ABSTRACT
The trend of global population aging is becoming increasingly evident, with the proportion of the elderly population continuously rising, making it one of the most profound demographic trends of the 21st century. As people age, their sensory functions generally decline, such as vision, hearing, smell, and taste, which not only affects their food choices and enjoyment but can also lead to health issues like malnutrition and weight loss. In addition, chronic diseases such as diabetes, cardiovascular diseases, and the COVID‐19 pandemic can exacerbate sensory impairments in older adults. Currently, most sensory evaluation methods are designed for healthy adults and have limitations when applied to the elderly, such as visual impairment making it difficult to see scoring sheets or linear scales, and hearing impairment preventing understanding of questions and requests from sensory analysts, leading to potential biases and inaccuracies in data collection. Therefore, there is an urgent need to develop sensory evaluation systems suitable for older adults to better understand and address their sensory changes due to aging and health conditions. This review summarizes the sensory abilities, cognitive functions, and physical health status of older adults; explores how to improve their food intake and appetite through flavor‐enhanced foods; and reviews current sensory evaluation methods, pointing out their limitations and the necessity for developing new approaches to meet the diverse needs of older adults. Future research should deepen the understanding of the neurobiological mechanisms of sensory decline, develop implicit sensory evaluation methods based on EEG and facial microexpressions, and optimize personalized sensory compensation strategies that are safe and sustainable in order to improve the dietary health and quality of life of older adults.
Keywords: aging population, dietary behavior, elderly nutrition, food product design, sensory evaluation, sensory impairment
Aging impacts sensory abilities in older adults. Sensory impairment in older people displays different degrees and aspects. Food sensory compensation strategies have different effects on food intake.
1. Introduction
The proportion of elderly people in the world's population is increasing, and population aging may become one of the most dominant social trends in the 21st century. According to the World Population Prospects 2022, the proportion of people aged 65 and over will be 10% globally in 2023 and is projected to reach 16.5% by 2050, which means that one in six people will be aged 65 and over (United Nations 2022). In 2022, 19.8% of China's population was aged 60, with 14.9% of the population aged 65 and over (National Bureau of Statistics of China 2020). It is expected that in 2035, China's elderly population will exceed 400 million (NHSC 2022).
Today, most of the products in the market are aimed at young people, and as the number of elderly people increases, there is a need to give more consideration to their needs and designing food products according to their own characteristics. Many studies have confirmed that as people age, their senses deteriorate to varying degrees and that sensory changes can have an impact on food selection and enjoyment (Davenport 2004). Impairments in sensory perception are often associated with aging. Visual and auditory impairments may be widely recognized as sensory impairments, followed by perceptual impairments such as taste, smell, and texture (Doty and Kamath 2014; Hutchings et al. 2014; Withers et al. 2013). Older adults also often face other physical sensory impairments such as dysphagia, wearing dentures, or dysfunction. They may also be taking medication and may have psychosensory impairments such as diminished cognition and poor mental performance, which may reduce their ability to perceive sensory stimulation and may lead to insufficient food intake, placing them at risk for protein deficiency and undernutrition (Doets and Kremer 2016). Older adults have a growing need for variety and richness in the products they consume, and understanding these needs and how they change is a key step in designing products for older adults. Our work summarizes the deficits in sensory functions (taste, smell, vision, hearing, chemosensation) and their causes, and the effects of COVID‐19 on sensory perception (mainly focusing on taste and smell) in older adults.
Sensory evaluation is the process of evaluating food in order to accurately measure human responses to it and to minimize potential nonsensory influences such as brand effects (Lawless and Heymann 2010). This work provides an overview of sensory evaluation methods currently used with older adults. These methods include not only difference tests, descriptive analysis tests, and ranking tests but also experiments measuring the emotions of older adults, such as preference, familiarity, and acceptability. Meiselman argues that dietary behaviors consist of three variables: the food, the person, and the environment (which includes all environmental factors; the physical environment in which the shopping and eating takes place, as well as the social and economic context) (Meiselman 2007). Currently, most sensory methods are conducted with “healthy” consumers as test subjects. The applicability of these methods to healthy or frail older adults has yet to be fully considered, and some researchers have not focused on older adults. However, in today's society, where there is a large elderly population, it is increasingly important to develop sensory evaluation systems that are appropriate for them and then to produce tasty and nutritious food for them. Some of the existing sensory testing methods may not be friendly to the elderly with sensory impairments; for example, the visually impaired are unable to see the scoring sheet or line scale; the hearing impaired are unable to understand the questions and requests from the sensory analyst, and have poor dexterity in tasting or categorizing the samples and have difficulty in writing and expressing themselves; and subjects may easily become fatigued if the sample size set is too large. All these factors can impose restrictions on the application of sensory evaluation methods in various ways. It is crucial to consider these aspects when designing sensory studies involving older consumers. However, aside from a few exceptions (Duffy et al. 1999; Griep et al. 1997; Mojet, Christ‐Hazelhof, et al. 2005, Mojet, Köster, et al. 2005), the majority of sensory studies overlook the specificity of older adults.
This work categorizes older adults, encompassing both “healthy” and “frail” older adults, as well as those living independently and residing in nursing homes. Older adults living in nursing homes, as well as those who are frail, are mentioned. Additionally, both older persons living independently and those residing in nursing homes are discussed. These groups of older adults differ considerably in terms of their sensory abilities (sight, hearing, taste, smell, and mouth), cognitive abilities, and physical health. The effects of flavor‐enhancing foods on food intake, appetite, preference, and acceptability in older adults and measures to increase food intake in older adults are now more widely accepted.
We conducted a systematic review of the sensory function deficits in older adults and their causes, as well as the methods used for sensory evaluation, food preference, and intake assessment in this population. Furthermore, we explored the need for and measures of increasing food intake in older adults through enhancing sensory attributes. Building on this foundation, we summarized the current research and proposed new ideas and perspectives.
2. Sensory Impairment and Influencing Factors in Older Adults
Food enjoyment is a key aspect of the human appetite and affects psychological and physical health, relationships, and self‐esteem, potentially leading to long‐term implications for recovery, especially for older adults (Drewnowski and Evans 2001; Lumbers and Raats 2006; Abrahams 2020). Age‐related changes in sensory perception have become a consensus. Sensory impairment in older adults displays different degrees and aspects. In fact, the phenomenon of impairment of sensory perception in older adults is associated with different reasons, such as physical state, psychological state, and others (Table 1). The impairment of oral function for food texture perception has been reviewed very recently by Liu et al. (2022). This section will examine the sensory perception state (especially, taste and smell) of older adults and the main causes of related impairment.
TABLE 1.
Examples of sensory impairment in the elderly.
Information reference | Participants' information | Main finding |
---|---|---|
Laska (2001) |
Age: 100 participants aged 65–88 years Area: Germany |
The ability to discriminate chemical stimuli with trigeminal properties decreases with aging in the elderly |
Hummel et al. (2003) |
Age: 35 patients with olfactory Dysfunction aged 28–69 years; 17 normosmic subjects aged 28–82 years Area: Germany |
The sensitivity of the intranasal trigeminal nerve decreases in older adults as they age |
Mojet (2003) |
Age: 21 participants aged 60–75 years Area: Wageningen |
Perception of salty taste decreases with age in the elderly |
Koskinen and Tuorila (2005) |
Age: 27 participants aged 66–83 years Area: Helsinki |
The elderly have some difficulty in distinguishing the intensity of odors |
Fukunaga et al. (2005) |
Age: 30 participants aged 65–85 years Area: Japan |
Compared to young people, older adults have higher thresholds with the four basic taste sensations |
Kremer et al. (2007b) |
Age: 22 participants aged 60–85 years Area: the Netherlands |
Decreased olfactory sensitivity in the elderly affects their evaluation of taste intensity |
Baraldi et al. (2007) |
Age: 211 participants aged 60–99 years Area: Brazil |
Hearing loss deepens with age and is greater in the 80–89‐year‐old age group with higher frequency damage |
Kawagishi et al. (2009) |
Age: 60 participants aged 66–91 years Area: Japan |
Compared to young people, the ability to recognize the tongue in three dimensions is significantly reduced in the elderly |
Solemdal et al. (2012a, 2012b) |
Age: 174 participants aged 70–103 years Area: Germany |
Oral diseases can affect the perception of sweet and salty tastes in the elderly |
Sasano et al. (2015) |
Age: 82 participants aged 65–89 years Area: Japan |
Aging causes the elderly to lose their sense of taste, but four basic taste sensations remain |
Sulmont‐Rosse et al. (2015) |
Age: 559 participants aged 65–99 years Area: France |
Aging can lead to impairment of chemosensory abilities in the elderly |
Yoshinaka et al. (2015) |
Age: 949 participants aged 79–81 years Area: Japan |
Loss of taste sensation in the elderly with aging |
Joussain et al. (2015) |
Age: 32 anosmic participants aged 30–72 years; 32 hyposmic participants aged 27–75 years; 32 controls participants aged 28–73 years Area: Lyon, Geneva, Helsinki |
The prevalence of olfactory dysfunction in people over 60 years (39%) are significant higher than people between 18 and 28 years (10%) |
Piotrowicz et al. (2016) |
Age: 3471 participants aged 65–104 years, mean age 78.3 years Area: Poland |
Vision impairment, hearing impairment, and cognitive impairment increased with age |
Cui et al. (2017) |
Age: 388 participants aged 50–59 years; 413 participants aged 60–69 years; 157 participants aged 70–79 years; 47 participants aged 80 years Area: China |
Vision impairment mainly occurred in Chinese participants aged 70–80 years and increases as the aging process progresses |
Chen et al. (2017) |
Age: 2975 participants aged 60 years Area: 1818 participants were non‐Hispanic White |
Declining cognitive function in the elderly can lead to distance vision impairment |
Birte‐Antina et al. (2017) |
Age: 91 participants aged 50 to 84 years Area: Germany |
As the elderly age, the sense of smell gradually loses its function |
Cavazzana et al. (2018) |
Age: 104 participants aged 50–100 years Area: Norway |
Hearing threshold changes significantly with age |
Mazzatenta et al. (2020) |
Age: 100 participants aged 28–94 years Area: United States |
COVID‐19 patients experience hyposmia and loss of smell, but no relationship was found between the severity of olfactory impairment and age |
Manesse et al. (2021) |
Age: 510 participants aged 50–59 years; 202 participants aged 60–89 years Area: France |
The prevalence of olfactory disorders is high in the French population and is more prevalent in the elderly population |
Chaaban et al. (2021) |
Age: 102 participants aged 19–69 years Area: Denmark |
COVID‐19 causes loss of the senses of smell and taste. |
Yang et al. (2022) |
Age: 24 participants aged 61–73 years Area: China |
Aging significantly reduced gustatory sensitivity (sweetness) but not chemical anesthesia sensitivity in older adults |
2.1. Impact of Aging on Sensory Impairment in Older Adults
Numerous studies on older adults have found that a person's physiological status is associated with their quality of life (Ahmed and Haboubi 2010; Doets and Kremer 2016; Crimmins 2017). In terms of sensory in people's physiological status, the formation of sensory perception cannot be achieved without the functioning of the nervous system. Sensory organs receive stimuli and convert them into nerve impulses, which are transmitted through afferent nerves to the cortical sensory centers of the brain, thus producing sensation (Watson et al. 2010; Connor et al. 2018). Throughout the natural progression from adolescence to adulthood and to old age, the nervous system undergoes various changes that can impair sensory functions, potentially leading to a decline in olfaction, taste, hearing, vision, and chemosensory perception in older adults (Figure 1).
FIGURE 1.
Schematic diagram of sensory decline.
2.1.1. Visual Impairment in Older Adults
Most of the information from the outside world is transmitted to the brain through the visual system. Visual impairment becomes more prevalent with age. Around the age of 40, changes in vision begin to appear (Pathai et al. 2013). Visual impairment in daily life may manifest as malfunctions in the optical system, which are preliminary indicators of the onset of disease or aging, such as optical aberrations and intraocular light scatter of the eye (Artal et al. 2002). The crystalline lens is an active optical element capable of changing the intensity of light throughout the eye by altering its shape, allowing the eye to focus on objects at different distances (Artal 2014). Aging can cause changes in the optical system of the eyes, leading to various visual impairments in the elderly. During the natural aging process, the shape, radii of curvature, dimensions, surface curvatures, and refractive index of the crystalline lens are continuously changing, thus altering the aberrations of the lens (Artal et al. 2002; Berrio et al. 2010; Dubbelman et al. 2001). These physiological changes in the eyes can affect visual image formation in older adults, thereby affecting their daily activities, such as reading, eating, sports, and recognition capabilities.
Vision loss in older adults has been widely documented in numerous studies. Piotrowicz et al. (2016) reported that the prevalence of clinical conditions such as vision impairment (affecting 42% of 3471 Polish participants with a mean age of 78.3 years), hearing impairment, and cognitive impairment increases with age. Similarly, Cui et al. (2017) found that vision impairment predominantly occurs in Chinese individuals aged 70–80 years and becomes more prevalent as people age. Their study involved 388 participants aged 50–59 years, 413 aged 60–69 years, 157 aged 70–79 years, and 47 aged 80 years in China. They noted that the severity and frequency of vision loss varied significantly by disease, with refractive errors and cataracts being more common, while glaucoma and diabetic retinopathy were less so. Nonrefractive eye diseases, particularly cataracts, also increased in incidence with age. Humes and Young (2016) reported similar findings. Cognitive dysfunction is more common in older adults with visual impairment. Jefferis et al. (2012) found that poor visual acuity is associated with poor performance on the MMSE visual items. Several studies have indicated that visual impairment is a significant factor influencing cognitive decline in older adults (Chen et al. 2017; Cavazzana et al. 2018; Nagarajan et al. 2022).
2.1.2. Hearing Impairment in Older Adults
Hearing impairment can reduce the perceived crispness or crunchiness of food, diminishing the sensation of freshness (Tam and Koppel 2021). The primary mechanism of hearing loss (HL) involves damage to the white matter tracts and gray matter in the cranial region. Specifically, the white matter tracts connect the eighth cranial nerve to subcortical nuclei (e.g., cochlear), while the gray matter is linked to the primary auditory cortices (e.g., Heschl's gyri) (Manno et al. 2021; Feng et al. 2018). Moreover, the effects of HL on the brain are multifocal, impacting all age groups, including adolescents, adults, and older adults (Manno et al. 2021). The auditory system relies on hair cells, sensory cells located on the basement membrane (Purves et al. 2004). Age‐related hearing impairment is primarily caused by degenerative changes in the inner ear structures, such as the loss of inner and outer hair cells, deterioration of spiral ganglion cells, and atrophy of the stria vascularis (Zampini and Spence 2004).
Compared to younger individuals, older adults experience a gradual decline in cognitive ability and speech perception, particularly in noisy or complex auditory environments, due to central auditory dysfunction. This decline is a significant factor in hearing impairment among the elderly (Rutherford et al. 2018; Lin et al. 2013). Neuroimaging studies of age‐related HL have shown that neural activation in response to auditory stimuli decreases with age and neural responses to sound gradually become less sensitive, leading to HL (Peelle et al. 2011; Sheline et al. 2009).
Gerontology accepts the idea that HL is a basic principle associated with aging. Studies have shown that age is one of the most significant factors in the prevalence of sensory impairment, particularly among older adults (Besser et al. 2018; Amieva and Ouvrard 2020; Ellis et al. 2021). Age‐related HL is similar to the cumulative effects of ototoxic substances, noise exposure, diseases, and medical treatments. Typical symptoms of HL include a decline in hearing sensitivity, difficulty interpreting speech in noisy environments, slower processing of central acoustic stimuli, and challenges in sound localization (Hill et al. 2017; Huang et al. 2023). According to Haile et al. (2021), substantial HL affects approximately 62.1% of seniors aged 60–64 years worldwide. With an aging population, this trend cannot be underestimated.
2.1.3. Olfactory Impairment in Older Adults
The olfactory system is stimulated by volatile food molecules, which enhance intake and pleasure and aid in food identification (Tan et al. 2018; Arikawa et al. 2020). The peri‐amygdala and pre‐parietal areas of the cerebral cortex, known as the primary olfactory cortex, are connected to the secondary olfactory cortex. Together, these areas are collectively responsible for the perception of smell (Watson et al. 2010; Wilson and East 2020). Olfactory receptor cells detect odors and transmit signals through nerves to the olfactory bulb, which processes the signals and transmits them to the brain (Cerf Ducastel and Murphy 2003; Connor et al. 2018). The neurological and anatomical morphology of the olfactory system undergoes various changes with age, such as decreased vascularization, fewer olfactory receptors, and olfactory bulb shrinkage, as well as reduced olfactory epithelium thickness and mucus secretion. Additionally, aging may reduce the activity of brain areas that process olfactory signals (Rouby et al. 2002; Manesse et al. 2021). The loss of olfactory neuron selectivity and decreased receptor selectivity to olfactory stimuli can also affect olfactory performance (Rawson et al. 2012; Doty and Kamath 2014; Manesse et al. 2021).
Olfactory function impacts the quality of life, enhances pleasantness and intake, and aids in food recognition (Croy et al. 2014; Manesse et al. 2017). Several studies have indicated that aging is primarily responsible for the decline in olfactory function (Annear et al. 2016; Doets and Kremer 2016; Manesse et al. 2021). According to large‐scale research of the French population by Manesse et al. (2021), they recruited 510 older adults aged 50–59 years and 202 aged 60–89 years. They found that the prevalence of olfactory dysfunction is > 30% in people aged over 60 years, compared to only 10% in those aged 4–12 years. Joussain et al. (2015) also found that the prevalence of olfactory dysfunction in people aged 18–28 years (10%) is significantly lower than in those over 60 years (39%). Additionally, several studies have noted that some older adults' olfactory function is similar to that of younger people, while others have minor deficiencies and some have substantial anosmia (Hummel et al. 2017; Manesse et al. 2021). For instance, Koskinen and Tuorila (2005) found that some elderly people showed similar levels to their younger counterparts in smell tests, while those with low ratings tended to have difficulty differentiating various scent intensities.
2.1.4. Taste Impairment in Older Adults
Taste can be fundamentally divided into five basic types: sweet, sour, bitter, salty, and umami (Gravina et al. 2013). The taste system perceives mainly water‐soluble molecules of taste‐presenting substances. Once these molecules enter the mouth, they dissolve in the saliva and interact with the taste receptors on the tongue (Purves et al. 2004).
Many physiological functions related to taste decline with aging, particularly in the oral cavity. Various types of papillae are distributed on the tongue, such as fungiform, foliate, and circumvallate papillae, with the fungiform papillae mainly located at the front of the tongue and surrounded by taste buds (Srur et al. 2010; Nordin 2017). As the density of taste buds decreases with age, these papillae can also be affected, leading to a decrease in taste perception in older adults (Nordin et al. 2006). Additionally, the oral mucosa becomes thinner with aging, with reduced keratinization and thinning of the epithelial structure (Löe and Karring 1971). The salivary glands, which secrete saliva and facilitate the swallowing of food, gradually atrophy, affecting taste perception in the elderly (Sasano et al. 2015; Imoscopi et al. 2012; Moreira et al. 2006). Dental integrity is also an important factor affecting chewing in the elderly, as tooth loss can lead to the use of dentures, which can impair chewing and oral movements (Kremer, Mojet et al. 2007; Wayler et al. 1984; Sergi et al. 2016).
