Skip to main content
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America logoLink to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
. 2000 Oct 17;97(22):12149–12152. doi: 10.1073/pnas.200196097

Social enhancement of fitness in yellow-bellied marmots

Kenneth B Armitage *,, Orlando A Schwartz
PMCID: PMC17309  PMID: 11035771

Abstract

The yellow-bellied marmot (Marmota flaviventris) is a social, ground-dwelling squirrel that lives either individually or in kin groups of from two to five adult females. Philopatry and daughter recruitment lead to the formation and persistence of matrilines at habitat sites. By using 37 years of demographic data for 12 habitat sites, we could determine long-term trends in the effects of group size on two measures of fitness, survivorship and net reproductive rate, which otherwise are obscured by annual fluctuations in these measures. Both size and number of matrilines varied among sites and survivorship and net reproductive rate varied among sites and among matriline sizes. The role of social organization was explored further by examining the effect of matriline size, averaged over all years and sites, on fitness. For both survivorship and net reproductive rate the relationship with matriline size was curvilinear. Fitness increased with the increase in matriline size and then decreased in the largest groups. Decreased fitness in matrilines of four or five was associated with agonistic behavior, a large number of 2-year-old females in the social group, and reproductive suppression. There is no evidence that females acted to increase their fitness by increasing indirect fitness; i.e., by assisting relatives, but attempted to increase direct fitness. Direct fitness increased when mortality and fission of large matrilines reduced group size and the surviving females increased reproduction.


Because sociality in mammals is potentially costly due to intraspecific competition, parasite transmission, or suppressed reproduction (1), sociality must enhance inclusive fitness directly through the production of offspring and possibly indirectly through the production of nondescendant relatives when compared with nonsocial conspecifics. However, in many societies, sociality is associated with reproductive skew; a few individuals do all or most of the breeding (2). Reproductive skew may be associated with a trade-off between reproduction and survival. For example, highly social species of marmots, such as Marmota olympus and M. vancouverensis, which do not reproduce before age 3, have higher survival at ages 1–4 than the less social M. flaviventris, which reproduces at age 2 (35). Increased group size is widely associated with reduced per capita reproductive success; e.g., increased social complexity among species of ground-dwelling sciurids is associated with decreased litter size and a smaller proportion of females breeding (6). Among cooperatively breeding species reproduction may be limited to a single female in a group (3, 7), and per capita reproductive success is lower in large coteries than in small coteries of black-tailed prairie dogs (8). In contrast, by using 37 years of field observations, we report that net reproductive rate (Ro), a direct measure of fitness (9), initially increases with increased group size in the yellow-bellied marmot (Marmota flaviventris), then decreases in the largest groups. Furthermore, our data indicate that females in large groups act to increase their direct fitness rather than increasing indirect fitness by assisting relatives.

Methods

Marmot Biology.

Yellow-bellied marmots typically inhabit montane meadows in the mountainous regions of western North America (10). Annual mortality of young and yearlings is approximately 50% and adult mortality by age class ranges from 20 to 40% (11). About 25% of yearling females live to age 4 and 10% of 4-year-old females live to age 9 (11). Mortality results from unsuccessful hibernation and predation (11, 12). Social factors affecting fitness include competition between matrilines and cooperative and competitive behavior within matrilines. Between matriline competition may be expressed by infanticide (13), agonistic behavior (14), and reproductive suppression (4, 15); competitive behavior within matrilines includes agonistic behavior directed toward close kin (15, 16) and reproductive suppression of daughters by their mothers (4). Cooperative behavior is expressed as amicable social interactions; i.e., allogrooming and greeting (16), and by defense of home range against conspecific intruders (4).

Yellow-bellied marmots form social groups on habitat patches that range in size from 0.15 to 7.2 ha (17). Social groups occur when yearling daughters are recruited into the natal group to form mother:daughter:sister matrilines that may persist for several generations (4, 18). About 50% of female yearlings are philopatric, whereas nearly all males disperse from their breeding units and dominant males establish and defend a territory that includes one or more matrilines on the same or different resource patches (4, 19, 20).

Calculations of Survivorship and Ro.

