Abstract
A wealth of information can be garnered from the face, such as current emotional state, perceived overall health, personality traits, and even attributed capabilities. It is well understood within aesthetic medicine that minimally invasive treatments positively influence how a patient feels about themselves from an emotional and physical standpoint; however, a growing body of literature suggests that facial aesthetic treatments can also impact how an individual is perceived by others. First impressions, which are formed within milliseconds of viewing a new face, allow us to intuit information about the person and anticipate their intentions. Multiple theories aim to explain why and how these initial social impressions are formed from relatively simple visual cues, and increasing evidence suggests that augmentation of facial appearance influences how others form first impressions. In this article, we discuss theories underlying the importance of first impressions and how minimally invasive aesthetic treatments can affect first impressions and social attributions. We also discuss how advancements in technology can enhance consultations and how technology may expand the world of aesthetic medicine to patients who had not previously considered engaging with aesthetic treatments.
Level of Evidence: 5 (Diagnostic)
The face can convey a wealth of information about an individual, from simple categorical assessments to perceived overall health, personality traits, and attributed skills. For fast and economic assessment, humans have developed an elaborate sensory system that rates facial characteristics as a proxy for broader impressions of traits and behaviors. Multiple studies have demonstrated that facial aesthetic treatments positively influence the physical and emotional well-being of patients.1-4 In controlled studies, the effectiveness of aesthetic treatments is often determined through clinical assessments of the degree of anatomical change (eg, physical change from baseline in severity of upper facial lines) and patient-reported outcomes, including quality of life data. Similarly, in reconstructive patients (eg, those who experienced a traumatic injury necessitating facial reconstruction), adjunctive treatment with minimally invasive injectables can improve the patient's satisfaction with their appearance, improve their quality of life, and reduce psychosocial distress.5 Often, the emphasis is placed on how an aesthetic treatment makes the individual feel about themselves.
But aesthetic medicine can offer advantages that go beyond physical improvement, and minimally invasive aesthetic treatments can have a positive social impact on patients.1,3,6-8 Indeed, one's outward appearance factors heavily into how they are perceived by others and how they might interact with others. When viewing the face of another individual, people form a first impression and almost instantaneously ascribe social attributions (ie, personality traits, potential behavioral outcomes) based on the appearance of that individual's face, whether their face is expressing an emotion or at rest. By incorporating a discussion of socially based treatment outcomes into the consultation, aesthetic practitioners can better help patients attain their desired outcome. Furthermore, the demonstrated ability of injectables to positively impact third-party impressions may lead individuals who have never previously considered treatment to be open to aesthetic intervention.
In this article, we discuss the psychology of first impressions, evidence for improved first impressions following aesthetic treatment, and how technology can be incorporated into consultations to educate prospective patients on socially based treatment outcomes.
PSYCHOLOGY OF FIRST IMPRESSIONS
First impressions are formed rapidly; 1 study reported that only <100 ms were required for personality traits such as extraversion to be inferred from an initial impression.9 Evaluation of others based on their facial appearance is inevitable, and the brain makes split-second judgments on personality traits when seeing a new face. Multiple theories exist to attempt explaining why and how initial social impressions are generated, including the overgeneralization theory and valence-dominance model.
