Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2010 Nov 1.
Published in final edited form as: Stroke. 2009 Sep 17;40(11):3563–3566. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.109.561969

Severity of Hypoperfusion in Distinct Brain Regions Predicts Severity of Hemispatial Neglect in Different Reference Frames

Peyman Shirani 1,2, Julia Thorn 3, Cameron Davis 4, Jennifer Heidler-Gary 4, Melissa Newhart 4, Rebecca F Gottesman 4, Argye E Hillis 3,4,5
PMCID: PMC2790042  NIHMSID: NIHMS153934  PMID: 19762699

Abstract

Background and Purpose

Hemispatial neglect is among the most common and disabling consequences of right hemisphere stroke. A variety of variables have been associated with the presence or severity of neglect, but have not evaluated the independent effects of location, severity, and volume of ischemia. Few have determined areas involved in different types of neglect. We identified the contributions of these variables to severity of viewer-centered versus stimulus-centered neglect in acute ischemic right hemisphere stroke.

Methods

We studied 137 patients within 24 hours of stroke onset with MR diffusionand perfusion-weighted imaging and a test of hemispatial neglect that distinguishes between viewer-centered and stimulus-centered neglect. Using multivariable linear regression, we identified the independent contributions of severity of ischemia in specific locations, volume of ischemia, and age in accounting for severity of each neglect type.

Results

Severity of hypoperfusion in angular gyrus was the only variable that significantly and independently contributed to severity of viewer-centered neglect. Volume of dysfunctional tissue and hypoperfusion in posterior frontal cortex also accounted for some variability in severity of viewer-centered neglect. Severity of hypoperfusion of superior temporal cortex was the only variable that independently and significantly contributed to severity of stimulus-centered neglect.

Conclusions/Relevance

Location, severity, and volume of ischemia together determine the type and severity of neglect after right hemisphere stroke. Results also show that perfusion-weighted MRI can be used as a semi-quantitative measure of tissue dysfunction in acute stroke and can account for substantial proportion of the variability in functional deficits in the acute stage.


Hemispatial neglect is a common and debilitating consequence of right hemisphere stroke. The severity of hemispatial neglect depends on volume of infarct, age, and degree of atrophy in the intact hemisphere, but not sex.14 Previous studies have shown that volume of tissue hypoperfusion (measured with perfusion weighted imaging time-to-peak, or TTP, maps) is even more strongly associated with severity of hemispatial neglect than is volume of infarct in acute stroke.57 Reduction in volume of hypoperfusion by restoring blood flow also correlates with degree of early recovery of neglect.8 These findings indicate that TTP maps represent dysfunctional brain tissue and can show areas where dysfunction results in neglect in acute stroke (irrespective of the more controversial issue of whether or not TTP maps are useful in predicting tissue that will progress to infarct). Severity of neglect may also depend on location of infarct or tissue dysfunction. Many studies have identified an association between hemispatial neglect and damage to inferior parietal cortex or temporoparietal junction.911 Others conclude superior temporal gyrus lesions are most common in patients with neglect.12 We previously found that the presence of ischemia in temporal, parietal, and frontal regions is associated with different forms of hemispatial neglect in acute stroke, distinguished by modality13 or by reference frame14. The presence of hypoperfusion of right parietal cortex was associated with left viewer-centered neglect (neglect of the contralesional side of the viewer), and the presence of hypoperfusion in right temporal cortex was associated with left stimulus-centered neglect (neglect of the contralesional side of individual stimuli, irrespective of the side of the viewer on which they are presented).14,15

Complementary evidence for the role of the parietal lobe in viewer-centered representations and the temporal lobe in stimulus-centered spatial representations would be obtained if severity of dysfunction in parietal cortex predicted severity of viewer-centered neglect and severity of dysfunction in temporal cortex predicted severity of stimulus-centered neglect after acute ischemic stroke. This hypothesis can be tested by identifying areas where the severity of ischemia is independently associated with severity of each type of neglect. Previous studies have indicated that severity of language impairment reflects severity of hypoperfusion in left temporal cortex in the first 24 hours of stroke.16 After the acute stage, chronic hypoperfusion can lead to reorganization of structure/function relationships.17,18 Therefore, we evaluated the association between severity of each type of neglect and severity of hypoperfusion in dorsal and ventral regions of interest in the first 24 hours of stroke onset, before substantial reorganization. We also evaluated the independent contributions of locations and volumes of tissue dysfunction and age in predicting severity of each type of neglect.