In fact, the decline in taste perception among older adults is mainly characterized by reduced taste sensitivity and decreased perception intensity. As reported by Murphy and Gilmore (1989), elderly individuals need a 74% increase in caffeine concentration to detect a change, while young individuals only require a 34% increase. This phenomenon occurs only with medium and high concentrations of caffeine, not with low concentrations. According to Yoshinaka et al. (2015), there are age‐related differences in taste sensitivity for the four basic tastes among young and old Japanese, with the sensitivity to sweet taste being more robust than the others. Aging was also found to significantly reduce taste sensitivity to sweetness in the experiments of Yang et al. (2022).
However, the extent of age's influence on detection thresholds varies significantly depending on the specific substances examined (Field and Duizer 2016). Methven et al. (2012) analyzed 69 studies on the impact of aging on taste perception. Some studies showed that the average elevations in detection thresholds were 1.4 times for sweet flavors, 1.5 times for sour flavors, 2.0 times for salty flavors, 2.2 times for bitter flavors, and 2.2 times for umami flavors. They concluded that aging causes a reduction in taste perception for all five basic tastes. The rate of degradation of taste perception with aging varies across different studies. Murphy and Gilmore (1989) found a more substantial impact on sweet taste thresholds with age compared to, for example, bitter taste thresholds. Sasano et al. (2015) found that some older individuals only experience the loss of umami taste sensation while still experiencing the other four basic tastes (sweet, salty, sour, and bitter).
2.1.5. Chemosensory Impairment in Older Adults
The trigeminal nerve primarily mediates chemesthetic sensations, such as stinging (e.g., from carbon dioxide in soda), burning (e.g., chili pepper), cooling (e.g., peppermint), or astringency (e.g., wine) (Nordin 2017). The decline in chemesthetic sensation with increasing age is influenced by multiple factors (Stamps 2019; Oleszkiewicz et al. 2020). Some research suggests that age‐related dysfunction is due to changes in the peripheral olfactory system, including a decrease in olfactory receptor neurons, impaired regeneration capacity, and hindered bacterial clearance (Doty and Kamath 2014; Doty 2018; Claus et al. 2022). Laska's (2001) study showed a significant decrease in older adults' ability to discriminate between chemical stimuli with trigeminal properties compared to younger adults. Hummel et al. (2003) found that older subjects had lower intranasal trigeminal sensitivity than younger subjects. The interaction between the intranasal trigeminal and olfactory systems may lead to a marked decline in trigeminal sensitivity. Kremer et al. (2007a, 2007b) and Kremer, Mojet, et al. (2007) also found that younger individuals performed better in trigeminal sensitivity tests, particularly in detecting menthol and ammonia.
However, Yang et al. (2022) found that the detection threshold for capsaicin (found in chili peppers and Sichuan pepper) did not significantly differ between older and younger adults. Similar results were observed in comparisons between young and old adults from the United States and Japan (Fukunaga et al. 2005; Wierenga et al. 2020). This discrepancy requires further investigation and verification, as there are limited studies on the effect of age on chemesthetic experience. Furthermore, some research has shown that younger adults can identify more test pieces with varied shapes than older adults, indicating that older adults have less tongue stereognosis capacity than younger people (Kawagishi et al. 2009).
2.2. Other Factors Influencing Sensory Abilities in Older Adults
Although impairment of sensory perception with age is common in the elderly population, studies have also shown that there is some individual variation in sensory loss in the elderly, which may be related to gender, occupation, environment, diseases, and psychological factors (Tseng et al. 2018). Most studies emphasized the challenge of identifying the ideal type and level of flavor because deficiencies associated with age may be quality dependent and differ in severity between people (Tan et al. 2018; Oleszkiewicz et al. 2020; Claus et al. 2022).
2.2.1. Pathological Conditions and Unhealthy Lifestyle
Age‐related declines in sensory abilities can be exacerbated by various pathological conditions (Solemdal et al. 2012a, 2012b; Sulmont‐Rosse et al. 2015; Gao et al. 2020; Völter et al. 2021). Chronic diseases such as chronic sinonasal disease, rhinosinusitis, and nasal polyposis can impair olfactory function, leading to a reduced ability to detect and enjoy smells (Schubert et al. 2011). Additionally, cardiovascular disease and Alzheimer's disease have been linked to a decline in sensory perception, particularly affecting the ability to process complex sensory information (Völter et al. 2021). Poor oral health, including dental caries and periodontal diseases, can also affect taste perception, as these conditions can alter the oral environment and the sensitivity of taste receptors (Jonathan A 1999). Vision‐related conditions like glaucoma, diabetic retinopathy, age‐related macular degeneration, and cataracts are common among older adults and can lead to significant vision impairments (Whillans et al. 2016). HL can be caused by conditions such as otitis media, which can lead to degeneration and loss of auditory function (Davis et al. 2016). A study by McKee et al. (2018) found that HL in older adults is independently associated with various diseases, including arthritis, cardiovascular disease, diabetes, emphysema, high blood pressure, and stroke. Unhealthy lifestyle habits further contribute to sensory impairments. Smoking has been shown to increase the risk of HL, possibly due to its detrimental effects on blood circulation and the inner ear (Dawes et al. 2014). In contrast, moderate alcohol consumption has been found to be protective against age‐related HL, potentially due to its antioxidant properties (Dawes et al. 2014). Dietary factors, such as high carbohydrate and sugar intake, have also been linked to HL in older adults, possibly due to their impact on metabolic health and inflammation (Gopinath et al. 2010).
2.2.2. Psychological Status
Many studies have highlighted the impact of psychological differences among older adults, particularly focusing on their quality of life, which is closely linked to social stimuli and activities (Fischer et al. 2009; Cavazzana et al. 2018; Olofsson et al. 2021). As Ellis et al. (2021) noted, poor quality of life, often characterized by social isolation, can lead to multiple psychological issues such as loneliness, depression, and anxiety, which in turn can result in sensory impairments. For instance, Robino et al. (2013) investigated 2956 older adults aged 60 and older and found that those with significant alexithymia traits had lower taste sensitivity than normal individuals. According to statistics, more than one in five people over the age of 60 years suffer from mental disorders such as depression, dementia, and anxiety (World Health Organization 2017), and these conditions can affect appetite and food intake.
The prevalence of depression varies globally and can manifest in different forms, including mild depression, bipolar disorder, and dysthymia. Social and biological factors, such as chronic illnesses and the long‐term use of certain medications, contribute to the development of depression in older adults (Rathod et al. 2022). Pause et al. (2001) found that individuals with depression have diminished olfactory ability, and a negative correlation exists between olfactory function and depression symptoms (Pause et al. 2001; Negoias et al. 2010). Alam et al. (2021) surveyed 400 older adults over 65 years in Bangladesh and found a strong association between depression and poor nutritional intake. Quandt et al. (2000) also noted that depression is often linked to loss of appetite and reduced food intake, particularly in women. Loneliness is more prevalent among older adults than in the general adult population (Ong et al. 2015). A study of 6786 community‐dwelling elderly Finnish people found a loneliness prevalence of 39.4% (Routasalo et al. 2006), and it was 56.63% among 3530 community‐dwelling adults aged 60 and older in the United States (Gerst‐Emerson and Jayawardhana 2015). In China, a study of 744 elderly participants residing in the community measured a loneliness prevalence of 26.2% (Zhong et al. 2019), while in Singapore, it was 51% (Chan et al. 2015). Ramic et al. (2011) investigated 200 older adults over the age of 65 years and found that those living alone consumed less food than those living with family, noting that loneliness was a significant predictor of anorexia nervosa in older adults. Whitelock and Ensaff (2018) conducted focus‐group discussions in the United Kingdom with 30 older adults aged 63–90 years and found that respondents reported a lack of motivation to cook due to having to eat alone. Tsofliou et al. (2020) surveyed 39 older adults and found that appetite was enhanced in the presence of a companion for meals.
Anxiety can lead to a decline in subjective well‐being and life satisfaction, particularly affecting cognitive functions related to food consumption (Taghiabadi et al. 2017; Malone and Wachholtz 2018; Divers et al. 2021). Paquet et al. (2003) observed 30 older adults aged 65–92 years and found that negative emotions such as anxiety, depression, and anger had a negative impact on food intake.
2.2.3. Gender Differences
Gender differences in sensory abilities are evident among older adults, with men generally experiencing more sensory impairments than women. Several studies have shown that older men more frequently reported HL than older women, particularly among those aged 62–90 years (Pinto et al. 2014; Uchida et al. 2019; Reavis et al. 2022). For instance, Park et al. (2016) conducted a study involving 2818 men and 4132 women aged 50–85 years and found that men had significantly lower hearing thresholds at frequencies of 3 kHz, 4 kHz, and 6 kHz compared to women. This suggests that men are more susceptible to hearing impairments at these frequencies. Furthermore, a study by Correia et al. (2016) involving 3005 US adults aged 57–85 years revealed that men had significantly poorer sensory function in hearing, taste, and smell compared to women. These findings align with previous research, indicating a consistent pattern of gender differences in sensory abilities among older adults (Schneider et al. 2012; Pinto et al. 2014).
2.2.4. Education, Income, and Occupation
Several studies have demonstrated that occupation and income can impact sensory abilities, particularly vision and hearing, in older adults (Andrade and López‐Ortega 2017; Tham et al. 2018; Wang et al. 2020). For instance, Andrade and López‐Ortega (2017) conducted a study involving 14,961 older adults aged 50–94 years in Mexico and found that women with lower levels of education were at a higher risk for hearing and visual impairments. Similarly, Tham et al. (2018) studied 3353 individuals aged 40 and older and observed that lower income and education levels were associated with bilateral vision impairment or blindness (p < 0.025).
Cao et al. (2021) used weighted multivariate logistic regression to analyze factors influencing sensory abilities among 15,176 Chinese seniors aged 65 and older. They found that vision impairment was prevalent and associated with age, marital status, economic status, educational level, occupation, and certain diseases. Adults with higher education and better self‐rated economic status had a lower risk of visual impairment, while those in traditional occupations (agriculture, forestry, animal husbandry, and fishing) and the unemployed had a higher likelihood of visual impairment, with all results being statistically significant (p < 0.05, p < 0.01). This suggests that lower education and economic status may lead to reduced health awareness and limited access to healthcare resources, thereby increasing the risk of sensory impairments.
Whillans et al. (2016) surveyed 2956 older adults over 60 years in the United Kingdom and found that visual stability varied with age, with those of higher social standing displaying better vision stability regardless of age.
In addition to occupation, the work environment and lifestyle also significantly affect sensory perception. Exposure to loud noises, such as those found in factories or from firearms, can lead to HL. Pinto et al. (2014) reported that older adults who worked in noisy environments were more likely to experience HL, and Yang et al. (2015) and Bowl and Dawson (2018) further supported this finding.
2.3. Impact of COVID‐19 on Sensory Impairment in Older Adults
COVID‐19 has significantly affected the global population, causing not only symptoms such as sore throat, headache, diarrhea, fatigue, muscle pain, shortness of breath, fever, and cough but also clinical issues like the loss of chemosensory function, vision, hearing, taste, and smell (Guo et al. 2020; Salcan et al. 2021; Nanjo et al. 2022; Chaaban et al. 2021). Studies have shown that many patients experience phantosmia (smelling odors without a source) and parosmia (distorted smell when a familiar odor is present) (Watson et al. 2010). Similar phenomena have been observed in taste (Chaaban et al. 2021). Additionally, COVID‐19 patients may develop sudden sensorineural hearing loss (SSNHL) and vision loss (Ricciardiello et al. 2021; Meng et al. 2022; van Rijssen et al. 2021; Swain and Pani 2021; Johansson et al. 2022).
While many patients regain their sense of taste and smell within weeks, approximately 10% report ongoing issues such as dysgeusia (distortion of basic tastes like bitter, sour, sweet, and salty), phantosmia, parosmia, hyposmia (reduced smell), anosmia (loss of smell), and decreased chemesthesis (reduced chemical sensations like the cooling of mint, the warmth of ginger, or the burn of chilies) (Burges Watson et al. 2021). These sensory dysfunctions can affect food preferences and reduce food intake and appetite, leading to weight loss (Fantozzi et al. 2020; Deer et al. 2022).
COVID‐19 impacts sensory loss through two possible mechanisms: receptor cell damage or infection and inflammatory cytokines (Iebba et al. 2021; Srinivasan et al. 2022). Older adults, who are more susceptible to inflammation, neurodegenerative diseases, and certain eye conditions, may face a higher risk of severe sensory loss compared to younger individuals (Hardan et al. 2021; Erausquin et al. 2021). Deer et al. (2022) found that men aged 60 and older consumed less total fruit and vegetables per day than recommended, highlighting the potential impact of COVID‐19 on the dietary quality of older adults. Furthermore, social distancing during lockdowns can exacerbate psychological issues and dietary patterns in older adults, further affecting their dietary quality (Piotrowicz et al. 2021). Visser et al. (2020) reported that 48.3% and 54.3% of older adults in the Netherlands aged 61 and above reduced their physical activity during the COVID‐19 pandemic, with older age groups at higher risk of undereating or weight loss.
In fact, recovery from COVID‐19 is also associated with “long COVID,” a term used to describe the persistence of various symptoms after acquiring a COVID‐19 infection, regardless of viral status (Raveendran et al. 2021). Numerous studies have indicated that long COVID is more prevalent among adult patients than among younger individuals (Sudre et al. 2021; Naidu et al. 2021; Jacobson et al. 2021). Long COVID not only affects the physical health of older adults but also has a negative impact on their mental health. Changes in lifestyle and prolonged sequelae can lead to more negative emotions, such as depression, loneliness, and anxiety (Shabbir et al. 2022). Nwachukwu et al. (2020) found that older adults in Canada (n = 762, aged 60 and above) scored lower on the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS), Generalized Anxiety Disorder 7‐item (GAD‐7) scale, and Patient Health Questionnaire‐9 (PHQ‐9) compared to younger individuals (≤ 25). Individuals aged 65 and older face a higher risk of death compared to younger age groups (Minnai et al. 2022). Although some studies have discussed strategies for older adults with long COVID, such as psychological support, consistent physiotherapy, and evaluation systems, there is a need for further investigation into age‐related sensory loss (Greenhalgh et al. 2021; Raveendran et al. 2021).
3. Evaluation Methodology
3.1. Sensory Evaluation Methods
Aging may affect the sensory abilities of older adults, but the sensory appeal of foods has important implications for food selection as well as intake (Chaffee and Ross 2023). For the specific group of older adults, this section summarizes the sensory evaluation methods that have been commonly used in the last decade in terms of testing dimensions such as intensity, variance, and liking. Taking into account the specificities of older adults, we draw out the shortcomings and strengths of the application of these methods in older adults. In fact, there are many difficulties faced in sensory testing with older adults, which may be related to the sensory testing methods, such as some sensory evaluation methods being more oriented toward the younger population or may be related to the situation of older adults as a group itself (Cliceri et al. 2017; Scott et al. 2017). For example, the special characteristics of the elderly, short memory, physical fitness decline, ease of fatigue, and decreased sensitivity of smell and taste pose challenges. Based on these factors, we summarize the existing mainstream sensory evaluation methods and point out their strengths and weaknesses in the evaluation of the elderly, as shown in Figure 2.
FIGURE 2.
Feature of different sensory methods.
3.1.1. Descriptive Analysis Methods
Descriptive analytical methods are commonly used in sensory evaluation, where traditional descriptive tests are conducted by trained evaluation teams. More accurate food quality parameters such as appearance, color, flavor, taste, and texture of the food can be obtained by descriptive analytical tests (Methven et al. 2016). In the sensory evaluation of older adults, due to their special characteristics, the chosen research method and the design of the evaluation task should take into account the degradation of vision, smell, and taste, as well as the decline in comprehension. The questionnaire and experimental process should be designed in a targeted manner. A summary of the application of the descriptive method in sensory tests of older adults is presented in Table 2.
TABLE 2.
Literature information on the descriptive method.
Method | Author | Publication date | Materials | Evaluators | Age | Article | Evaluation content |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intensity test | De J. N. | 1996 | Orange lemonade, strawberry jam, strawberry yogurt, grain, porridge, chocolate paste |
Younger: 35 Older: 29 |
Younger: 22 ± 2 years old Older: 79 ± 6 years old |
Effect of sucrose in breakfast items on pleasantness and food intake in the elderly | Sweet |
9‐point intensity scale | S. Koskinen | 2003 | Oat bran product |
Older: 50 Younger: 58 |
Older: 63–85 years old Younger: 18–34 years old |
Flavor enhancement as a tool for increasing pleasantness and intake of a snack product among the elderly | Odor and taste intensity |
100‐mm visual analog scale | Stefanie Kremer | 2005 | Chicken‐flavored white cream soup |
Younger: 12 Older: 15 (9 normal sense of smell,6 impaired sense of smell) |
Younger: 23 ± 3.4 years old Older: 68 ± 7.3 years old |
Perception of texture and flavor in soups by elderly and young subjects. Journal of Texture Studies | Thickness, fluffiness, roughness, stickiness, swallow ability, creaminess, taste intensity, mushroom flavor, saltiness, chicken flavor |
7‐point scale | S. Koskinen | 2005 | Hams | 60 (54 females, 6 males) | 61–86 years old | Intakes of cold cuts in the elderly are predicted by olfaction and mood, but not by flavor type or intensity of the products | Odor and taste intensity |
100 mm horizontal visual analog scale | Stefanie Kremer | 2007 | Waffles |
Younger: 16 Older: 22, 13 normal sense of smell, 9 impaired sense of smell |
Younger: 18–35 individuals Older: 60–85 individuals |
Differences in perception of sweet and savory waffles between elderly and young subjects | Fat, fluffiness, dryness, elasticity, swallow ability, aftertaste, sweetness, vanilla flavor, saltiness, cheese |
15 cm line scale | Seo H S | 2009 | Green tea, coffee |
Older: 30 normal sense of smell, 30 impaired sense of smell Younger: 30 |
Older: 60–75 years old (normal sense of smell), 62–79 years old (impaired sense of smell) younger: 18–30 years old |
Effects of olfactory dysfunction on sensory evaluation and preparation of foods | Overall odor intensity |
TI | Hutchings S. C. | 2014a | Cashews |
Younger: 16 Older: 16 |
Younger: 22–30, 26.3 ± 2.5 years old Older: 56–70, 63.2 ± 4.5 years old |
Investigating changes in sensory stickiness perception with age | Differences in viscosity perception between elderly and young individuals |
TI | Luckett C. R. | 2016 | Potato chips | 68 |
Younger: 20–25 years old Middle‐aged: 40–45 years old Older: 65+ years old |
Crispness level of potato chips affects temporal dynamics of flavor perception and mastication patterns in adults of different age group | Differences in crispness perception between elderly and young individuals |
TI | Hitomi Sato | 2022 | NaCl solutions |
Older: 31 (18 male, 13 female) Younger: 43 (21 male,22 female) |
Older: 60–81 years old Younger: 21–39 years old |
Differences in dynamic perception of salty taste intensity between young and older adults | Differences in salty taste perception between elderly and young individuals |
TDS | A. Thomas | 2018 | Oral nutritional supplements (ONS) | 62 | 60–75 years old | Temporal drivers of liking for ONS for older adults throughout the day with monitoring of hunger and thirst status | Attributes influencing likability |
TDS | Hutchings S. C. | 2014b | Nuts |
Older: 20 Younger: 20 |
Older: 62.9 ± 4.8 years old Younger: 25.5 ± 2.5 years old |
Temporal dominance of sensations: A comparison between younger and older subjects for the perception of food texture | Key attributes: hardness, crispness, stickiness, low oil |
3.1.1.1. Intensity Rating Methods (Rating Methods)
Quantification using rating methods typically requires the establishment of an evaluation team trained to achieve consistency in scoring and reproducibility of results (Aguiar et al. 2018). The main rating methods include categorical scales, linear scales, affective magnitude estimation scales, magnitude estimation scales, N‐point scales, and 100 mm horizontal visual analog scale (Methven et al. 2016). In sensory evaluation for older adults, these methods have been applied to products such as orange lemonade, strawberry jam, strawberry yogurt, cereal porridge, chocolate sauce (De Jong et al. 1996), oat bran products (Koskinen et al. 2003), cream soup (Kremer et al. 2005), ham (Koskinen and Tuorila 2005), waffles (Kremer et al. 2007b), green tea, and coffee (Seo and Hummel 2009).
