of waterbody related terms. Topics Cogn. Sci. 15, 560–583 (2023). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Bratman G. N., Olvera-Alvarez H. A., Gross J. J., The affective benefits of nature exposure. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 15, e12630 (2021). [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Bratman G. N., Mehta A., Olvera-Alvarez H., Spink K. M., Levy C., White M. P., Kubzansky L. D., Gross J. J., Associations of nature contact with emotional ill-being and well-being: The role of emotion regulation. Cognit. Emot. 1–20 (2024). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Alaoui-Ismaili O., Vernet-Maury E., Dittmar A., Delhomme G., Chanel J., Odor hedonics: Connection with emotional response estimated by autonomic parameters. Chem. Senses 22, 237–248 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 144.Vernet-Maury E., VAlaoui-Ismaïli O., Dittmar A., Delhomme G., Chanel J., Basic emotions induced by odorants: A new approach based on autonomic pattern results. J. Auton. Nerv. Syst. 75, 176–183 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Hedblom M., Gunnarsson B., Iravani B., Knez I., Schaefer M., Thorsson P., Lundström J. N., Reduction of physiological stress by urban green space in a multisensory virtual experiment. Sci. Rep. 9, 10113 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Ravreby I., Snitz K., Sobel N., There is chemistry in social chemistry. Sci. Adv. 8, eabn0154 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 147.Frumin I., Perl O., Endevelt-Shapira Y., Eisen A., Eshel N., Heller I., Shemesh M., Ravia A., Sela L., Arzi A., Sobel N., A social chemosignaling function for human handshaking. eLife 4, e05154 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 148.Lübke K. T., Pause B. M., Always follow your nose: The functional significance of social chemosignals in human reproduction and survival. Horm. Behav. 68, 134–144 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 149.Croy I., Frackowiak T., Hummel T., Sorokowska A., Babies smell wonderful to their parents, teenagers do not: An exploratory questionnaire study on children’s age and personal odor ratings in a polish sample. Chemosens. Percept. 10, 81–87 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 150.Li W., Moallem I., Paller K. A., Gottfried J. A., Subliminal smells can guide social preferences. Psychol. Sci. 18, 1044–1049 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 151.Agron S., de March C. A., Weissgross R., Mishor E., Gorodisky L., Weiss T., Furman-Haran E., Matsunami H., Sobel N., A chemical signal in human female tears lowers aggression in males. PLOS Biol. 21, e3002442 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 152.Liu D. T., Prem B., Sharma G., Kaiser J., Besser G., Mueller C. A., Eating behavior in patients with smell loss. Front. Nutr. 9, 993639 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 153.Rawal S., Duffy V. B., Berube L., Hayes J. E., Kant A. K., Li C. M., Graubard B. I., Hoffman H. J., Self-reported olfactory dysfunction and diet quality: Findings from the 2011–2014 national health and nutrition examination survey (NHANES). Nutrients 13, 4561 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 154.Seo H.-S., Pramudya R. C., Singh A., Hummel T., Recent evidence for the impacts of olfactory disorders on food enjoyment and ingestive behavior. Curr. Opin. Food Sci. 42, 187–194 (2021). [Google Scholar]
  • 155.Spence C., What is the relationship between the presence of volatile organic compounds in food and drink products and multisensory flavour perception? Foods 10, 1570 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 156.Joussain P., Rouby C., Bensafi M., A pleasant familiar odor influences perceived stress and peripheral nervous system activity during normal aging. Front. Psychol. 5, 113 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 157.Lee E. J., Saraiva L. R., Hanchate N. K., Ye X., Asher G., Ho J., Buck L. B., Odor blocking of stress hormone responses. Sci. Rep. 12, 8773 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 158.Croy I., Hummel T., Olfaction as a marker for depression. J. Neurol. 264, 631–638 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 159.Yang X., Geng L., Zhou K., Environmental pollution, income growth, and subjective well-being: Regional and individual evidence from China. Environ. Sci. Pollut. Res. 27, 34211–34222 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 160.Wilker E. H., Osman M., Weisskopf M. G., Ambient air pollution and clinical dementia: Systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ 381, e071620 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 161.Shi L., Zhu Q., Wang Y., Hao H., Zhang H., Schwartz J., Amini H., van Donkelaar A., Martin R. V., Steenland K., Sarnat J. A., Caudle W. M., Ma T., Li H., Chang H. H., Liu J. Z., Wingo T., Mao X., Russell A. G., Weber R. J., Liu P., Incident dementia and long-term exposure to constituents of fine particle air pollution: A national cohort study in the United States. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 120, e2211282119 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 162.Li Z., Yan H., Zhang X., Shah S., Yang G., Chen Q., Han S., Zhang D., Weinberger D. R., Yue W., Tan H. Y., Air pollution interacts with genetic risk to influence cortical networks implicated in depression. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 118, e2109310118 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 163.Daily G. C., Polasky S., Goldstein J., Kareiva P. M., Mooney H. A., Pejchar L., Ricketts T. H., Salzman J., Shallenberger R., Ecosystem services in decision making: Time to deliver. Front. Ecol. Environ. 7, 21–28 (2009). [Google Scholar]
  • 164.Daily G. C., Ruckelshaus M., 25 years of valuing ecosystems in decision-making. Nature 606, 465–466 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 165.Foley J. A., DeFries R., Asner G. P., Barford C., Bonan G., Carpenter S. R., Chapin F. S., Coe M. T., Daily G. C., Gibbs H. K., Helkowski J. H., Holloway T., Howard E. A., Kucharik C. J., Monfreda C., Patz J. A., Prentice I. C., Ramankutty N., Snyder P. K., Global consequences of land use. Science 309, 570–574 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 166.Guerry A. D., Polasky S., Lubchenco J., Chaplin-Kramer R., Daily G. C., Griffin R., Ruckelshaus M., Bateman I. J., Duraiappah A., Elmqvist T., Feldman M. W., Folke C., Hoekstra J., Kareiva P. M., Keeler B. L., Li S., McKenzie E., Ouyang Z., Reyers B., Ricketts T. H., Rockström J., Tallis H., Vira B., Natural capital and ecosystem services informing decisions: From promise to practice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 7348–7355 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 167.Ruckelshaus M., McKenzie E., Tallis H., Guerry A., Daily G., Kareiva P., Polasky S., Ricketts T., Bhagabati N., Wood S. A., Bernhardt J., Notes from the field: Lessons learned from using ecosystem service approaches to inform real-world decisions. Ecol. Econ. 115, 11–21 (2015). [Google Scholar]
  • 168.L. Mandle, Z. Ouyang, J. Salzman, G. C. Daily, Green Growth that Works: Natural Capital Policy and Finance Mechanisms Around the World. (Island Press (Chinese, Science Press of China), 2019). [Google Scholar]
  • 169.Kitson J., Leiva M., Christman Z., Dalton P., Evaluating urban odor with field olfactometry in Camden, NJ. Urban Sci. 3, 93 (2019). [Google Scholar]
  • 170.Hoover K. C., The geography of smell. Cartogr.: Int. J Geogr. Inf. Geovis. 44, 237–239 (2009). [Google Scholar]
  • 171.Song X., Wu Q., Study on smellscape perception and landscape application of fragrant plants. Urban Forest. Urban Green. 67, 127429 (2022). [Google Scholar]
  • 172.Chen Z., Hermes J., von Haaren C., Mapping and assessing natural soundscape quality: An indicator-based model for landscape planning. J. Environ. Manage. 354, 120422 (2024). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 173.Gorman R., Smelling therapeutic landscapes: Embodied encounters within spaces of care farming. Health Place 47, 22–28 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 174.Whitcroft K., Altundag A., Balungwe P., Boscolo-Rizzo P., Douglas R., Enecilla M. L. B., Fjaeldstad A. W., Fornazieri M. A., Frasnelli J., Gane S., Gudziol H., Gupta N., Haehner A., Hernandez A. K., Holbrook E. H., Hopkins C., Hsieh J. W., Huart C., Husain S., Kamel R., Kim J. K., Kobayashi M., Konstantinidis I., Landis B. N., Lechner M., Macchi A., Mazal P. P., Miri I., Miwa T., Mori E., Mullol J., Mueller C. A., Ottaviano G., Patel Z. M., Philpott C., Pinto J. M., Ramakrishnan V. R., Roth Y., Schlosser R. J., Stjärne P., Van Gerven L., Vodicka J., Welge-Luessen A., Wormald P. J., Hummel T., Position paper on olfactory dysfunction: 2023. Rhinology 61, 1–108 (2023). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 175.Doty R. L., Olfactory dysfunction in neurodegenerative diseases: Is there a common pathological substrate? Lancet Neurol. 16, 478–488 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 176.Pence T. S., Reiter E. R., DiNardo L. J., Costanzo R. M., Risk factors for hazardous events in olfactory-impaired patients. JAMA Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 140, 951–955 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 177.Doty R. L., Olfactory dysfunction in Parkinson disease. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 8, 329–339 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 178.Haehner A., Hummel T., Reichmann H., Olfactory loss in Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsons Dis. 2011, 450939 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 179.Oleszkiewicz A., Kunkel F., Larsson M., Hummel T., Consequences of undetected olfactory loss for human chemosensory communication and well-being. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 375, 20190265 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 180.Stevenson R. J., An initial evaluation of the functions of human olfaction. Chem. Senses 35, 3–20 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 181.Eliyan Y., Wroblewski K. E., McClintock M. K., Pinto J. M., Olfactory dysfunction predicts the development of depression in older US adults. Chem. Senses 46, bjaa075 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 182.Haehner A., Hummel T., Hummel C., Sommer U., Junghanns S., Reichmann H., Olfactory loss may be a first sign of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 22, 839–842 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 183.Hummel T., Sekinger B., Wolf S. R., Pauli E., Kobal G., ‘Sniffin’ Sticks’: Olfactory performance assessed by the combined testing of odor identification, odor discrimination and olfactory threshold. Chem. Senses 22, 39–52 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 184.Birte-Antina W., Ilona C., Antje H., Thomas H., Olfactory training with older people. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 33, 212–220 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 185.Oleszkiewicz A., Abriat A., Doelz G., Azema E., Hummel T., Beyond olfaction: Beneficial effects of olfactory training extend to aging-related cognitive decline. Behav. Neurosci. 135, 732–740 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 186.Pieniak M., Oleszkiewicz A., Avaro V., Calegari F., Hummel T., Olfactory training–Thirteen years of research reviewed. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 141, 104853 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 187.Sabiniewicz A., Hoffmann L., Haehner A., Hummel T., Symptoms of depression change with olfactory function. Sci. Rep. 12, 5656 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 188.Cooper K. W., Brann D. H., Farruggia M. C., Bhutani S., Pellegrino R., Tsukahara T., Weinreb C., Joseph P. V., Larson E. D., Parma V., Albers M. W., Barlow L. A., Datta S. R., di Pizio A., COVID-19 and the chemical senses: Supporting players take center stage. Neuron 107, 219–233 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 189.Doty R. L., Olfactory dysfunction in COVID-19: Pathology and long-term implications for brain health. Trends Mol. Med. 28, 781–794 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 190.Parma V., Ohla K., Veldhuizen M. G., Niv M. Y., Kelly C. E., Bakke A. J., Cooper K. W., Bouysset C., Pirastu N., Dibattista M., Kaur R., Liuzza M. T., Pepino M. Y., Schöpf V., Pereda-Loth V., Olsson S. B., Gerkin R. C., Rohlfs Domínguez P., Albayay J., Farruggia M. C., Bhutani S., Fjaeldstad A. W., Kumar R., Menini A., Bensafi M., Sandell M., Konstantinidis I., di Pizio A., Genovese F., Öztürk L., Thomas-Danguin T., Frasnelli J., Boesveldt S., Saatci Ö., Saraiva L. R., Lin C., Golebiowski J., Hwang L. D., Ozdener M. H., Guàrdia M. D., Laudamiel C., Ritchie M., Havlícek J., Pierron D., Roura E., Navarro M., Nolden A. A., Lim J., Whitcroft K. L., Colquitt L. R., Ferdenzi C., Brindha E. V., Altundag A., Macchi A., Nunez-Parra A., Patel Z. M., Fiorucci S., Philpott C. M., Smith B. C., Lundström J. N., Mucignat C., Parker J. K., van den Brink M., Schmuker M., Fischmeister F. P. S., Heinbockel T., Shields V. D. C., Faraji F., Santamaría E., Fredborg W. E. A., Morini G., Olofsson J. K., Jalessi M., Karni N., D’Errico A., Alizadeh R., Pellegrino R., Meyer P., Huart C., Chen B., Soler G. M., Alwashahi M. K., Welge-Lüssen A., Freiherr J., de Groot J. H. B., Klein H., Okamoto M., Singh P. B., Hsieh J. W.; GCCR Group Author, Abdulrahman O., Dalton P., Yan C. H., Voznessenskaya V. V., Chen J., Sell E. A., Walsh-Messinger J., Archer N. S., Koyama S., Deary V., Roberts S. C., Yanık H., Albayrak S., L. M. Nováková, Croijmans I., Mazal P. P., Moein S. T., Margulis E., Mignot C., Mariño S., Georgiev D., Kaushik P. K., Malnic B., Wang H., Seyed-Allaei S., Yoluk N., Razzaghi-Asl S., Justice J. M., Restrepo D., Reed D. R., Hummel T., Munger S. D., Hayes J. E., More than smell—COVID-19 is associated with severe impairment of smell, taste, and chemesthesis. Chem. Senses 45, 609–622 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 191.Zazhytska M., Kodra A., Hoagland D. A., Frere J., Fullard J. F., Shayya H., McArthur N. G., Moeller R., Uhl S., Omer A. D., Gottesman M. E., Firestein S., Gong Q., Canoll P. D., Goldman J. E., Roussos P., tenOever B. R., Overdevest J. B., Lomvardas S., Non-cell-autonomous disruption of nuclear architecture as a potential cause of COVID-19-induced anosmia. Cell 185, 1052–1064.e1012 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 192.Hunter S. R., Dalton P. H., The need for sensory nutrition research in individuals with smell loss. Clin. Nutr. Open Sci. 46, 35–41 (2022). [Google Scholar]