Methods

Participants

We studied a consecutive series of 137 consenting patients with right hemisphere acute ischemic stroke who had neglect testing, Diffusion Weighted Imaging (DWI), and Perfusion Weighted Imaging (PWI) within 24 hours. This study involved a new population, compared to our previously published studies; and viewer-centered and stimulus-centered neglect were defined somewhat differently (using a single test in this study, as described below). Exclusion criteria included: reduced level of consciousness (as indicated by the ability to understand the task directions and to remain awake and responsive throughout the task), left-handedness (scores of <0.70 on Edinburgh Handedness Scale), previous neurological disease or uncorrected visual loss, sedation, or hemorrhage. All patients provided informed consent for the study, using methods and consent forms approved by the Johns Hopkins Institutional Review Board.

Imaging

MRI sequences included: axial T2, fluid-attenuated inversion recovery, gradient echo, diffusion-weighted images (DWI) trace images, apparent diffusion coefficient (ADC) maps, dynamic contrast perfusion-weighted images (PWI), and magnetic resonance angiogram of the circle of Willis. The reported analyses used DWI (after acuity of the lesion was confirmed on ADC maps) and PWI (co registered to T2 to provide anatomical boundaries less visible on PWI). DWI and PWI scans were 5 mm thick, with whole-brain coverage. Severity of hypoperfusion in various regions was determined on time-to-peak (TTP) maps, using ImageJ (http://rsb.info.nih.gov/ij). A trained technician without knowledge of the neglect test results outlined 8 regions of interest (ROIs) in each hemisphere, and determined the mean delay in TTP in each right hemisphere ROI relative to the homologous region in the left (normal) hemisphere. Areas that were entirely bright on DWI and dark on ADC maps were assigned a relative delay in TTP of 7 sec, because previous studies have indicated that a relative delay in TTP of 7 seconds is equivalent to infarct, in terms of total dysfunction. 18. Delays of < 7 sec appear to be associated with less severe dysfunction, at least in language cortex.16.

ROIs included atlas-based definitions of BA 6, 7, 9, 22, 37, 39, 40, and 44/45.19 We recognize that atlas-defined Brodmann’s areas do not necessarily reflect underlying cytoarchitecture, due to individual variability in the cytoarchitectural fields20; but are widely recognized as general cortical regions that may have associated functions and allow a reproducible methodology. We use BA numbers to provide the reader information about the location within the frontal, parietal, or temporal designated as each ROI by referring to the published template. This information makes the study reproducible; we do not mean to imply that a particular cytoarchitecture was present in that ROI in any given individual. These ROIs were selected because they have been previously implicated in neglect915 or because they have shown activation in functional imaging of spatial attention.21

Neglect Testing

Patients were given a battery of tests of hemispatial neglect, described previously.4,16 However, for this study we analyzed performance only a gap detection test that can distinguish between viewer-centered and stimulus-centered neglect in a single test with a single set of directions.22 In this test, 30 circles are presented on a page: 10 with left gaps, 10 with right gaps, and 10 without gaps. Patients are instructed to circle the complete circles and draw an “X” through any circles with gaps. Severity of viewer-centered neglect was measured as percent of circles, with or without gaps, that were ignored on the left sides of the pages (including any circle to the left of the left most marked stimulus). Severity of stimulus-centered neglect was measured as total percent of circles with left gaps that were marked as complete circles (failure to detect left gaps), irrespective of the side of the page. Among 59 healthy control subjects, with a mean age of 64 (range 42–81 years), there were no errors made on this test (unpublished data).

