Skip to main content
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2008 Jan 11.
Published in final edited form as: J Res Adolesc. 2007 Mar;17(1):117–144. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2007.00514.x

TABLE 2.

Hierarchical Regressions Predicting Contextual Correlates of Two Dimensions of Positive Peer Regard Among Suburban and Urban Adolescents

    Perceived Admiration
Social Preference
  Step β sri2 β sri2
Demographics 1   .01     .01  
Gendera   .01   .00 .01   .00
Race/ethnicityb   −.06   .00 .08   .00
School   .05   .00 .17   .00
Peer regard 2   .24***     .24***  
Positive peer regardc   .16   .02*** .30   .03***
Student attributes 3   .43***     .18***  
Grades   .01   .00 −.05   .00
Academic application   .17   .02*** .01   .00
Academic disengagement   .06   .00 −.09   .00
Aggression   .15   .01** −.37   .06***
Substance use   −.01   .00 .04   .00
Delinquency   .02   .00 .00   .00
Athletic ability   .14   .01** .19   .02***
Physical attractiveness   .59   .18*** .27   .02***
Two-way interactions 4   .02     .02  
Three-way interactions 5   .04***     .02  
Gender × context × academic application
  Substance Use   .08   .01*      
  Athletic Ability   −.42   .02**      
  Physical Attractiveness   .10   .02*      
Total R²     .74***     .48***  
a

Girl=1, boy=0.

b

White=1, ethnic minority=0.

c

This variable is the dimension of positive peer regard not being considered as an outcome.

*

p<.05

**

p<.01

***

p<.001.

Note. As recommended by Aiken and West (1991), (a) interaction terms in these analyses involve centered variables, (b) β values for main effects are standardized, and (c) β values for interaction effects are unstandardized. Only those interactions that showed significant effects in one or both analyses are presented.