The overgeneralization theory states that the evaluation of facial appearance by our emotional detection system is often overgeneralized to cues that guide future behavior. For example, cues about the emotional state of a person (eg, a smile) are overgeneralized, so people whose natural facial appearance resembles a smile leads to these people being judged as having a happier or friendlier personality. Also, encountering a person while smiling can be overgeneralized to assuming this person is always smiling. Similar overgeneralizations can be made when someone has a facial appearance that resembles a particular emotion (the emotion face overgeneralization), babies (the baby face overgeneralization), or a particular known identity (the familiar face overgeneralization).10 These singular events and social judgments are then viewed as an invariable rule of personality that dictates future interactions,10-16 although the effect sizes of these overgeneralizations can be small.9
An alternative theory, Oosterhof and Todorov's valence-dominance model, posits that social judgments can be mapped to 2 underlying dimensions: valence (ie, trustworthiness) and dominance.17 From an evolutionary standpoint, it is important to anticipate others’ intentions toward us (ie, trustworthiness) and assess the others’ capability to pursue their intentions (ie, dominance) to increase survival and reproduction rate. Importantly, this model has been shown to generalize across multiple cultures.18-20
In practice, facial judgment influences a variety of perceptions and outcomes. For example, physical appearance (eg, rounder vs more square face) contributes to perceptions of attractiveness, age, and gender.10,21-23 There are also universal inferences (ie, evolutionary mechanisms of perceived reproductive fitness), cultural specificity (face typicality, or how similar 1 face is to an average face in a cultural or ethnic group), and idiosyncratic factors (eg, resemblance to self or significant others) that affect how initial first impressions may be formed and then acted upon.23,24 First impressions also influence behavior. Without conscious awareness, interactions with others are influenced by first impressions and, in line with the self-fulfilling prophecy, people might believe in the trait judgments formed from first impressions and align their behavior accordingly. Thus, it can be argued that our facial appearance can strongly influence the outcomes of social events, such as judgments of attractiveness influencing romantic relationships, judgments of competence influencing professional and financial success, and several other judgments based on facial attributes influencing predictions of social outcomes (eg, voting, investment, sentencing decisions, punishment severity).24-31
THE INFLUENCE OF AESTHETIC MEDICINE ON SOCIAL PERCEPTIONS
A growing body of literature demonstrates that changing an individual's facial appearance (ie, with aesthetic treatments) can influence first impressions and social perceptions. An early study had nonphysicians (patients and general population) grade randomized preoperative and postoperative photographs of 14 patients who had received various facial surgeries (eg, rhinoplasty, blepharoplasty, chin implant) on multiple categories of first impressions. All categories were rated as higher for postoperative photographs, with the highest increases seen for attractiveness, attributed social skills, and attributed dating success.1 Similarly, multimodal treatment with neurotoxin, soft-tissue fillers, and eyelash growth treatment was associated with patients being rated as more attractive, successful, friendly, approachable, younger, and more socially adept.3 A recent cross-sectional online survey study investigated whether minimally invasive procedures in female patients affect 3 domains of facial information according to independent observers who do not have a background in aesthetic medicine: attractiveness (young, attractive, and healthy), competence (knowledgeable, competent, intelligent, confident, masculine, and dominant), and trustworthiness (friendly, approachable, aggressive, credible, honest, and trustworthy). There was significantly higher agreement that patients appeared competent, trustworthy, and attractive after treatment than before treatment. An exploratory domain of naturalness was not changed by treatment. All but 1 subdomain was significantly changed posttreatment; perceptions of dominance did not significantly change posttreatment, which authors proposed as being due to increased femininity (ie, significantly decreased “masculinity”).6 Although all patients in the study were female with low Fitzpatrick skin phototypes, and therefore it is not clear whether these findings are applicable to men, or women with higher Fitzpatrick skin phototypes, the results suggest that overall impressions are formed holistically, not necessarily from a single facial area, and that facial features are interconnected. Other studies have shown that treatment with hyaluronic acid injectables is associated with better overall first impressions, attractiveness, youthfulness, confidence, and happiness, as well as attributed social skills, athletic skills, and dating success.7,8 The importance of the quality of the treatment result also appears to influence first impressions, with higher ratings of perceived attractiveness for patients whose treatment results appeared more “natural-looking.”7 A limitation of these studies is that they often rely on static images, which may not capture relevant dynamic facial movement and may not directly translate to first impressions in a real social context.