  • 193.Chen B., Stein A., Olesch F.-T., Hummel T., Odor deprivation influences human olfactory function. Physiol. Behav. 262, 114090 (2023). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 194.Wu K. N., Tan B. K., Howard J. D., Conley D. B., Gottfried J. A., Olfactory input is critical for sustaining odor quality codes in human orbitofrontal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 1313–1319 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 195.Chen B., Wang Q., Zhong X., Heyne L., Ning Y., Hummel T., Can we assess the sense of smell through a face mask? Int. Forum Allergy Rhinol. 10, 1264–1265 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 196.Chen B., Boesen A., O. FT, H. T, A life without smell: Olfactory function in people working in odorless rooms. Laryngoscope 134, 382–387 (2024). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 197.Woo C. C., Leon M., Environmental enrichment as an effective treatment for autism: A randomized controlled trial. Behav. Neurosci. 127, 487–497 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 198.Ulrich R., Simons R. F., Losito B. D., Fiorito E., Miles M. A., Zelson M., Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 11, 201–230 (1991). [Google Scholar]
  • 199.Berman M. G., Kross E., Krpan K. M., Askren M. K., Burson A., Deldin P. J., Kaplan S., Sherdell L., Gotlib I. H., Jonides J., Interacting with nature improves cognition and affect for individuals with depression. J. Affect. Disord. 140, 300–305 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 200.Bratman G. N., Daily G. C., Levy B. J., Gross J. J., The benefits of nature experience: Improved affect and cognition. Landsc. Urban Plan. 138, 41–50 (2015). [Google Scholar]
  • 201.Ohly H., White M. P., Wheeler B. W., Bethel A., Attention restoration theory: A systematic review of the attention restoration potential of exposure to natural environments. J. Toxicol. Environ. Health B. 19, 305–343 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 202.Devanand D. P., Lee S., Manly J., Andrews H., Schupf N., Masurkar A., Stern Y., Mayeux R., Doty R. L., Olfactory identification deficits and increased mortality in the community. Ann. Neurol. 78, 401–411 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 203.Djordjevic J., Jones-Gotman M., De Sousa K., Chertkow H., Olfaction in patients with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 29, 693–706 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 204.Marin C., Vilas D., Langdon C., Alobid I., López-Chacón M., Haehner A., Hummel T., Mullol J., Olfactory dysfunction in neurodegenerative diseases. Curr. Allergy Asthma Rep. 18, 42 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 205.Roberts R. O., Christianson T. J. H., Kremers W. K., Mielke M. M., Machulda M. M., Vassilaki M., Alhurani R. E., Geda Y. E., Knopman D. S., Petersen R. C., Association between olfactory dysfunction and amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease dementia. JAMA Neurol. 73, 93–101 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 206.Greenberg M. I., Curtis J. A., Vearrier D., The perception of odor is not a surrogate marker for chemical exposure: A review of factors influencing human odor perception. Clin. Toxicol. 51, 70–76 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 207.M. Nakagawa, H. Nagai, M. Nakamura, W. Fujii, T. Inui, Influence of odors on human mental stress and fatigue. R. L. Doty, D. Müller-Schwarze (Eds.) Chemical Signals in Vertebrates 6. (Springer, 1992), pp. 581–585. [Google Scholar]
  • 208.Wells N. M., Evans G. W., Nearby nature: A buffer of life stress among rural children. Environ. Behav. 35, 311–330 (2003). [Google Scholar]
  • 209.Balvanera P., Pfisterer A. B., Buchmann N., He J. S., Nakashizuka T., Raffaelli D., Schmid B., Quantifying the evidence for biodiversity effects on ecosystem functioning and services. Ecol. Lett. 9, 1146–1156 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 210.Balvanera P., Siddique I., Dee L., Paquette A., Isbell F., Gonzalez A., Byrnes J., O’Connor M. I., Hungate B. A., Griffin J. N., Linking biodiversity and ecosystem services: Current uncertainties and the necessary next steps. Bioscience 64, 49–57 (2014). [Google Scholar]
  • 211.Y.-F. Tuan, Passing Strange and Wonderful: Aesthetics, Nature, and Culture. (Island Press, 1994). [Google Scholar]
  • 212.M. A. Lindquist, “Aesthetics, olfaction, and environment” in Theoretical Perspectives on Smell, A. Keller, B. D. Young, Eds. (Routledge, 2023). [Google Scholar]
  • 213.Marselle M. R., Hartig T., Cox D. T. C., de Bell S., Knapp S., Lindley S., Triguero-Mas M., Böhning-Gaese K., Braubach M., Cook P. A., de Vries S., Heintz-Buschart A., Hofmann M., Irvine K. N., Kabisch N., Kolek F., Kraemer R., Markevych I., Martens D., Müller R., Nieuwenhuijsen M., Potts J. M., Stadler J., Walton S., Warber S. L., Bonn A., Pathways linking biodiversity to human health: A conceptual framework. Environ. Int. 150, 106420 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 214.Michaiel A. M., Bernard A., Neurobiology and changing ecosystems: Toward understanding the impact of anthropogenic influences on neurons and circuits. Front Neural Circuits 16, 995354 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 215.Kahn P. H. Jr., Weiss T., The importance of children interacting with big nature. Child. Youth Environ. 27, 7–24 (2017). [Google Scholar]