Statistical analysis

First, stepwise linear regression analysis determined ROIs where degree of hypoperfusion/infarct was independently associated with severity of each neglect type. The alpha level to include a ROI was p<.05; alpha level to exclude a ROI was p> 0.1. We evaluated for collinearity by checking the variance inflation factor (VIF) for included and excluded factors. We then carried out multivariable regression to identify which variables independently contributed to severity of each type of neglect, by entering together severity of hypoperfusion/infarct in all ROIs (for all 137 patients), total volume of tissue dysfunction, and age. Total volume of tissue dysfunction was determined by measuring the total area of dense ischemia on DWI and/or hypoperfusion on PWI defined as > 4 sec delay in TTP compared to the homologous voxels in the left hemisphere.

Results

Age ranged from 31 to 90 years (mean = 62.9 ± 14.0); 50.8 % were female. Education ranged from 3 to 20 years (mean = 12.0 ± 3.1). Of the 137 patients, 22 (16.1 %) had some degree of viewer-centered neglect. Scores ranged from 7% to 100 % (mean = 55.7 ± 27.7%) errors in detecting stimuli on the left side of the page. A total of 25 (18.2 %) patients had some degree of stimulus-centered neglect. Scores ranged from 10% errors to 90% (mean = 23.05 ± 19 %) errors in detecting left gaps in stimuli on the both sides of the page. Patients with viewer-centered neglect and those with stimulus-centered neglect did not differ in terms of volume of dysfunctional tissue (80.6 ± 85.3 vs. 67.6 ± 77.8 cc) or age (67.0 ± 12.3 vs. 69.2 ± 13.1 years). Stepwise linear regression revealed that right BA 39 (angular gyrus) was the area where relative delay in TTP or infarct most strongly predicted severity of viewer-centered neglect, independently of delay in TTP in other regions (t=4.1; p<.0001). This variable alone accounted for a small amount of the variability (r2= 0.16) in severity of stimulus-centered neglect (r=.40; F=16.8; p<0.0001). When we regressed severity of hypoperfusion/infarct in each area along with total volume of dysfunctional tissue and age, the following model accounted for more of the variability (r2 =.38) in severity of viewer-centered neglect (where BA represents severity of hypoperfusion in each right hemisphere Brodmann area):

SevEgoNeg=BA9(.76)+BA39(.37)+BA44/45(.21)+voltissuedysfunction(.06)+age(.000076)+BA37(.10)BA22(.14)BA40(.24)BA6(.46)BA7(.53).F=4.2;r=.61(p<0.0001)

The only variables that were positively correlated with viewer-centered neglect severity were delay in TTP in BA 9 (dorsal prefrontal cortex), BA 39 (angular gyrus), BA 44/45 (inferior frontal cortex), age, and total volume of dysfunctional tissue. Relative delay in TTP in other regions included in the model was negatively correlated with severity of viewer-centered neglect.

In contrast, using stepwise linear regression, right BA 22 (superior temporal cortex) was the only ROI where relative delay in TTP or infarct predicted severity of stimulus-centered neglect, measured by percentage of left gaps missed, independently of delay in TTP of other ROIs (t=3.2; p=0.002). No other area showed a relationship between severity of hypoperfusion and severity of stimulus-centered neglect, after controlling for severity of hypoperfusion in right BA 22. However, this variable alone accounted for only a small proportion of the variability of severity of stimulus-centered neglect (r2=.10; F=9.99; p=0.002). But when we regressed severity of hypoperfusion/infarct in each area along with total volume of dysfunctional tissue and age, the following model accounted for more of the variability (r2=0.22) in severity of stimulus-centered neglect (where BA represents severity of hypoperfusion in each right hemisphere Brodmann area):

SevAlloNeglect=BA22(.31)+BA44/45(.07)+age(.02)+voldystissue(.01)BA37(.25)BA39(.13)BA6(.09)BA40(.05)BA9(.04).82