Facial differences, either congenital or acquired, also impact how an individual is perceived by others and correction may positively influence first impressions. Asymmetries produced by facial paralysis are associated with more negative perceptions of affect and lower ratings of attractiveness,32,33 which eye-tracking studies suggest may be because of attentional bias toward the asymmetry of the paralyzed face (ie, the mouth at rest or smiling).34 Patients with dentofacial deformities are perceived as possessing lower degrees of positive personality traits, such as confidence and intelligence, and higher degrees of negative traits such as aggressiveness and brutality.35 Aesthetic medicine may help these individuals augment their appearance in a way that can potentially reduce stigmatization and negative interactions with others. Indeed, multiple studies have demonstrated that surgical treatment of facial differences is associated with more positive personality and trait assessments by others.36-39
Although most published literature describes how social perceptions are influenced by the correction of facial differences using surgical intervention, minimally invasive treatments may also be an effective tool. Indeed, a growing number of articles report and highlight improved levels of satisfaction in relation to facial appearance and quality of life compared with levels of satisfaction with their facial differences before adjunctive facial aesthetic treatments.40-46 This is a powerful finding because many patients with acquired facial differences have deep levels of facial dissatisfaction, anxiety, and depression. Partial or total resolution of their facial-related anxieties may not only be related to an improvement in facial appearance but the “corrective” results from injectables may erode the strong physical cues that stimulate the daily traumatic recollections and posttraumatic stress. However, published data on the effects of minimally invasive treatments in patients with facial differences are limited, and it is not currently known whether minimally invasive treatments can stand alone in meaningfully improving first impressions or social interactions, or whether these treatments need to be adjunctive to surgical methods.47 Additional research is warranted to better understand the role and relative effectiveness of minimally invasive vs surgical treatments in influencing first impressions in this population.
THE GROWING ROLE OF MOBILE TECHNOLOGY AND ARTIFICIAL INTELLIGENCE IN AESTHETIC MEDICINE
As technology continues to advance, practitioners will have tools at their disposal to analyze how patients’ facial appearance may influence social perceptions as well as how specific aesthetic treatments might augment these social perceptions. Articles describing recent preliminary studies have reported on the use of artificial intelligence (AI), augmented reality, and/or machine learning for surgical planning of reconstructive patients,48 to predict facial morphology following orthognathic surgery,49 and to classify severity of facial wrinkles.50 AI-based applications have been used to show improvements in perceived attractiveness and age following aesthetic surgery procedures.51-53 This technology has also been used to show that bilateral brow lift surgery or facial rejuvenation surgery (facelift with or without brow lift, blepharoplasty, or fat grafting) decreases ratings of perceived sadness or anger with concurrent increases in detection of happiness.54,55
Published data on technology for assessing social outcomes of minimally invasive aesthetic treatments is lacking, and although the use of this technology is currently limited, it is growing in use. As we look to the future, the use of smartphone-based imaging coupled with AI creates a journey where a patient can capture a clinical-grade selfie using their smartphone, have an initial AI-powered assessment of facial features, select a treatment plan comprising minimally invasive treatments, visualize the before and after images of potential treatments, and assess outcomes across a wide array of clinical outcome assessment dimensions (eg, social perception). Providers are just beginning to leverage the power and potential of clinically validated technology in aesthetic medicine, and technological developments in the future will enable richer engagements between patients and technology and providers and technology, thus enriching the overall provider and patient experience.
FROM THEORY TO CLINICAL PRACTICE
Patients may engage with aesthetic medicine to receive a specific treatment, to address a concern with an anatomical facial feature, or to address a concern with a social feature (eg, looking sad).56 Although healthcare professionals are trained to provide anatomical or technical solutions to aesthetic requests, recognizing and even addressing the social benefits that these solutions provide has only recently come to our attention. By becoming aware of the social aspects of what we radiate and the potential impact of our treatments on our patients’ social lives, aesthetic providers can better understand and cater to the needs and expectations of our patients. This also accounts for patients seeking reconstructive treatments (eg, lightening the appearance of scars, correcting injury-induced volume and/or shape deficits), where the social aspects of the face are traditionally considered secondary to physical optimization.