Earlier studies have shown that older adults can use a variety of scales to perceive the overall odor intensity and overall flavor intensity of samples and can perceive and rate sweetness, elasticity, thickness, and flavor attributes such as creaminess and chicken flavor. In a study by De Jong et al. (1996), the authors used a 10‐point category scale to rate the sweetness of five types of products such as orange lemonade, strawberry jam, strawberry yogurt, cereal porridge, and chocolate sauce. They found that older adults (29 participants, mean age 79 ± 6 years) could distinguish well between the intensities of sweetness in different samples. Similarly, a larger group of individuals (50–60 participants, mean age 73.7 years) could accurately distinguish samples with differences in aroma intensity using either a 7‐point scale or a 9‐point scale (Koskinen et al. 2003; Koskinen et al. 2005). Notably, Seo and Hummel (2009), along with others, specifically recruited 30 young people with normal olfactory function, 30 elderly individuals, and 30 elderly people with olfactory dysfunction to assess the overall odor, flavor intensity, and bitterness of three different concentrations of green tea and coffee. Their study clearly demonstrated that individuals with olfactory dysfunction have a significantly reduced ability to perceive intensity (Seo and Hummel 2009).
Conducting intensity ratings using older adults is not limited to a single attribute; they can also evaluate multiple attributes simultaneously. Kremer et al. (2005) obtained the intensity of four attributes—sweetness, saltiness, cheesiness, and vanilla flavor—in a cream soup consumed by 15 older adults aged 60–85 years using a 100 mm horizontal visual analog scale. They also assessed six additional attributes: fatness, elasticity, dryness, fluffiness, swallowability, and aftertaste. In another study, Kremer et al. (2007b) evaluated the intensity of sweet/creamy, mushroom, chicken, savory, flavor intensity, thickness, and aftertaste in creamy soup waffles from 22 older adults aged 60–84 years using the same type of scale. The findings from Stefanie Kremer et al. indicated that the perception of creaminess, along with certain textures and flavors, was diminished in older adults.
The rating method demands a high level of competence from evaluators. Therefore, unless specifically required, potential subjects should undergo a survey and screening process to ensure they possess normal olfactory function, as assessed by the “Sniffin Sticks” test (Kremer et al. 2007b, 2005; Seo and Hummel 2009; Koskinen and Tuorila 2005), and normal cognitive abilities, as measured by the MMSE (Seo and Hummel 2009; Koskinen and Tuorila 2005). This process helps to identify any influences of subjects' backgrounds or illnesses on the study outcomes. Furthermore, the decline in perceptual abilities among older adults can complicate group training and research, potentially necessitating a larger sample size and a more extended training period to ensure data validity (Daute et al. 2021).
3.1.1.2. Time‐Intensity (TI) Method
The TI method is a widely used dynamic descriptive analysis technique that focuses on scoring a single attribute of a sample. When multiple attributes are involved, results must be obtained through multiple tastings, which can significantly increase the cost of the experiment. Therefore, the TI method is not suitable for the evaluation of multiple attributes (Methven et al. 2016). In sensory evaluation involving elderly individuals, it has been applied to the evaluation of foods such as cashew nuts (Hutchings et al. 2014), crisps (Luckett et al. 2016), and salt solution (Sato et al. 2022).
In a study by Hutchings et al. (2014), the authors used the TI method to evaluate the dynamic viscosity perception of cashew nuts among16 older subjects (55–70 years old) and younger subjects (22–30 years old). It was found that the younger subjects reached the highest intensity earlier than the older subjects, confirming the decline in taste perception among the elderly. Another study using the TI method to test the temporal dynamics of crispiness perception involved 68 subjects from different age groups (young: 20–25 years, middle aged: 40–45 years, elderly: 65+ years) and showed that older adults perceived a significantly lower initial intensity of flavor compared to younger individuals (Luckett et al. 2016). A study on the savory flavor intensity in 31 elderly people (aged 60–81 years) revealed that while older adults could correctly identify salty flavors, their perception of specific intensities took longer. All of these studies highlight the effects of aging on taste perception (Sato et al. 2022).
When using the TI method for research, compensatory effects occur in the elderly population due to decreased chewing ability, so chewing time can be extended or the number of chews increased (Luckett et al. 2016). Additionally, because the TI method is specific to the evaluation of a single attribute, more subjects need to be recruited for the experiment to obtain more accurate group differences (Hutchings et al. 2014).
3.1.1.3. Temporal Dominance of Sensations (TDS)
The TDS method is a dynamic descriptive analysis method that considers the dynamic processes of food digestion and flavor release (Oliver et al. 2018a) and allows for the simultaneous evaluation of multiple attributes in response to the dynamics of the food (Di Monaco et al. 2014). Usually completed by a trained panel, the TDS method can be completed by untrained subjects if the intensity of the attribute is not being evaluated (Di Monaco et al. 2014). In sensory evaluation of older adults, it has been applied to nuts (Hutchings et al. 2014), oral solutions (Thomas et al. 2018), and other products.
In a study by Hutchings et al. (2014), four types of nuts, almonds, roasted cashews, macadamia nuts, and roasted peanuts, were evaluated by 20 older and 20 younger people, and taste change curves were obtained for different age groups for different samples as the number of chews increased. The study proved that healthy subjects with a mean age of 62.9 ± 4.8 years were able to obtain clear TDS curves, as well as young people, and the attributes selected were consistent for both age groups. Thomas et al. (2018) evaluated different types of oral liquids with 62 older adults aged 60–75 years and found that the sensation of thirst in older adults was predominantly characterized by dryness attributes. Samples with higher protein content received higher preference due to their persistent nutty and coffee aromas, as well as their brief metallic taste and dryness.
These studies have validated the use of older adults as subjects to investigate the dynamics of sensory perception in the oral cavity, enabling them to identify which sensations are dominant (Albert et al. 2012). However, older subjects may take longer to understand the content of the table, which could be related to the decline in memory and visual abilities commonly experienced by older adults. Therefore, when designing experiments, it is important to consider suitable ways to present the table or other forms of information (Hutchings et al. 2014).
3.1.2. Rapid Sensory Analysis Methods
With the development of sensory methods, rapid sensory analysis methods such as Sorting, Projective Mapping (PM), Just About Right (JAR), and Check All That Apply (CATA) have gradually emerged. These methods are completed by untrained evaluators, and the tests are simple and easy to understand, making them suitable for use in sensory evaluation of older adults. Combined with the preference tests, they can uncover the specific drivers of preferences that influence older adults. A summary of the application of these rapid methods in sensory tests for older adults is presented in Table 3.
TABLE 3.
Literature information on rapid sensory methods.
Method | Author | Publication date | Materials | Evaluators | Age | Article | Evaluation content |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sorting | Scott N. | 2017 | Pudding |
Younger: 60 Older: 60 (36 females, 24 males) |
A: 18–30 years B: 60–88 years | Exploring the use of rapid profiling techniques for use in older adult populations | Food sensory characteristics |
Sorting | Natalia Riquelme | 2022 | Dessert | 87 | > 60 | Understanding older people perceptions about desserts using word association and sorting task methodologies | Views on desserts |
Free sorting | Danny Cliceri | 2017 | Canned pea, canned sweet corn |
Canned pea control group: 34 females, 21 males Canned sweet corn control group: 38 females, 20 males Experimental group: 166;148 |
Canned pea control group: 34 females, 21males; canned sweet corn control group: 38 females, 20 males; experimental group: 65–69 years old, 70–79 years old | Exploring salient dimensions in a free sorting task: A cross‐country study within the elderly population | Perception and preference |
Projective Mapping | Scott N | 2017 | Pudding |
Younger: 60 (42 females, 18 males) Older: 60 (36 females, 24 males) |
A: 18–30 years old B: 60–88 years old | Exploring the use of rapid profiling techniques for use in older adult populations | Texture, flavor |
QMA | Behannis Mena | 2020 | Snack |
China: 21 (17 females, 4 males) Australia: 16 (13 females, 3 males) |
China: 60–81 years old Australia: 65–79 years old |
Exploring meal and snacking behavior of older adults in Australia and China | Food preferences |
5‐JAR scale | Rutkowska J. | 2022 | Kefir | 256 (158 females, 98 males) | 65–76 | Volatile composition and sensory profile of lactose‐free kefir, and its acceptability by elderly consumers | Perception and hedonic acceptability |
5‐JAR scale | Ares G. | 2010 | Milk desserts | 100 | 18–63 | Comparison of attribute liking and JAR scales to evaluate the adequacy of sensory attributes of milk desserts | Overall liking, thickness, creaminess, sweetness, vanilla flavor rating |
CATA | Scott N | 2017 | Pudding |
Younger: 60 (42 females, 18 males) Older: 60 (36 females, 24 males) |
A: 18–30 years B: 60–88 years | Exploring the use of rapid profiling techniques for use in older adult populations | Texture, flavor, acceptability |
CATA | Regan E | 2019 | Oral nutritional supplements (ONS) |
Experimental group: 80 Control group: 80 |
Experimental group: untrained,35 males, 45 females, > 65 years old Control group: untrained,35 females, 35 males, 18–35 years old | Exploring how age influences sensory perception, thirst and hunger during the consumption of oral nutritional supplements using the check‐all‐that‐apply methodology | The impact of thirst, hunger, and satiety on supplement preference |
CATA/TCATA | Schumaker, M. R. | 2019 | Commercial Shiraz wine | 105 (58 females, 37 males) | 21–86 years | Influence of wine composition on consumer perception and acceptance of Brettanomyces metabolites using temporal check‐all‐that‐apply methodology | Aroma, taste, and mouth feel |
CATA | Jaeger, S. R. | 2020 | Apple juice, red tea | 636 | 19–68 years | Check‐all‐that‐apply four case studies with beverages | Similarities or differences in describing sensory product characteristics |
CATA | Mahieu, B. | 2021 | French red wines, milk chocolates | A: 120, B: 147 |
A: 18–60 years B: 18–65 years |
An investigation of the stability of free‐comment and check‐all‐that‐apply in two consumer studies on red wines and milk chocolates | The stability of the same product assessed using Free‐Choice Profiling (FC) method versus Check‐All‐That‐Apply (CATA) method |
CATA | Regan E | 2021 | ONS | 80 | 51 (65–74 years old), 29 (> 75 years old), 74 ± 8 years old (65–97) 35 males, 45 females | Exploring how age, medication usage, and dentures effect the sensory perception and liking of oral nutritional supplements in older adults | Hunger, satiety, food cravings, and thirst |
3.1.2.1. Sorting
The sorting method, with its roots in psychology, is a relatively new approach in sensory evaluation (Dominique et al. 2018). This method requires subjects to group samples based on their overall similarity and typically involves 9–15 trained subjects or 9–98 untrained subjects (Fleming et al. 2015). It has been applied to the sensory evaluation of foods such as peas and corn (Cliceri et al. 2017), puddings (Scott et al. 2017), and desserts (Riquelme et al. 2021) in studies involving older adults.
In a study by Scott et al. (2017), seven puddings were evaluated using the sorting method. The results demonstrated that untrained older subjects (aged 60–88 years) could effectively sort the samples according to the degree of similarity, with 75% of the older adults completing the sorting experiment correctly. In the evaluation of the desserts, all older adults expressed a clear opinion on all 12 desserts (Riquelme et al. 2021). In the study of canned peas and sweet corn, 10 types of canned peas and 8 types of canned sweet corn were evaluated, respectively, using the categorization method, and older adults (over 65 years) still demonstrated good classification skills (Cliceri et al. 2017).
However, the sorting method can be influenced by the familiarity of the samples. Cliceri et al. (2017) found that for samples with a high level of familiarity, the categorization result plots were highly reliable across all age groups. In contrast, in the evaluation of samples with low familiarity, there were differences in the evaluation criteria used by the subjects, which led to a decrease in the reliability of the results. Therefore, it is recommended to conduct experiments based on the familiarity of the test population (e.g., gender, age) with the samples (Cliceri et al. 2017). Additionally, some elderly subjects were unable to categorize the samples based on multiple criteria. For this reason, the authors suggest that selecting a group of samples with relatively small differences for further testing (Scott et al. 2017).
3.1.2.2. Projective Mapping
PM has a long history in the field of psychology, and it is a relatively new evaluation method in the sensory field. The PM method is completed by at least 20 subjects, who are generally untrained consumers (Mena et al. 2020). Subjects place the samples on corresponding coordinate paper based on the overall similarity of the samples (Dominique et al. 2018). Qualitative multivariate analysis (QMA) is a derivative of the PM method, and its basic process and requirements are similar to PM (Daute et al. 2021; Mena et al. 2020; Dehlholm et al. 2012).
Scott et al. (2017) recruited 60 older subjects (aged 60–88 years) and 60 younger subjects (aged 18–30 years) to evaluate the similarity of seven puddings and two replicate samples using the PM method. The results showed that older subjects could correctly place the duplicate samples on the coordinate paper. Comparison of the result plots showed that there was a correlation of 0.89 between the evaluation results of the older subjects and the results of the younger subjects (Scott et al. 2017). Mena et al. (2020) recruited 21 Chinese (aged 60–81 years) and 16 Australian (aged 65–79 years) older subjects to evaluate jerky, chocolate, berries using the QMA method. The criteria for placement were x‐axis: “every day” to “indulgence,” y‐axis: “likely to eat” to y‐axis: “likely to eat” to “unlikely to eat.” The results clearly concluded that Chinese older adults have healthier eating habits than Australian older adults. It was also found that foods that met the criteria of “ready‐to‐eat” and moderate size were preferred more by the older population.
Some of the elderly subjects were unable to place the samples using the PM method considering multiple attributes at the same time. It is suggested that the subjects focus on one attribute at a time and be guided through the experiment in multiple sessions (Scott et al. 2017). As there are also differences within the elderly, such as age, health status, and nutritional need, it is recommended to consider individual differences in elderly subjects when conducting research (Mena et al. 2020).
3.1.2.3. Napping
Napping, developed by Pagès (2003), is a specific variant of the PM method. It is a relatively recent and rapid sensory description technique used for sensory evaluation. Napping involves projecting food samples onto a two‐dimensional plane (typically sized 60 × 40 cm), where products with similar attributes are placed closer together and those that are more dissimilar are placed further apart (Reinbach et al. 2014; Pickup et al. 2017). This method has been applied in the context of the UFP (ultra‐flash profiling) for the sensory evaluation of confectionery products (Reinbach et al. 2014; Pickup et al. 2017). Napping is often used in conjunction with UFP and requires data analysis using multiple factor analysis (MFA). It has been utilized in experiments involving confectionery gels (Frantz Lairy Obas et al. 2021), caramel corn (Mayhew et al. 2016), and green tea (Kim et al. 2013), among others. Typically, napping is suitable for experiments conducted by 10–12 untrained evaluators. However, Liu et al. (2016) found that method‐trained groups could group wines more clearly in their study of small sensory differences, suggesting that training in methods and samples may lead to more reliable results.
In addition, the variation and accuracy of the results may be influenced by the familiarity of the samples. In an experiment by Park et al. (2014), a group of people aged 34–68 years (mean age 51.6 years) participated in napping sensory evaluations of traditional Korean biscuits. They found that younger consumers tended to place biscuits with strong odors and ginger flavors together, both of which may be perceived as pungent. Older consumers, on the other hand, placed two types of biscuits from the same company, produced according to traditional methods, closer together, possibly due to their greater exposure to these traditional biscuits. There were also differences in the frequency of use of perceptual and sensory terms between older and younger consumers, with terms like “very oily” used more often by the older group and “hard” by the younger group. This experiment suggests that napping can reveal sensory differences among consumers. Napping is less time consuming than traditional analytical methods, making it suitable for the elderly population who are prone to fatigue. It can also better highlight differences between samples (Oliver et al. 2018b; Liu et al. 2016).
3.1.2.4. Just About Right
JAR scale is one of the commonly used methods in consumer sensory evaluation to obtain the consumers' acceptance of an aspect or overall perception of a sample (Ares et al. 2009). The JAR method is often used in conjunction with preference testing and is generally completed by a minimum of 25 untrained evaluators (Lee et al. 2021). In sensory evaluations involving older adults, it has been applied in studies of kefir (Rutkowska et al. 2022), milk desserts (Ares et al. 2009), and cheese (Esmerino et al. 2015).
Rutkowska et al. (2022) investigated the perception and acceptance of lactose‐free kefir (LFK) products among older consumers (aged 65–76 years) using a JAR scale. A 5‐point JAR scale (1 = too little, 2 = slightly too little, 3 = just right, 4 = slightly too much, 5 = too much) was used in the study to rate the sweetness, tartness, and refreshing effect of kefir. The study showed that 63% of the elderly subjects rated the sweetness of LFK as “just right” and 73% rated the acidity of LFK as lower than that of traditional kefir. Nearly 50% of the subjects rated LFK as “like it a lot,” indicating that both acidity and sweetness are attributes that influence consumer preference. Ares et al. (2009) used a 5‐point JAR scale to evaluate the overall preference, creaminess, thickness, sweetness, and vanilla flavor of eight milk desserts among subjects aged 18–63 years. The results showed that subjects could accurately assess the relationship between each attribute of the samples and their level of preference using the JAR scale, and the samples that were closest to the ideal were identified.
With the development of sensory methods, the JAR method is considered a good judgment method because subjects can independently use this method to rate different attributes. The JAR method can help subjects think about the reasons for high or low preferences for samples, suggest deficiencies, and even provide suggestions (Ares et al. 2009). This can be very helpful in uncovering the factors that influence the preference drivers of older adults.