  • 216.Weiss T., Kahn P. H. Jr., Lam L.-W., Children’s interactions with relatively wild nature associated with more relational behavior: A model of child-nature interaction in a forest preschool. J. Environ. Psychol. 86, 101941 (2023). [Google Scholar]
  • 217.White M. P., Bratman G. N., Pahl S., Young G., Cracknell D., Elliott L. R., Affective reactions to losses and gains in biodiversity: Testing a prospect theory approach. J. Environ. Psychol. 72, 101502 (2020). [Google Scholar]
  • 218.A. François, From Polysemy to Semantic Change: Towards a Typology of Lexical Semantic Associations, in Semantic maps and the typology of colexification: Intertwining polysemous networks across languages. (Benjamins, 2008), vol. 106, pp. 163–215. [Google Scholar]
  • 219.Young D., The smell of greenness: Cultural synaesthesia in the Western Desert. Etnofoor 18, 61–77 (2005). [Google Scholar]
  • 220.Perciavalle V., Blandini M., Fecarotta P., Buscemi A., di Corrado D., Bertolo L., Fichera F., Coco M., The role of deep breathing on stress. Neurol. Sci. 38, 451–458 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 221.Bensafi M., Porter J., Pouliot S., Mainland J., Johnson B., Zelano C., Young N., Bremner E., Aframian D., Khan R., Sobel N., Olfactomotor activity during imagery mimics that during perception. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 1142–1144 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 222.Lazarini F., Roze E., Lannuzel A., Lledo P. M., The microbiome-nose-brain axis in health and disease. Trends Neurosci. 45, 718–721 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 223.Celebi Sozener Z., Ozdel Ozturk B., Cerci P., Turk M., Gorgulu Akin B., Akdis M., Altiner S., Ozbey U., Ogulur I., Mitamura Y., Yilmaz I., Nadeau K., Ozdemir C., Mungan D., Akdis C. A., Epithelial barrier hypothesis: Effect of the external exposome on the microbiome and epithelial barriers in allergic disease. Allergy 77, 1418–1449 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 224.Koskinen K., Reichert J. L., Hoier S., Schachenreiter J., Duller S., Moissl-Eichinger C., Schöpf V., The nasal microbiome mirrors and potentially shapes olfactory function. Sci. Rep. 8, 1296 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 225.Biswas K., Wagner Mackenzie B., Ballauf C., Draf J., Douglas R. G., Hummel T., Loss of bacterial diversity in the sinuses is associated with lower smell discrimination scores. Sci. Rep. 10, 16422 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 226.Ruokolainen L., Paalanen L., Karkman A., Laatikainen T., von Hertzen L., Vlasoff T., Markelova O., Masyuk V., Auvinen P., Paulin L., Alenius H., Fyhrquist N., Hanski I., Mäkelä M. J., Zilber E., Jousilahti P., Vartiainen E., Haahtela T., Significant disparities in allergy prevalence and microbiota between the young people in Finnish and Russian Karelia. Clin. Exp. Allergy 47, 665–674 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 227.E. Yong, An immense world: How animal senses reveal the hidden realms around us. (Knopf Canada, 2022). [Google Scholar]
  • 228.Drew L., Olfactory receptors are not unique to the nose. Nature 606, S14–S17 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 229.Chan K. M., Balvanera P., Benessaiah K., Chapman M., Díaz S., Gómez-Baggethun E., Gould R., Hannahs N., Jax K., Klain S., Luck G. W., Martín-López B., Muraca B., Norton B., Ott K., Pascual U., Satterfield T., Tadaki M., Taggart J., Turner N., Why protect nature? Rethinking values and the environment. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 113, 1462–1465 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 230.Kida H., Fukutani Y., Mainland J. D., de March C. A., Vihani A., Li Y. R., Chi Q., Toyama A., Liu L., Kameda M., Yohda M., Matsunami H., Vapor detection and discrimination with a panel of odorant receptors. Nat. Commun. 9, 4556 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 231.Lee B. K., Mayhew E. J., Sanchez-Lengeling B., Wei J. N., Qian W. W., Little K. A., Andres M., Nguyen B. B., Moloy T., Yasonik J., Parker J. K., Gerkin R. C., Mainland J. D., Wiltschko A. B., A principal odor map unifies diverse tasks in olfactory perception. Science 381, 999–1006 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 232.A.-S. Barwich, Smellosophy: What the nose tells the mind. (Harvard Univ. Press, 2020). [Google Scholar]