Nearly all yellow-bellied marmots at study sites in the East River Valley, Gunnison County, CO, were live-trapped, permanently marked, and observed yearly from 1962 to 1998. Not all sites were trapped every year, as some sites were added during the study period and some sites were unoccupied by marmots for one or more years. For the 12 sites where matrilines were observed, we determined annual survivorship (lx) and maternity (mx) for marmots living individually and in matriline by group size (here all called matriline size), and used the product to calculate net reproductive rate (Ro) for each matriline size at each site for each year. In a second analysis, we calculated Ro across all sites for each year for all matrilines of the same size combined. These calculations produced Ro values for 36 years, as we could not know if the animals in the 37th year (1998) survived to 1999. We obtained yearly means to avoid variation introduced by differences among years.

Second-order polynomials were calculated for each site using the weighted average of annual survivorship and net reproductive rate as dependent variables and mean matriline size as the independent variable over 36 years for each habitat patch (= site). In subsequent analyses, survivorship and net reproductive rate (Ro) were determined for each year for each matriline size (15) over all habitat sites and the average per year determined, and second-order polynomials were calculated. We also calculated gross reproductive rate (GRR, mx) over all years and sites for each matriline size.

Because large (≥4) matrilines are short-lived, we predicted that per capita reproduction should increase when the matrilines became smaller and that the increase should be greater among those females that lived in those matrilines with low Ro. Therefore, we divided the matrilines into two groups, more productive and less productive. Initially there were 15 matrilines; however, one matriline had no resident male the following year and was removed from the analysis, which left 14 large matrilines that were divided into two groups of seven each. The more productive matrilines were those in which 50% or more of the females weaned litters in the first year, and the less productive group consisted of matrilines in which 25% or fewer of the females weaned litters the first year. We compared the same females in the two sequential years.

Results

The number of matrilines used in the analysis varied among sites and mean matriline size among sites varied from 1.17 to 2.24 (Table 1). The total number of matrilines was 544 (Table 2). Fitness as measured by survivorship and net reproductive rate varied among sites (Fig. 1). Survivorship and net reproductive rates increased and then declined with mean matriline size. Apparently site quality affected mean matriline size and the level of a resident's fitness. Although we could not directly index site quality, factors known to affect it are length of snow cover (21), number of burrows, area, angle of vision, and mean distance to trees (22). Because area could be a surrogate for quality, e.g., larger areas could have more resources such as food and burrow sites, we tested for the effect of area by Spearman rank correlation. Ro was not significantly related to area (rs = 0.035, P > 0.5) but survivorship was nearly so (rs = 0.45, 0.1 > P > 0.05). Because the number of females on a habitat patch is positively correlated with area (rs = 0.54, 0.05 > P > 0.02), the greater number of females on a patch may provide increased detection of predators. The lack of significant area effects suggests that differences among sites in fitness are a consequence of differences in habitat quality unrelated to area.

Table 1.

Mean matriline size for each site (= colony)

Site n Mean size SE 95% C.I.
Copper Creek 48 1.17 0.054 ± 0.109
Beaver 22 1.27 0.097 ± 0.201
North Picnic 89 1.34 0.064 ± 0.127
Bend 27 1.52 0.098 ± 0.201
Marmot Meadow 66 1.65 0.098 ± 0.196
Upper Picnic 98 1.82 0.111 ± 0.220
Cliff 40 1.90 0.147 ± 0.297
Gothic Townsite 65 1.98 0.100 ± 0.199
Lower Picnic 106 2.05 0.088 ± 0.175
Boulder 59 2.08 0.178 ± 0.356
River 174 2.24 0.100 ± 0.197

n = number of females. 

Table 2.

The relationship between matriline size and gross reproductive rate (GRR)

Matriline size n Mean GRR 95% C.I.
1 395 0.82 ± 0.125
2 109 1.01 ± 0.168
3 26 1.32 ± 0.428
4 12 1.25 ± 0.473
5 2 0.20 ± 0.861

Figure 1.

Figure 1

The relationship between survivorship, net reproductive rate, and matriline size by site. The relationships are described by significant second order polynomials. For survivorship, Y = −0.553 + 1.248X − 0.311X2; P = 0.006, R2 = 0.67; for Ro, Y = −2.388 + 3.281X − 0.865X2; P = 0.018, R2 = 0.59. The names of the 12 sites where matrilines were observed are abbreviated on the figure: CC, Copper Creek; BR, Beaver Talus; NP, North Picnic; RA, River Annex; BD, Bend; MM, Marmot Meadow; UP, Upper Picnic; CL, Cliff; GO, Gothic Townsite; LP, Lower Picnic; BO, Boulder; RI, River.