The social relevance of the above makes it important to discuss perceptions as part of a consultation. Awareness among aesthetic providers can be accomplished by sharing information through published articles, photographs, etc, about the importance of first impressions. Additionally, it is important that there is alignment between the outcomes desired by the patient and the goals of the provider (eg, natural-looking outcomes, improving how the patient feels about themself, noticeable differences in appearance). Recent technological developments (eg, treatment simulation) can help both the aesthetic provider as well as the patient in better decision-making regarding the applied treatments, resulting in higher patient satisfaction levels. AI-based solutions could also allow patients to “see” how minimally invasive aesthetic treatments may affect different social perceptions (eg, aesthetics, social, professional), or these AI-based solutions may allow physicians to tailor treatments to the desired outcome.
CONCLUSIONS
Aesthetic medicine can have far-reaching benefits for patients that extend beyond beautification and rejuvenation, as aesthetic treatment can significantly influence third-party impressions and impact how treated individuals interact with others, thus leading to a socially transformative outcome. Furthermore, raised awareness of the benefits of minimally invasive aesthetic treatments beyond the physical improvement may open up these treatments to those who may not have previously considered them.
Acknowledgments
Medical writing and editorial assistance were provided by Sarah J. Cross, PhD, of AbbVie Inc, and funded by AbbVie Inc (North Chicago, IL).
Disclosures
Dr Dayan is an investigator, speaker, or consultant for Merz (Raleigh, NC), GDN, Croma Pharma UK (Winnersh, United Kingdom), Teoxane (Geneva, Switzerland), Revance (Nashville, TN), Evolus (Newport Beach, CA), and Allergan Aesthetics, an AbbVie company (Irvine, CA). Dr Ogilvie is a consultant, speaker, and investigator for Allergan Aesthetics, an AbbVie company, and an investigator, board member, and consultant for Evolus, Merz, Galderma (Lausanne, Switzerland), Vivacy (Paris, Ile-de-France, France), Aptos (Alpharetta, GA), Dior (Paris, France), Procter & Gamble (Cincinnati, OH), Revance, and Amway (Ada, MI). Dr Bucker is a consultant and speaker for Allergan Aesthetics, an AbbVie company; and CSO and co-founder with a financial interest in RealFaceValue B.V. (Amsterdam, the Netherlands). Dr van der Meulen is a consultant and speaker for Allergan Aesthetics, an AbbVie company; and co-founder with a financial interest in RealFaceValue B.V., the Netherlands. Dr Blythe is a consultant and speaker for Allergan Aesthetics, an AbbVie company. Mr Caiazza and Drs Silberberg and Musumeci are employees of AbbVie Inc. and may hold company stock.
Funding
Allergan Aesthetics, an AbbVie company (Irvine, CA), provided funding support for this publication and participated in the design, interpretation, review, and approval of the publication.