The only variable that was positively and significantly correlated with neglect severity independently of the other variables was delay in TTP or severity of hypoperfusion in BA 22 (p<0.001). Volume of dysfunctional tissue did not independently account for variability in severity of stimulus-centered neglect (p=0.06). Relative delay in TTP in other significantly associated regions was negatively correlated with severity of neglect (see Figure 1 for illustrative cases). In the above analyses, collinearity was acceptable, with a VIF of 1.113 to 3.010 for variables included in the models.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Examples of neglect performance and DWI (left) and TTP maps (right) of two patients. Case 1 has only allocentric neglect, with infarct and hypoperfusion within BA 22 (superior temporal gyrus; red arrows) and no infarct or hypoperfusion in BA 39 (angular gyrus; yellow arrows). Case 2 has mild allocentric neglect with hypoerfusion in BA 22 and egocentric neglect with hypoperfusion of BA 39.

Discussion

Results confirmed the hypothesis that severity of hypoperfusion (or infarct) in right parietal cortex (specifically, BA 39) predicted severity of viewer-centered neglect; while severity of hypoperfusion within more ventral right cortical areas (specifically, BA 22) predicted severity of stimulus-centered neglect. We also found that hypoperfusion of right posterior frontal cortex, BA 44/45 and BA 9 independently contributed to the severity of viewer-centered neglect. Neglect due to right frontal regions has been previously described after stroke and may form a part of a dorsal network of spatial attention critical for modulating attention in an viewer-centered reference frame.10 The localization of the different types of neglect is similar to those of previous studies14,15, although the precise BAs are not identical. BAs vary across individuals, but those implicated in viewer-centered neglect are reliably more dorsal (frontal and parietal), while those implicated in stimulus-centered neglect are more ventral (temporal) across studies. Results are consistent with the hypothesis that viewer-centered spatial representations are processed in parietal (and frontal) cortex, while stimulus-centered representations are processed more ventrally, in temporal cortex.

The role of right BA 22 in neglect is controversial. Karnath and colleagues found it to be the cortical region where infarct was most associated with neglect;12 but others report that chronic lesions in angular gyrus are more strongly associated with neglect11,14, although ischemia in right BA 22 is associated with stimulus-centered but not viewer-centered neglect.14 The current results provide novel evidence for this last hypothesis. Results confirm an essential role of posterior parietal cortex for representing where an object is relative to the viewer and how to respond to it, and an essential role of posterior temporal cortex for stimulus-centered representations necessary for identifying objects. Correlational evidence for these distinct roles of posterior parietal cortex and more ventral temporal cortex was provided in a PET study in normal subjects that showed bilateral temporo-occipital activation associated with object-centered processing and right posterior parietal activation associated with viewer-centered processing.23

The clinical importance of our results is two-fold. First, many patients with acute stroke are unable to have perfusion imaging, either because of unavailability of hardware, software, or technical expertise, or because contraindications to contrast or lack of intravenous access. Our results provide the basis for predicting the site and severity of hypoperfusion, which in combination with imaging of the completed infarct on DWI or CT, allows the clinician to estimate the location of dysfunctional, but potentially salvageable, tissue. This sort of information can be useful in clinical decision making.24 Secondly, stimulus-centered and viewer-centered neglect are likely to be amenable to different rehabilitation approaches. The site of dysfunctional tissue can therefore be useful information in planning rehabilitation of neglect. Patients with parietal lesions, who are more likely to have viewer-centered neglect, may respond more to treatment directed toward shifting the window of attention toward the contralesional side (eg prism adaptation). Patients with temporal infarcts are more likely to have stimulus-centered neglect and respond to treatment designed toward expanding the window of attention toward individual stimuli (irrespective of their location relative to the viewer), so that the entire stimulus is processed.