Although area and the number of resident adult females are correlated, area is not correlated with either the mean number (rs = 0.39, 0.2 > P > 0.1) or the mean size (rs = 0.36, 0.2 > P > 0.1) of matrilines. However, the mean number of resident females is significantly correlated with the mean number (rs = 0.797, 0.005 > P > 0.0005) and the mean size (rs = 0.601, 0.025 > P > 0.01) of matrilines. However, mean size and mean number of matrilines are not correlated (rs = 0.084, P > 0.5). Two different processes affect matrilineal number and size: matrilineal size increases when daughters are retained in their natal group and number of matrilines increases because of immigration and the fission of large matrilines (14, 15, 17, 18). Fission of large matrilines is much more important than immigration in those habitat patches that typically support more than one matriline (16). The rank order of sites for survivorship is not significantly correlated (rs = 0.426, 0.1 > P > 0.05) with the rank order of sites for Ro, which suggests that resources and social organization act somewhat differently on these two measures of fitness.

The role of social organization was further explored by examining the effects of matriline size on fitness. First, we looked at the relationship between matriline size and GRR. The relationship between GRR and matriline size (Table 2) was similar to that for the relationship between Ro and matriline size (Fig. 2). The relationship between mean GRR and matriline size was not statistically significant (1-way ANOVA, F4,539 = 1.96, P = 0.099), but the relationship was described by a significant polynomial equation (y = 0.393 + 0.494X − 0.078X2, P = 0.044.). GRR increased with increasing matriline size and then decreased. The relationship was also curvilinear for both survivorship and Ro (Fig. 2). Survivorship was less affected by matriline size, whereas Ro was strongly affected. Of particular interest is the effect of small and large matrilines on Ro. Matrilines of one had reduced Ro for two major reasons. First, immigrants form matrilines of one; of 77 immigrant adult females, only 20 (26%) reproduced in their year of immigration. Immigration was highest at North Picnic and Copper Creek colonies with low mean group size and low Ro (Fig. 1). Second, matrilines of one reproduced significantly less often than expected when they were contiguous with a matriline of two or more (15). A matriline of one may persist for several years; as a consequence, reproductive output of immigrants measured as mean number of female young (0.753 ± 0.09, n = 216) did not differ significantly (t = 0.71, P = 0.24) from that of philopatric females (0.849 ± 0.15, n = 140) when each group formed matrilines of one.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

The relationship between survivorship, net reproductive rate, and matriline size; n, number of years in which that matriline size occurred. Vertical bars indicate the 95% confidence intervals of the mean. For survivorship, Y = 0.460 + 0.179X −0.025X2, P = 0.008, R2 = 0.97. Differences among means were tested by one-way ANOVA, F4,93 = 2.75, P = 0.033; prior/posterior tests did not identify any group size as different from another. For Ro, Y = 0.158 + 0.751X −0.122X2, P = 0.0028, R2 = 0.12. One-way ANOVA (F4,93 = 4.44, P = 0.0025) was significant and a LSD test reveals that Ro was significantly lower for matrilines of 5 than for matrilines of 3.

The decline of Ro in matrilines of four and five suggests that females in large matrilines did not benefit equally and that some females incurred a reproductive cost in matrilines of four or five that was greater than such costs in smaller matrilines. Only two matrilines of five occurred; in one a 6-year-old female was associated with four 2-year-old daughters, in the other a 4-year-old female was associated with four 2-year-old daughters and nieces. Of these 10 females, only the 4-year-old weaned a litter. No reproduction occurred in one of 13 matrilines of four. This matriline consisted of four 2-year-old sisters living next to a matriline of two. The 6-year-old reproductive female of the smaller matriline frequently chased the 2-year-old females, who were either maternal half-sisters or nieces of the 6-year-old female. Two-year-old females are less likely to reproduce when living with older females than when living alone or with other 2-year-old females (4, 11). However, GRR (0.46 ± 0.14, n = 51) of 2-year-old females in matrilines of one did not differ significantly (t = 0.63, df = 182, P = 0.27) from GRR (0.57 ± 0.91, n = 135) of 2-year-old females living in matrilines of two to five. Only 2 of the 15 large (≥4) matrilines persisted as large matrilines into the following year; none persisted longer than that. The number of reproductive females in these two matrilines of four did not differ between the 2 years. The 13 remaining matrilines were reduced in size the following year by mortality (15 animals presumably died in hibernation), dispersal (one female moved to a nearby habitat patch where she formed a matriline of 1), and matrilineal fission (4 females formed matrilines of 1, and 2 littermate sisters formed a matriline of 2).