REFERENCES
- 1. Dayan S, Clark K, Ho AA. Altering first impressions after facial plastic surgery. Aesthetic Plast Surg. 2004;28:301–306. doi: 10.1007/s00266-004-1017-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Dayan SH, Arkins JP, Patel AB, Gal TJ. A double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled health-outcomes survey of the effect of botulinum toxin type a injections on quality of life and self-esteem. Dermatol Surg. 2010;36:2088–2097. doi: 10.1111/j.1524-4725.2010.01795.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Dayan S, Rivkin A, Sykes JM, et al. Aesthetic treatment positively impacts social perception: analysis of subjects from the HARMONY study. Aesthet Surg J. 2019;39:1380–1389. doi: 10.1093/asj/sjy239 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Demesh D, Cristel RT, Gandhi ND, Kola E, Issa TZ, Dayan SH. Effects of hyaluronic acid filler injection for non-surgical rhinoplasty on first impressions and quality of life (FACE-Q scale). J Cosmet Dermatol. 2022;21:3351–3357. doi: 10.1111/jocd.14980 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Bertossi D, Giampaoli G, Lucchese A, et al. The skin rejuvenation associated treatment-Fraxel laser, Microbotox, and low G prime hyaluronic acid: preliminary results. Lasers Med Sci. 2019;34:1449–1455. doi: 10.1007/s10103-019-02738-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. van den Elzen HE, Barends AJ, van Vugt M, et al. Facial aesthetic minimally invasive procedure: more than just vanity, a social-psychological approach. J Cosmet Dermatol. 2023;22:2063–2070. doi: 10.1111/jocd.15678 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Dayan S, Fabi S, Nogueira A. Lay rater evaluation of naturalness and first impression following treatment of lower face wrinkles with hyaluronic acid fillers. J Cosmet Dermatol. 2021;20:1091–1097. doi: 10.1111/jocd.13927 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Othman S, Cohn JE, Daggumati S, Bloom JD. The impact of dermal fillers on perceived personality traits and attractiveness. Aesthetic Plast Surg. 2021;45:273–280. doi: 10.1007/s00266-020-01888-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Willis J, Todorov A. First impressions: making up your mind after a 100-ms exposure to a face. Psychol Sci. 2006;17:592–598. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01750.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Zebrowitz LA, Montepare JM. Social psychological face perception: why appearance matters. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2008;2:1497. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00109.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Said CP, Sebe N, Todorov A. Structural resemblance to emotional expressions predicts evaluation of emotionally neutral faces. Emotion. 2009;9:260–264. doi: 10.1037/a0014681 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Engell AD, Haxby JV, Todorov A. Implicit trustworthiness decisions: automatic coding of face properties in the human amygdala. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19:1508–1519. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.9.1508 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Winston JS, Strange BA, O'Doherty J, Dolan RJ. Automatic and intentional brain responses during evaluation of trustworthiness of faces. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5:277–283. doi: 10.1038/nn816 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Todorov A, Said CP, Engell AD, Oosterhof NN. Understanding evaluation of faces on social dimensions. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008;12:455–460. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.10.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Todorov A, Pakrashi M, Oosterhof NN. Evaluating faces on trustworthiness after minimal time exposure. Soc Cogn. 2009;27:813–833. doi: 10.1521/soco.2009.27.6.813 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Zebrowitz LA. Ecological and social approaches to face perception. In: Calder AJ, Rhodes G, Johnson MH, Haxby JV, eds. Oxford Handbook of Face Perception. Oxford University Press; 2011:Chap. 3. [Google Scholar]
- 17. Oosterhof NN, Todorov A. The functional basis of face evaluation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105:11087–11092. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0805664105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Jones BC, DeBruine LM, Flake JK, et al. To which world regions does the valence-dominance model of social perception apply? Nat Hum Behav. 2021;5:159–169. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-01007-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Sutherland CAM, Liu X, Zhang L, Chu Y, Oldmeadow JA, Young AW. Facial first impressions across culture: data-driven modeling of Chinese and British perceivers’ unconstrained facial impressions. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2018;44:521–537. doi: 10.1177/0146167217744194 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Wang H, Han C, Hahn AC, et al. A data-driven study of Chinese participants’ social judgments of Chinese faces. PLoS One. 2019;14:e0210315. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0210315 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Goodman GJ. The oval female facial shape—a study in beauty. Dermatol Surg. 2015;41:1375–1383. doi: 10.1097/dss.0000000000000571 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Swift A, Remington BK. The mathematics of facial beauty. In: Jones DH, Swift A, ed. Injectable Fillers: Facial Shaping and Contouring. John Wiley & Sons Ltd.; 2019:29–61. [Google Scholar]