3.1.2.5. CATA Method
The CATA method is a relatively recent approach for rapid sensory analysis. It is typically conducted by untrained subjects. A study by Ares et al. (2014) clarified that the number of subjects should be limited to 60–80 if there are significant differences between the evaluation samples in order to obtain reliable results. During the experiment, subjects are required to tick all the words that can characterize the samples from the list of descriptors provided to them (Jaeger et al. 2020). This allows researchers to identify the ideal product and product characteristics in the minds of the subjects. In the elderly population, this method has been applied in the sensory evaluation of food products such as pudding (Scott et al. 2017), wine (Schumaker et al. 2019; Mahieu et al. 2021), apple juice, and iced tea (Jaeger et al. 2020) and oral nutritional supplements (ONS) (Regan et al. 2019, 2021).
Older adults can identify sample differences and obtain product characteristics through the CATA method, which offers advantages in sensory evaluation for this age group. Scott et al. (2017) evaluated seven types of puddings by 60 older adults (aged 60–88 years) and 60 younger adults, successfully obtaining the corresponding product characteristics with RV coefficients of up to 0.98 for both age groups. This suggests that the CATA method is applicable for sensory evaluation in older adults and yields convincing results. Regan et al. (2019) characterized the sensory attributes of different flavors of ONS using 80 older adults and 80 young adults. They obtained the characteristics of the samples while enhancing the understanding of the preference factors of older adults. In Schumaker et al. (2019)'s sensory evaluation of wine, the CATA method was used to compare the aroma of seven wine samples, and elderly subjects (over 60 years old) clearly distinguished the differences in aroma among all the samples. Similarly, Jaeger et al. (2020) confirmed the ability of older adults (over 60 years old) to accurately complete the evaluation task for apple juice and iced tea with varying levels of acidity and sweetness.
Older subjects differed from younger subjects in the frequency of selection of an attribute. In a study of oral supplements of different protein types, younger subjects more frequently chose “hazelnut,” “metal,” “milk,” and “vanilla” to describe the nonconcentrated protein type of supplements. For protein concentrate nutrients, older subjects (mean age 73.7 ± 7.9 years) frequently chose “chocolate” to characterize them (Regan et al. 2019). Additionally, older subjects (29 participants, aged over 75 years) still frequently chose attributes to characterize the samples when different age groups of older adults were the subjects (Regan et al. 2021). This study also demonstrates the applicability of the CATA method in sensory evaluation of elderly subjects of different ages.
Overall, the CATA method is simple to operate, the evaluation consumes relatively little time, and it can effectively avoid issues such as reduced attention and increased fatigue, making it an ideal method for obtaining older adults' perceptions of sample features.
3.1.2.6. Methods for Assessing Appetite and Intake in Older Adults
Appetite and food intake are crucial for maintaining the nutritional balance of older adults (Regan et al. 2019), and changes in these factors are important indicators of their physical health (Methven et al. 2016). When studying these issues, it is essential to consider the decline in smell and taste that often occurs with aging. Therefore, methods that are less fatiguing, quick, and simple should be used (Allen et al. 2014). Current methods applied to appetite and food intake in older adults include the 5‐point visual scale (Koskinen et al. 2003), the 7‐point hedonic scale (Koskinen et al. 2005), 100 mm visual analog scale (100 mm VAS) (Regan et al. 2019, 2021), [0–10] continuous scale (Thomas et al. 2018), and TDS (Thomas et al. 2018).
In an earlier study, De De Jong et al. (1996) recruited 33 young adults with a mean age of 23 ± 2 years and 25 older adults with a mean age of 82 ± 5 years. They used a crossover design to analyze the intake of five products, including orange lemonade, strawberry jam, strawberry yogurt, cereal porridge, and chocolate sauce, with varying sucrose concentrations. The study found that different sucrose concentrations did not significantly affect intake in either older or younger subjects (De Jong et al. 1996).
In a study by Koskinen et al. (2003) on the impact of flavor enhancement on intake, a group of 50 older subjects (aged 63–85 years) and 58 younger subjects (aged 18–34 years) were tested using a 5‐point visual scale. They evaluated the intake of three cups of regular aroma concentration oat bran and three cups of intensely flavored oat bran. Each score from 1 to 5 represented 0% (tasting sample only), 25%, 50%, 75%, and 100% consumption, respectively. The results showed that the mean intake of older subjects was numerically lower than that of younger subjects, but there was no significant difference (Koskinen et al. 2003).
A cross‐sectional study combined with a 7‐point hedonic scale was used to divide 60 older subjects (aged 61–86 years) into two groups to evaluate four types of commercial ham and three types of homemade ham with enhanced flavors (Koskinen et al. 2005). The study asked for a first preference rating of all samples received, followed by a rating of the samples consumed to obtain differences in intake. The authors found that a decrease in olfactory ability (as represented by poor health, age, and emotional state) might actually increase intake, especially in commercial hams (Koskinen et al. 2005).
In recent years, Regan et al. (2019, 2021) recruited 80 elderly subjects to use 100 mm VAS to assess the effects of ONS on appetite. The elderly subjects in the study were > 65 years of age. The study effectively demonstrated that ONS can lead to decreased appetite in older adults, potentially influenced by various physiological, psychological, and digestive factors (Regan et al. 2019, 2021).
The study by Thomas et al. (2018) recruited 62 older participants (aged 60–75 years), and used a [0–10 scale combined with the TDS method. They analyzed the factors affecting food intake using the mean values of hunger, desire to eat, desire to consume, and 100% satiety as references. The study found that “the eating environment (e.g., laboratory vs. home) affects intake,”“continuous drinking has less effect on hunger and more on thirst,” and “appetite is greater in the afternoon than in the morning” (Thomas et al. 2018).
Overall, first, when designing studies related to appetite and intake, it is often necessary to consider a balanced crossover sequence to eliminate experimental error. Second, due to the influence of subjects' own health conditions, a larger number of subjects may need to be involved to obtain significant and accurate results (Methven et al. 2016).
4. Measures and Effects of Food Sensory Compensation in Older Adults
The deterioration of sensory perception is a pivotal factor influencing food preferences and intake in older adults. As discussed in Section 2, this decline can lead to reduced dietary intake, contributing to issues like weight loss and frailty. To counteract these effects, it is essential to explore and implement food sensory compensation strategies that enhance older adults' enjoyment and consumption of food (Figure 3). This section provides an in‐depth analysis of various compensation strategies targeting different sensory perceptions, highlighting their potential to improve older adults' food preferences and intake (See details in Table 4).
FIGURE 3.
Sensory improvement strategies on older adults' daily intake.
TABLE 4.
Summarized literature on food compensation strategies.
Modal | Elderly population | Decline/impairment of sensory perception | Food system | Compensatory strategies | Effect of compensatory strategies | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sample size | Age | Living condition | Taste | Order | Auditory | Version | ||||
Taste | ||||||||||
Sweet | N = 191 | 70–75 | — | — | — | — | — | A variety of foods | Sugar | Positive |
N = 35 | 59–88 | — | — | — | — | — | Apple juice | Sweetness | Positive | |
N = 36 | 82.8 (F)78.8 (M) | Nursing home | Yes | Yes | — | — | Peach juice | Sweetness | Positive | |
N = 25 | 79 ± 6 | Nursing home | — | — | — | — | Orange lemonade, strawberry jam, strawberry yogurt | Sweetness | Positive | |
Sour | N = 52 | 60–85 | Community dwelling | — | — | — | — | Desserts | Sour | Positive |
N = 76 | 64–97 | Clinical Rehabilitation Institute | Yes | Yes | Yes | — | Functional drink | Lower sourness | No effect | |
Salt | N = 35 | 62–83 | Nursing home | — | — | — | — | Meat | Enhance salt | Positive |
N = 84 | 65.2–68.5 | Community dwelling | Yes | Yes and No | — | — | Mashed potatoes | Salt | No effect | |
N = 114 | 63–102 | Nursing home | Yes and no | — | — | — | Finger foods | Salt | Positive | |
N = 52 | 71.1 ± 4.6 | Community dwelling | Yes | — | — | — | Lunch meal and evening meal | Sauce | Positive | |
N = 97 | > 65 | Nursing home | — | — | — | — | Cooked meal | Flavor | No effect | |
Smell | N = 71 | 65–93 | Nursing home | — | — | — | — | Oat bran frankfurters | Spices | Positive |
N = 71 | > 65 | Nursing home | — | — | — | — | Cooked meals | Flavor powders | Positive | |
N = 104 | 79–101 | Nursing home | — | — | — | — | Appetizer | Flavor | Positive | |
N = 84 | 65.2–68.5 | Community dwelling | Yes | Yes and no | — | — | Gravy and both stews | Flavor | No effect | |
Version | N = 71 | 65–93 | Nursing home | — | — | — | — | In‐between meals | Flavor | Positive |
Auditory | N = 30 | 66–75 | — | — | — | — | — | Nursing care foods | Auditory | Positive |
Taste–smell | N = 100 | 65–93 | Community dwelling | — | — | — | — | Lemon‐flavored beverage base | Sucrose and citric acid | Positive |
N = 52 | 60–85 | Living independently | Yes | — | — | — | Desserts | Cherry flavor and cream flavor | Positive | |
N = 25 | 65–83 | Community dwelling | — | — | — | — | Meal | Tastes and flavors | Positive | |
Taste‐texture | N = 29 | 84.4 ± 4 | Nursing home | — | — | — | Yes and no | Main meals | Tastes and flavors | Positive |
N = 62 | 70 ± 5 | — | Yes | Yes | — | — | Yogurt | Sweetness and hardness | No effect | |
N = 37 | 60–81 | Community dwelling | Yes | Yes | — | — | Meal and snacking | Flavor–texture | Positive | |
Taste–appearance | N = 122 | 79.4 ± 1.1 | — | — | — | — | — | Maize soup | Vision and taste | Positive |
N = 37 | 60–75 | — | — | — | — | — | Bread | Color and taste | Positive | |
Appearance–texture | N = 37 | 65–101 | Nursing home | — | — | — | — | A variety of foods | Color and hardness | Positive |
N = 20 | 51–64& > 80 | Nursing home | — | — | — | — | Ice cream and desserts | Color and hardness | Positive |
4.1. Compensatory Strategies for Single Sensory
Overall, compensation strategies targeting taste and aroma show potential in increasing older adults' preference and intake of food. By increasing the intensity of one or several sensory attributes in food or by enhancing the dining environment, the acceptability of food for older adults can be effectively improved (Song et al. 2016). However, the effectiveness of these strategies may vary depending on individual preferences and the specific types of food involved, necessitating personalized adjustments in practical applications to better meet the dietary needs of older adults (Thomas et al. 2021). In addition to increasing sugar concentration, using sweeteners such as sugar alcohols and artificial sweeteners can enhance sweet taste perception without adding calories. These alternatives can satisfy older adults' desire for sweetness without increasing the risk of diabetes.
4.1.1. Sweet Taste
It has been found that sweet taste compensation strategies indicate that as age increases, older adults' taste thresholds for sweetness also rise (Schiffman et al. 1981; Easterby‐Smith et al. 1994; Methven et al. 2012), so increasing the concentration of sweetness in food can enhance their preference and intake to a certain extent (Schiffman et al. 1981; Toffanello et al. 2010; Pouyet et al. 2015). Studies have found that older adults tend to prefer beverages with higher sugar content, such as apple juice and peach juice (Kozlowska et al. 2003; Laureati et al. 2008). However, some studies have pointed out that simply increasing the concentration of sweetness does not always significantly increase food intake, which may be related to older adults' tolerance to sweetness, other sensory attributes of the food, and their overall dietary habits (De Jong et al. 1996). Therefore, when applying sweet taste compensation strategies, it is necessary to consider other factors comprehensively to achieve the best results.
4.1.2. Sour Taste
The results of research on sour taste compensation strategies are relatively complex. On one hand, some studies have shown that adding low concentrations of sour taste components (such as cherry flavor) to desserts can increase older adults' preference and intake of food (Kremer et al. 2007a), which may be due to the synergistic effect of sour taste with other flavors, making the overall taste of the food richer and more pleasant. On the other hand, other studies have found that enhancing sour taste in beverages does not always effectively promote intake in older adults (Arganini et al. 2013), which may be related to their sensitivity to sour taste, preferences, and other sensory attributes of the beverages. In addition, there are differences in the acceptance of sour taste among different individuals, so when applying sour taste compensation strategies in practice, it is necessary to fully consider the individual differences of older adults and the specific types of food to achieve the best compensation effect.
4.1.3. Salty Taste
Research on salty taste compensation strategies indicates that older adults have a higher taste threshold for saltiness, and appropriately increasing the saltiness in food can improve their preference for food to a certain extent (Pangborn and Braddock 1989; Dermiki et al. 2013). For example, studies have found that increasing the saltiness in mashed potatoes and meatloaf can enhance older adults' preference for these foods (Kremer et al. 2014). Additionally, studies have found that adding sauces to food (Pouyet et al. 2014; Kimura et al. 2019; Appleton 2018; Best and Appleton 2011) leads to older subjects always preferring food with added sauces over standard samples, with the enhancement of saltiness effectively increasing their preference and intake. However, some studies have pointed out that adding seasonings or monosodium glutamate (MSG) when cooking meat does not always significantly increase older adults' dietary intake (Essed et al. 2007), which may be related to their sensitivity to saltiness, other sensory attributes of the food, and their overall dietary habits. Moreover, excessive intake of salt can have adverse effects on the health of older adults, so when applying salty taste compensation strategies, it is necessary to find a balance between improving food preference and ensuring health, while also considering the individual differences of older adults and the specific types of food.
4.1.4. Aroma
Aroma compensation strategies play an important role in increasing older adults' preference and intake of food. Research has shown that adding spices and seasonings to food, especially natural spices such as herbs, onions, and garlic, can significantly enhance older adults' interest and intake of food (Thomas et al. 2021; Kimura et al. 2019; Best and Appleton 2011; van den Heuvel et al. 2020). For example, studies by van den van den Heuvel et al. (2018) and Henry et al. (2003) have found that adding spices can increase the attractiveness of food, making older adults more willing to consume it; adding cheese flavor to sausages is also favored by older adults (Elsner et al. 1998). Additionally, enhancing the aroma of food can also increase older adults' hunger and daily dietary intake (Mathey et al. 2001). In the study by Pouyet et al. (2015), soft bread and aubergine sauce samples were used as blank control samples, and flavorings such as lemon, roasted garlic, salt, and pepper were added, showing that older adults had a higher intake of the flavor‐enhanced samples. However, Koskinen et al. (2003) found that adding redcurrant aroma to oatmeal yogurt and enhancing the samples' aroma showed a positive correlation with the preferences of older subjects, but aroma enhancement did not directly increase intake in older adults. There are also studies pointing out that enhancing aroma in stews does not always significantly improve older adults' preference and intake of food (Kremer et al. 2014), which may be related to other sensory attributes of the food and the individual differences of older adults. Therefore, when applying aroma compensation strategies, it is necessary to consider the multiple sensory attributes of food and the specific needs of older adults comprehensively to achieve the best effect.
4.1.5. Vision
Visual compensation strategies play a vital role in the diet of older adults. With the decline of olfactory and gustatory functions, the importance of vision in food perception is increasingly highlighted (Wijk et al. 1999). Research has shown that older adults have better discrimination and higher preferences for bright colors such as red and yellow, so enhancing the visual impact of food by adding food coloring can effectively promote their food intake (Schiffman 2000; Garber et al. 2001). For example, colorful foods such as ice cream and cheesecake are more favored by older adults (Wendin et al. 2021). In addition, using larger tableware and increasing the portion size of food can also significantly increase food intake (Wansink and van Ittersum 2003; Wansink et al. 2005). However, there are relatively few studies on the impact of single visual compensation on the diet of older adults at present, and more related research is urgently needed in the future to further explore the mechanism of action and application strategies of visual factors in the diet of older adults, providing a scientific basis for improving their dietary experience and nutritional intake.
4.1.6. Hearing
Auditory compensation strategies play an undeniable role in the perception of food flavor. Research has shown that various sounds related to food, such as the sound of unwrapping packaging, the sound of food preparation, and the sound of chewing, can affect older adults' perception of food taste (Wang et al. 2019; Endo et al. 2017). For example, providing older adults with prerecorded chewing sounds can improve their perception of the taste of unevenly textured food, while playing fake chewing sounds that do not match the texture of the food may change their perceptual experience. In addition, sound stimuli in the environment, such as music, can also cause emotional changes, which in turn affect food preferences and dietary intake (Guéguen et al. 2008; Crisinel et al. 2012). When music with a sweet atmosphere is played, it can enhance people's perception of the sweetness of food, while music with a sad atmosphere may increase the perception of bitterness (Guedes et al. 2023). This indicates that the emotional response elicited by sound can regulate people's sensory evaluation of food. Although there are relatively few studies on auditory compensation strategies at present, their potential in improving the dietary experience and promoting healthy eating in older adults is worth further exploration.
4.2. Implementation and Effectiveness of Multimodal Food Compensation Strategies
Multimodal food compensation strategies have shown significant advantages in increasing older adults' preference and intake of food. The effectiveness of single‐sensory compensation strategies is limited, while multimodal strategies can compensate for the insufficiency of single‐sensory perception through the interaction and synergistic effects between different senses, thereby more effectively enhancing the dietary experience of older adults (Wang et al. 2022). In recent years, some studies have explored food compensation strategies for older adults from multiple dimensions such as food odor, appearance, and texture simultaneously (Klosse et al. 2004; van der Meij et al. 2015). Improving the taste, texture, and appearance of food through multimodal strategies can enhance consumers' sensory pleasure, increase their preference for food, and thus promote food intake. The following sections will focus on reviewing the impact of four types of sensory interactions, namely, taste–aroma, taste and aroma–texture, appearance–taste, and appearance–texture, on older adults' food preferences and intake.
4.2.1. Taste and Aroma
The olfactory and gustatory systems are interconnected and activated in the same areas of the brain, so adding aroma and taste modifiers to food can enhance older adults' preference and intake of food (Bellisle et al. 1991; Henry et al. 2003; Mathey et al. 2001; Gagnon et al. 2014; Biswas and Szocs 2019). For example, sweetness and specific flavor compounds increase older adults' preference for and intake of ONS (Lester et al. 2022). Adding sucrose and citric acid to beverages at appropriate concentrations can significantly enhance older adults' liking for the beverages (Murphy and Withee 1986); adding two different aroma compounds, cream and a subtle hint of cherry, to desserts can also significantly increase the preference of most older adults (Kremer et al. 2007a). Meals fortified with a variety of flavors and odors (herbs, spices, onions, and garlic) enhance food liking, thereby increasing food intake (Thomas et al. 2021). Appleton (2009) and Best and Appleton (2011), respectively, investigated whether adding sauces to food could increase older adults' food intake and showed that consuming meals fortified with sauces containing a variety of flavors and aromas resulted in a significant increase in older adults' energy, protein, and fat intake. However, the degree of preference for different flavors among older adults may vary depending on their olfactory and gustatory abilities, and they may only show a higher preference for certain specific flavors in specific food systems (Kremer et al. 2014), which suggests that when applying taste–aroma compensation strategies, it is necessary to fully consider the individual differences of older adults and the specific types of food to achieve the best compensation effect.
4.2.2. Taste–Aroma and Texture
Texture and flavor are key factors affecting older adults' food choices, and enhancing the texture and nourishing flavor of food simultaneously can effectively improve their preference for food, thereby promoting their food intake (Mena et al. 2020; Forsberg et al. 2022). For example, finger snacks developed for Swedish older adults with motor difficulties were made softer by adding whey protein, soy protein, and fat, and off‐flavors in the sauce were reduced with sweet, sour, and bitter flavors, providing an excellent taste experience for these older adults (Forsberg et al. 2022). However, whether changes in the sensory characteristics of food can effectively enhance older adults' preferences also depends on the characteristics of the food itself and the individual differences of older adults. For example, in yogurt, texture has less impact on older adults (Kalviainen et al. 2002), but in cream soups and waffles, improvements in texture and flavor are extremely important for older consumers (Kremer et al. 2005, 2007b). Recent studies have also found that adding plum sauce (as a lubricant, while enhancing sweet and sour flavors) has no significant effect on older adults (Behannis et al. 2022). Therefore, when applying such compensation strategies, it is necessary to consider the multiple sensory attributes of food and the specific needs of older adults comprehensively to achieve the best compensation effect.
4.2.3. Appearance and Taste–Aroma
The color, shape, and other appearance characteristics of food have an important impact on consumers' attraction, and their combination with taste and aroma can further enhance the attractiveness of food (Piqueras‐Fiszman and Spence 2014). Research has found that pink and red are most strongly associated with sweet taste, yellow and green with sour taste, white and blue with salty taste, and brown/black and purple with bitter taste (Spence et al. 2015). For example, using ingredients such as cilantro, red peppers, bacon, and sour cream to improve the appearance and taste of high‐energy corn soup can significantly increase older adults' preference for it, thereby promoting their intake (Zhou et al. 2021). In addition, improving the appearance (such as darker color) and taste (with the typical smell and flavor of bread) of bread can also enhance older consumers' satisfaction and preference for bread (Moretton et al. 2023). This indicates that when people choose food, they not only pay attention to its taste and smell but also place great importance on its appearance characteristics. For example, strawberry and chocolate beverages are favored for their sweet taste and bright colors (Zellner et al. 2014), and cakes are favored for their unique shape. The visual stimulation of color may trigger a preference for expected taste perceptions.
4.2.4. Appearance and Texture
The appearance and texture of food not only attract consumers' attention but also affect their preferences and intake. For example, biscuits with smooth surfaces and crispy textures are more favored by consumers (Jansson‐Boyd and Kobescak 2020). Older adults and those with dysphagia are prone to anorexia and consequently malnutrition due to the unattractive appearance and texture of food (Pereira et al. 2021). Research has found that older adults with reduced appetite prefer food with color variations, high fiber, and firm texture (van der Meij et al. 2015). In addition, older adults show a preference for vanilla and lemon‐flavored ice cream as well as Swedish cheesecake, which is mainly attributed to the vibrant appearance and light, moist texture of these three food items (Wendin et al. 2021). There are also studies that have used 3D technology to transform the unappealing and difficult‐to‐swallow texture of wood ear pudding into visually appealing food, suitable for consumption by older adults with swallowing difficulties or related issues (Xing et al. 2022). This indicates that improving the appearance and texture of food can effectively increase older adults' acceptance and intake of food, thereby improving their nutritional status and quality of life.
5. Conclusions and Future Perspectives
5.1. Conclusions
This review comprehensively examines sensory decline in older adults and its impact on their dietary behavior, thoroughly analyzes the limitations of existing sensory evaluation methods in the elderly population, and summarizes various sensory compensation strategies that can enhance older adults' food preferences and intake. The study highlights that aging is associated with a general decline in sensory functions such as vision, hearing, olfaction, and taste, which can lead to reduced food perception, selection, and consumption, potentially resulting in malnutrition (Cavazzana et al. 2018; Doets and Kremer 2016). The exacerbation of sensory decline due to COVID‐19 further complicates dietary management in older adults (Burges Watson et al. 2021; Chaaban et al. 2021). While most sensory evaluation methods are designed for healthy adults, some can be adapted for use with older adults after modifications. For example, the TI method can assess dynamic sensory attributes like viscosity or flavor release (Hutchings et al. 2014; Luckett et al. 2016), and rapid methods like sorting and PM are accessible to older adults due to their simplicity (Scott et al. 2017; Mena et al. 2020). Additionally, the CATA method and JAR scale simplify the evaluation process, making them suitable for assessing older adults' food acceptance and preferences (Ares et al. 2014; Rutkowska et al. 2022). However, individual differences in cognitive function and sensory sensitivity among older adults must be considered when applying these methods. The review also presents a range of compensation strategies, including enhancing individual sensory attributes like sweetness and aroma, as well as multisensory approaches that improve food appearance, texture, and sound, thereby increasing older adults' food preferences and intake (Song et al. 2016; Thomas et al. 2021). These strategies offer promising solutions to mitigate the dietary challenges posed by sensory decline in older adults, contributing to improved nutritional status and quality of life.
5.2. Future Perspectives
To effectively address the challenges posed by sensory decline in older adults and improve their food preferences and nutritional intake, future research should focus on several strategic areas. First and foremost, it is crucial to deepen the understanding of the neurobiological mechanisms behind sensory decline. This includes investigating how these changes affect dietary behavior and nutritional intake through neurobiological pathways; examining individual differences, such as genetic factors, lifestyle, and chronic diseases, which play a significant role in modulating sensory decline, providing a solid foundation for developing personalized prevention and intervention strategies (Reuter‐Loryea et al. 2020; Wang et al. 2020); and additionally, continuing research on the long‐term impact of the pandemic on sensory functions, particularly the effects of neuroinflammation and damage to olfaction and taste (Iebba et al. 2021; Srinivasan et al. 2022). Second, developing innovative sensory evaluation methods for older adults, especially those in poor health, is essential. Leveraging information technology and neurobiological tools, such as EEG and FaceReader, can provide deeper insights into sensory processing (Poldrack and Wagner 2004). Lastly, a comprehensive exploration of sensory compensation strategies is necessary. The implementation of these strategies should be continuously optimized to improve their effectiveness. This includes developing personalized plans for older adults with varying health conditions, dietary habits, and cultural backgrounds (Wang et al. 2019). Long‐term studies are crucial for assessing the sustained impact of these strategies on health and nutritional status, ensuring their safety and sustainability (Regan et al. 2019, 2021). Interdisciplinary collaboration will further enhance the scientific validity and effectiveness of these strategies by integrating insights from psychology, nutrition, and food science (Fischer et al. 2009; Oleszkiewicz et al. 2020). Through ongoing research and innovation, we can provide older adults with more effective sensory compensation methods, improving their dietary health and overall quality of life.
Author Contributions
Yilin Li: conceptualization (lead), formal analysis (lead), visualization (lead), writing – original draft (lead). Shuying Wang: manuscript preparation, data acquisition and analysis. Lanxin Zhang: data curation (equal), formal analysis (equal). Qianhui Dong: data curation (equal), formal analysis (equal). Xinyu Hu: data curation (equal), formal analysis (equal). Yuxin Yang: data curation (equal), formal analysis (equal). Ting Liu: conceptualization (equal), resources. Baopei Wu: writing – review and editing. Bingqi Shan: writing – review and editing. Chuncao Yin: writing – review and editing (equal). Qinggang Xie: conceptualization (equal). Baoqing Zhu: writing – review and editing, supervision, resources, project administration, conceptualization (equal). Chengdong Zheng: conceptualization (equal), project administration (equal).
Conflicts of Interest
Yilin Li, Ting Liu, Bingqi Shan, Qinggang Xie, and Chengdong Zheng are employed by Feihe Dairy Co. Ltd., Tsitsihar, China. The other authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.
Acknowledgments
The authors have nothing to report.
Yilin Li, and Shuying Wang are contributed equally to this work.
Contributor Information
Baoqing Zhu, Email: zhubaoqing@bjfu.edu.cn.
Chengdong Zheng, Email: zhengchengdong@feihe.com.
Data Availability Statement
Data will be made available upon request.
References
- Abrahams, C. 2020. The Impact of COVID‐19 to Date on Older People's Mental and Physical Health. Age UK. [Google Scholar]
- Aguiar, L. A. , Melo L., and de Lacerda Oliveira L.. 2018. “Validation of Rapid Descriptive Sensory Methods Against Conventional Descriptive Analyses: A Systematic Review.” Critical Reviews in Food Science and Nutrition 59, no. 16: 2535–2552. 10.1080/10408398.2018.1459468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahmed, T. , and Haboubi N.. 2010. “Assessment and Management of Nutrition in Older People and Its Importance to Health.” Clinical Interventions in Aging 5: 207. 10.2147/cia.s9664. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alam, M. R. , Karmokar S., Reza S., Kabir M. R., Ghosh S., and Mamun M. A. A.. 2021. “Geriatric Malnutrition and Depression: Evidence From Elderly Home Care Population in Bangladesh.” Preventive Medicine Reports 23: 101478. 10.1016/j.pmedr.2021.101478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Albert, A. , Salvador A., Schlich P., and Fiszman S.. 2012. “Comparison Between Temporal Dominance of Sensations (TDS) and Key‐Attribute Sensory Profiling for Evaluating Solid Food With Contrasting Textural Layers: Fish Sticks.” Food Quality and Preference 24, no. 1: 111–118. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2011.10.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Allen, V. J. , Withers C. A., Hough G., Gosney M. A., and Methven L.. 2014. “A New Rapid Detection Threshold Method for Use With Older Adults: Reducing Fatigue Whilst Maintaining Accuracy.” Food Quality and Preference 36: 104–110. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2014.03.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Amieva, H. , and Ouvrard C.. 2020. “Does Treating Hearing Loss in Older Adults Improve Cognitive Outcomes? A Review.” Journal of Clinical Medicine 9, no. 3: 805. 10.3390/jcm9030805. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andrade, F. C. D. , and López‐Ortega M.. 2017. “Educational Differences in Health Among Middle‐Aged and Older Adults in Brazil and Mexico.” Journal of Aging and Health 29, no. 6: 923–950. 10.1177/0898264317705781. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Annear, M. J. , Otani J., Gao X., and Keeling S.. 2016. “Japanese Perceptions of Societal Vulnerability to Disasters During Population Ageing: Constitution of a New Scale and Initial Findings.” International Journal of Disaster Risk Reduction 18: 32–40. 10.1016/j.ijdrr.2016.06.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Appleton, K. M. 2009. “Increases in Energy, Protein and Fat Intake Following the Addition of Sauce to an Older Person's Meal.” Appetite 52, no. 1: 161–165. 10.1016/j.appet.2008.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Appleton, K. M. 2018. “Limited Compensation at the Following Meal for Protein and Energy Intake at a Lunch Meal in Healthy Free‐Living Older Adults.” Clinical Nutrition 37, no. 3: 970–977. 10.1016/j.clnu.2017.03.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ares, G. , Barreiro C., and Gimenze A.. 2009. “Comparison of Attribute Liking and Jar Scales to Evaluate the Adequacy of Sensory Attributes of Milk Desserts.” Journal of Sensory Studies 24, no. 5: 664–676. 10.1111/j.1745-459x.2009.00232.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ares, G. , Tárrega A., Izquierdo L., and Jaeger S. R.. 2014. “Investigation of the Number of Consumers Necessary to Obtain Stable Sample and Descriptor Configurations From Check‐All‐That‐Apply (CATA) Questions.” Food Quality and Preference 31: 135–141. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2013.08.012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Arganini, C. , Donini L. M., Peparaio M., and Sinesio F.. 2013. “Influence of Chemosensory Impairment on Intake and Liking of a Functional Drink in Undernourished Institutionalized Elderly People.” Journal of Aging Research and Lifestyle 2014: 1. [Google Scholar]
- Arikawa, E. , Kaneko N., Nohara K., Yamaguchi T., Mitsuyama M., and Sakai T.. 2020. “Influence of Olfactory Function on Appetite and Nutritional Status in the Elderly Requiring Nursing Care.” Journal of Nutrition, Health & Aging 24, no. 4: 398–403. 10.1007/s12603-020-1334-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Artal, P. 2014. “Optics of the Eye and Its Impact in Vision: ATutorial.” Advances in Optics and Photonics 6, no. 3: 340. 10.1364/aop.6.000340. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Artal, P. , Berrio E., Guirao A., and Piers P.. 2002. “Contribution of the Cornea and Internal Surfaces to the Change of Ocular Aberrations With Age.” Journal of the Optical Society of America A 19, no. 1: 137. 10.1364/josaa.19.000137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baraldi, G. d. S. , de Almeida L. C., and Borges A. C. d. C.. 2007. “Hearing Loss in Aging.” Revista Brasileira de Otorrinolaringologia 73, no. 1: 64–70. 10.1590/S0034-72992007000100010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Behannis, M. , Scott C. H., Minh H., Hollis A., Phyllis J. S., and Robyn D. W.. 2022. “Effect of Age on Sensory Perception of Beef Patties With Varying Firmness.” Meat Science 192: 108869. 10.1016/j.meatsci.2022.108869. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bellisle, F. , Monneuse M. O., Chabert M., Larue‐Achagiotis C., Lanteaume M. T., and Louis‐Sylvestre J.. 1991. “Monosodium Glutamate as a Palatability Enhancer in the European Diet.” Physiology & Behavior 49, no. 5: 869–873. 10.1016/0031-9384(91)90196-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berrio, E. , Tabernero J., and Artal P.. 2010. “Optical Aberrations and Alignment of the Eye With Age.” Journal of Vision 10, no. 14: 34. 10.1167/10.14.34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Besser, J. , Stropahl M., Urry E., and Launer S.. 2018. “Comorbidities of Hearing Loss and the Implications of Multimorbidity for Audiological Care.” Hearing Research 369: 3–14. 10.1016/j.heares.2018.06.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Best, R. L. , and Appleton K. M.. 2011. “Comparable Increases in Energy, Protein and Fat Intakes Following the Addition of Seasonings and Sauces to an Older Person's Meal.” Appetite 56, no. 1: 179–182. 10.1016/j.appet.2010.10.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birte‐Antina, W. , Ilona C., Antje H., and Thomas H.. 2017. “Olfactory Training With Older People.” International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 33, no. 1: 212–220. 10.1002/gps.4725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Biswas, D. , and Szocs C.. 2019. “The Smell of Healthy Choices: Cross‐Modal Sensory Compensation Effects of Ambient Scent on Food Purchases.” Journal of Marketing Research 56, no. 1: 123–141. 10.1177/0022243718820585. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bowl, M. R. , and Dawson S. J.. 2018. “Age‐Related Hearing Loss.” Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Medicine 9, no. 8: a033217. 10.1101/cshperspect.a033217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burges Watson, D. L. , Campbell M., Hopkins C., Smith B., Kelly C., and Deary V.. 2021. “Altered Smell and Taste: Anosmia, Parosmia and the Impact of Long Covid‐19.” PLoS One 16, no. 9: e0256998. 10.1371/journal.pone.0256998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cao, G. , Wang K., Chen Z., et al. 2021. “Analysis of Current Status and Influencing Factors of Vision Impairment in Chinese Seniors.” Chinese Journal of Prevention and Control of Chronic Diseases 29: 1004–6194. 10.16386/j.cjpccd.issn.1004-6194.2021.01.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cavazzana, A. , Röhrborn A., Garthus‐Niegel S., Larsson M., Hummel T., and Croy I.. 2018. “Sensory‐Specific Impairment Among Older People. An Investigation Using Both Sensory Thresholds and Subjective Measures Across the Five Senses.” PLoS One 13, no. 8: e0202969. 10.1371/journal.pone.0202969. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cerf Ducastel, B. , and Murphy C.. 2003. “FMRI Brain Activation in Response to Odors Is Reduced in Primary Olfactory Areas of Elderly Subjects.” Brain Research 986, no. 1–2: 39–53. 10.1016/s0006-8993(03)03168-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaaban, N. , Høier A. T. Z. B., and Andersen B. V.. 2021. “A Detailed Characterisation of Appetite, Sensory Perceptional, and Eating‐Behavioural Effects of COVID‐19: Self‐Reports From the Acute and Post‐Acute Phase of Disease.” Food 10, no. 4: 892. 10.3390/foods10040892. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chaffee, O. , and Ross C. F.. 2023. “Older Adults' Acceptance of Ready‐To‐Eat Meals in Relation to Food Choice and Sensory Ability.” Journal of Food Science 88, no. 6: 2611–2628. 10.1111/1750-3841.16573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chan, A. , Raman P., Ma S., and Malhotra R.. 2015. “Loneliness and All‐Cause Mortality in Community‐Dwelling Elderly Singaporeans.” Demographic Research 32: 1361–1382. [Google Scholar]
- Chen, S. P. , Bhattacharya J., and Pershing S.. 2017. “Association of Vision Loss With Cognition in Older Adults.” JAMA Ophthalmology 135, no. 9: 963. 10.1001/jamaophthalmol.2017.2838. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Claus, L. E. , Leland E. M., Tai K. Y., et al. 2022. “Olfactory Loss and Beyond: A Practical Review of Chemosensory Dysfunction.” Journal of the American Board of Family Medicine 35, no. 2: 406–419. 10.3122/jabfm.2022.02.210373. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cliceri, D. , Dinnella C., Depezay L., et al. 2017. “Exploring Salient Dimensions in a Free Sorting Task: A Cross‐Country Study Within the Elderly Population.” Food Quality and Preference 60: 19–30. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2017.03.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Connor, E. E. , Zhou Y., and Liu G. E.. 2018. “The Essence of Appetite: Does Olfactory Receptor Variation Play a Role?” Journal of Animal Science 96, no. 4: 1551–1558. 10.1093/jas/sky068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Correia, C. , Lopez K. J., Wroblewski K. E., et al. 2016. “Global Sensory Impairment in Older Adults in the United States.” Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 64, no. 2: 306–313. 10.1111/jgs.13955. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crimmins, E. 2017. “Trends in Mortality, Disease, and Physiological Status in the Older Population.” In Future Directions for the Demography of Aging. National Academies. [Google Scholar]
- Crisinel, A.‐S. , Cosser S., King S., Jones R., Petrie J., and Spence C.. 2012. “A Bittersweet Symphony: Systematically Modulating the Taste of Food by Changing the Sonic Properties of the Soundtrack Playing in the Background.” Food Quality and Preference 24, no. 1: 201–204. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2011.08.009. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Croy, I. , Nordin S., and Hummel T.. 2014. “Olfactory Disorders and Quality of Life—An Updated Review.” Chemical Senses 39, no. 3: 185–194. 10.1093/chemse/bjt072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cui, Y. , Zhang L., Zhang M., et al. 2017. “Prevalence and Causes of Low Vision and Blindness in a Chinese Population With Type 2 Diabetes: The Dongguan Eye Study.” Scientific Reports 7, no. 1: 11195. 10.1038/s41598-017-11365-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daute, M. , Jack F., Baxter I., Harrison B., Grigor J., and Walker G.. 2021. “Comparison of Three Approaches to Assess the Flavour Characteristics of Scotch Whisky Spirit.” Applied Sciences 11, no. 4: 1410. 10.3390/app11041410. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Davenport, R. 2004. “The Flavor of Aging.” Science of Aging Knowledge Environment 2004: 12. 10.1126/sageke.2004.12.ns1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis, A. , McMahon C. M., Pichora‐Fuller K. M., et al. 2016. “Aging and Hearing Health: The Life‐Course Approach.” Gerontologist 56, no. Suppl 2: S256–S267. 10.1093/geront/gnw033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dawes, P. , Cruickshanks K. J., Moore D. R., et al. 2014. “Cigarette Smoking, Passive Smoking, Alcohol Consumption, and Hearing Loss.” Journal of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology 15, no. 4: 663–674. 10.1007/s10162-014-0461-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Jong, N. , Graaf C., and Van Staveren W. A.. 1996. “Effect of Sucrose in Breakfast Items on Pleasantness and Food Intake in the Elderly.” Physiology & Behavior 60, no. 6: 1453–1462. 10.1016/s0031-9384(96)00306-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deer, R. R. , Hosein E., Harvey M., et al. 2022. “Impact of COVID‐19 Infection and Persistent Lingering Symptoms on Patient Reported Indicators of Nutritional Risk and Malnutrition.” Nutrients 14, no. 3: 642. 10.3390/nu14030642. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dehlholm, C. , Brockhoff P. B., Meinert L., Aaslyng M. D., and Bredie W. L. P.. 2012. “Rapid Descriptive Sensory Methods – Comparison of Free Multiple Sorting, Partial Napping, Napping, Flash Profiling and Conventional Profiling.” Food Quality and Preference 26, no. 2: 267–277. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2012.02.012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dermiki, M. , Mounayar R., Suwankanit C., et al. 2013. “Maximising Umami Taste in Meat Using Natural Ingredients: Effects on Chemistry, Sensory Perception and Hedonic Liking in Young and Old Consumers.” Journal of the Science of Food and Agriculture 93, no. 13: 3312–3321. 10.1002/jsfa.6177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Monaco, R. , Su C., Masi P., and Cavella S.. 2014. “Temporal Dominance of Sensations: A Review.” Trends in Food Science and Technology 38, no. 2: 104–112. 10.1016/j.tifs.2014.04.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Divers, R. , Robinson A., Miller L., De Vito A. N., Pugh E., and Calamia M.. 2021. “Beyond Depression: Examining the Role of Anxiety and Anxiety Sensitivity on Subjective Cognition and Functioning in Older Adults.” Aging & Mental Health 11: 1–7. 10.1080/13607863.2021.1966747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doets, E. L. , and Kremer S.. 2016. “The Silver Sensory Experience – A Review of Senior Consumers' Food Perception, Liking and Intake.” Food Quality and Preference 48: 316–332. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2015.08.010. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dominique, V. , Sylvie C., Michael N., and Hervé A.. 2018. “Projective Mapping & Sorting Tasks.” In Descriptive Analysis in Sensory Evaluation, 535–559. John Wiley & Sons Ltd. [Google Scholar]
- Doty, R. L. 2018. “Measurement of Chemosensory Function.” World Journal of Otorhinolaryngology — Head and Neck Surgery 4, no. 1: 11–28. 10.1016/j.wjorl.2018.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doty, R. L. , and Kamath V.. 2014. “The Influences of Age on Olfaction: A Review.” Frontiers in Psychology 5: 20. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drewnowski, A. , and Evans W. J.. 2001. “Nutrition, Physical Activity, and Quality of Life in Older Adults: Summary.” Journal of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences 56, no. Supplement 2: 89–94. 10.1093/gerona/56.suppl_2.89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dubbelman, M. , Van der Heijde G. L., and Weeber H. A.. 2001. “The Thickness of the Aging Human Lens Obtained From Corrected Scheimpflug Images.” Optometry and Vision Science 78, no. 6: 411–416. 10.1097/00006324-200106000-00013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duffy, V. B. , Cain W. S., and Ferris A. M.. 1999. “Measurement of Sensitivity to Olfactory Flavor: Application in a Study of Aging and Dentures.” Chemical Senses 24, no. 6: 671–677. 10.1093/chemse/24.6.671. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Easterby‐Smith, V. , Besford J., and Heath M.. 1994. “The Effect of Age on the Recognition Thresholds of Three Sweeteners: Sucrose, Saccharin and Aspartame.” Gerodontology 11, no. 1: 39–45. 10.1111/j.1741-2358.1994.tb00101.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ellis, S. , Sheik Ali S., and Ahmed W.. 2021. “A Review of the Impact of Hearing Interventions on Social Isolation and Loneliness in Older People With Hearing Loss.” European Archives of Oto‐Rhino‐Laryngology 278, no. 12: 4653–4661. 10.1007/s00405-021-06847-w. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elsner, F. , Chang H. C., and Carpenter J. A.. 1998. “Determination of Acceptability of Flavored Oat Bran Frankfurters by Older Adults.” International Journal of Food Sciences and Nutrition 49, no. 3: 219–223. 10.3109/09637489809086415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Endo, H. , Ino S., and Fujisaki W.. 2017. “Texture‐Dependent Effects of Pseudo‐Chewing Sound on Perceived Food Texture and Evoked Feelings in Response to Nursing Care Foods.” Appetite 116: 493–501. 10.1016/j.appet.2017.05.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Erausquin, G. A. , Snyder H., Carrillo M., Hosseini A. A., Brugha T. S., and Seshadri S.. 2021. “The Chronic Neuropsychiatric Sequelae of COVID‐19: The Need for a Prospective Study of Viral Impact on Brain Functioning.” Alzheimer's & Dementia 17, no. 6: 1056–1065. 10.1002/alz.12255. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Esmerino, E. A. , Paixão J. A., Cruz A. G., Garitta L., Hough G., and Maria H.. 2015. “Survival Analysis: A Consumer‐Friendly Method to Estimate the Optimum Sucrose Level in Probiotic Petit Suisse.” Journal of Dairy Science 98, no. 11: 7544–7551. 10.3168/jds.2015-9651. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Essed, N. H. , van Staveren W. A., Kok F. J., and de Graaf C.. 2007. “No Effect of 16 Weeks Flavor Enhancement on Dietary Intake and Nutritional Status of Nursing Home Elderly.” Appetite 48, no. 1: 29–36. 10.1016/j.appet.2006.06.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fantozzi, P. J. , Pampena E., Di Vanna D., et al. 2020. “Xerostomia, Gustatory and Olfactory Dysfunctions in Patients With COVID‐19.” American Journal of Otolaryngology 41, no. 6: 102721. 10.1016/j.amjoto.2020.102721. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feng, G. , Ingvalson E. M., Grieco‐Calub T. M., et al. 2018. “Neural Preservation Underlies Speech Improvement From Auditory Deprivation in Young Cochlear Implant Recipients.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 115, no. 5: E1022–E1031. 10.1073/pnas.1717603115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Field, K. , and Duizer L. M.. 2016. “Food Sensory Properties and the Older Adult.” Journal of Texture Studies 47, no. 4: 266–276. 10.1111/jtxs.12197. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fischer, M. E. , Cruickshanks K. J., Klein B. E. K., Klein R., Schubert C. R., and Wiley T. L.. 2009. “Multiple Sensory Impairment and Quality of Life.” Ophthalmic Epidemiology 16, no. 6: 346–353. 10.3109/09286580903312236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fleming, E. E. , Ziegler G. R., and Hayes J. E.. 2015. “Check‐All‐That‐Apply (CATA), Sorting, and Polarized Sensory Positioning (PSP) With Astringent Stimuli.” Food Quality and Preference 45: 41–49. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2015.05.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forsberg, S. , Olsson V., Bredie W. L. P., Verstraelen E., Krona A., and Wendin K.. 2022. “Proposal of Development of Finger Foods for Older Adults With Motoric Eating Difficulties—a Study Based on Creative Design.” International Journal of Gastronomy and Food Science 28: 100516. 10.1016/j.ijgfs.2022.100516. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fukunaga, A. , Uematsu H., and Sugimoto K.. 2005. “Influences of Aging on Taste Perception and Oral Somatic Sensation.” Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences 60, no. 1: 109–113. 10.1093/gerona/60.1.109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gagnon, L. , Vestergaard M., Madsen K. H., et al. 2014. “Neural Correlates of Taste Perception in Congenital Olfactory Impairment.” Neuropsychologia 62: 297–305. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.07.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gao, S. S. , Chen K. J., Duangthip D., Lo E. C. M., and Chu C. H.. 2020. “The Oral Health Status of Chinese Elderly People With and Without Dementia: A Cross‐Sectional Study.” International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17, no. 6: 1913. 10.3390/ijerph17061913. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garber, L. L. , Hyatt E. M., and Starr R. G.. 2001. “Placing Food Color Experimentation Into a Valid Consumer Context.” Journal of Food Products Marketing 7, no. 3: 3–24. 10.1300/j038v07n03_02. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Gerst‐Emerson, K. , and Jayawardhana J.. 2015. “Loneliness as a Public Health Issue: The Impact of Loneliness on Health Care Utilization Among Older Adults.” American Journal of Public Health 105, no. 5: 1013–1019. 10.2105/ajph.2014.302427. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gopinath, B. , Flood V. M., McMahon C. M., Burlutsky G., Brand‐Miller J., and Mitchell P.. 2010. “Dietary Glycemic Load Is a Predictor of Age‐Related Hearing Loss in Older Adults.” Journal of Nutrition 140, no. 12: 2207–2212. 10.3945/jn.110.128462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gravina, S. A. , Yep G. L., and Khan M.. 2013. “Human Biology of Taste.” Annals of Saudi Medicine 33, no. 3: 217–222. 10.5144/0256-4947.2013.217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greenhalgh, T. , Knight M., Inada‐Kim M., Fulop N. J., Leach J., and Vindrola‐Padros C.. 2021. “Remote Management of Covid‐19 Using Home Pulse Oximetry and Virtual Ward Support.” BMJ 372: n677. 10.1136/bmj.n677. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griep, M. I. , Mets T. F., Collys K., Vogelaere P., Laska M., and Massart D. L.. 1997. “Odour Perception in Relation to Age, General Health, Anthropometry and Dental State.” Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics 25, no. 3: 263–275. 10.1016/s0167-4943(97)00017-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guedes, D. , Prada M., Lamy E., and Garrido M. V.. 2023. “Sweet Music Influences Sensory and Hedonic Perception of Food Products With Varying Sugar Levels.” Food Quality and Preference 104: 104752. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2022.104752. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Guéguen, N. , Jacob C., Le Guellec H., Morineau T., and Lourel M.. 2008. “Sound Level of Environmental Music and Drinking Behavior: A Field Experiment With Beer Drinkers.” Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 32, no. 10: 1795–1798. 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00764.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guo, J. , Radloff C. L., Wawrzynski S. E., and Cloyes K. G.. 2020. “Mining Twitter to Explore the Emergence of COVID‐19 Symptoms.” Public Health Nursing 37, no. 6: 934–940. 10.1111/phn.12809. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haile, L. M. , Kamenov K., Briant P. S., et al. 2021. “Hearing Loss Prevalence and Years Lived With Disability, 1990–2019: Findings From the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019.” Lancet 397, no. 10278: 996–1009. 10.1016/S0140-6736(21)00516-X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hardan, L. , Filtchev D., Kassem R., et al. 2021. “COVID‐19 and Alzheimer's Disease: A Literature Review.” Medicina 57, no. 11: 1159. 10.3390/medicina57111159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henry, C. J. K. , Woo J., Lightowler H. J., et al. 2003. “Use of Natural Food Flavours to Increase Food and Nutrient Intakes in Hospitalized Elderly in Hong Kong.” International Journal of Food Sciences and Nutrition 54, no. 4: 321–327. 10.1080/0963748031000148718. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van den Heuvel, E. , Murphy J., and Appleton K.. 2020. “The Provision of Recipes and Single‐Use Herb/Spice Packets to Increase Egg and Protein Intake in Community‐Dwelling Older Adults: ARandomised Controlled Trial.” Public Health Nutrition 20: 1–14. 10.1017/s1368980020002712. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hill, S. , Gregory M., Regan C., Fleming S., and Hards S.. 2017. What Works: Hearing Loss and Healthy Ageing. NHS. [Google Scholar]
- Huang, A. R. , Jiang K., Lin F. R., Deal J. A., and Reed N. S.. 2023. “Hearing Loss and Dementia Prevalence in Older Adults in the US.” JAMA 329, no. 2: 171–173. 10.1001/jama.2022.20954. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Humes, L. E. , and Young L. A.. 2016. “Sensory–Cognitive Interactions in Older Adults.” Ear and Hearing 37: 52S–61S. 10.1097/aud.0000000000000303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hummel, T. , Futschik T., Frasnelli J., and Hüttenbrink K.‐B.. 2003. “Effects of Olfactory Function, Age, and Gender on Trigeminally Mediated Sensations: A Study Based on the Lateralization of Chemosensory Stimuli.” Toxicology Letters 140‐141: 273–280. 10.1016/s0378-4274(03)00078-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hummel, T. , Whitcroft K. L., Andrews P., et al. 2017. “Position Paper on Olfactory Dysfunction.” Rhinology Journal 54, no. 26: 1–30. 10.4193/rhino16.248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hutchings, S. C. , Foster K. D., Grigor J. M. V., and Morgenstern M. P.. 2014. “Investigating Changes in Sensory Stickiness Perception With Age.” Food Quality and Preference 37: 1–9. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2014.04.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Iebba, V. , Zanotta N., Campisciano G., et al. 2021. “Profiling of Oral Microbiota and Cytokines in COVID‐19 Patients.” Frontiers in Microbiology 12: 1813. 10.3389/fmicb.2021.671813. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Imoscopi, A. , Inelmen E. M., Sergi G., Miotto F., and Manzato E.. 2012. “Taste Loss in the Elderly: Epidemiology, Causes and Consequences.” Aging Clinical and Experimental Research 24, no. 6: 570–579. 10.3275/8520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacobson, K. B. , Rao M., Bonilla H., et al. 2021. “Patients With Uncomplicated Coronavirus Disease 2019 (COVID‐19) Have Long‐Term Persistent Symptoms and Functional Impairment Similar to Patients With Severe COVID‐19: A Cautionary Tale During a Global Pandemic.” Clinical Infectious Diseases 73, no. 3: e826–e829. 10.1093/cid/ciab103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jaeger, S. R. , Beresford M. K., Lo K. R., Hunter D. C., Chheang S. L., and Ares G.. 2020. “What Does It Mean to Check‐All‐That‐Apply? Four Case Studies With Beverages.” Food Quality and Preference 80: 103794. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2019.103794. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Jansson‐Boyd, C. V. , and Kobescak M.. 2020. “To See Is to Hold: Using Food Surface Textures to Communicate Product Healthiness.” Food Quality and Preference 81: 103866. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2019.103866. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Jefferis, J. M. , Collerton J., Taylor J.‐P., et al. 2012. “The Impact of Visual Impairment on Mini‐Mental State Examination Scores in the Newcastle 85+ Study.” Age and Ageing 41, no. 4: 565–568. 10.1093/ageing/afs042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johansson, J. , Levi R., Jakobsson M., Gunnarsson S., and Samuelsson K.. 2022. “Multiprofessional Neurorehabilitation After COVID‐19 Infection Should Include Assessment of Visual Function.” Archives of Rehabilitation Research and Clinical Translation 4, no. 2: 100184. 10.1016/j.arrct.2022.100184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jonathan A, S. 1999. “The Influence of Aging on Oral Health and Consequences for Taste and Smell.” Physiology & Behavior 66, no. 2: 209–215. 10.1016/s0031-9384(98)00267-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joussain, P. , Bessy M., Faure F., et al. 2015. “Application of the European Test of Olfactory Capabilities in Patients With Olfactory Impairment.” European Archives of Oto‐Rhino‐Laryngology 273, no. 2: 381–390. 10.1007/s00405-015-3536-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kalviainen, N. , Salovaara H., and Tuorila H.. 2002. “Sensory Attributes and Preference Mapping of Muesli Oat Flakes.” Journal of Food Science 67, no. 1: 455–460. 10.1111/j.1365-2621.2002.tb11428.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kawagishi, S. , Kou F., Yoshino K., Tanaka T., and Masumi S.. 2009. “Decrease in Stereognostic Ability of the Tongue With Age.” Journal of Oral Rehabilitation 36, no. 12: 872–879. 10.1111/j.1365-2842.2009.02005.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim, Y.‐K. , Jombart L., Valentin D., and Kim K.‐O.. 2013. “A Cross‐Cultural Study Using Napping®: Do Korean and French Consumers Perceive Various Green Tea Products Differently?” Food Research International 53, no. 1: 534–542. 10.1016/j.foodres.2013.05.015. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kimura, A. , Yamaguchi K., Tohara H., et al. 2019. “Addition of Sauce Enhances Finger‐Snack Intake Among Japanese Elderly People With Dementia.” Clinical Interventions in Aging 14: 2031–2040. 10.2147/cia.s225815. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klosse, P. R. , Riga J., Cramwinckel A. B., and Saris W. H. M.. 2004. “The Formulation and Evaluation of Culinary Success Factors (CSFs) That Determine the Palatability of Food.” Food Service Technology 4, no. 3: 107–115. 10.1111/j.1471-5740.2004.00097.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Koskinen, S. , Nenonen A., and Tuorila H.. 2005. “Intakes of Cold Cuts in the Elderly Are Predicted by Olfaction and Mood, but Not by Flavor Type or Intensity of the Products.” Physiology & Behavior 85, no. 3: 314–323. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.04.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koskinen, S. , Kälviäinen N., and Tuorila H.. 2003. “Flavor Enhancement as a Tool for Increasing Pleasantness and Intake of a Snack Product Among the Elderly.” Appetite 41, no. 1: 87–96. 10.1016/s0195-6663(03)00054-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Koskinen, S. , and Tuorila H.. 2005. “Performance on an Odor Detection and Identification Test as a Predictor of Ortho‐ and Retronasal Odor Intensity Ratings in the Young and Elderly.” Food Quality and Preference 16, no. 5: 383–392. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2004.10.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kozlowska, K. , Jeruszka M., Matuszewska I., Roszkowski W., Barylko‐Pikielna N., and Brzozowska A.. 2003. “Hedonic Tests in Different Locations as Predictors of Apple Juice Consumption at Home in Elderly and Young Subjects.” Food Quality and Preference 14, no. 8: 653–661. 