If reproduction of some females is suppressed in large matrilines, then in the following year with reduced matriline sizes, we predict improved reproductive performance. In the first year, 43.1% of the females weaned litters; there were 1.97 young per resident female. In the following year, reproduction improved: 59.1% of the females weaned litters that produced 2.43 young per resident female. The difference between years in the percentage of reproductive females was not statistically significant (G = 2.57, P = 0.11). However, we reasoned that the improvement should have occurred primarily among females that initially were in matrilines of low Ro. Therefore, we compared the weaning success of females who were in the more productive group (50% or more of the females weaned litters in the first year) with those in the less productive group (25% or fewer of the females weaned litters the first year). By definition, the more productive matrilines weaned litters at a much higher rate than the less productive matrilines in the first year (71.4% vs. 16.7%, G = 9.38, P = 0.002). In the following year, there was no significant difference in reproductive rate (more productive matrilines, 63.6%; less productive matrilines, 54.5%; G = 0.44, P = 0.49). The proportion of females reproducing each year in the more productive matrilines did not differ significantly (G = 0.40, P = 0.45), whereas the proportion of reproductive females significantly increased in the less productive matrilines (G = 8.36, P = 0.003). The increase in weaning by females in the less productive matrilines cannot be attributed to whether they reproduced the previous year. Of 24 females that reproduced the first year, 15 reproduced the second year; of 23 females that were barren the first year, 11 reproduced the second year and 12 remained barren. Thus, reproductive success in one year does not necessarily increase or decrease reproductive success the next year (G = 1.02, P ≫ 0.1). Movement out of large matrilines enhanced reproductive performance. Six of the seven females weaned litters in the year of movement; only two reproduced the previous year (G = 5.0, P = 0.025). All seven females survived to the year after movement; five weaned litters.

Discussion

The optimal group size is three, but matrilines of three occur only 4.7% of the time, and the overall mean matriline size is 1.38. Why is the optimal group size so uncommon? The main reason seems to be habitat availability. Small sites never develop large matrilines. For example, at Copper Creek and Beaver, only three matrilines as large as two occurred at each site. Twenty-two of the 40 large matrilines (≥3) occurred at Lower Picnic, River, and Marmot Meadow. These sites are among the larger in area, which is a necessary but not sufficient condition for the development of large matrilines. These three sites also have numerous burrow sites (22) and large meadows with abundant forage (23, 24). Thus, resources at these sites are sufficient to support a large population of yellow-bellied marmots, which is evident in the average number of matrilines and mean size of matrilines at these sites in comparison to other sites (17). By contrast, the widely spaced, small resource patches at North Picnic, the largest site, result in a large number of small matrilines (17).

Although matriline size significantly affected reproductive success, there was great variation (Fig. 2). Some of the variation is a consequence of habitat differences; matrilines of similar size have markedly different Ro values (Figs. 1 and 2). The low Ro of matrilines of five and of many matrilines of four (Fig. 2) suggest that competition was more severe in large matrilines. Why would a yearling female join a large matriline with the prospect of poor reproductive success? The alternative is to disperse as a yearling, but mortality of dispersing yearlings is significantly higher than that of yearling residents, and there is no future compensatory decrease in the age of first reproduction or increase in the frequency of reproduction (25). Because Ro is higher in matrilines of 2, 3, and 4 than in a matriline of 1, philopatric females may gain indirect fitness. For example, they may increase survivorship of infant kin by alarm-calling when predators are present. However, 42% of the variation in alarm-calling was a function of whether the caller was a female with emergent pups. Measures of indirect fitness were not significantly related to alarm calling (26). Nor is there any evidence for social thermoregulation in this species at these study sites (3) nor any evidence that allogrooming removes external parasites. Alternatively, philopatric females (those remaining in their natal site) may increase their direct fitness; by foregoing dispersal, they increase the probability of reaching reproductive age, and they can escape reproductive suppression either by moving away from competitors and budding off a new matriline or by waiting for mortality to reduce the size of the matriline and, hence, the number of competitors. Philopatry, movement, alarm calling, and social interactions between and within matrilines (15, 27) strongly suggest that females act to enhance direct fitness and that any indirect fitness gains are a by-product of a direct fitness strategy.