  • 233.Han P., Zang Y., Akshita J., Hummel T., Magnetic resonance imaging of human olfactory dysfunction. Brain Topogr. 32, 987–997 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 234.Hedner M., Larsson M., Arnold N., Zucco G. M., Hummel T., Cognitive factors in odor detection, odor discrimination, and odor identification tasks. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 32, 1062–1067 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 235.Hummel T., Olgun S., Gerber J., Huchel U., Frasnelli J., Brain responses to odor mixtures with sub-threshold components. Front. Psychol. 4, 786 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 236.Shepherd G. M., Smell images and the flavour system in the human brain. Nature 444, 316–321 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 237.Sorokowska A., Sorokowski P., Hummel T., Huanca T., Olfaction and environment: Tsimane’ of Bolivian rainforest have lower threshold of odor detection than industrialized German people. PLOS ONE 8, e69203 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 238.Zufall F., Leinders-Zufall T., The cellular and molecular basis of odor adaptation. Chem. Senses 25, 473–481 (2000). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 239.Liu Y., Toet A., Krone T., van Stokkum R., Eijsman S., van Erp J. B. F., A network model of affective odor perception. PLOS ONE 15, e0236468 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 240.Trimmer C., Keller A., Murphy N. R., Snyder L. L., Willer J. R., Nagai M. H., Katsanis N., Vosshall L. B., Matsunami H., Mainland J. D., Genetic variation across the human olfactory receptor repertoire alters odor perception. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116, 9475–9480 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 241.Keller A., Gerkin R. C., Guan Y., Dhurandhar A., Turu G., Szalai B., Mainland J. D., Ihara Y., Yu C. W., Wolfinger R., Vens C., schietgat, de Grave K., Norel R., DREAM Olfaction Prediction Consortium, Stolovitzky G., Cecchi G. A., Vosshall L. B., Meyer P., Predicting human olfactory perception from chemical features of odor molecules. Science 355, 820–826 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 242.Keller A., Hempstead M., Gomez I. A., Gilbert A. N., Vosshall L. B., An olfactory demography of a diverse metropolitan population. BMC Neurosci. 13, 122 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 243.Malnic B., Godfrey P. A., Buck L. B., The human olfactory receptor gene family. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 2584–2589 (2004). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 244.Jacobs L. F., The PROUST hypothesis: The embodiment of olfactory cognition. Anim. Cogn. 26, 59–72 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 245.Reep R., Finlay B., Darlington R., The limbic system in mammalian brain evolution. Brain Behav. Evol. 70, 57–70 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 246.Ache B. W., Young J. M., Olfaction: Diverse species, conserved principles. Neuron 48, 417–430 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 247.Baker K. L., Dickinson M., Findley T. M., Gire D. H., Louis M., Suver M. P., Verhagen J. V., Nagel K. I., Smear M. C., Algorithms for olfactory search across species. J. Neurosci. 38, 9383–9389 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 248.Jacobs L. F., From chemotaxis to the cognitive map: The function of olfaction. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 10693–10700 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 249.Jacobs L. F., How the evolution of air breathing shaped hippocampal function. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 377, 20200532 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 250.Jacobs L. F., Schenk F., Unpacking the cognitive map: The parallel map theory of hippocampal function. Psychol. Rev. 110, 285–315 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 251.Jayakumar S., Murthy V. N., A new angle on odor trail tracking. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 119, e2121332119 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 252.Reddy G., Murthy V. N., Vergassola M., Olfactory sensing and navigation in turbulent environments. Annu. Rev. Condens. 13, 191–213 (2022). [Google Scholar]
  • 253.Rokni D., Murthy V. N., Analysis and synthesis in olfaction. ACS Chem. Nerosci. 5, 870–872 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 254.Majid A., Speed L., Croijmans I., Arshamian A., What makes a better smeller? Perception 46, 406–430 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 255.Doty R. L., Shaman P., Applebaum S. L., Giberson R., Siksorski L., Rosenberg L., Smell identification ability: Changes with age. Science 226, 1441–1443 (1984). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 256.Doty R. L., Cameron E. L., Sex differences and reproductive hormone influences on human odor perception. Physiol. Behav. 97, 213–228 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 257.Hummel T., Guel H., Delank W., Olfactory sensitivity of subjects working in odorous environments. Chem. Senses 29, 533–536 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 258.Kuehn M., Welsch H., Zahnert T., Hummel T., Changes of pressure and humidity affect olfactory function. Eur. Arch. Otorhinolaryngol. 265, 299–302 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 259.Oleszkiewicz A., Alizadeh R., Altundag A., Chen B., Corrai A., Fanari R., Farhadi M., Gupta N., Habel R., Hudson R., Hughes J. L., Joshi A., Kamrava S. K., Luckett C., Mahmut M. K., Masala C., Mori E., Pellegrino R., Piras R., Resler K., Rivas-Castro A., Saluja S., Sharma S., Shimmura H., Soler G. M., Stefańczyk M. M., Sun Z., Thaploo D., Wei Y., Yan X., Hummel T., Global study of variability in olfactory sensitivity. Behav. Neurosci. 134, 394–406 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 260.Lötsch J., Reichmann H., Hummel T., Different odor tests contribute differently to the evaluation of olfactory loss. Chem. Senses 33, 17–21 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 261.Oleszkiewicz A., Behl O., Grahl T., Hummel T., Odor discrimination in children aged 4-12 years. Chem. Senses 47, 1–8 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 262.Stevenson R. J., Mahmut M., Sundqvist N., Age-related changes in odor discrimination. Dev. Psychol. 43, 253–260 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 263.Wilson D. A., Stevenson R. J., Olfactory perceptual learning: The critical role of memory in odor discrimination. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 27, 307–328 (2003). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 264.Majid A., Kruspe N., Hunter-gatherer olfaction is special. Curr Biol. 28, 409–413.e402 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 265.Doty R. L., Smith R., McKeown D. A., Raj J., Tests of human olfactory function: Principal components analysis suggests that most measure a common source of variance. Percept. Psychophys. 56, 701–707 (1994). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 266.De Luca R., Botelho D., The unconscious perception of smells as a driver of consumer responses: A framework integrating the emotion-cognition approach to scent marketing. AMS Rev. 11, 145–161 (2021). [Google Scholar]
  • 267.Holland R. W., Hendriks M., Aarts H., Smells like clean spirit. Nonconscious effects of scent on cognition and behavior. Psychol. Sci. 16, 689–693 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 268.Croy I., Krone F., Walker S., Hummel T., Olfactory processing: Detection of rapid changes. Chem. Senses 40, 351–355 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 269.Koster E. P., Moller P., Mojet J., A “Misfit” theory of spontaneous conscious odor perception (MITSCOP): Reflections on the role and function of odor memory in everyday life. Front. Psychol. 5, 64 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 270.Doty R. L., The olfactory vector hypothesis of neurodegenerative disease: Is it viable? Ann. Neurol. 63, 7–15 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 271.Lucchini R., Dorman D., Elder A., Veronesi B., Neurological impacts from inhalation of pollutants and the nose–brain connection. Neurotoxicology 33, 838–841 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 272.Calderón-Garcidueñas L., Solt A. C., Henríquez-Roldán C., Torres-Jardón R., Nuse B., Herritt L., Villarreal-Calderón R., Osnaya N., Stone I., García R., Brooks D. M., González-Maciel A., Reynoso-Robles R., Delgado-Chávez R., Reed W., Long-term air pollution exposure is associated with neuroinflammation, an altered innate immune response, disruption of the blood-brain barrier, ultrafine particulate deposition, and accumulation of amyloid β-42 and α-synuclein in children and young adults. Toxicol. Pathol. 36, 289–310 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 273.Fowler D., Pilegaard K., Sutton M. A., Ambus P., Raivonen M., Duyzer J., Simpson D., Fagerli H., Fuzzi S., Schjoerring J. K., Granier C., Neftel A., Isaksen I. S. A., Laj P., Maione M., Monks P. S., Burkhardt J., Daemmgen U., Neirynck J., Personne E., Wichink-Kruit R., Butterbach-Bahl K., Flechard C., Tuovinen J. P., Coyle M., Gerosa G., Loubet B., Altimir N., Gruenhage L., Ammann C., Cieslik S., Paoletti E., Mikkelsen T. N., Ro-Poulsen H., Cellier P., Cape J. N., Horváth L., Loreto F., Niinemets Ü., Palmer P. I., Rinne J., Misztal P., Nemitz E., Nilsson D., Pryor S., Gallagher M. W., Vesala T., Skiba U., Brüggemann N., Zechmeister-Boltenstern S., Williams J., O'Dowd C., Facchini M. C., de Leeuw G., Flossman A., Chaumerliac N., Erisman J. W., Atmospheric composition change: Ecosystems–atmosphere interactions. Atmos. Environ. 43, 5193–5267 (2009). [Google Scholar]