Finally, our results demonstrate that TTP maps can be used as semi-quantitative indications of tissue dysfunction, whether or not they predict the risk of the tissue proceeding to infarct (a more controversial topic). Although maps of regional cerebral blood flow, calculated with appropriate territorial arterial input functions are likely to provide more precise and quantitative measures of tissue dysfunction25, software for computing TTP maps is more widely available. Our study shows that delay in TTP in particular brain regions is correlated with severity of functional deficits associated with those regions.

There are also limitations of this study. First, we elected to analyze results by BAs where ischemia predicted each type of neglect, rather than using a voxel-based approach, because this ROI analysis allowed us to identify significant lesion-deficit associations, even after correcting for multiple comparisons, with a relatively small population. We recognize the individual variability in the precise location of cytoarchitecture (BAs) in the brain. However, almost certainly, the cytoarchitecture rather than the location or voxel itself is related to function, so it is reasonable to estimate location of BAs. Additionally, mean delay in TTP in a region of interest must be compared to the mean TTP in the homologous region in the opposite hemisphere. A single voxel is too small to serve as the ROI, because brains are not precisely symmetrical. A more important limitation of our study is that it was based on subjects whose location of tissue dysfunction was a result of poor perfusion. The cortex is not homogeneous with respect to its vulnerability to ischemia.26 Only areas where there are sufficient numbers of patients who have ischemia (as well as those who do not) will have the power to show a significant association with the deficit. It is likely that more ventral areas of the temporal cortex are associated with stimulus-centered neglect, but these areas (eg BA 20, 21) are less commonly ischemic. Nevertheless, this study provides evidence for the broad conclusion that severity of tissue dysfunction in the right parietal and frontal cortex and volume of tissue dysfunction together account for severity of viewer-centered neglect; and severity of tissue dysfunction in the right temporal cortex accounts for severity of stimulus-centered neglect in acute ischemic stroke.