- 23. Dion K, Berscheid E, Walster E. What is beautiful is good. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1972;24:285–290. doi: 10.1037/h0033731 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Zebrowitz LA. First impressions from faces. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2017;26:237–242. doi: 10.1177/0963721416683996 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Blair IV, Judd CM, Fallman JL. The automaticity of race and Afrocentric facial features in social judgments. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2004;87:763–778. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.6.763 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Eberhardt JL, Davies PG, Purdie-Vaughns VJ, Johnson SL. Looking deathworthy: perceived stereotypicality of black defendants predicts capital-sentencing outcomes. Psychol Sci. 2006;17:383–386. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01716.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Little AC, Burriss RP, Jones BC, Roberts SC. Facial appearance affects voting decisions. Evol Hum Beh. 2007;28:18–27. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.09.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Zebrowitz LA, McDonald SM. The impact of litigants’ baby-facedness and attractiveness on adjudications in small claims courts. Law Hum Behav. 1991;15:603–623. doi: 10.1007/bf01065855 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Zebrowitz LA, Andreoletti C, Collins MA, Lee SY, Blumenthal J. Bright, bad, babyfaced boys: appearance stereotypes do not always yield self-fulfilling prophecy effects. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1998;75:1300–1320. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.75.5.1300 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Langlois JH, Kalakanis L, Rubenstein AJ, Larson A, Hallam M, Smoot M. Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychol Bull. 2000;126:390–423. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.3.390 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Todorov A, Olivola CY, Dotsch R, Mende-Siedlecki P. Social attributions from faces: determinants, consequences, accuracy, and functional significance. Annu Rev Psychol. 2015;66:519–545. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143831 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Wong EM, Ormiston ME, Haselhuhn MP. A face only an investor could love: CEOs’ facial structure predicts their firms’ financial performance. Psychol Sci. 2011;22:1478–1483. doi: 10.1177/0956797611418838 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Hong Y, Chua K-W, Freeman JB. Reducing facial stereotype bias in consequential social judgments: intervention success with white male faces. Psychol Sci. 2023;35:21–33. doi: 10.1177/09567976231215238 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Ishii LE, Godoy A, Encarnacion CO, Byrne PJ, Boahene KDO, Ishii M. What faces reveal: impaired affect display in facial paralysis. Laryngoscope. 2011;121:1138–1143. doi: 10.1002/lary.21764 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Ishii L, Godoy A, Encarnacion CO, Byrne PJ, Boahene KDO, Ishii M. Not just another face in the crowd: Society's perceptions of facial paralysis. Laryngoscope. 2012;122:533–538. doi: 10.1002/lary.22481 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Denadai R, Chou P-Y, Su Y-Y, et al. Facial appearance and psychosocial feature sin orthognathic surgery: a FACE-Q- and 3D facial image-based comparative study of patient-, clinician-, and lay-observer-reported outcomes. J Clin Med. 2019;8(6):909. doi: 10.3390/jcm8060909 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Posnick JC, Kinard BE. Orthognathic surgery has a significant positive effect on perceived personality traits and perceived emotional facial expressions in subjects with primary maxillary deficiency. Plast Reconstr Surg Glob Open. 2019;7:e2198. doi: 10.1097/GOX.0000000000002198 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Posnick JC, Susarla SM, Kinard BE. The effect of cleft orthognathic and nasal reconstruction on perceived social traits. Plast Reconstr Surg Glob Open. 2019;7(10):e2422. doi: 10.1097/GOX.0000000000002422 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Dey JK, Ishii M, Boahene KDO, Byrne PJ, Ishii LE. Changing perception: facial reanimation surgery improves attractiveness and decreases negative facial perception. Laryngoscope. 2014;124:84–90. doi: 10.1002/lary.24262 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Alam M, Vitarella D, Ahmad W, Abushakra S, Mao C, Brin MF. Botulinum toxin type E associated with reduced itch and pain during wound healing and acute scar formation following excision and linear repair on the forehead: a randomized controlled trial. J Am Acad Dermat. 2023;89:1317–1319. doi: 10.1016/j.jaad.2023.08.072 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Lin MJ, Bernstein DM, Torbeck RL, Dubin DP, Rosenberg JD, Khorasani H. Botulinum toxin improves forehead scars after Mohs surgery: a randomized, double-blinded, controlled study. J Am Acad Dermatol. 2022;86:964–966. doi: 10.1016/j.jaad.2021.03.110 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Bertossi D, Dell'Acqua I, Albanese M, Marchetti P, Nocini P. Face treatment using nonsurgical mini-invasive techniques as postsurgical procedure for traumatic injury. Aesthet Surg J 2019;39:Np266–Np278. doi: 10.1093/asj/sjz017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. D'Emilio R, Salerno T, Rosati G. Full face and neck treatment with onabotulinumtoxinA in patients with facial palsy. J Drugs Dermatol. 2021;20:560–564. doi: 10.36849/jdd.5417 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Manzoni APD, Venier NAB. Hyaluronic acid filler correction of scars and asymmetries secondary to cleft lip repair. Dermatol Surg. 2022;48:1264–1266. doi: 10.1097/DSS.0000000000003615 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Rammal A, Mogharbel A. Effectiveness of botulinum toxin-A on face, head, and neck scars: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Facial Plast Surg Aesthet Med. 2023;26:379–385. doi: 10.1089/fpsam.2023.0170 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Rauso R, Lo Giudice G, Zerbinati N, Tartaro G. Hyaluronic acid injections as nonsurgical alternative in case of delayed diagnosis of malar arch fracture: case report and literature review. Case Rep Surg. 2019;2019:1360741. doi: 10.1155/2019/1360741 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Jaeger B, Bucker B, van der Meulen J, van Vugt M. Face value: The effect of facial aesthetic treatment on first impressions and partner preferences. PsyArXiv, doi: 10.31234/osf.io/9k4hg, August 24, 2024, preprint: not peer reviewed. [DOI]
- 48. Boyaci O, Serpedin E, Stotland MA. Personalized quantification of facial normality: a machine learning approach. Sci Rep. 2020;10:21375. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-78180-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Tanikawa C, Yamashiro T. Development of novel artificial intelligence systems to predict facial morphology after orthognathic surgery and orthodontic treatment in Japanese patients. Sci Rep. 2021;11:15853. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-95002-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Carlos da Silva G, Barbosa MB, Júnior FBC, Moreira PL, Werka R, Martin AA. Detection of skin wrinkles and quantification of roughness using a novel image processing technique from a dermatoscope device. Skin Res Technol. 2023;29:e13335. doi: 10.1111/srt.13335 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Khetpal S, Peck C, Parsaei Y, et al. Perceived age and attractiveness using facial recognition software in rhinoplasty patients: a proof-of-concept study. J Craniofac Surg. 2022;33:1540–1544. doi: 10.1097/scs.0000000000008625 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Zhang BH, Chen K, Lu SM, et al. Turning back the clock: artificial intelligence recognition of age reduction after face-life surgery correlates with patient satisfaction. Plast Reconstr Surg. 2021;148:45–54. doi: 10.1097/prs.0000000000008020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Gibstein AR, Chen K, Nakfoor B, et al. Facelift surgery turns back the clock: artificial intelligence and patient satisfaction quantitate value of procedure type and specific techniques. Aesthet Surg J. 2021;41:987–999. doi: 10.1093/asj/sjaa238 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Boonipat T, Lin J, Bite U. Detection of baseline emotion in brow lift patients using artificial intelligence. Aesthetic Plast Surg. 2021;45:2742–2748. doi: 10.1007/s00266-021-02430-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Boonipat T, Hebel N, Zhu A, Lin J, Shapiro D. Using artificial intelligence to analyze emotion and facial action units following facial rejuvenation surgery. J Plast Reconstr Aesthet Surg. 2022;75:3628–3651. doi: 10.1016/j.bjps.2022.08.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Liew S, Silberberg M, Chantrey J. Understanding and treating different patient archetypes in aesthetic medicine. J Cosmet Dermatol. 2020;19:296–302. doi: 10.1111/jocd.13227 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]