  • 274.Gomes Alves E., Taylor T., Robin M., Pinheiro Oliveira D., Schietti J., Duvoisin Júnior S., Zannoni N., Williams J., Hartmann C., Gonçalves J. F. C., Schöngart J., Wittmann F., Piedade M. T. F., Seasonal shifts in isoprenoid emission composition from three hyperdominant tree species in central Amazonia. Plant Biol. 24, 721–733 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 275.Lelieveld J., Butler T. M., Crowley J. N., Dillon T. J., Fischer H., Ganzeveld L., Harder H., Lawrence M. G., Martinez M., Taraborrelli D., Williams J., Atmospheric oxidation capacity sustained by a tropical forest. Nature 452, 737–740 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 276.Kammer J., Perraudin E., Flaud P. M., Lamaud E., Bonnefond J. M., Villenave E., Observation of nighttime new particle formation over the French Landes forest. Sci. Total Environ. 621, 1084–1092 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 277.Kesselmeier J., Staudt M., Biogenic volatile organic compounds (VOC): An overview on emission, physiology and ecology. J. Atmos. Chem. 33, 23–88 (1999). [Google Scholar]
  • 278.Wagner P., Kuttler W., Biogenic and anthropogenic isoprene in the near-surface urban atmosphere–A case study in Essen, Germany. Sci. Total Environ. 475, 104–115 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 279.Yassaa N., Williams J., Enantiomeric monoterpene emissions from natural and damaged Scots pine in a boreal coniferous forest measured using solid-phase microextraction and gas chromatography/mass spectrometry. J. Chromatogr. A 1141, 138–144 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 280.J. Williams, “Mass spectrometric methods for atmospheric trace gases” in Analytical Techniques for Atmospheric Measurement, D. E. Heard, Ed. (Blackwell Publishing Ltd., 2006). [Google Scholar]
  • 281.Guenther A., Geron C., Pierce T., Lamb B., Harley P., Fall R., Natural emissions of non-methane volatile organic compounds, carbon monoxide, and oxides of nitrogen from North America. Atmos. Environ. 34, 2205–2230 (2000). [Google Scholar]
  • 282.Pressley S., Lamb B., Westberg H., Guenther A., Chen J., Allwine E., Monoterpene emissions from a Pacific Northwest old-growth forest and impact on regional biogenic VOC emission estimates. Atmos. Environ. 38, 3089–3098 (2004). [Google Scholar]
  • 283.Hakola H., Laurila T., Rinne J., Puhto K., The ambient concentrations of biogenic hydrocarbons at a northern European, boreal site. Atmos. Environ. 34, 4971–4982 (2000). [Google Scholar]
  • 284.Holopainen J. K., Virjamo V., Ghimire R. P., Blande J. D., Julkunen-Tiitto R., Kivimarnpaa M., Climate change effects on secondary compounds of forest trees in the northern hemisphere. Front. Plant Sci. 9, 1445 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 285.Choi K., Ko D. W., Kim K. W., Shin M. Y., A modeling approach for quantifying human-beneficial terpene emission in the forest: A pilot study applying to a recreational forest in South Korea. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19, 8278 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 286.Williams J., Yassaa N., Bartenbach S., Lelieveld J., Mirror image hydrocarbons from Tropical and Boreal forests. Atmos. Chem. Phys. 7, 973–980 (2007). [Google Scholar]
  • 287.Mori K., Chirality and insect pheromones. Chirality 10, 578–586 (1998). [Google Scholar]
  • 288.Brook R. D., Rajagopalan S., Pope C. A. III, Brook J. R., Bhatnagar A., Diez-Roux A. V., Holguin F., Hong Y., Luepker R. V., Mittleman M. A., Peters A., Siscovick D., Smith S. C. Jr., Whitsel L., Kaufman J. D., Particulate matter air pollution and cardiovascular Disease. Circulation 121, 2331–2378 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 289.Dockery D. W., Pope C. A., Xu X., Spengler J. D., Ware J. H., Fay M. E., Ferris B. G. Jr., Speizer F. E., An association between air pollution and mortality in six U.S. cities. N. Engl. J. Med. 329, 1753–1759 (1993). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 290.Franklin B. A., Brook R., Arden Pope C. III, Air pollution and cardiovascular disease. Curr. Probl. Cardiol. 40, 207–238 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 291.Killin L. O., Starr J. M., Shiue I. J., Russ T. C., Environmental risk factors for dementia: A systematic review. BMC Geriatr. 16, 1–28 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 292.Yuchi W., Sbihi H., Davies H., Tamburic L., Brauer M., Road proximity, air pollution, noise, green space and neurologic disease incidence: A population-based cohort study. Environ. Health 19, 8 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 293.Hart B. L., Biological basis of the behavior of sick animals. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 12, 123–137 (1988). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 294.Lee C.-H., Giuliani F., The role of inflammation in depression and fatigue. Front. Immunol. 10, 1696 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 295.Frank P., Jokela M., Batty G. D., Cadar D., Steptoe A., Kivimäki M., Association between systemic inflammation and individual symptoms of depression: A pooled analysis of 15 population-based cohort studies. Am. J. Psychiatry 178, 1107–1118 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Science Advances are provided here courtesy of American Association for the Advancement of Science

RESOURCES