Acknowledgments

The field work was performed at the Rocky Mountain Biological Laboratory, Colorado. We thank the National Science Foundation for grants to K.B.A. and UNI for a sabbatical leave for O.A.S. We thank D. Blumstein, J. Hoogland, G. Michener, P. Sherman, and two anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.

Abbreviation

GRR

gross reproductive rate

Footnotes

This paper was submitted directly (Track II) to the PNAS office.

Article published online before print: Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 10.1073/pnas.200196097.

Article and publication date are at www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.200196097

References

  • 1.Alexander R D. Annu Rev Ecol Syst. 1974;5:325–383. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Sherman P W, Lacy E A, Reeve H K, Keller L. Behav Ecol. 1995;6:102–108. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Blumstein D T, Armitage K B. Oikos. 1999;84:369–382. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Armitage K B. J Mamm. 1998;79:385–393. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Armitage K B. J Mamm. 1999;80:1–10. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Blumstein D T, Armitage K B. Behav Ecol. 1998;9:8–19. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Solomon N G, French J A, editors. Cooperative Breeding in Mammals. Cambridge, U.K.: Cambridge Univ. Press; 1997. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hoogland J L. The Black-Tailed Prairie Dog. Chicago: Univ. of Chicago Press; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Stearns S C. The Evolution of Life Histories. Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Frase B A, Hoffmann R S. Mamm Species. 1980;135:1–8. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Schwartz O A, Armitage K B, Van Vuren D. J Zool. 1998;246:337–346. [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Armitage K B. In: Biodiversité chez les Marmottes/Biodiversity in Marmots. LeBerre M, Ramousse R, LeGuelte L, editors. Moscow/Lyon: International Marmot Network; 1996. pp. 113–128. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Brody A K, Melcher J. Anim Behav. 1984;33:673–674. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Armitage K B. Ethol Ecol Evol. 1989;1:111–121. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Armitage K B. In: Ecological Aspects of Social Evolution. Rubenstein D S, Wrangham R W, editors. Princeton: Princeton Univ. Press; 1986. pp. 303–331. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Armitage K B. Annu Rev Ecol Syst. 1991;22:379–407. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Armitage K B. In: The Ecology of Social Behavior. Slobodchikoff C N, editor. New York: Academic; 1988. pp. 131–155. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Armitage K B. In: Biology of Ground-Dwelling Squirrels. Murie J O, Michener G R, editors. Lincoln: Univ. Nebraska Press; 1984. pp. 377–403. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Armitage K B. J Zool. 1974;172:233–265. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Salsbury C M, Armitage K B. Can J Zool. 1995;73:1791–1797. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Van Vuren D, Armitage K B. Can J Zool. 1991;69:1755–1758. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Svendsen G E. Ecology. 1974;55:760–771. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Kilgore D L, Jr, Armitage K B. Ecology. 1978;59:78–88. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Frase B A, Armitage K B. Ethol Ecol Evol. 1989;1:353–366. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Van Vuren D, Armitage K B. Oikos. 1994;69:179–181. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Blumstein D T, Steinmetz J, Armitage K B, Daniel J C. Anim Behav. 1997;53:173–184. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Armitage K B. In: First International Symposium on Alpine Marmot and on Genus Marmota. Bassano B, Durio P, Gallo Orsi V, Macchi E, editors. Epidemiologia ed Ecologia, Torino, Italy: Dipartimento di Produzioni Animali; 1992. pp. 89–94. [Google Scholar]

Articles from Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America are provided here courtesy of National Academy of Sciences

RESOURCES