10.1016/s0950-3293(02)00207-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kremer, S. , Bult J. H. F., Mojet J., and Kroeze J. H. A.. 2007a. “Compensation for Age‐Associated Chemosensory Losses and Its Effect on the Pleasantness of a Custard Dessert and a Tomato Drink.” Appetite 48, no. 1: 96–103. 10.1016/j.appet.2006.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kremer, S. , Bult J. H. F., Mojet J., and Kroeze J. H. A.. 2007b. “Food Perception With Age and Its Relationship to Pleasantness.” Chemical Senses 32, no. 6: 591–602. 10.1093/chemse/bjm028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kremer, S. , Holthuysen N., and Boesveldt S.. 2014. “The Influence of Olfactory Impairment in Vital, Independently Living Older Persons on Their Eating Behaviour and Food Liking.” Food Quality and Preference 38: 30–39. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2014.05.012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kremer, S. , Mojet J., and Kroeze J. H. A.. 2005. “Perception of Texture and Flavor in Soups by Elderly and Young Subjects.” Journal of Texture Studies 36, no. 3: 255–272. 10.1111/j.1745-4603.2005.00015.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kremer, S. , Mojet J., and Kroeze J. H. A.. 2007. “Differences in Perception of Sweet and Savoury Waffles Between Elderly and Young Subjects.” Food Quality and Preference 18, no. 1: 106–116. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2005.08.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Laska, M. 2001. “Perception of Trigeminal Chemosensory Qualities in the Elderly.” Chemical Senses 26, no. 6: 681–689. 10.1093/chemse/26.6.681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laureati, M. , Pagliarini E., and Calcinoni O.. 2008. “Does the Enhancement of Chemosensory Stimuli Improve the Enjoyment of Food in Institutionalized Elderly People?” Journal of Sensory Studies 23, no. 2: 234–250. 10.1111/j.1745-459x.2008.00152.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lawless, H. T. , and Heymann H.. 2010. Sensory Evaluation of Food: Principles and Practices. Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Lee, S. , Kwak H. S., Kim S. S., and Lee Y.. 2021. “Combination of the Check‐All‐That‐Apply (CATA) Method and Just‐About‐Right (JAR) Scale to Evaluate Korean Traditional Rice Wine (Yakju).” Food 10, no. 8: 1895. 10.3390/foods10081895. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lester, S. , Kleijn M., Cornacchia L., Hewson L., Taylor M. A., and Fisk I.. 2022. “Factors Affecting Adherence, Intake, and Perceived Palatability of Oral Nutritional Supplements: A Literature Review.” Journal of Nutrition, Health & Aging 26, no. 7: 663–674. 10.1007/s12603-022-1819-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin, F. R. , Yaffe K., Xia J., et al. 2013. “Hearing Loss and Cognitive Decline in Older Adults.” JAMA Internal Medicine 173, no. 4: 293. 10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.1868. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu, F. , Yin J., Wang J., and Xu X.. 2022. “Food for the Elderly Based on Sensory Perception: A Review.” Current Research in Food Science 5: 1550–1558. 10.1016/j.crfs.2022.09.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu, J. , Grønbeck M. S., Di Monaco R., Giacalone D., and Bredie W. L. P.. 2016. “Performance of Flash Profile and Napping With and Without Training for Describing Small Sensory Differences in a Model Wine.” Food Quality and Preference 48: 41–49. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2015.08.008. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Löe, H. , and Karring T.. 1971. “The Three‐Dimensional Morphology of the Epithelium‐Connective Tissue Interface of the Gingiva as Related to Age and Sex.” European Journal of Oral Sciences 79, no. 3: 315–326. 10.1111/j.1600-0722.1971.tb02017.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luckett, C. R. , Meullenet J.‐F., and Seo H.‐S.. 2016. “Crispness Level of Potato Chips Affects Temporal Dynamics of Flavor Perception and Mastication Patterns in Adults of Different Age Groups.” Food Quality and Preference 51: 8–19. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2016.02.013. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lumbers, M. , and Raats M.. 2006. “Food Choices in Later Life.” Psychology of Food Choice 3: 289–310. 10.1079/9780851990323.0289. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mahieu, B. , Visalli M., Thomas A., and Schlich P.. 2021. “An Investigation of the Stability of Free‐Comment and Check‐All‐That‐Apply in Two Consumer Studies on Red Wines and Milk Chocolates.” Food Quality and Preference 90: 104159. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2020.104159. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Malone, C. , and Wachholtz A.. 2018. “The Relationship of Anxiety and Depression to Subjective Well‐Being in a Mainland Chinese Sample.” Journal of Religion and Health 57, no. 1: 266–278. 10.1007/s10943-017-0447-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manesse, C. , Ferdenzi C., Mantel M., et al. 2021. “The Prevalence of Olfactory Deficits and Their Effects on Eating Behavior From Childhood to Old Age: A Large‐Scale Study in the French Population.” Food Quality and Preference 93: 104273. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2021.104273. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Manesse, C. , Ferdenzi C., Sabri M., et al. 2017. “Dysosmia‐Associated Changes in Eating Behavior.” Chemosensory Perception 10, no. 4: 104–113. 10.1007/s12078-017-9237-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Manno, F. A. M. , Rodríguez‐Cruces R., Kumar R., Ratnanather J. T., and Lau C.. 2021. “Hearing Loss Impacts Gray and White Matter Across the Lifespan: Systematic Review, Meta‐Analysis and Meta‐Regression.” NeuroImage 231: 117826. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.117826. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mathey, M.‐F. A. M. , Siebelink E., de Graaf C., and Van Staveren W. A.. 2001. “Flavor Enhancement of Food Improves Dietary Intake and Nutritional Status of Elderly Nursing Home Residents.” Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences 56, no. 4: M200–M205. 10.1093/gerona/56.4.m200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayhew, E. , Schmidt S., and Lee S.‐Y.. 2016. “Napping‐Ultra Flash Profile as a Tool for Category Identification and Subsequent Model System Formulation of Caramel Corn Products.” Journal of Food Science 81, no. 7: S1782–S1790. 10.1111/1750-3841.13338. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mazzatenta, A. , Neri G., D'Ardes D., et al. 2020. “Smell and Taste in Severe CoViD‐19: Self‐Reported vs. Testing.” Frontiers in Medicine 7: 9409. 10.3389/fmed.2020.589409. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McKee, M. M. , Stransky M. L., and Reichard A.. 2018. “Hearing Loss and Associated Medical Conditions Among Individuals 65 Years and Older.” Disability and Health Journal 11, no. 1: 122–125. 10.1016/j.dhjo.2017.05.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van der Meij, B. S. , Wijnhoven H. A. H., Finlayson G. S., Oosten B. S. H., and Visser M.. 2015. “Specific Food Preferences of Older Adults With a Poor Appetite. A Forced‐Choice Test Conducted in Various Care Settings.” Appetite 90: 168–175. 10.1016/j.appet.2015.03.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meiselman, H. L. 2007. “Consumer‐Led Food Product Development.” In Integrating Consumer Responses to Food Products, 3–33. Woodhead Publishing Ltd. [Google Scholar]
- Mena, B. , Ashman H., Dunshea F. R., Hutchings S., Ha M., and Warner R. D.. 2020. “Exploring Meal and Snacking Behaviour of Older Adults in Australia and China.” Food 9, no. 4: 426. 10.3390/foods9040426. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meng, X. , Wang J., Sun J., and Zhu K.. 2022. “COVID‐19 and Sudden Sensorineural Hearing Loss: A Systematic Review.” Frontiers in Neurology 13: 749. 10.3389/fneur.2022.883749. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Methven, L. , Allen V., Withers C., and Gosney M.. 2012. “Conference on “Malnutrition Matters” Nutrition Society Symposium: Muscle Wasting With Age: ANew Challenge in Nutritional Care; Part 1 ‐The Underlying Factor Ageing and Taste.” Proceedings of the Nutrition Society 71: 556–565. 10.1017/S0029665112000742. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Methven, L. , Jiménez‐Pranteda M. L., and Lawlor J. B.. 2016. “Sensory and Consumer Science Methods Used With Older Adults: A Review of Current Methods and Recommendations for the Future.” Food Quality and Preference 48: 333–344. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2015.07.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Minnai, F. , De Bellis G., Dragani T. A., and Colombo F.. 2022. “COVID‐19 Mortality in ItalyVaries by Patient Age, Sex and Pandemic Wave.” Scientific Reports 12, no. 1: 4604. 10.1038/s41598-022-08573-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mojet, J. 2003. “Taste Perception With Age: Generic or Specific Losses in Supra‐Threshold Intensities of Five Taste Qualities?” Chemical Senses 28, no. 5: 397–413. 10.1093/chemse/28.5.397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mojet, J. , Christ‐Hazelhof E., and Heidema J.. 2005. “Taste Perception With Age: Pleasantness and Its Relationships With Threshold Sensitivity and Supra‐Threshold Intensity of Five Taste Qualities.” Food Quality and Preference 16, no. 5: 413–423. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2004.08.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mojet, J. , Köster E. P., and Prinz J. F.. 2005. “Do Tastants Have a Smell?” Chemical Senses 30, no. 1: 9–21. 10.1093/chemse/bjh251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moreira, C. R. , Azevedo L. R., Lauris J. R. P., Taga R., and Damante J. H.. 2006. “Quantitative Age‐Related Differences in Human Sublingual Gland.” Archives of Oral Biology 51, no. 11: 960–966. 10.1016/j.archoralbio.2006.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moretton, M. , Cattaneo C., Mosca A. C., et al. 2023. “Identification of Desirable Mechanical and Sensory Properties of Bread for the Elderly.” Food Quality and Preference 104: 104716. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2022.104716. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy, C. , and Gilmore M. M.. 1989. “Quality‐Specific Effects of Aging on the Human Taste System.” Perception & Psychophysics 45, no. 2: 121–128. 10.3758/bf03208046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy, C. , and Withee J.. 1986. “Age‐Related Differences in the Pleasantness of Chemosensory Stimuli.” Psychology and Aging 1, no. 4: 312–318. 10.1037//0882-7974.1.4.312. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nagarajan, N. , Assi L., Varadaraj V., et al. 2022. “Vision Impairment and Cognitive Decline Among Older Adults: ASystematic Review.” BMJ Open 12, no. 1: e047929. 10.1136/bmjopen-2020-047929. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naidu, S. B. , Shah A. J., Saigal A., et al. 2021. “The High Mental Health Burden of “Long COVID” and Its Association With On‐Going Physical and Respiratory Symptoms in all Adults Discharged From Hospital.” European Respiratory Journal 27: 626–631. 10.1183/13993003.04364-2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nanjo, Y. , Okuma T., Kuroda Y., et al. 2022. “Multiple Types of Taste Disorders Among Patients With COVID‐19.” Internal Medicine 61, no. 14: 2127–2134. 10.2169/internalmedicine.9065-21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- National Bureau of Statistics of China . 2020. “The Seventh National Census in 2020.” Www.stats.gov.cn. http://www.stats.gov.cn/tjsj/pcsj/.
- Negoias, S. , Croy I., Gerber J., et al. 2010. “Reduced Olfactory Bulb Volume and Olfactory Sensitivity in Patients With Acute Major Depression.” Neuroscience 169, no. 1: 415–421. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.05.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- NHSC . 2022. “Transcript of NHSC's September 20, 2022.” Press Conference. http://www.nhc.gov.cn/xcs/s3574/202209/ee4dc20368b440a49d270a228f5b0ac1.shtml.
- Nordin, S. 2017. “Chapter 3 ‐ Sensory Perception of Food and Aging.” In Food for the Aging Population, 57–82. Woodhead Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Nordin, S. , Brämerson A., Bringlöv E., Kobal G., Hummel T., and Bende M.. 2006. “Substance and Tongue‐Region Specific Loss in Basic Taste‐Quality Identification in Elderly Adults.” European Archives of Oto‐Rhino‐Laryngology 264, no. 3: 285–289. 10.1007/s00405-006-0169-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nwachukwu, I. , Nkire N., Shalaby R., et al. 2020. “COVID‐19 Pandemic: Age‐Related Differences in Measures of Stress, Anxiety and Depression in Canada.” International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17, no. 17: 6366. 10.3390/ijerph17176366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Obas, F. L. , Lee S. Y., Thomas L. C., and Schmidt S. J.. 2021. “Characterization and Categorization of Commercial Confectionary Gels Through Napping‐Ultra Flash Profile (UFP) and Hierarchical Clustering Analysis.” Journal of Food Science 86, no. 6: 2655–2670. 10.1111/1750-3841.15764. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oleszkiewicz, A. , Kunkel F., Larsson M., and Hummel T.. 2020. “Consequences of Undetected Olfactory Loss for Human Chemosensory Communication and Well‐Being.” Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences 375, no. 1800: 20190265. 10.1098/rstb.2019.0265. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oliver, P. , Cicerale S., Pang E., and Keast R.. 2018a. “A Comparison of Temporal Dominance of Sensation (TDS) and Quantitative Descriptive Analysis (QDATM) to Identify Flavors in Strawberries.” Journal of Food Science 83, no. 4: 1094–1102. 10.1111/1750-3841.14096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oliver, P. , Cicerale S., Pang E., and Keast R.. 2018b. “Comparison of Quantitative Descriptive Analysis to the Napping Methodology With and Without Product Training.” Journal of Sensory Studies 33, no. 3: e12331. 10.1111/joss.12331. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Olofsson, J. K. , Ekström I., Larsson M., and Nordin S.. 2021. “Olfaction and Aging: A Review of the Current State of Research and Future Directions.” I‐Perception 12, no. 3: 204166952110203. 10.1177/20416695211020331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ong, A. D. , Uchino B. N., and Wethington E.. 2015. “Loneliness and Health in Older Adults: A Mini‐Review and Synthesis.” Gerontology 62, no. 4: 443–449. 10.1159/000441651. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pagès, J. 2003. “Recueil Direct de Distances Sensorielles: Application à l'évaluation de dix Vins Blancs du Val‐de‐Loire.” Sciences des Aliments 23, no. 5–6: 679–688. 10.3166/sda.23.679-688. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pangborn, R. M. , and Braddock K. S.. 1989. “Ad Libitum Preferences for Salt in Chicken Broth.” Food Quality and Preference 1, no. 2: 47–52. 10.1016/s0950-3293(89)80001-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Paquet, C. , St‐Arnaud‐McKenzie D., Kergoat M.‐J., Ferland G., and Dube L.. 2003. “Direct and Indirect Effects of Everyday Emotions on Food Intake of Elderly Patients in Institutions.” Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences 58, no. 2: M153–M158. 10.1093/gerona/58.2.m153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park, H. S. , Lê S., Hong J. H., and Kim K. O.. 2014. “Sensory Perception of Yackwa (Korean Traditional Fried Cookie) by Consumer Groups of Different Age Using the Sorted Napping Procedure.” Journal of Sensory Studies 29, no. 6: 425–434. 10.1111/joss.12123. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Park, Y. H. , Shin S.‐H., Byun S. W., and Kim J. Y.. 2016. “Age‐ and Gender‐Related Mean Hearing Threshold in a Highly‐Screened Population: The Korean National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey 2010–2012.” PLoS One 11, no. 3: e0150783. 10.1371/journal.pone.0150783. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pathai, S. , Shiels P. G., Lawn S. D., Cook C., and Gilbert C.. 2013. “The Eye as a Model of Ageing in Translational Research – Molecular, Epigenetic and Clinical Aspects.” Ageing Research Reviews 12, no. 2: 490–508. 10.1016/j.arr.2012.11.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pause, B. M. , Miranda A., Göder R., Aldenhoff J. B., and Ferstl R.. 2001. “Reduced Olfactory Performance in Patients With Major Depression.” Journal of Psychiatric Research 35, no. 5: 271–277. 10.1016/s0022-3956(01)00029-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peelle, J. E. , Troiani V., Grossman M., and Wingfield A.. 2011. “Hearing Loss in Older Adults Affects Neural Systems Supporting Speech Comprehension.” Journal of Neuroscience 31, no. 35: 12638–12643. 10.1523/jneurosci.2559-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pereira, T. , Barroso S., and Gil M. M.. 2021. “Food Texture Design by 3D Printing: A Review.” Food 10, no. 2: 320. 10.3390/foods10020320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pickup, W. , Bremer P., and Peng M.. 2017. “Comparing Conventional Descriptive Analysis and Napping®‐UFP Against Physiochemical Measurements: ACase Study Using Apples.” Journal of the Science of Food and Agriculture 98, no. 4: 1476–1484. 10.1002/jsfa.8616. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pinto, J. M. , Kern D. W., Wroblewski K. E., Chen R. C., Schumm L. P., and McClintock M. K.. 2014. “Sensory Function: Insights From Wave 2 of the National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project.” Journals of Gerontology: Series B 69, no. Suppl_2: S144–S153. 10.1093/geronb/gbu102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piotrowicz, K. , Gąsowski J., Michel J.‐P., and Veronese N.. 2021. “Post‐COVID‐19 Acute Sarcopenia: Physiopathology and Management.” Aging Clinical and Experimental Research 33: 2887–2898. 10.1007/s40520-021-01942-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piotrowicz, K. , Pac A., Skalska A., et al. 2016. “Clustering of Geriatric Deficits Emerges to Be an Essential Feature of Ageing ‐ Results of a Cross‐Sectional Study in Poland.” Aging (Albany NY) 8, no. 10: 2437–2448. 10.18632/aging.101055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piqueras‐Fiszman, B. , and Spence C.. 2014. “Colour, Pleasantness, and Consumption Behaviour Within a Meal.” Appetite 75: 165–172. 10.1016/j.appet.2014.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poldrack, R. A. , and Wagner A. D.. 2004. “What Can Neuroimaging Tell Us About the Mind? Insights From Prefrontal Cortex.” Current Directions in Psychological Science 13, no. 5: 177–181. 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00302.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pouyet, V. , Cuvelier G., Benattar L., and Giboreau A.. 2015. “Influence of Flavour Enhancement on Food Liking and Consumption in Older Adults With Poor, Moderate or High Cognitive Status.” Food Quality and Preference 44: 119–129. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2015.04.014. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pouyet, V. , Giboreau A., Benattar L., and Cuvelier G.. 2014. “Attractiveness and Consumption of Finger Foods in Elderly Alzheimer's Disease Patients.” Food Quality and Preference 34: 62–69. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2013.12.011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Purves, D. , Augustine G., Fitzpatrick D., et al. 2004. Neuroscience. 3rd ed. Sinauer. [Google Scholar]