References

  • 1.Gottesman RF, Kleinman JT, Davis C, Heidler-Gary J, Newhart M, Hillis AE. The NIHSS-plus: improving cognitive assessment with the NIHSS. Behavioural Neurology. doi: 10.3233/BEN-2009-0259. (in press) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Gottesman RF, Kleinman JT, Davis C, Heidler-Gary J, Newhart M, Kannan V, Hillis AE. Unilateral neglect is more severe and common in older patients with right hemispheric stroke. Neurology. 2008;71:1439–1444. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000327888.48230.d2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Levine DN, Warach JD, Benowitz L, Calvanio R. Left spatial neglect: effects of lesion size and premorbid brain atrophy on severity and recovery following right cerebral infarction. Neurology. 1986;36:362–366. doi: 10.1212/wnl.36.3.362. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Kleinman JT, Gottesman RF, Davis C, Newhart M, Heidler-Gary J, Hillis AE. Gender differences in unilateral spatial neglect within 24 hours of ischemic stroke. Brain and Cognition. 2008;68:49–52. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.02.122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Hillis AE, Barker P, Beauchamp N, Gordon B, Wityk R. MR perfusion imaging reveals regions of hypoperfusion associated with aphasia and neglect. Neurology. 2000;55:782–788. doi: 10.1212/wnl.55.6.782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Karnath HO, Zopf R, Johannsen L, Berger MF, Nagele T, Klose U. Normalized perfusion MRI to identify common areas of dysfunction: patients with basal ganglia neglect. Brain. 2005;128:2462–2469. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh629. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Leibovitch FS, Black SE, Caldwell CE, McIntosh AR, Ehrlich LE, Szalai JP. Brain SPECT imaging and left hemispatial neglect covaried using partial least squares: The Sunnybrook Stroke Study. Human Brain Mapping. 1999;7:244–253. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)7:4&#x0003c;244::AID-HBM3&#x0003e;3.0.CO;2-K. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hillis AE, Wityk RJ, Barker PB, Ulatowski JA, Jacobs MA. Change in perfusion in acute nondominant hemisphere stroke may be better estimated by tests of hemispatial neglect than by the National Institutes of Health Stroke Scale. Stroke. 2003;34:2392–2396. doi: 10.1161/01.STR.0000089681.84041.69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Vallar G, Perani D. The anatomy of unilateral neglect after right hemisphere stroke lesions. A clinical CT scan correlation study in man. Neuropsychologia. 1986;24:609–622. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(86)90001-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Heilman KM, Watson RT, Valenstein E. Neglect and related disorders. In: Heilman KM, Valenstein E, editors. Clinical Neuropsychology. London: Oxford University Press; 1993. pp. 279–336. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Mort DJ, Malhotra P, Mannan SK, Rorden C, Pambakian A, Kennard C, Husain M. The anatomy of visual neglect. Brain. 2003;126:1986–1997. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Karnath HO, Ferber S, Himmelbach M. Spatial awareness is a function of the temporal not the posterior parietal lobe. Nature. 2001;411:950–953. doi: 10.1038/35082075. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Hillis AE, Chang S, Heidler-Gary J, Newhart M, Kleinman JT, Davis C, Barker PB, Aldrich E, Ken L. Neural correlates of modality-specific spatial extinction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2006;18:1889–1898. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.11.1889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hillis AE, Newhart M, Heidler J, Barker PB, Degaonkar M. Anatomy of spatial attention: insights from perfusion imaging and hemispatial neglect in acute stroke. Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:3161–3167. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4468-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Medina J, Kannan V, Pawlak M, Kleinman JT, Newhart M, Davis C, Heidler-Gary JE, Herskovits EH, Hillis AE. Neural substrates of visuospatial processing in distinct reference frames: evidence from unilateral spatial neglect. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2008;71:1439–1444. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.21160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Hillis AE, Wityk RJ, Tuffiash E, Beauchamp NJ, Jacobs MA, Barker PB, Selnes OA. Hypoperfusion of Wernicke’s area predicts severity of semantic deficit in acute stroke. Annals of Neurology. 2001;50:561–566. doi: 10.1002/ana.1265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Marshall RS, Lazar RM, Pile-Spellman J, Young WL, Duong DH, Joshi S, Ostapkovich N. Recovery of brain function during induced cerebral hypoperfusion. Brain. 2001;124:1208–1217. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.6.1208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Prabhakaran V, Raman S, Grunwald M, Mahadevia A, Hussain N, Lu H, Van zijl PCM, Hillis AE. Neural substrates of word generation during stroke recovery: the influence of cortical hypoperfusion. Behavioral Neurology. 2007;18:45–52. doi: 10.1155/2007/430402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Damasio H, Damasio A. Lesion analysis in neuropsychology. New York: Oxford University Press; 1989. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Amunts K, Schleicher A, Burgel U, Mohlberg H, Uylings HBM, Zilles K. Broca’s Region Revisited: Cytoarchitecture and Intersubject Variability. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1999;412:319–341. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19990920)412:2<319::aid-cne10>3.0.co;2-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Corbetta M, Kincade MJ, Lewis C, Snyder AZ, Sapir A. Neural basis and recovery of spatial attention deficits in spatial neglect. Nature Neuroscience. 2005;8:1603–1610. doi: 10.1038/nn1574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Ota H, Fujii T, Suzuki K, Fukatsu R, Yamadori A. Dissociation of body-centered and stimulus-centered representations in unilateral neglect. Neurology. 2001;57:2064–2069. doi: 10.1212/wnl.57.11.2064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Honda M, Wise SP, Weeks RA, Deiber MP, Hallett M. Cortical areas with enhanced activation during object-centered spatial information processing: A PET study. Brain. 1998;121:2145–2158. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.11.2145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Reineck L, Agarwal S, Hillis AE. The “diffusion-clinical mismatch” predicts early language recovery in acute stroke. Neurology. 2005;64:828–833. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000152983.52869.51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Ostergaard L, Weisskoff RM, Chesler DA, Gyldensted C, Rosen BR. High-resolution measurement of cerebral blood flow using intravascular bolus passages. Part I. Mathematical approach and statistical analysis. Magnetic Resonance Medicine. 1996;36:715–725. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910360510. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES