Table 3.
Using extreme group membership to predict choice impulsivity in the MIDA task (boys only).
Extreme group (top 5%) |
n (total) | IRR1 | SE2 | p | IRR 95% CI3 | Discrete change %points (rewards)4 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Post-reward delay condition | ||||||
ADHD-IN 5 | 32(542) | 1.15 | 0.28 | .57 | (0.71, 1.84) | 1.94 (0.39) |
ADHD-H/I 6 | 25(542) | 1.05 | 0.30 | .87 | (0.60, 1.84) | 0.64 (0.13) |
ADHD-CO 7 | 19(542) | 1.37 | 0.35 | .21 | (0.83, 2.26) | 4.88 (0.98) |
No post-reward delay condition | ||||||
ADHD-IN 5 | 32(542) | 1.45 | 0.22 | .015 | (1.07, 1.97) | 12.33 (2.47) |
ADHD-H/I 6 | 25(542) | 0.99 | 0.21 | .98 | (0.66, 1.51) | −0.14 (−0.03) |
ADHD-CO 7 | 19(542) | 1.32 | 0.24 | .12 | (0.93, 1.88) | 8.95 (1.79) |
Note: Negative binomial regression models have been used to examine whether extreme group membership can predict choice impulsivity under each task condition.
Incidence rate ratio.
Robust standard error.
Confidence intervals.
Expected change in preference for smaller-immediate rewards with extreme group membership expressed in percentage points or raw rewards for a typical participant.
ADHD-Inattentive extreme group.
ADHD-Hyperactive/impulsive extreme group.
ADHD-Combined subtype extreme group. MIDA: Maudsley Index of Childhood Delay Aversion.