- Quandt, S. , McDonald J., Arcury T., Bell R., and Vitolins M.. 2000. “Nutritional Self‐Management of Elderly Widows in Rural Communities.” Gerontologist 40, no. 1: 86–96. 10.1093/geront/40.1.86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramic, E. , Pranjic N., Batic‐Mujanovic O., and Alibasic E.. 2011. “The Effect of Loneliness on Malnutrition in Elderly Population.” Medical Archives 65, no. 2: 92–95. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/51139459. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rathod, S. , Denee T., Eva J., et al. 2022. “Health‐Related Quality of Life Burden Associated With Treatment‐Resistant Depression in UKPatients: Quantitative Results From a Mixed‐Methods Non‐Interventional Study.” Journal of Affective Disorders 300: 551–562. 10.1016/j.jad.2021.12.090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raveendran, A. V. , Jayadevan R., and Sashidharan S.. 2021. “Long COVID: An Overview.” Diabetes and Metabolic Syndrome: Clinical Research and Reviews 15, no. 3: 869–875. 10.1016/j.dsx.2021.04.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rawson, N. E. , Gomez G., Cowart B. J., Kriete A., Pribitkin E., and Restrepo D.. 2012. “Age‐Associated Loss of Selectivity in Human Olfactory Sensory Neurons.” Neurobiology of Aging 33, no. 9: 1913–1919. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.09.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reavis, K. M. , Bisgaard N., Canlon B., et al. 2022. “Sex‐Linked Biology and Gender‐Related Research Is Essential to Advancing Hearing Health.” Ear and Hearing 44, no. 1: 10–27. 10.1097/aud.0000000000001291. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Regan, E. , Feeney E. L., Hutchings S. C., O'Neill G. J., and O'Riordan E. D.. 2021. “Exploring How Age, Medication Usage, and Dentures Effect the Sensory Perception and Liking of Oral Nutritional Supplements in Older Adults.” Food Quality and Preference 92: 104224. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2021.104224. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Regan, E. , O'Neill G. J., Hutchings S. C., and O'Riordan D.. 2019. “Exploring How Age Influences Sensory Perception, Thirst and Hunger During the Consumption of Oral Nutritional Supplements Using the Check‐All‐That‐Apply Methodology.” Food Quality and Preference 78: 103736. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2019.103736. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Reinbach, H. C. , Giacalone D., Ribeiro L. M., Bredie W. L. P., and Frøst M. B.. 2014. “Comparison of Three Sensory Profiling Methods Based on Consumer Perception: CATA, CATAWith Intensity and Napping®.” Food Quality and Preference 32: 160–166. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2013.02.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Reuter‐Loryea, K. M. , Bisgaard N., Canlon B., et al. 2020. “Sex‐Linked Biology and Gender‐Related Research Is Essential to Advancing Hearing Health.” Ear and Hearing 41, no. 6: 1415–1425. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ricciardiello, F. , Pisani D., Viola P., et al. 2021. “Sudden Sensorineural Hearing Loss in Mild Covid‐19: Case Series and Analysis of the Literature.” Audiology Research 11, no. 3: 313–326. 10.3390/audiolres11030029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riquelme, N. , Robert P., and Arancibia C.. 2021. “Understanding Older People Perceptions About Desserts Using Word Association and Sorting Task Methodologies.” Food Quality and Preference 96: 104423. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2021.104423. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Robino, A. , Carlino D., Pirastu N., De Vanna M., Tepper B. J., and Gasparini P.. 2013. The Role of Personality Traits on Taste Perception and Food Preferences. Publications UniTS. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rouby, C. , Schaal B., Dubois D., Gervais R., and Holley A.. 2002. Olfaction, Taste, and Cognition. Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Routasalo, P. E. , Savikko N., Tilvis R. S., Strandberg T. E., and Pitkälä K. H.. 2006. “Social Contacts and Their Relationship to Loneliness Among Aged People – A Population‐Based Study.” Gerontology 52, no. 3: 181–187. 10.1159/000091828. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rutherford, B. R. , Brewster K., Golub J. S., Kim A. H., and Roose S. P.. 2018. “Sensation and Psychiatry: Linking Age‐Related Hearing Loss to Late‐Life Depression and Cognitive Decline.” American Journal of Psychiatry 175, no. 3: 215–224. 10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.17040423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rutkowska, J. , Antoniewska‐Krzeska A., Żbikowska A., Cazón P., and Vázquez M.. 2022. “Volatile Composition and Sensory Profile of Lactose‐Free Kefir, and Its Acceptability by Elderly Consumers.” Molecules 27, no. 17: 5386. 10.3390/molecules27175386. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salcan, İ. , Karakeçili F., Salcan S., et al. 2021. “Is Taste and Smell Impairment Irreversible in COVID‐19 Patients?” European Archives of Oto‐Rhino‐Laryngology 278, no. 2: 411–415. 10.1007/s00405-020-06560-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sasano, T. , Satoh‐Kuriwada S., and Shoji N.. 2015. “The Important Role of Umami Taste in Oral and Overall Health.” Flavour 4, no. 1: 1–5. 10.1186/2044-7248-4-10. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sato, H. , Wada H., Matsumoto H., Takagiwa M., and Goto T. K.. 2022. “Differences in Dynamic Perception of Salty Taste Intensity Between Young and Older Adults.” Scientific Reports 12, no. 1: 7558. 10.1038/s41598-022-11442-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schiffman, S. S. 2000. “Intensification of Sensory Properties of Foods for the Elderly.” Journal of Nutrition 130, no. 4: 927S–930S. 10.1093/jn/130.4.927s. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schiffman, S. S. , Lindley M. G., Clark T. B., and Makino C.. 1981. “Molecular Mechanism of Sweet Taste: Relationship of Hydrogen Bonding to Taste Sensitivity for Both Young and Elderly.” Neurobiology of Aging 2, no. 3: 173–185. 10.1016/0197-4580(81)90018-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider, J. , Gopinath B., McMahon C., et al. 2012. “Prevalence and 5‐Year Incidence of Dual Sensory Impairment in an Older Australian Population.” Annals of Epidemiology 22, no. 4: 295–301. 10.1016/j.annepidem.2012.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schubert, C. R. , Cruickshanks K. J., Fischer M. E., et al. 2011. “Olfactory Impairment in an Adult Population: The Beaver Dam Offspring Study.” Chemical Senses 37, no. 4: 325–334. 10.1093/chemse/bjr102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schumaker, M. R. , Diako C., Castura J. C., Edwards C. G., and Ross C. F.. 2019. “Influence of Wine Composition on Consumer Perception and Acceptance of Brettanomyces Metabolites Using Temporal Check‐All‐That‐Apply Methodology.” Food Research International 116: 963–972. 10.1016/j.foodres.2018.09.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scott, N. , Grygorczyk A., Gilbert C. C., and Duizer L. M.. 2017. “Exploring the Use of Rapid Profiling Techniques for Use in Older Adult Populations.” Food Quality and Preference 62: 199–207. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2017.06.012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Seo, H.‐S. , and Hummel T.. 2009. “Effects of Olfactory Dysfunction on Sensory Evaluation and Preparation of Foods.” Appetite 53, no. 3: 314–321. 10.1016/j.appet.2009.07.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sergi, G. , Bano G., Pizzato S., Veronese N., and Manzato E.. 2016. “Taste Loss in the Elderly: Possible Implications for Dietary Habits.” Critical Reviews in Food Science and Nutrition 57, no. 17: 3684–3689. 10.1080/10408398.2016.1160208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shabbir, M. A. , Mehak F., Khan Z. M., et al. 2022. “Delving the Role of Nutritional Psychiatry to Mitigate the COVID‐19 Pandemic Induced Stress, Anxiety and Depression.” Trends in Food Science and Technology 120: 25–35. 10.1016/j.tifs.2021.12.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheline, Y. I. , Barch D. M., Price J. L., et al. 2009. “The Default Mode Network and Self‐Referential Processes in Depression.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106, no. 6: 1942–1947. 10.1073/pnas.0812686106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Solemdal, K. , Sandvik L., Willumsen T., and Mowe M.. 2012a. “Taste Ability in Hospitalised Older People Compared With Healthy, Age‐Matched Controls.” Gerodontology 31, no. 1: 42–48. 10.1111/ger.12001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Solemdal, K. , Sandvik L., Willumsen T., Mowe M., and Hummel T.. 2012b. “The Impact of Oral Health on Taste Ability in Acutely Hospitalized Elderly.” PLoS One 7, no. 5: e36557. 10.1371/journal.pone.0036557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Song, X. , Giacalone D., Bølling Johansen S. M., Frøst M. B., and Bredie W. L. P.. 2016. “Changes in Orosensory Perception Related to Aging and Strategies for Counteracting Its Influence on Food Preferences Among Older Adults.” Trends in Food Science and Technology 53: 49–59. 10.1016/j.tifs.2016.04.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Spence, C. , Wan X., Woods A., et al. 2015. “On Tasty Colours and Colourful Tastes? Assessing, Explaining, and Utilizing Crossmodal Correspondences Between Colours and Basic Tastes.” Flavour 4, no. 1: 383. 10.1186/s13411-015-0033-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Srinivasan, A. , Wong F., Couch L. S., and Wang B. X.. 2022. “Cardiac Complications of COVID‐19 in Low‐Risk Patients.” Viruses 14, no. 6: 1322. 10.3390/v14061322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Srur, E. , Stachs O., Guthoff R., Witt M., Pau H. W., and Just T.. 2010. “Change of the Human Taste Bud Volume Over Time.” Auris Nasus Larynx 37, no. 4: 449–455. 10.1016/j.anl.2009.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stamps, J. J. 2019. Chemosensory Function During Neurologically Healthy Aging, 68–94. Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sudre, C. H. , Murray B., Varsavsky T., et al. 2021. “Attributes and Predictors of Long COVID.” Nature Medicine 27, no. 4: 626–631. 10.1038/s41591-021-01292-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sulmont‐Rosse, C. , Maitre I., Amand M., et al. 2015. “Evidence for Different Patterns of Chemosensory Alterations in the Elderly Population: Impact of Age Versus Dependency.” Chemical Senses 40, no. 3: 153–164. 10.1093/chemse/bju112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swain, S. K. , and Pani S. R.. 2021. “Incidence of Hearing Loss in COVID‐19 Patients: A COVID Hospital‐Based Study in the Eastern Part of India.” International Journal of Current Research and Review 13, no. 3: 103–107. 10.31782/ijcrr.2021.13329. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Taghiabadi, M. , Kavosi A., Mirhafez S. R., Keshvari M., and Mehrabi T.. 2017. “The Association Between Death Anxiety With Spiritual Experiences and Life Satisfaction in Elderly People.” Electronic Physician 9, no. 3: 3980–3985. 10.19082/3980. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tam, M. , and Koppel K.. 2021. “Sensory Impairment: Natural Result of Aging.” Journal of Sensory Studies 36, no. 5: e12693. 10.1111/joss.12693. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Tan, S.‐Y. , Tey S., and Brown R.. 2018. “Can Nuts Mitigate Malnutrition in Older Adults? A Conceptual Framework.” Nutrients 10, no. 10: 1448. 10.3390/nu10101448. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tham, Y.‐C. , Lim S.‐H., Shi Y., et al. 2018. “Trends of Visual Impairment and Blindness in the Singapore Chinese Population Over a Decade.” Scientific Reports 8, no. 1: 12224. 10.1038/s41598-018-30004-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas, A. , Boobyer C., Borgonha Z., van den Heuvel E., and Appleton K. M.. 2021. “Adding Flavours: Use of and Attitudes Towards Sauces and Seasonings in a Sample of Community‐Dwelling UK Older Adults.” Food 10, no. 11: 2828. 10.3390/foods10112828. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thomas, A. , van der Stelt A. J., Schlich P., and Lawlor J. B.. 2018. “Temporal Drivers of Liking for Oral Nutritional Supplements for Older Adults Throughout the Day With Monitoring of Hunger and Thirst Status.” Food Quality and Preference 70: 40–48. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2017.05.001. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Toffanello, E. D. , Inelmen E. M., Minicuci N., et al. 2010. “Ten‐Year Trends in Dietary Intake, Health Status and Mortality Rates in Free‐Living Elderly People.” Journal of Nutrition, Health & Aging 14, no. 4: 259–264. 10.1007/s12603-010-0058-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tseng, Y.‐C. , Liu S. H.‐Y., Lou M.‐F., and Huang G.‐S.. 2018. “Quality of Life in Older Adults With Sensory Impairments: ASystematic Review.” Quality of Life Research 27, no. 8: 1957–1971. 10.1007/s11136-018-1799-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tsofliou, F. , Grammatikopoulou M. G., Lumley R., Gkiouras K., Lara J., and Clark C.. 2020. “Effects of Lunch Club Attendance on the Dietary Intake of Olderadults in the UK: A Pilot Cross‐Sectional Study.” Nutrition and Health 26, no. 3: 209–214. 10.1177/0260106020921047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uchida, Y. , Sugiura S., Nishita Y., Saji N., Sone M., and Ueda H.. 2019. “Age‐Related Hearing Loss and Cognitive Decline — The Potential Mechanisms Linking the Two.” Auris Nasus Larynx 46, no. 1: 1–9. 10.1016/j.anl.2018.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- United Nations . 2022. World Population Prospects: The 2022 Revision. Department of Economic and Social Affairs, Population Division. [Google Scholar]
- van den Heuvel, E. , Murphy J. L., and Appleton K. M.. 2018. “Could Eggs Help Increase Dietary Protein Intake in Older Adults? ‐ Exploring Reasons for the Consumption and Non‐Consumption of Eggs in People Over 55 Years Old.” Journal of Nutrition in Gerontology and Geriatrics 37, no. 3–4: 292–309. 10.1080/21551197.2018.1505577. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Rijssen, L. B. , Derks W., Hoffmans R., et al. 2021. “No COVID‐19 in Patients With Sudden Sensorineural Hearing Loss (SSNHL).” Otology & Neurotology 43, no. 2: 170–173. 10.1097/mao.0000000000003438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Visser, M. , Schaap L. A., and Wijnhoven H. A. H.. 2020. “Self‐Reported Impact of the COVID‐19 Pandemic on Nutrition and Physical Activity Behaviour in Dutch Older Adults Living Independently.” Nutrients 12, no. 12: 3708. 10.3390/nu12123708. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Völter, C. , Thomas J. P., Maetzler W., Guthoff R., Grunwald M., and Hummel T.. 2021. “Sensory Dysfunction in Old Age.” Deutsches Aerzteblatt Online 118: 29–30. 10.3238/arztebl.m2021.0212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang, J. , Xiao L. D., Wang K., Luo Y., and Li X.. 2020. “Gender Differences in Cognitive Impairment Among Rural Elderly in China.” International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17, no. 10: 3724. 10.3390/ijerph17103724. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang, Q. J. , Mielby L. A., Junge J. Y., et al. 2019. “The Role of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Sensory Factors in Sweetness Perception of Food and Beverages: A Review.” Food 8, no. 6: 211. 10.3390/foods8060211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang, Y. , Zhong K., Shi B., et al. 2022. “Cross‐Modal Effect of Capsaicin and Pepper Oleoresin on the Enhancement of Saltiness Perception in a NaCl Model Solution.” Food Quality and Preference 98: 104542. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2022.104542. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wansink, B. , Painter J. E., and North J.. 2005. “Bottomless Bowls: Why Visual Cues of Portion Size May Influence Intake.” Obesity Research 13, no. 1: 93–100. 10.1038/oby.2005.12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wansink, B. , and van Ittersum K.. 2003. “Bottoms Up! The Influence of Elongation on Pouring and Consumption Volume.” Journal of Consumer Research 30, no. 3: 455–463. 10.1086/378621. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Watson, C. , Kirkcaldie M., and Paxinos G.. 2010. “Gathering Information–the Sensory Systems.” Brain 20: 75–96. 10.1016/b978-0-12-373889-9.50006-1. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wayler, A. H. , Muench M. E., Kapur K. K., and Chauncey H. H.. 1984. “Masticatory Performance and Food Acceptability in Persons With Removable Partial Dentures, Full Dentures and Intact Natural Dentition.” Journal of Gerontology 39, no. 3: 284–289. 10.1093/geronj/39.3.284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wendin, K. , Biörklund‐Helgesson M., Andersson‐Stefanovic K., Lareke A., Böök O., and Skjöldebrand C.. 2021. “Liking, Preference and Practical Implications of Protein and Energy Enriched in‐Between‐Meals Designed for Elderly People.” Food & Nutrition Research 65: 5635. 10.29219/fnr.v65.5635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whillans, J. , Nazroo J., and Matthews K.. 2016. “Trajectories of Vision in Older People: The Role of Age and Social Position.” European Journal of Ageing 13, no. 2: 171–184. 10.1007/s10433-015-0360-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitelock, E. , and Ensaff H.. 2018. “On Your Own: Older Adults' Food Choice and Dietary Habits.” Nutrients 10, no. 4: 413. 10.3390/nu10040413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wierenga, M. , Crawford C. R., and Running C. A.. 2020. “Older US Adults Like Sweetened Colas, but Not Other Chemesthetic Beverages.” Journal of Texture Studies 51, no. 5: 722–732. 10.1111/jtxs.12549. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wijk, H. , Berg S., Sivik L., and Steen B.. 1999. “Color Discrimination, Color Naming and Color Preferences in 80‐Year Olds.” Aging Clinical and Experimental Research 11, no. 3: 176–185. 10.1007/bf03399660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson, D. A. , and East B. S.. 2020. Function of the Olfactory Cortex, 661–674. Elsevier EBooks. [Google Scholar]
- Withers, C. , Gosney M. A., and Methven L.. 2013. “Perception of Thickness, Mouth Coating and Mouth Drying of Dairy Beverages by Younger and Older Volunteers.” Journal of Sensory Studies 28, no. 3: 230–237. 10.1111/joss.12039. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization . 2017. Mental Health of Older Adults. World Health Organization. [Google Scholar]
- Xing, X. , Chitrakar B., Hati S., et al. 2022. “Development of Black Fungus‐Based 3D Printed Foods as Dysphagia Diet: Effect of Gums Incorporation.” Food Hydrocolloids 123: 107173. 10.1016/j.foodhyd.2021.107173. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Yang, C.‐H. , Schrepfer T., and Schacht J.. 2015. “Age‐Related Hearing Impairment and the Triad of Acquired Hearing Loss.” Frontiers in Cellular Neuroscience 9: 2769. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yang, T. , Zhang L., Xu G., et al. 2022. “Investigating Taste Sensitivity, Chemesthetic Sensation and Their Relationship With Emotion Perception in Chinese Young and Older Adults.” Food Quality and Preference 96: 104406. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2021.104406. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Yoshinaka, M. , Ikebe K., Uota M., et al. 2015. “Age and Sex Differences in the Taste Sensitivity of Young Adult, Young‐Old and Old‐Old Japanese.” Geriatrics & Gerontology International 16, no. 12: 1281–1288. 10.1111/ggi.12638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zampini, M. , and Spence C.. 2004. “The Role of Auditory Cues in Modulating the Perceived Crispness and Staleness of Potato Chips.” Journal of Sensory Studies 19, no. 5: 347–363. 10.1111/j.1745-459x.2004.080403.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Zellner, D. A. , Loss C. R., Zearfoss J., and Remolina S.. 2014. “It Tastes as Good as It Looks! The Effect of Food Presentation on Liking for the Flavor of Food☆.” Appetite 77: 31–35. 10.1016/j.appet.2014.02.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhong, B.‐L. , Xu Y.‐M., Xie W.‐X., Liu X.‐J., and Huang Z.‐W.. 2019. “Depressive Symptoms in Elderly Chinese Primary Care Patients: Prevalence and Sociodemographic and Clinical Correlates.” Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology 32, no. 6: 312–318. 10.1177/0891988719862620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou, X. , Hartvig D. L., Perez‐Cueto F. J. A., and Bredie W. L. P.. 2021. “Provision of Visually Appetising and High‐Energy Maize Soup as an in‐Between Meal for Older Consumers.” Food Quality and Preference 88: 104069. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2020.104069. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Data will be made available upon request.